Abstract
Bitter is a taste modality associated with toxic substances evoking aversive behaviour in most animals, and the valence of different taste modalities is conserved between mammals and Drosophila. Despite knowledge gathered in the past on the peripheral perception of taste, little is known about the identity of taste interneurons in the brain. Here we show that hugin neuropeptide-containing neurons in the Drosophila larval brain are necessary for avoidance behaviour to caffeine, and when activated, result in cessation of feeding and mediates a bitter taste signal within the brain. Hugin neuropeptide-containing neurons project to the neurosecretory region of the protocerebrum and functional imaging demonstrates that these neurons are activated by bitter stimuli and by activation of bitter sensory receptor neurons. We propose that hugin neurons projecting to the protocerebrum act as gustatory interneurons relaying bitter taste information to higher brain centres in Drosophila larvae.
Bitter taste evokes aversive behaviour in animals, but little is known about the central nervous system mechanisms that convey this taste modality. Hückesfeld et al. identify a set of second order neurons in Drosophila that contain hugin neuropeptide and are responsible for conveying bitter taste to the protocerebrum.
Detailed knowledge exists on the anatomical distribution and function of gustatory receptors in mammals and Drosophila1,2,3,4,5. In Drosophila 60 gustatory receptor genes encode 68 gustatory receptors6,7,8, with the majority of these receptors detecting bitter compounds9. Although gustatory receptors in Drosophila share no homology to mammalian taste receptors, the strategy used in both to detect a taste molecule, process its information and the valence of aversive bitter and appetitive sweet stimuli share similarities4. In contrast to the extensive knowledge on the peripheral coding of taste in flies and mammals, much less is known about the central pathways that relay and translate these signals into meaningful behaviour. Although broad regions in different parts of the brain have been shown to respond to various taste cues, there is little information on the molecular identity of specific neurons that convey different taste modalities to the higher brain centres10,11. Recently, secondary neurons that relay sweet taste from subesophageal zone (SEZ) to the antennal mechanosensory motor centre of adult Drosophila were characterized12. Analogous secondary neurons for other taste modalities have not yet been identified.
A candidate for conveying bitter taste from the SEZ to higher brain centres are neurons that express the hugin neuropeptide13,14 (referred to as hugin neurons), whose arborizations in Drosophila larvae overlap with that of bitter gustatory receptor neurons (GRNs) expressing the caffeine receptor GR66a15,16,17. In adult Drosophila, GR66a was shown to represent a bitter receptor for detection of caffeine9,16,17 and inactivation of GR66a positive neurons leads to impairment of caffeine aversion17. In Drosophila larvae, artificial activation of GR66a positive neurons leads to aversive behaviour18. Thus, hugin neurons were good candidates for acting as a central relay for bitter information from sensory neurons.
Using classical two-choice behavioural experiments, electrophysiological measurements as well as calcium imaging analysis, we now show that hugin neurons relay caffeine as well as other bitter taste signals from sensory neurons to the protocerebrum, acting as bitter taste interneurons in the Drosophila larval brain.
Results
Hugin neurons are required for caffeine avoidance response
We first asked whether the hugin neurons make contacts with caffeine responsive GR66a neurons. Using the GFP reconstitution across synaptic partners (GRASP) approach19, we could indeed observe a GRASP signal in the SEZ, indicating that caffeine receptor neurons and hugin neurons are in close proximity to each other (Fig. 1a,b).
Activating the hugin neurons causes the larvae to stop feeding and move out of a strongly appetitive food source (yeast)20, which could be due to activation of an aversive bitter taste pathway. We therefore tested the behavioural response to caffeine in a two choice assay. When hugin neurons are activated with the temperature sensitive cation channel dTrpA1, the animals showed impaired aversion to caffeine stimuli (Fig. 1c). We next asked how the animals would behave if hugin neuronal activity was suppressed, by ablating the hugin neurons. These animals showed significantly less avoidance to caffeine (Fig. 1d,e). To exclude potential developmental effects, we also expressed the temperature sensitive mutant form of dynamin (shibireTS) in the hugin neurons, which leads to a block of synaptic release in a temperature dependent manner. The loss of proper bitter aversion still persisted (Fig. 1d,f).
