Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2016 Sep 19;5:e16349. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16349

AMPK acts as a molecular trigger to coordinate glutamatergic signals and adaptive behaviours during acute starvation

Moloud Ahmadi 1, Richard Roy 1,*
Editor: Kang Shen2
PMCID: PMC5028190  PMID: 27642785

Abstract

The stress associated with starvation is accompanied by compensatory behaviours that enhance foraging efficiency and increase the probability of encountering food. However, the molecular details of how hunger triggers changes in the activity of neural circuits to elicit these adaptive behavioural outcomes remains to be resolved. We show here that AMP-activated protein kinase (AMPK) regulates neuronal activity to elicit appropriate behavioural outcomes in response to acute starvation, and this effect is mediated by the coordinated modulation of glutamatergic inputs. AMPK targets both the AMPA-type glutamate receptor GLR-1 and the metabotropic glutamate receptor MGL-1 in one of the primary circuits that governs behavioural response to food availability in C. elegans. Overall, our study suggests that AMPK acts as a molecular trigger in the specific starvation-sensitive neurons to modulate glutamatergic inputs and to elicit adaptive behavioural outputs in response to acute starvation.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.001

Research Organism: C. elegans

eLife digest

Animals often need to adapt to changes in food availability in order to survive. When food is in short supply and animals are starving, their energy reserves are low. To conserve energy, behaviours that are not essential to survival, like mating, are put on hold. Instead, animals channel their energies into foraging strategies that may help them find new food sources. These behavioural changes are likely to be caused by changes in brain activity triggered by starvation.

It is not entirely clear how starvation changes the brain and consequently how an animal behaves. It is also difficult to study how the brain regulates behaviour in response to environmental changes like food availability in larger animals with more complex nervous systems. Instead, scientists often study less complex animals like a type of worm called C. elegans, because this model organism has a simpler nervous system and it is easier to observe its feeding behaviours. Previous observations have revealed that well-fed worms travel backwards when they are hungry, revisiting sites where they have previously found food. Yet, when the worms are starving, they move forward more frequently, presumably to find new sources of food.

Now, Ahmadi and Roy show starving worms activate an enzyme called AMP-activated protein kinase (or AMPK for short). Worms genetically engineered to lack this enzyme tend to move backward when they are starved, instead of moving forward like typical starving worms. This shows that AMPK triggers a wider search for new food sources. Further experiments showed that AMPK acts to inhibit two receptors, which in turn, affects the activity of two different neurons. These two neurons work together to change the animal’s behaviour and boost the likelihood the animal will be able to find a new food source when food is scarce.

More complex animals, including humans, also have the receptors and brain cells targeted by AMPK in response to starvation. Future studies are needed to determine whether a similar chain of events occurs in creatures with more complicated nervous systems.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.002

Introduction

Most organisms are faced with unpredictable fluctuations in their natural environment that often lead to periods of limited food resources. Their ability to adapt to these changes in resource availability is critical for survival and is often a driving force in evolution (Gray et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2005). When resources are scarce, pathways associated with energy conservation at both the organismal and the cellular levels become activated, and these are often complemented by behavioural modifications that simultaneously enhance foraging efficiency (Wang et al., 2005; Ashrafi, 2006).

The mammalian central nervous system (CNS) integrates internal and external cues that signal energy demand and availability and coordinately regulates outputs ranging from energy expenditure to feeding and associated locomotory behaviours (Cone, 2005; Balthasar et al., 2005; Belgardt et al., 2009; Dietrich and Horvath, 2011; Yang et al., 2011; Aponte et al., 2011; Sternson et al., 2013; Dietrich et al., 2015). In mice, the Agouti-related protein (AGRP)- and Pro-opiomelanocortin (POMC)-expressing neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus form a core circuit to regulate food intake and energy expenditure through the modulation of their neuronal activity in response to hormonal signals linked to metabolic status (Cowley et al., 2001; Bewick et al., 2005; Yang et al., 2011). The activity of both of these neuronal populations is mediated by engaging signalling pathways that control the strength and/or plasticity of rapid, excitatory glutamatergic transmission (Bito et al., 2010; Collingridge et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2012), but how energy stress results in changes in neuronal activity to elicit adaptive, or even compulsive behaviours are just now beginning to be elucidated (Dietrich et al., 2015).

