Abstract
PERK (EIF2AK3) is an ER-resident eIF2α kinase required for memory flexibility and metabotropic glutamate receptor-dependent long-term depression, processes known to be dependent on new protein synthesis. Here we investigated PERK’s role in working memory, a cognitive ability that is independent of new protein synthesis, but instead is dependent on cellular Ca2+ dynamics. We found that working memory is impaired in forebrain-specific Perk knockout and pharmacologically PERK-inhibited mice. Moreover, inhibition of PERK in wild-type mice mimics the fear extinction impairment observed in forebrain-specific Perk knockout mice. Our findings reveal a novel role of PERK in cognitive functions and suggest that PERK regulates both Ca2+ -dependent working memory and protein synthesis-dependent memory flexibility.
Introduction
Working memory is the cognitive capacity to actively and temporarily maintain information for the purpose of task execution [1]. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in primates, which is homologous to the medium prefrontal cortex in rodents [2, 3], is essential for working memory as evidenced by lesion studies [4], electrophysiological recordings [5] and brain imaging [6, 7]. At the cellular level, sustained neuronal firing was observed during the delay period of working memory, which is now considered an important neuronal correlate of working memory [8]. The molecular mechanisms underlying working memory have been studied extensively in recent years, and it has been shown that intracellular Ca2+ signaling as stimulated by muscarinic acetylcholine or metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR) is critical for working memory [9–14].
PERK, an eIF2α kinase, is well known for its role in eIF2α-dependent protein synthesis and translational control. Upon activation PERK phosphorylates the α subunit of the translation initiation factor eIF2, which can subsequently modulate protein translation in two opposite ways: repression of global protein synthesis [15] and induction of translation of specific genes including CREB2/ATF4 [16]. Since both de novo protein synthesis and CREB2 are key regulators of long-term memory storage [17, 18], PERK’s role in protein synthesis-dependent cognition has been comprehensively studied, where it has been shown that PERK is required for normal flexibility in learning and memory [19] and mGluR-dependent long-term depression [20].
Besides its role in cognition, the function of PERK has been most extensively studied in the pancreatic insulin-secreting β-cells where it regulates cell proliferation, proinsulin trafficking through the secretory pathway, and insulin secretion [21, 22]. Unexpectedly the mechanism of PERK-dependent regulation of insulin secretion was found to be independent of eIF2α phosphorylation and protein synthesis. By acutely inhibiting PERK kinase activity using a newly available pharmacological inhibitor, it was discovered that PERK regulates Ca2+ dynamics in β-cells [23], which underlies glucose-stimulated insulin secretion. Considering the critical role of Ca2+ signaling in learning and memory, we hypothesized that PERK might regulate working memory, which is independent of new protein synthesis, but is largely driven by Ca2+ dynamics. We show herein that PERK regulates working memory. Moreover, pharmacological PERK inhibition in wild-type mice mimics the memory flexibility impairment observed in Perk knockout mice. These findings illustrate a novel role of PERK in cognitive function, and suggest that PERK regulates both Ca2+ dynamics-dependent working memory and protein synthesis-dependent memory flexibility.
Materials and Methods
Mouse strains
BrPerk KO mice were generated by crossing Perk-floxed mice[24] with αCaMKII-Cre mice T29-1 strain [25] in C57BL/6J background. Adult BrPerk KO mice and their wild-type littermates aged 3–7 months old were used to ensure the maximum knockdown of PERK in the forebrain [26]. Wild-type mice used in the pharmacological PERK inhibition experiments were 3 month old, and in C57BL/6J background (purchased from the Jackson laboratory). All the animal procedures were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Penn State University (IACUC# 43379).
Brain tissue collection
To compare PERK knockdown efficiency in different brain regions, BrPerk KO mice and their wild-type littermates were euthanized by CO2 inhalation in accordance with the IACUC protocol approved by Penn State University. Different brain regions including prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and cerebellum were isolated for western blot analysis.
Western blot analysis
Protein lysates from different brain regions and whole cells were prepared using RIPA buffer with 1X protease inhibitor and 1X phosphatase inhibitor cocktails from Sigma. Denatured protein samples were generated by boiling in 2X Laemmli buffer for 5 min. NuPAGE 4–12% Bis-Tris Midi Gel (Thermo Fisher Scientific) was used for electrophoresis. To enable the comparison of PERK knockdown efficiency in different brain regions, protein quantification was performed on protein lysates from brain tissue using Peirce BCA protein assay kit (Thermo scientific, # 23227), and 50μg protein per sample was loaded for western blot. The following primary antibodies were used in western blot analysis: monoclonal anti-PERK produced in rabbit (1:500, cell signaling, #3192), monoclonal anti-β-actin produced in mouse (1:1000, GenScript, A00702), monoclonal anti-p-PERK produced in rabbit (1:500, cell signaling, #3179), polyclonal anti-eIF2α [pS52] produced in rabbit (1:1000, Invitrogen, 44728G), monoclonal anti-α-tubulin produced in mouse (1:1000, Sigma, T5168).