We next asked if hugin neurons were involved in other taste modalities. We first tested high salt (2 M NaCl), which is very aversive for larvae21, in two-choice assays. As with caffeine, activation of the hugin neurons led to animals showing impaired aversion to high salt (Fig. 2a). However, no difference to control was observed when hugin neurons were ablated, indicating that hugin neurons are not required for the behavioural response to high salt (Fig. 2b). High caffeine and salt levels are both aversive gustatory stimuli. To determine how manipulation of hugin neuronal activity affected the response to an appetitive cue, we performed two-choice assays with 1 M fructose. In this case, activation led to impaired attraction to fructose, whereas ablation had no effect relative to control (Fig. 2c,d; Supplementary Fig. 1). Thus, when hugin neurons are activated, the larvae are no longer able to respond properly to different taste modalities, such as yeast protein20, fructose, high salt and caffeine. This chemosensory response was specific for taste, as olfactory behaviour was not affected (Fig. 3), showing that activation of hugin neurons selectively disrupts gustatory chemosensory processing. However, inhibiting the hugin neurons results in the inability to respond appropriately to caffeine, indicating that hugin neurons process bitter taste information. In sum, when hugin neurons are activated, larvae perceive all tested substrates (water, caffeine, salt and fructose) as bitter tasting. In contrast, when hugin neurons are inactivated, only the bitter substrate choice is affected since proper detection of bitter compounds no longer occurs.
Distinct hugin neurons modulate taste and feeding behaviour
As the hugin neuronal cluster is composed of different classes with different projection targets13,14, we next wanted to determine which class was responsible for taste processing and feeding regulation. Through promoter deletion analysis, we generated a hugin promoter-Gal4 line which showed target gene expression exclusively in the eight hugin cells (four per hemisphere) that project to the protocerebrum (HugPC-Gal4, Fig. 4a). To see whether this subset of hugin neurons (huginPC neurons) is necessary for proper bitter taste processing, we ablated these neurons (Fig. 4b) and tested larvae in the caffeine two-choice experiment. Ablation of the huginPC neurons resulted in the inability to appropriately avoid caffeine as compared with control animals (Fig. 4c). Consistent with the ablation of all hugin neurons (Fig. 2), ablating just the huginPC neurons had no effect compared with controls on high salt or fructose (Fig. 4d,e, controls are re-plotted from Fig. 2b,d). We note that, in the case of 2 M NaCl, there was a significant difference to one of the controls (UAS-rpr;;hid control larvae). Therefore, we additionally tested the animals on a lower, but still aversive salt concentration (500 mM), which showed that huginPC neurons are not necessary for proper high salt aversion (Supplementary Fig. 2, for descriptive statistics see Supplementary Table 7). Activating just the huginPC neurons was sufficient to suppress food intake, induce wandering-like behaviour (Fig. 5a; Supplementary Fig. 3), as well as decrease the motor pattern of pharyngeal muscle contractions (Fig. 5b). These results indicated that the hugin neurons responsible for feeding and caffeine taste mediated behaviours are those that project to the protocerebrum.