The many signalling networks that are triggered throughout the nervous system that mediate the action of small molecules, hormones and nutrients on energy balance are of major interest due to their implication in, or treatment of various disorders. One of the key factors of paramount importance for metabolic homeostasis and survival is a highly conserved heterotrimeric protein kinase called AMP-activated protein kinase (AMPK). AMPK is regulated by the ratio of cellular AMP/ATP and by upstream activating kinases (Hardie, 2008; Hardie et al., 2012). It functions as a ‘fuel gauge’ to monitor cellular energy status by inhibiting anabolic pathways and activating catabolic pathways so as to generate sufficient levels of metabolic substrates required to maintain a minimal threshold of basal cellular activities (Hardie, 2008; Hardie et al., 2012; Hardie and Ashford, 2014)

Energy stress has been demonstrated to induce adaptive behaviours in a neuronal AMPK-dependent manner (Lee et al., 2008; Cunningham et al., 2014). Moreover, accumulating evidence has implicated AMPK in the hypothalamic regulation of metabolic rate and food intake behaviour (Kola, 2008; Lopez et al., 2010; Lim et al., 2010; Yang et al., 2011; Schneeberger and Claret, 2012). However, our understanding of how starvation influences adaptive foraging behaviours in an AMPK-dependent manner is still largely unknown, mostly due to the overwhelming complexity of the response in higher animals (Dietrich et al., 2015).

In C. elegans these foraging behaviours are comparatively simple, consisting of a series of forward or backward movements, specific turns, or changes in direction (Gray et al., 2005; Piggott et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2013; Hendricks, 2015). Food availability has been demonstrated to affect various aspects of these key elements in C. elegans locomotion (Sawin et al., 2000; Gray et al., 2005Chalasani et al., 2007; Flavell et al., 2013). In the absence of food, well-fed animals reverse frequently, a behavioural pattern that reflects a sensory memory of food that is expressed by the navigation circuit and results in efficient exploration of a limited area. In contrast, starvation suppresses reversals and induces forward movement (runs) that allow animals to explore distal areas; a strategy that is referred to as dispersal behaviour or alternatively, distal exploration (Gray et al., 2005).

The simplicity of the neural circuits and locomotory behaviours in conjunction with its amenability to genetic manipulation makes C. elegans an ideal model to investigate the mechanisms through which AMPK regulates neuronal activity and adaptive locomotory behaviour (searching for food) in response to hunger. Unlike most other organisms studied to date, C. elegans mutants that lack all AMPK signalling are viable, but show clear phenotypes when subjected to energy stress (Narbonne and Roy, 2006; 2009). Therefore, using calcium imaging, cell type–specific optogenetic techniques, and classic genetic analysis, we identified and characterized the neural circuit in which AMPK functions as a molecular switch. This circuit includes the AIB and AIY interneurons; two neurons that form one of the primary circuits that dictate appropriate food- and odour-evoked behaviours (Gray et al., 2005; Chalasani et al., 2007). We discovered that AMPK modulates AIB and AIY activity through two distinct mechanisms to ultimately ensure that adaptive foraging behaviours are appropriately triggered during periods of starvation. In the AIB interneurons AMPK regulates the abundance of the α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA) receptor GLR-1 in the postsynaptic elements, presumably by phosphorylation of serine 907 and 924 resulting in changes in synaptic strength and specific behavioural outputs. In addition, we also demonstrate that AMPK modulates a key metabotropic glutamate receptor called MGL-1 in the AIY interneuron at both mRNA and protein levels leading to an increase in AIY neuronal activity in starved animals. Together, our results indicate that AMPK acts as a starvation-inducible molecular trigger in the nervous system that modulates glutamatergic neuronal activity by at least two distinct mechanisms to modify behavioural outcomes in response to energy stress.

Results

Neuronal AMPK signalling triggers distal exploratory behaviour in starved animals

AMPK has been implicated in the regulation of feeding behaviours in higher animals (Minokoshi et al., 2004; Yang et al., 2011) although its essential role in development and cellular homeostasis has made it very difficult to study its role outside this closed circuit. Accordingly, the identification of the neuronal targets of this protein kinase that may be important for these behaviours has been virtually impossible to interrogate. Fortunately, C. elegans mutants that completely lack AMPK signalling are viable, thus providing us with an opportunity to investigate how AMPK regulates characteristic foraging behaviours in response to acute periods of starvation.