Gavage administration of PERK inhibitor GSK2606414
GSK2606414 was suspended in vehicle (0.5% HPMC+0.01% tween-80 in water with pH 4.0) at the concentration of 15mg/ml. Mice were gavaged with GSK2606414 at the dosage of 150mg/kg body weight or equivalent amount of vehicle during the experiment.
Spontaneous alternation Y-maze task
Spontaneous alternation Y-maze task was performed as described [27]. Each mouse was put in the middle of a symmetrical Y maze with three identical arms (arms 35 cm long X 16 cm high) and given free access to the arms during an 8 min period. The test was video-recorded and the results including percentage alternation and number of arms entered were blindly analyzed later. The first 2 min were for habituation and the subsequent 6 min were analyzed for the sequence of entrance. An entry was recorded only when all of the animal’s limbs were within the arm. Percentage alternation was calculated as follows: the number of triads containing entries into three arms/maximum possible alternations (total number of arms entered-2) X 100. The chance level for the alternation rate is 22.2% [28].
Reference memory test using radial-arm water maze
Reference memory was measured in radial-arm water maze, which was prepared similar to what has been previously described [29]. A circular pool with an interior diameter of 92 cm and a depth of 63 cm was used. Six identical, black, plastic 60° bended, V-shaped inserts with 45 cm in height were put into the pool to form six arms (30 cm long X 10 cm wide). The pool was filled with water made opaque by mixing with non-toxic white paint so that the platform was invisible. Visual cues were put on the conjunction of every other V-shaped insert to help orient the mouse.
Each mouse was given 8 trials per day to locate a hidden escape platform placed at the end of a fixed arm. In each trial, the animal was released from a starting arm, and given 60 s to locate the hidden platform. Once the animal got onto the platform, it was immediately taken out as a positive reward. The platform stayed in the same arm during the experiment, but the starting arm was changed every trial to avoid forming the swimming pattern. In the first 12 trials of training, the platform was altered between visible and invisible to accelerate the initial training process. The number of errors (number of wrong arms entered before getting onto the platform) was manually recorded throughout the experiment. Wild-type animals typically take 4 to 5 days to meet the criterion that they make less than 1 error on average in a trial block (4 trials per block) before locating the platform.
The delay match-to-place task using radial-arm water maze
We performed the delay match-to-place task in the same radial-arm water maze described above. The test was consisted of two days, day 1 for training and day 2 for testing. On each day, there were five sessions for each mouse. A session consisted of a location trial and a match trial, which both lasted 120 s. In the location trial, a hidden platform was placed at the end of one arm and the animal was released from the starting arm. If the mouse successfully got onto the platform, it would be quickly taken out as a positive reward. If the mouse failed to locate the platform within the time, it would be guided onto the platform and placed on it for 10 s before being taken out. 5 s after the location trial, the mouse was released again from the starting arm to start the match trial, with the platform staying in the same location. The escape latency (time taken to get onto the platform) and the number of errors (number of wrong arms entered before getting onto the platform) were manually recorded throughout the experiment. The position of the platform was changed between sessions, while the starting arm remained the same (If the six arms were labeled as A-F clockwise, the platform was placed in different arms with the order of B-D-F-C-E, with A being the starting arm).
Fear extinction test
The fear extinction test was carried out in the pre-pulse inhibition box from Panlab. The mice were fear conditioned on day 0 by 5 pairs of auditory conditional stimulus (85dB white noise lasting for 30 s) and unconditional stimulus (0.65mA foot-shock for 2 s) in context A (the chamber was cleaned with 1% acetic acid solution and the light was turned on in both chamber and test room). There was a 1 min interval between each pair of stimuli. Fear extinction was performed 24 hrs after fear conditioning in an altered environment context B (the chamber was cleaned with 70% ethanol, both chamber light and test room light were turned off with red fluorescent lights on as the replacement). During the extinction session, animals received 15 consecutive presentations of auditory conditional stimulus (85dB white noise lasting for 30 s) without foot-shock everyday for 2–3 days. There was a 1 min-interval between each auditory stimulus.