Activity of huginPC neurons upon bitter stimulation
To further investigate the connection between bitter taste and hugin neurons, we asked if the huginPC neurons could be activated by caffeine using CaMPARI (Calcium Modulated Photoactivatable Ratiometric Integrator), that allows monitoring of calcium activity in intact animals. Calcium activity and simultaneous presence of ultraviolet-light (405 nm) lead to an irreversible conversion from green to red fluorescence of the neurons of interest22. When we placed intact larvae in solutions containing water and water mixed with caffeine, fructose, high NaCl or yeast (Fig. 6a), huginPC neurons were strongly activated by caffeine (Fig. 6b,c). They also showed a concentration dependent increase in calcium activity with increasing caffeine concentrations (Fig. 6d). Other bitter tastants, such as quinine and denatonium, also activated huginPC neurons (Fig. 6e). We tested the behavioural relevance for this rise in calcium activity for denatonium in two-choice experiments, since denatonium led to robust avoidance behaviour in control larvae at 32 °C (experimental temperature). The calcium activity in huginPC neurons was behaviourally relevant since ablation of these neurons resulted in larvae with impaired aversion to the taste of denatonium (Fig. 6f) (no difference when tested against chance levels, see Supplementary Table 5). Unexpectedly, we observed decrease in huginPC calcium activity in larvae placed in high salt, fructose and yeast relative to water alone (Fig. 6c). Although the functional significance of this repression is not clear, it has been shown that bitter taste pathways can inhibit sweet pathways in adult23 and larval24 Drosophila, reflecting an interaction between pathways involving different taste modalities. This cross-regulation might be a general strategy to efficiently detect distinct taste modalities, since in case of the huginPC neurons, a sweet taste stimulus would inhibit the activity of bitter interneurons.
Finally we asked if there is a functional connection, in addition to the anatomical connection (Fig. 1a,b), between the caffeine sensing GR66a neurons and hugin neurons. To this end, we activated the GR66a neurons and then monitored the activity of hugin neurons using the calcium indicator GCaMP6s (Fig. 7a,b). Activation of GR66a by dTrpA1 and simultaneous calcium imaging of huginPC projections resulted in the induction of rhythmically occurring calcium peaks, demonstrating a functional connection between GR66a and huginPC neurons (Fig. 7b).
Discussion
The bitter taste rejection response is important for all animals that encounter toxic or harmful food in their environment. Here we showed that the hugin neurons in the Drosophila larval brain function as a relay between bitter sensory neurons and higher brain centres (Fig. 8). Strikingly, activation of the hugin neurons made the animals significantly more insensitive to substrates with negative valence like bitter (caffeine) and salt (high NaCl), as well as positive valence like sweet (fructose). In other words, when the hugin neurons are active these animals ‘think' they are tasting bitter and therefore become insensitive to other gustatory cues. This is in line with observations made in mice, in which optogenetically activating bitter cortex neurons caused animals to avoid an empty chamber illuminated with blue light. In this situation, although mice do not actually taste something bitter, they avoid the empty chamber since the bitter perception has been optogenetically induced in the central nervous system (CNS) and the mice ‘think' they are tasting a bitter substance25.
In our previous work, activation of all hugin neurons led to behavioural and physiological phenotypes such as decreased feeding, decrease in neural activity of the antennal nerve (AN), and induction of a wandering-like behaviour20. We have now pinpointed the neurons responsible specifically to those that project to the protocerebrum. These neurons not only respond to bitter stimuli, but also show a concentration dependent increase in calcium activity in response to caffeine. Dose dependent coding of bitter taste stimuli was previously shown to occur in peripheral bitter sensory neurons, where bitter sensilla exhibit dose dependent responses to various bitter compounds9. Larvae in which the huginPC neurons have been ablated still showed some avoidance to caffeine. Whether this implies the existence of other interneurons being involved in caffeine taste processing remains to be determined. Interestingly, the huginPC neurons are inhibited when larvae taste other modalities like salt (NaCl), sugar (fructose) or protein (yeast). This may indicate that taste pathways in the brain are segregated, but influence each other, as previously suggested10.
Bitter compounds may be able to inhibit the sweet-sensing response to ensure that bitter taste cannot be masked by sweet tasting food. This provides an efficient strategy for the detection of potentially harmful or toxic substances in food26,27. For appetitive tastes like fructose and yeast, bitter interneurons neurons like the huginPC neurons in the CNS may become inhibited to ensure appropriate behaviour to pleasant food. Salt is a bivalent taste modality, that is, low doses of salt drive appetitive behaviour, whereas high doses of salt are aversive to larval21,28 and adult29,30 Drosophila. Inhibition of huginPC neurons when larvae are tasting salt might be due to a different processing circuit for different concentrations of salt and the decision to either take up low doses or reject high doses.