We and others found that removal of aak-2, the more prominent of two catalytic subunits of AMPK present in C. elegans, disrupts most cellular AMPK signalling and affects various aspects of C. elegans foraging behaviour (Lee et al., 2008Cunningham et al., 2014). More specifically, aak-2 mutants exhibit a behavioural profile in response to acute starvation that is more typical of satiated animals, where animals tend to explore locally rather than foraging in more distant locations. This is reflected by the frequency of body bends; which is considered as a representative readout for locomotory movement away from a nutrient-depleted environment in search of new resources, and reversals; a behaviour most often associated with local exploration (Gray et al., 2005). Both of these behaviours are affected in starved aak-2 mutants (Figure 1A). These behaviours appear to be regulated predominantly by aak-2 activity since removal of both catalytic subunits and hence eliminating all AMPK signalling (aak(0)) only modestly enhanced the phenotype of aak-2 mutants in all our behavioural assays suggesting that aak-2 mutants recapitulate a severe loss of AMPK function.

Figure 1. Neuronal AMPK regulates distal exploratory behaviour in starved animals.

(A) Starved aak-2 mutants display defective transition from local to distal exploration indicated by (i) decreased forward locomotion and (ii) increased reversal rate during acute bouts of starvation. These phenotypes are not dependent on aak-1 (n>40), (one-way ANOVA **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001). (B) AMPK mutants display persistent decreased forward locomotion rate (i) and increased reversal frequency (ii) during periods of acute starvation (n>10), Error bars represent ± SEM (Student's t-test, *p<0.05, **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001). (C) aak-2 reconstitution within the nervous system using the pan-neural [Punc-119::aak-2] transgene rescues the defective distal exploratory behaviour in starved aak-2 mutants while its reconstitution within the body wall muscle using [Punc-54::aak-2] transgene does not improve the distal exploratory behaviour of aak-2 mutants (n>15), (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001). (D,E,F) aak-2 is highly expressed throughout the nervous system. Scale bars are 40 μm. In the box and whisker plots (A,C) the central line is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.003

Figure 1—source data 1. Locomotory behaviour in AMPK mutant animals.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.004
Figure 1—source data 2. Food and stimuli-related behaviours in AMPK mutants.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.005
Figure 1—source data 3. Locomotory behaviour upon depletion of aak-2 with 1535 RNAi.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.006

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. AMPK function is not required for other food-related behaviours rather than distal exploratory behaviour.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) AMPK mutants display normal locomotory behaviour in the presence of food (i and ii) (n>20), (one-way ANOVA). In the box and whisker plots the central line is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points (B) Well-fed aak-2 mutants display normal basal locomotory behaviour once transferred to food (n>10). (C) Starved aak-2 mutants show normal enhanced slowing response once returned to food after 30 min of starvation (n>15). (D) aak-2 mutants display normal locomotory speed upon exposure to a gentle touch in the posterior part of their body in the absence of food (n>15). In (B,C) Error bars represent SEM (two-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001).
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Reduced forward locomotion rate in aak-2 mutants is not a consequence of developmental defect in their nervous system.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A) AMPK depletion in the late L2 stage by knocking down aak-2 in the nervous system resulted in a decreased forward locomotion rate and increased reversal frequency under starvation condition (n>15), (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001). In the box and whisker plots the central line is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points.

Since aak-2 is a major regulator of foraging behaviour in C. elegans, we transferred mid-fourth larval stage (L4) aak-2 mutant larvae to bacteria-free plates and monitored their movements for 20 hr to delineate sub-behaviours underlying aak-2 deficiency. We found that starved aak-2 mutants show defects in their rate of forward locomotion measured in the absence of food, which is compounded by their inability to appropriately suppress reversal behaviour during periods of more prolonged starvation (Figure 1B). Because we observe changes in each of these AMPK-dependent parameters in starved animals, and given the antagonistic relationship between forward and backward locomotion where increased reversal frequency affects the onset and/or duration of forward movement (Burbea et al., 2002; Juo et al., 2007), we chose to assess both behaviours in starved animals throughout our study.