Mice used in behavior experiments
In the behavior experiments performed on BrPerk KO mice, different groups of mice were used for each experiment. In the behavior experiments performed on pharmacologically PERK-inhibited mice, the animals used in the spontaneous alternation Y-maze task were further subjected to either short-term PERK inhibition or long-term PERK inhibition fear extinction test. Animals used in one fear extinction test were not used in the other to avoid residual effect.
Primary neuronal culture
Mouse primary cortical neurons were prepared as previously described [30]. Briefly, cerebral cortex from day 0 wild-type pups was isolated and the cortex from each pup was dissociated in 5ml 0.05% trypsin-EDTA with DNase1 for 30 min at 37 C°(5% CO2). The tissue was then washed twice with HBSS containing 10% FBS, and mechanically triturated in 2ml neuronal medium with DNase1. 8ml fresh neuronal medium was added after trituration and the cells were collected by centrifugation at 120Xg for 5 min. The cells were re-suspended in 2ml fresh neuronal medium and plated on glial-coated 12mm glass coverslips in a 24-well plate at the density around 150,000 cells per well. The neuronal medium used was the MEM based medium with 5% FBS (GEMINI bio-products), 2% B27 (Invitrogen), 1mM L-Glutamine (Gibco), 20mM D-Glucose, 2μM Ara–C, 40 units/ml penicillin, 40μg/ml streptomycin and 100ng/ml Amphotericin B. Final pH was adjusted to 7.4 with NaHCO3 (100mg/500ml). Total medium was changed on DIV 1 and 50% of the medium was changed on DIV 3 and DIV 5. DIV 14–21 neurons were used for western blot analysis.
Results
Forebrain-specific Perk knockout mice are impaired in spontaneous alternation Y-maze task
To study the function of PERK in learning and memory, we generated forebrain-specific Perk knockout (BrPerk KO) mice by crossing αCamKII-Cre with Perk-floxed mice previously generated in our lab [24]. The expression of Cre recombinase in the αCaMKII-Cre strain occurs primarily in postnatal forebrain peaking at 3 months old [26]. We therefore restricted our studies to adult BrPerk KO mice at least 3 months old. Previously it has been reported that Cre recombinase is expressed in both cortex and hippocampus in the αCaMKII-Cre strain [31, 32]. Western blot analysis of homogenates of different brain regions confirmed that the expression of PERK was substantially knocked down in the prefrontal cortex (Fig 1A), which is the major brain region involved in working memory and memory flexibility in rodents [1, 33]. Significant knockdown of PERK was also observed in the hippocampus but not in the cerebellum (Fig 1A). The small amount of residual PERK seen in BrPerk KO mice forebrain was likely due to PERK’s expression in glia and in non-pyramidal neurons, and to incomplete deletion of PERK in pyramidal neurons. The generation of BrPerk KO mice enabled us to specifically look at the function of PERK in the forebrain at adult stage without disturbing PERK’s function in other tissues or in earlier neurodevelopment.
As a specific form of memory, working memory lasts for only several seconds and does not require new protein synthesis. To determine if PERK is required for working memory, BrPerk KO mice were subjected to the spontaneous alternation Y-maze task, which is a spatial working memory test that is based on the willingness of rodents to explore a new environment [34]. In this task, an animal must remember the arm it had visited previously in order to alternate the arm entrance in the next trial. Thus the alternation rate is an indicator of the animal’s spatial working memory capacity. BrPerk KO mice exhibited significantly less alternation rate when compared to their wild-type littermates, but did not differ in the total number of arms entered (Fig 1B). These results show that BrPerk KO mice are impaired in spatial working memory, but exhibit similar levels of locomotion and exploratory behavior.
BrPerk KO mice are impaired in the delay match-to-place task
To further examine the working memory capacity in BrPerk KO mice, we challenged the BrPerk KO mice with a more complex working memory test: the delay match-to-place task using radial-arm water maze. Successful accomplishment of this task requires good spatial reference memory and working memory. Previously it has been shown that BrPerk KO mice possess normal hippocampus-dependent spatial reference memory when tested in Morris water maze [19]. We repeated the spatial reference memory test in radial-arm water maze, and confirmed that there is no difference between BrPerk KO mice and their wild-type littermates (Fig 2A). After the reference memory test, BrPerk KO mice were subjected to a delay match-to-place test in radial-arm water maze. In this test, the mouse was given five sessions each day, with the location of the hidden platform changed between successive sessions. Each session consisted of two trials, a location trial followed by a match trial. A mouse with intact working memory would remember the location of the hidden platform in the location trial, and use visual cues to orient itself, thus displaying substantial savings of escape latency in the match trial [35]. The BrPerk KO mice made more errors and required longer escape latency in locating the platform in match trials compared to their wild-type littermates (Fig 2B), suggesting an impairment in working memory.