Taken together, we propose that hugin neuropeptide neurons projecting to the protocerebrum represent a hub between bitter gustatory receptor neurons and higher brain centres that integrate bitter sensory information in the brain, and through its activity, influences the decision of the animal to avoid a bitter food source. The identification of second order gustatory neurons for bitter taste will not only provide valuable insights into bitter taste pathways in Drosophila, but may also help in assigning a potentially novel role of its mammalian homologue, Neuromedin U, in taste processing.
Methods
Fly lines
Wild type (OrgR) crossed to UAS-dTrpA1 (Bloomington #26263) served as control in Figs 1, 2, 3 and 5. Hugin-Gal4 (HugS3-Gal4 (ref. 13), Bloomington# 58769), GR66a-Gal4 (second Chr., gift from K.Scott31(formerly described as GR66C1)), GR66a-Gal4 (third Chr., Bloomington# 57670 used in Fig. 7b), Hugin-lexA (Hug1.2-lexA attp40 (ref. 32)), HugPC-Gal4 (see generation of this Gal4-line below), UAS-eNpHR-YFP (Bloomington# 41753, referred here as UAS-YFP in Supplementary Fig. 3, since this homozygous line together with HugPC-Gal4 was used as fluorescent marker only), UAS-CaMPARI (Bloomington #58761), UAS-rpr;;UAS-hid (UAS-rpr (Bloomington# 5823) crossed homozygous to UAS-hid33), UAS-shibireTS34, lexAop-CD4::spGFP11 and UAS-CD4::spGFP1-10 were gifts from K. Scott19, 13x LexAop2-IVS-GCamP6s-p10 (Bloomington #44274).
Fly care
Adult flies and larvae were kept on 25 °C under 12 h light/dark conditions. For electrophysiological and food intake experiments 4 h egg collections were made on apple juice agar plates containing a spot of yeast–water paste. After 48 h, larvae were transferred into food vials containing lab standard fly food. For other experiments (two-choice, CaMPARI, GCamP) larvae were raised in vials containing standard fly food with a spot of yeast for 4 days. All larvae used for the experiments were 98±2 h old. Only feeding third instar larvae were used for the experiments.
Generation of transgenic flies
For HugPC-Gal4 line, a 544 bp Hugin fragment 155 bp upstream of the ATG was amplified with primer1 (AAG GGT TTG GTT TAA TTT ATT TAT GTC ATA) and primer2 (GAG CCT GAT TAG GTC CCT GAT GTT TAA ACT T) and cloned into pCaSpeR-AUG-Gal4-X vector (Addgene plasmid 8,378. The construct was injected into w[1118]).
Two-choice gustatory assay
To measure the preference index of larvae towards given appetitive or aversive substrates, 9 mm diameter petri dishes were filled with 20 ml warm water agar (2.125% Agar-Agar, Kobe). After 20 min of air drying half of the agar was cut away and discarded. Compounds (2 M NaCl, 1 M Fructose, 200 mM Caffeine, 10 mM Denatonium) were diluted in warm agar in the given concentration until the agar fluid was clear, and filled in the other half of the petri dish (10 ml). After air drying again for 20 min, petri dishes were prewarmed in the 32 °C incubator 1.5 h prior to the experiment. All two-choice experiments were performed at 32 °C for comparability between all genotypes. For each experiment 30 larvae were taken out of standard fly food and washed with tap water. Larvae were then placed on the water side of the two-choice dishes and videotaped for 20 min. Videos were processed with FIJI (ImageJ) and analysed using a custom written script for FIJI. Analysis of PI values started 60 s after the start of the experiment to ensure proper tracking of larvae due larval accumulation at the beginning by placing them on the pure agar side. PIgustatory was calculated as (#larvaesubstrate−#larvaewater)/#larvaetotal.
One-choice olfactory assay
For testing response to an attractive odour (apple vinegar), a one-choice assay was performed. Agar plates were used (as described earlier for two-choice petri dishes) and placed into the incubator at 32 °C for 1.5 h. Larvae were videotaped at 32 °C for 5 min, and then an Eppendorf cup (1.5 ml) with filter paper soaked with apple vinegar was placed on one side of the water agar plate above the larvae such that they were not able to reach the odorant source. Movement of larvae was analysed using the custom made FIJI macro.