Pharmacologically PERK-inhibited mice are impaired in spontaneous alternation Y-maze task
To investigate if a similar behavioral impairment would occur following pharmacological inhibition of PERK activity, we systematically treated the wild-type mice with a newly developed PERK inhibitor (PERKi), GSK2606414. GSK2606414 is a highly specific inhibitor of PERK, which acts by competing for the ATP binding domain in the catalytic site [36], and has been proven to be effective in acutely inhibiting PERK’s activity in cultured cells [36]. To verify the effectiveness of PERKi in vitro, primary cortical neurons were pretreated with 500nM PERKi for 15 min followed by activation of PERK with 100nM thapsigargin for 30 min. Thapsigargin is a non-reversible SERCA pump inhibitor [37], that activates the PERK kinase activity by causing ER calcium depletion [38]. 15 min-pretreatment of 500nM PERKi inhibited thapsigargin-induced PERK activation and eIF2α phosphorylation, demonstrating that the PERKi effectively inhibits PERK’s activity in primary neurons (Fig 3A). Due to the low level of PERK activation in mice brain at steady state (preliminary data), we were unable to validate the efficacy of PERKi in vivo by western blot analysis. On the other hand, the efficacy of PERKi in vivo has been validated by others using liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry measurements, and it was demonstrated that orally administered GSK2606414 penetrates the blood-brain barrier in mice [39].
Oral gavage is the only effective means of administering the PERKi, however oral gavage results in acute stress [40] and short-term elevation of corticosterone levels [41]. To relieve gavage-induced stress, mice were acclimated to gavage administration by dosing with vehicle daily for 5 days prior to the administration of the inhibitor. After gavage acclimation, the animals were administered with 2 doses of PERKi or vehicle, and their working memory was assessed in spontaneous alternation Y-maze task 8 hours after the 2nd dose of PERKi (Fig 3B). The selection of 8 hours allowed dissipation of gavage-induced stress while retaining sufficient inhibitor levels in circulation (preliminary data). Since saturating concentrations of GSK2606414 in mice brains have been reported 14 hours after 2 doses of compound administration at the concentration of 50mg/kg [39], a sufficient amount of the PERKi should be present in the brain 8 hours after 2 doses of GSK2606414 administration at the concentration of 150mg/kg. The PERK-inhibited mice exhibited a lower alternation rate compared to the vehicle controls (Fig 3C), suggesting the impairment of spatial working memory. Similar to genetic Perk ablation, no difference was seen in the total number of arms entered between the PERKi and vehicle groups (Fig 3C). However mice received gavage treatment showed significantly less total arm entrance when compared to the mice with no gavage treatment (number of arms entered in Fig 1B and Fig 3C were compared; p<0.001, one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s post-hoc test), which suggests the possible impairment in animal performance due to the residual gavage-induced stress.
Pharmacological PERK inhibition mimics fear extinction impairment in BrPerk KO mice
Previous studies reported that BrPerk KO mice are impaired in protein synthesis-dependent memory flexibility, as shown by Y-water maze reversal task and fear extinction task [19]. We tested the memory flexibility of the BrPerk KO using the Y-water maze reversal task and confirmed this result (S1 Fig). To examine whether pharmacological PERK inhibition could mimic the memory flexibility impairments observed in genetic Perk knockout mice, we measured fear extinction memory in wild-type mice orally administered with PERKi. We chose to test fear extinction memory because this task requires less precise cognition compared to Y-water maze reversal task, and thus is less likely to be confounded by gavage-induced stress.
Fear extinction memory was measured in wild-type mice which received short-term or long-term PERKi treatment. In the short-term PERKi treatment group, the mice were first acclimated to gavage for 5 days by daily vehicle administration, followed by 2 doses of PERKi or vehicle before the behavioral test (Fig 3B), and 1 dose daily during the course of the experiment. Short-term PERK inhibition did not affect fear conditioning (Fig 4A). However, whereas the vehicle group started to exhibit a progressive decline of freezing rate with repeated exposure to CS on the 2nd and 3rd day of extinction session, the freezing rate of PERKi group remained elevated (Fig 4B and 4C). This result mimics the extinction impairment observed previously in BrPerk KO mice. It is noteworthy that neither group exhibited significant fear extinction on day 1. This was likely the result of gavage-induced stress, because in our initial trials when mice were not first acclimated to gavage neither vehicle nor PERKi group showed significant decline in the freezing rate over 2 days’ extinction training (S2 Fig).
In the long-term PERKi treatment group, mice were orally administered with inhibitor or vehicle for 2 weeks prior to the behavioral test. The mice in this experiment were not acclimated to gavage because of long-term gavage administration. As with the short-term PERK inhibition, no difference in fear conditioning was observed between treatments (Fig 5A). With longer inhibitor treatment, while both groups exhibited significant fear extinction on day 1, PERK-inhibited mice exhibited delayed extinction over the 2 days (Fig 5B and 5C).
Discussion
Previous study of PERK’s function in the brain has focused on protein synthesis-dependent cognitive abilities where it has been demonstrated that PERK is required for memory flexibility and normal expression of mGluR-dependent long-term depression via eIF2α-dependent protein translational control. Two other eIF2α kinases, GCN2 and PKR, have also been shown to be involved in protein synthesis-dependent memory regulation [42, 43]. Motivated by the recent discovery that PERK acutely regulates Ca2+ dynamics in β-cells [23] and primary neurons (preliminary data), which is independent of phosphorylation of eIF2α and protein translational control, we explored the possibility that PERK may regulate Ca2+ dynamics-dependent working memory. We found that BrPerk KO mice are impaired in working memory, as shown by spontaneous alternation Y-maze task and the delay match-to-place task. Since over the course of development the genetic Perk knockout mice may have acquired a compensatory mechanism that could potentially confound the interpretation of underlying mechanisms, we examined the effects of pharmacologically ablating PERK activity in wild-type mice by oral administration of a specific and potent PERK inhibitor. PERK-inhibited mice exhibited significant impairment in working memory in the spontaneous alternation Y-maze task. Moreover, oral administration of PERKi mimics the fear extinction deficit observed in forebrain-specific Perk knockout mice. Together, these results suggest that the behavior phenotypes observed in BrPerk KO mice are attributable to the absence of PERK rather than to a developmental compensatory response.
Spontaneous alternation Y-maze task and the delay match-to-place task in radial-arm water maze were used to test spatial working memory in this study. Y-maze spontaneous alternation occurs because of rodents’ natural exploratory behavior, which causes them to alternate spontaneously in the Y-maze, at the rate significantly higher than that could be due to chance [34]. Although the neuronal mechanisms underlying spontaneous alternation are still unclear, this test has been widely used to measure spatial working memory, because it does not require excessive animal handling and measures subject’s locomotion in the meantime [44]. In addition to spontaneous alternation Y-maze task, we also subjected BrPerk KO mice to the delay match-to-place task. The delay match-to-place task is a robust behavioral test, but it is also a more complex test for measuring spatial working memory. Since the initial introduction by Morris and coworkers [45], modifications have been made to simplify the procedure without reducing the maze’s complexity [35, 46]. We performed the delay match-to-place task in radial-arm water maze, which preserves the spatial complexity of the Morris water tank, but simplifies scoring procedure with the benefit of a defined swim path.
The neuronal mechanisms underlying PERK’s regulation of working memory is unknown. We speculate that PERK regulates working memory via changes in Ca2+ dynamics based upon PERK’s demonstrated function in regulating Ca2+ dynamics in β-cells. Our lab has shown that PERK regulates ER Ca2+ re-uptake and cytosol Ca2+ influx from extracellular space in β-cells, which underlies normal glucose-stimulated insulin secretion [23]. Similarly, in neurons, it has been shown that metabotropic receptor-coupled intracellular Ca2+ ([Ca2+]i) rise is important for working memory in several ways. Firstly, the induced [Ca2+]i rise is known to activate several Ca2+-dependent proteins including phosphatase calcineurin, kinase CaMKII and PKC, all of which have been shown to regulate working memory capacity [47]. Moreover, metabotropic receptor-coupled [Ca2+]i can directly affect intrinsic neuronal properties. For example, it has been shown that mGluR mediated [Ca2+]i regulates SK channel-dependent neuronal hyperpolarization and TRPC-dependent neuronal depolarization, indicating the amplitude of [Ca2+]i can influence neuronal excitability [14, 48]. In our preliminary studies (unpublished), we have observed that acute PERK inhibition impairs metabotropic receptor-coupled [Ca2+]i due to impaired receptor-operated Ca2+ influx in primary cortical neurons, which suggests that these pathways may also underlie the neuronal mechanism of PERK’s regulation of working memory in vivo.