Food intake assay
Apple juice agar plates were prepared with a spot of red yeast paste in the middle of the plate. Plates were then placed in incubators precooled to 18 °C or prewarmed to 32 °C for 2 h. After 30 min starvation, five larvae were transferred on top of the yeast paste of a plate and videotaped for 20 min. After 20 min of videotaping, larvae were transferred into a small cell strainer and washed with 60 °C hot water. Larvae were then transferred to glass slides for photo documentation and analysed with the open source software FIJI (ImageJ) and a custom written analysis macro, which calculated the percentage of the red stained surface of the body compared with the whole body of the larva. To calculate the fold change of food intake from 18 to 32 °C, the value of all 32 °C values was divided by the mean value of all 18 °C values.
Electrophysiology
Third instar larvae were dissected in 35 mm petri dishes coated with 5 ml two-component silicone (Elastosil RT). Larvae were pinned down dorsal side up at the anterior and posterior end using 77 μm thick sharp etched tungsten needles. The larva was cut open longitudinally along the dorsal midline and the cuticle was pinned aside with 40 μm tungsten needles. Interior organs like fat body, intestine or salivary glands were removed except for the cephalopharyngeal skeleton and CNS with attached nerves of interest. Eye and leg imaginal discs were also removed. A transversal cut of the cuticle was performed beneath the CNS to reveal the AN. Nerves not needed for the respective recording were cut. A piece of thinned Parafilm was placed beneath the nerve of interest. This nerve was isolated from the surrounding solution with two adjacent jelly pools. Motor output of the AN was measured using custom made silver wire electrodes connected to a preamplifier (Model MA103, Ansgar Büschges group electronics lab). The preamplifier was connected to a four-channel amplifier/signal conditioner (Model MA 102, Ansgar Büschges group electronics lab). All recorded signals were amplified (amplification factor: 5,000) and filtered (bandpass: 0.1–3 kHz). Recordings were sampled at 20 kHz. Data were acquired with Micro3 1,401 A/D board (Cambridge Electronic Design) and Spike2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design).
Calcium imaging with CaMPARI
High-power LED of 405 nm (Thorlabs, M405L2–UV (405 nm) Mounted LED, 1,000 mA, 410 mW) was driven with a LED controller (Thorlabs, LEDDB1 driven with 1000, mA) and positioned 18 cm above the solution with the larva. PCR plate of 96-well was filled with 50 μl containing the given taste stimuli in tap water. Larvae were placed into the solution for 2 min and then ultraviolet light was applied for 30 s.
Afterwards brains were dissected in PBS and mounted onto a Poly-L-lysine (Sigma, Lot # SLBG4596V) coated cover slide with a drop of PBS. All z-stacks of the HugPC neurons were aquired using a ZEISS LSM 780 Laser scanning microscope with LCI Plan-Neofluar 25 × /0.8 Imm Korr DIC M27. For quantification of green to red photoconversion maximum intensity projections of the acquired z-stacks were used and a portion of the cytoplasmatic region of each cell was analysed to obtain data for green and red fluorescence intensity. Red fluorescence intensity was divided by green fluorescence intensity to get Fred/Fgreen ratio. A ‘no ultraviolet light' control was included to show that scanning of the CNS without being exposed to ultraviolet light does not convert green to red fluorescence.
GRASP
Genotypes used for GFP reconstitution across synaptic partners (GRASP) method19 were: Hug1.2lexA;lexAop-CD4::spGFP11 and GR66a-Gal4;UAS-CD4::spGFP1-10. Larval CNS was dissected and stained with anti-mouse-GFP (Abcam, 1:500, secondary antibody was anti-mouse-Al488 (Invitrogen, 1:500)). Images were acquired using a ZEISS LSM 780 Laser scanning microscope with LCI Plan-Neofluar 25 × per 0.8 Imm Korr DIC M27.