Fear extinction is a form of inhibitory learning, a process by which a pre-conditioned fear response is extinguished after repeated exposure to neutral conditioned stimulus without the aversive stimulus. Previously it has been shown that BrPerk KO mice are impaired in fear extinction, which is associated with reduced eIF2α-dependent ATF4 expression [19]. Here we showed that pharmacological PERK inhibition mimics the fear extinction impairment observed in BrPerk KO mice, which suggests that the behavior phenotype observed in BrPerk KO mice is attributable to the absence of PERK, and confirms PERK’s role in regulating memory flexibility in addition to working memory.
Although different molecular mechanisms have been suggested underlying working memory and memory flexibility, both cognitive functions are considered as core components of executive function, regulated in different sub-regions in prefrontal cortex, but modulated by similar neurotransmitters [1, 33]. Thus, the possibility remains that the working memory and memory flexibility deficits observed in BrPerk mice may be due to the impairment in a common pathway that is normally regulated by PERK.
Based on cell culture experiments, PERK was initially identified as an effector of unfolded protein and ER stress response [15, 38, 49] and from studies it was speculated that the function of PERK in whole organisms was to temporarily repress protein synthesis in order to relieve the client protein load in the ER. However subsequent studies in Perk KO mice, which display an array of dysfunctions, failed to find evidence in support of this hypothesis [21, 50]. Rather we found that PERK plays important developmental and physiological regulatory functions that are unrelated to stress throughout the body [21, 22, 50, 51]. In this study, we investigated PERK’s role in cognition, and showed for the first time that PERK regulates new protein synthesis-independent working memory. This discovery reveals a novel physiological role of PERK in cognitive function, which is unrelated to stress responses. Moreover, eIF2α dephosphorylation has been shown to be critical for late phase long-term potentiation (L-LTP) elicitation and long-term memory (LTM) storage, as genetic reduction of eIF2α phosphorylation by knockdown of eIF2α kinases GCN2 [42] or PKR [43], or single allele phosphorylation site mutation of eIF2α (Serine to Alanine at phosphorylation site Ser51) [52] lowers the threshold for L-LTP and facilitates LTM, while pharmacological promotion of eIF2α phosphorylation impairs L-LTP and LTM [52]. On the other hand, forebrain-specific Perk knockout mice exhibit normal LTP and LTM [19, 20], but impaired memory flexibility [19]. The significant phenotype difference between Perk knockout mice and other mice models with reduced eIF2α phosphorylation suggest that PERK may possess additional regulation of cognition that is eIF2α-independent. The discovery of PERK’s regulation over Ca2+ dynamics-dependent working memory supports the above hypothesis. Further study is needed to elucidate the specific pathways that participate in PERK’s regulation of the different cognitive functions.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We thank Dr. Yingwei Mao for advice on experimental design and sharing behavioral testing equipment, and Cavener lab member Jingjie Hu for animal handling assistance.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health grant DK088140 (to DRC), http://www.nih.gov. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Khan ZU, Muly EC. Molecular mechanisms of working memory. Behavioural brain research. 2011;219(2):329–41. 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.12.039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kolb B, Tees RC. The Cerebral cortex of the rat. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press; 1990. xii, 645 p. p. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Guldin WO, Pritzel M, Markowitsch HJ. Prefrontal cortex of the mouse defined as cortical projection area of the thalamic mediodorsal nucleus. Brain Behav Evol. 1981;19(3–4):93–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jacobsen CF. Functions of frontal association area in primates. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry. 1935; 33:558–569. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fuster JM, Alexander GE. Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science. 1971;173(3997):652–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.D'Esposito M, Detre JA, Alsop DC, Shin RK, Atlas S, Grossman M. The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature. 1995;378(6554):279–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Noll DC, Jonides J, et al. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature. 1997;386(6625):604–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Goldman-Rakic PS. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron. 1995;14(3):477–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Penetar DM, McDonough JH Jr. Effects of cholinergic drugs on delayed match-to-sample performance of rhesus monkeys. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior. 1983;19(6):963–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Granon S, Poucet B, Thinus-Blanc C, Changeux JP, Vidal C. Nicotinic and muscarinic receptors in the rat prefrontal cortex: differential roles in working memory, response selection and effortful processing. Psychopharmacology. 1995; 119:139–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hayashi K, Yoshihara T, Ichitani Y. Involvement of hippocampal metabotropic glutamate receptors in radial maze performance. Neuroreport. 2007; 18:719–723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mikami A, Masuoka T, Yasuda M, Yamamoto Y, Kamei C. Participation of cholinergic system in memory deficits induced by blockade of hippocampal mGlu(1) receptors. European journal of pharmacology. 2007;575(1–3):82–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Egorov AV, Hamam BN, Fransen E, Hasselmo ME, Alonso AA. Graded persistent activity in entorhinal cortex neurons. Nature. 2002;420(6912):173–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hagenston AM, Fitzpatrick JS, Yeckel MF. MGluR-mediated calcium waves that invade the soma regulate firing in layer V medial prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons. Cerebral cortex. 2008;18(2):407–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Harding HP, Zhang Y, Bertolotti A, Zeng H, Ron D. Perk is essential for translational regulation and cell survival during the unfolded protein response. Molecular cell. 2000;5(5):897–904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Vattem KM, Wek RC. Reinitiation involving upstream ORFs regulates ATF4 mRNA translation in mammalian cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(31):11269–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Davis HP, Squire LR. Protein synthesis and memory: a review. Psychol Bull. 1984;96(3):518–59. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Chen A, Muzzio IA, Malleret G, Bartsch D, Verbitsky M, Pavlidis P, et al. Inducible enhancement of memory storage and synaptic plasticity in transgenic mice expressing an inhibitor of ATF4 (CREB-2) and C/EBP proteins. Neuron. 2003;39(4):655–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Trinh MA, Kaphzan H, Wek RC, Pierre P, Cavener DR, Klann E. Brain-specific disruption of the eIF2alpha kinase PERK decreases ATF4 expression and impairs behavioral flexibility. Cell reports. 2012;1(6):676–88. 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Trinh MA, Ma T, Kaphzan H, Bhattacharya A, Antion MD, Cavener DR, et al. The eIF2alpha kinase PERK limits the expression of hippocampal metabotropic glutamate receptor-dependent long-term depression. Learning & memory. 2014;21(5):298–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Zhang W, Feng D, Li Y, Iida K, McGrath B, Cavener DR. PERK EIF2AK3 control of pancreatic beta cell differentiation and proliferation is required for postnatal glucose homeostasis. Cell metabolism. 2006;4(6):491–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gupta S, McGrath B, Cavener DR. PERK (EIF2AK3) regulates proinsulin trafficking and quality control in the secretory pathway. Diabetes. 2010;59(8):1937–47. 10.2337/db09-1064 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wang R, McGrath BC, Kopp RF, Roe MW, Tang X, Chen G, et al. Insulin secretion and Ca2+ dynamics in beta-cells are regulated by PERK (EIF2AK3) in concert with calcineurin. The Journal of biological chemistry. 2013;288(47):33824–36. 10.1074/jbc.M113.503664 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Zhang P, McGrath B, Li S, Frank A, Zambito F, Reinert J, et al. The PERK eukaryotic initiation factor 2 alpha kinase is required for the development of the skeletal system, postnatal growth, and the function and viability of the pancreas. Molecular and cellular biology. 2002;22(11):3864–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tsien JZ, Chen DF, Gerber D, Tom C, Mercer EH, Anderson DJ, et al. Subregion- and cell type-restricted gene knockout in mouse brain. Cell. 1996;87(7):1317–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Minichiello L, Korte M, Wolfer D, Kuhn R, Unsicker K, Cestari V, et al. Essential role for TrkB receptors in hippocampus-mediated learning. Neuron. 1999;24(2):401–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Oakley H, Cole SL, Logan S, Maus E, Shao P, Craft J, et al. Intraneuronal beta-amyloid aggregates, neurodegeneration, and neuron loss in transgenic mice with five familial Alzheimer's disease mutations: potential factors in amyloid plaque formation. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2006;26(40):10129–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wolf A, Bauer B, Abner EL, Ashkenazy-Frolinger T, Hartz AM. A Comprehensive Behavioral Test Battery to Assess Learning and Memory in 129S6/Tg2576 Mice. PloS one. 2016;11(1):e0147733 10.1371/journal.pone.0147733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Alamed J, Wilcock DM, Diamond DM, Gordon MN, Morgan D. Two-day radial-arm water maze learning and memory task; robust resolution of amyloid-related memory deficits in transgenic mice. Nature protocols. 2006;1(4):1671–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Chen G, Trombley PQ, van den Pol AN. Excitatory actions of GABA in developing rat hypothalamic neurones. The Journal of physiology. 1996;494 (Pt 2):451–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Madisen L, Zwingman TA, Sunkin SM, Oh SW, Zariwala HA, Gu H, et al. A robust and high-throughput Cre reporting and characterization system for the whole mouse brain. Nature neuroscience. 2010;13(1):133–40. 