Calcium imaging with GCaMP
Freshly dissected CNS of feeding third instar larvae of the genotypes UAS-dTrpA1/Hug1.2lexA; GR66a-Gal4/lexAop-GCamP6s-p10 (experiment) and Hug1.2lexA/CyO; lexAop-GCamP6s/TM3, Sb (control) were placed with the SEZ region up on a Poly-L-lysine coated coverslide in a drop of saline. Coverslide was attached to a custom built heating device consisting of a 1.5 cm2 Peltier element for shifting the temperature from 20 to 30 °C by applying specific voltage values. Images were acquired with a Zeiss LSM 780 laser scanning microscope as time series with a speed of 781.96 ms (∼1.28 Hz) using a Zeiss LCI ‘Plan-Neofluar' 25 × per 0.8 Imm Korr DIC M27 objective dipped in the saline solution. Region of interest covered the complete HugPC neuronal ‘sprinkler-like' arborization pattern in the protocerebrum.
Statistics
For comparison of two groups in the two choice assays Mann-Whitney-Rank-Sum-Test was used. Statistical data were acquired as cumulative PI values of the last 5 min of the 20 min experiment and displayed as boxplots. For food intake analysis 32 °C values (% of red yeast in gut relative to whole body) were divided by the mean of all 18 °C values to gather the fold change of food intake. Fold changes were then compared with the Mann-Whitney-Rank-sum-Test. For electrophysiological data cycle frequencies were analysed at 18 °C for 60 s and 32 °C for 60 s. The fold change was calculated between 18 and 32 °C. Per larva a maximum of four temperature steps could be applied to the CNS during recordings. All fold changes of one genotype were then compared with the Mann-Whitney-Rank-Sum-Test with the other genotypes. The relative fold change is displayed as subtraction of the mean of OrgR × dTrpA1 control values from the fold change values of all genotypes (OrgR × dTrpA1 set to 0). CaMPARI data were analysed by calculating the mean Fred/Fgreen value of all eight HuginPC neurons of one larva to determine one mean Fred/Fgreen value per larva. Values were then compared with the Mann-Whitney-Rank-Sum-Test to water control.
Data availability
The authors declare that the data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article (and its Supplementary Information files), or available from the authors upon request.
Additional information
How to cite this article: Hückesfeld, S. et al. Central relay of bitter taste to the protocerebrum by peptidergic interneurons in the Drosophila brain. Nat. Commun. 7:12796 doi: 10.1038/ncomms12796 (2016).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Ingo Zinke and Andreas Schoofs for comments and discussions on the manuscript, Andreas Schoofs for help with electrophysiology, Philipp Schlegel and Anton Miroschnikow for sharing analysis scripts, Marius Wegner for reagents and Daniela Müller for help with behavioural experiments. We thank Kristin Scott, Andreas Thum and the Bloomington stock centre for sharing fly lines. This work was supported by grants from DFG (PA 787/7-1) to MJP and Bonn Cluster of Excellence ImmunoSensation.
Footnotes
Author contributions S.H. and M.J.P. designed experiments and wrote the manuscript. S.H. carried out the experiments and analysed the data. M.P. performed the GRASP experiment.