10.1038/nn.2467 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Yamanaka T, Tosaki A, Kurosawa M, Akimoto K, Hirose T, Ohno S, et al. Loss of aPKClambda in differentiated neurons disrupts the polarity complex but does not induce obvious neuronal loss or disorientation in mouse brains. PloS one. 2013;8(12):e84036 10.1371/journal.pone.0084036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Logue SF, Gould TJ. The neural and genetic basis of executive function: attention, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition. Pharmacology, biochemistry, and behavior. 2014;123:45–54. 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lalonde R. The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2002;26(1):91–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Runyan JD, Moore AN, Dash PK. A role for prefrontal calcium-sensitive protein phosphatase and kinase activities in working memory. Learning & memory. 2005;12(2):103–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Axten JM, Medina JR, Feng Y, Shu A, Romeril SP, Grant SW, et al. Discovery of 7-methyl-5-(1-{[3-(trifluoromethyl)phenyl]acetyl}-2,3-dihydro-1H-indol-5-yl)-7H-p yrrolo[2,3-d]pyrimidin-4-amine (GSK2606414), a potent and selective first-in-class inhibitor of protein kinase R (PKR)-like endoplasmic reticulum kinase (PERK). Journal of medicinal chemistry. 2012;55(16):7193–207. 10.1021/jm300713s [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lytton J, Westlin M, Hanley MR. Thapsigargin inhibits the sarcoplasmic or endoplasmic reticulum Ca-ATPase family of calcium pumps. The Journal of biological chemistry. 1991;266(26):17067–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Harding HP, Zhang Y, Ron D. Protein translation and folding are coupled by an endoplasmic-reticulum-resident kinase. Nature. 1999;397(6716):271–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Moreno JA, Halliday M, Molloy C, Radford H, Verity N, Axten JM, et al. Oral treatment targeting the unfolded protein response prevents neurodegeneration and clinical disease in prion-infected mice. Sci Transl Med. 2013;5(206):206ra138 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006767 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Balcombe JP, Barnard ND, Sandusky C. Laboratory routines cause animal stress. Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci. 2004;43(6):42–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Brown AP, Dinger N, Levine BS. Stress produced by gavage administration in the rat. Contemp Top Lab Anim Sci. 2000;39(1):17–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Costa-Mattioli M, Gobert D, Harding H, Herdy B, Azzi M, Bruno M, et al. Translational control of hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory by the eIF2alpha kinase GCN2. Nature. 2005;436(7054):1166–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Zhu PJ, Huang W, Kalikulov D, Yoo JW, Placzek AN, Stoica L, et al. Suppression of PKR promotes network excitability and enhanced cognition by interferon-gamma-mediated disinhibition. Cell. 2011;147(6):1384–96. 10.1016/j.cell.2011.11.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hughes RN. The value of spontaneous alternation behavior (SAB) as a test of retention in pharmacological investigations of memory. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2004;28(5):497–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Morris RG, Hagan JJ, Rawlins JN. Allocentric spatial learning by hippocampectomised rats: a further test of the "spatial mapping" and "working memory" theories of hippocampal function. Q J Exp Psychol B. 1986;38(4):365–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Morgan D, Diamond DM, Gottschall PE, Ugen KE, Dickey C, Hardy J, et al. A beta peptide vaccination prevents memory loss in an animal model of Alzheimer's disease. Nature. 2000;408(6815):982–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Dash PK, Moore AN, Kobori N, Runyan JD. Molecular activity underlying working memory. Learning & memory. 2007;14(8):554–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.El-Hassar L, Hagenston AM, D'Angelo LB, Yeckel MF. Metabotropic glutamate receptors regulate hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neuron excitability via Ca(2)(+) wave-dependent activation of SK and TRPC channels. The Journal of physiology. 2011;589(Pt 13):3211–29. 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.209783 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Shi Y, Vattem KM, Sood R, An J, Liang J, Stramm L, et al. Identification and characterization of pancreatic eukaryotic initiation factor 2 alpha-subunit kinase, PEK, involved in translational control. Molecular and cellular biology. 1998;18(12):7499–509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wei J, Sheng X, Feng D, McGrath B, Cavener DR. PERK is essential for neonatal skeletal development to regulate osteoblast proliferation and differentiation. J Cell Physiol. 2008;217(3):693–707. 10.1002/jcp.21543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Li Y, Iida K, O'Neil J, Zhang P, Li S, Frank A, et al. PERK eIF2alpha kinase regulates neonatal growth by controlling the expression of circulating insulin-like growth factor-I derived from the liver. Endocrinology. 2003;144(8):3505–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Costa-Mattioli M, Gobert D, Stern E, Gamache K, Colina R, Cuello C, et al. eIF2alpha phosphorylation bidirectionally regulates the switch from short- to long-term synaptic plasticity and memory. Cell. 2007;129(1):195–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.