References
- Kwon J. Y., Dahanukar A., Weiss L. A. & Carlson J. R. Molecular and cellular organization of the taste system in the Drosophila larva. J. Neurosci. 31, 15300–15309 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apostolopoulou A. A., Rist A. & Thum A. S. Taste processing in Drosophila larvae. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 9, 1–9 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslin P. A. S. & Spector A. C. Mammalian taste perception. Curr. Biol. 18, R148–R155 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yarmolinsky D. A., Zuker C. S. & Ryba N. J. P. Common sense about taste: from mammals to insects. Cell 139, 234–244 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joseph R. M. & Carlson J. R. Drosophila chemoreceptors: a molecular interface between the chemical world and the brain. Trends Genet. 31, 683–695 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clyne P. J. Candidate taste receptors in Drosophila. Science 287, 1830–1834 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott K. et al. A chemosensory gene family encoding candidate gustatory and olfactory receptors. Cell 104, 661–673 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunipace L., Meister S., McNealy C. & Amrein H. Spatially restricted expression of candidate taste receptors in the Drosophila gustatory system. Curr. Biol. 11, 822–835 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss L. A., Dahanukar A., Kwon J. Y., Banerjee D. & Carlson J. R. The molecular and cellular basis of bitter taste in Drosophila. Neuron 69, 258–272 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris D. T., Kallman B. R., Mullaney B. C. & Scott K. Representations of taste modality in the Drosophila brain. Neuron 86, 1449–1460 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carleton A., Accolla R. & Simon S. A. Coding in the mammalian gustatory system. Trends Neurosci. 33, 326–334 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kain P. & Dahanukar A. Secondary taste neurons that convey sweet taste and starvation in the Drosophila brain. Neuron 85, 819–832 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melcher C. & Pankratz M. J. Candidate gustatory interneurons modulating feeding behavior in the Drosophila brain. PLoS Biol. 3, e305 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bader R. et al. Genetic dissection of neural circuit anatomy underlying feeding behavior in Drosophila: distinct classes of hugin-expressing neurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 502, 848–856 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marella S. et al. Imaging taste responses in the fly brain reveals a functional map of taste category and behavior. Neuron 49, 285–295 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moon S. J., Köttgen M., Jiao Y., Xu H. & Montell C. A taste receptor required for the caffeine response in vivo. Curr. Biol. 16, 1812–1817 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorne N., Chromey C., Bray S. & Amrein H. Taste perception and coding in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 14, 1065–1079 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez-Nunez L. et al. Reverse-correlation analysis of navigation dynamics in Drosophila larva using optogenetics. Elife 4, 1–4 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon M. D. & Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron 61, 373–384 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoofs A. et al. Selection of motor programs for suppressing food intake and inducing locomotion in the Drosophila brain. PLoS Biol. 12, e1001893 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niewalda T. et al. Salt processing in larval Drosophila: choice, feeding, and learning shift from appetitive to aversive in a concentration-dependent way. Chem. Senses 33, 685–692 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fosque B. F. et al. Labeling of active neural circuits in vivo with designed calcium integrators. Science 347, 755–760 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chu B., Chui V., Mann K. & Gordon M. D. Presynaptic gain control drives sweet and bitter taste integration in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 24, 1978–1984 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- König C., Schleyer M., Leibiger J., El-Keredy A. & Gerber B. Bitter-sweet processing in larval Drosophila. Chem. Senses 40, 445–445 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peng Y. et al. Sweet and bitter taste in the brain of awake behaving animals. Nature 527, 512–515 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- French A. S. et al. Dual mechanism for bitter avoidance in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 35, 3990–4004 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meunier N., Marion-Poll F., Rospars J.-P. & Tanimura T. Peripheral coding of bitter taste in Drosophila. J. Neurobiol. 56, 139–152 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alves G., Sallé J., Chaudy S., Dupas S. & Manière G. High-NaCl perception in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurosci. 34, 10884–10891 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y. V., Ni J. & Montell C. The molecular basis for attractive salt-taste coding in Drosophila. Science 340, 1334–1338 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura M., Baldwin D., Hannaford S., Palka J. & Montell C. Defective proboscis extension response (DPR), a member of the Ig superfamily required for the gustatory response to salt. J. Neurosci. 22, 3463–3472 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Z., Singhvi A., Kong P. & Scott K. Taste representations in the Drosophila brain. Cell 117, 981–991 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bader R. et al. The IGFBP7 homolog Imp-L2 promotes insulin signaling in distinct neurons of the Drosophila brain. J. Cell Sci. 126, 2571–2576 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou L. et al. Cooperative functions of the reaper and head involution defective genes in the programmed cell death of Drosophila central nervous system midline cells. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 94, 5131–5136 (1997). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kitamoto T. Conditional modification of behavior in Drosophila by targeted expression of a temperature-sensitive Shibire allele in defined neurons. J. Neurobiol. 47, 81–92 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The authors declare that the data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article (and its Supplementary Information files), or available from the authors upon request.