The aak-2-dependent foraging defects we observed in starved animals are not likely to be due to a more general role of AMPK in the regulation of appropriate motor neuron development or function since AMPK mutants were comparable to wild type controls when reversal frequency and forward locomotion were quantified during conditions when food was abundant (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). Moreover, aak-2 mutants displayed a normal fleeing speed in response to a mechanical stimulus that was applied to the posterior (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). In addition, we observed that aak-2 (RNAi) performed at the late L2 stage, when the majority of neurons have already been generated, resulted in a reduction in forward locomotion compounded with an increase in reversal frequency in starved animals. This suggests that the reduced locomotory speed is unlikely to be the consequence of a general defect in motor function or neuromuscular development (Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

To discern whether aak-2 is specifically required in neurons, muscle, or both tissues to modulate distal exploratory behaviours we used tissue-specific promoters to drive aak-2 expression and test its ability to correct the behavioural defects typical of AMPK mutants. While an aak-2 cDNA (Narbonne and Roy, 2009) expressed under the control of a pan neuronal promoter [Punc-119::aak-2] rescued the distal exploratory defect of aak-2 mutants, aak-2 mutants continued to exhibit prolonged local exploration when the same aak-2 cDNA was reconstituted in body wall muscle using [Punc-54::aak-2] transgene (Figure 1C). Consistent with these results, we observed that a rescuing translational fusion reporter [Paak-2::aak-2::GFP] was broadly expressed throughout the nervous system (Figure 1D,E,F). These data are consistent with previous observations indicating that AMPK is required in the nervous system to regulate locomotory behaviour in response to food availability (Lee et al., 2008Cunningham et al., 2014).

AMPK is not required for all starvation-related behaviours

The ability of AMPK mutants to appropriately respond to starvation could result from a global AMPK-dependent defect in their nervous system rendering them incapable of responding to multiple cues in addition to starvation. However, this seems unlikely since two other food-related behaviours namely, basal slowing response and enhanced slowing response, which are triggered in well-fed and starved animals that are reintroduced to food, respectively, remain unaffected in AMPK mutants. In its natural habitat, a well-fed animal would be more likely to risk exploring distant locations for high quality food sources, whereas a starved animal would be less likely to stray far from a recently discovered vital food supply. These paradigms regulate behavioural plasticity in response to starvation in C. elegans and are mediated by distinct dopaminegic and serotonergic signalling (Sawin et al., 2000). When we re-introduced well-fed or starved aak-2 mutants to food we did not detect any significant difference in either the basal slowing response or the enhanced slowing response (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B,C) suggesting that AMPK is not involved in the behavioural plasticity that occurs after re-introduction of animals to food and that the neural circuitry that mediates such behavioural plasticity is presumably intact and functional in animals that lack AMPK.

AAK-2 is required in both the AIY and the AIB interneurons to mediate the transition from local to distal exploration in response to starvation

Since pan neuronal aak-2 expression rescued the distal exploration defect of aak-2 mutants, we next sought to identify the individual neurons that require aak-2 activity to trigger this behaviour. The neural circuitry that dictates these simple behavioural outcomes has been well described (White et al., 1976, 1986; Chalfie et al., 1985; Wicks et al., 1996; Gray et al., 2005). In particular, five pairs of command interneurons are required for the control of coordinated movement. The PVC and AVB interneurons are primarily required for the initiation of forward movement, while the AVA, AVD, and AVE interneurons control reversals (White et al., 1976; Chalfie et al., 1985; Wicks et al., 1996; Gray et al., 2005). Sensory inputs and serotonergic signalling together contribute to the behavioural changes that occur following removal from food (Gray et al., 2005). Moreover, several studies have underscored a critical role for both the AIY and the AIB interneurons, which are independently required to suppress and enhance reversals, respectively, resulting in longer or shorter durations of forward movement (Tsalik and Hobert, 2003; Wakabayashi et al., 2004; Gray et al., 2005; Chalasani et al., 2007; Luo et al., 2014). The RIM interneurons have also been determined as regulators of reversal frequency (Gray et al., 2005Gordus et al., 2015).

To map the precise sites of AMPK function in its regulation of distal exploratory behaviour, we introduced aak-2 in different subsets of neurons using neuronal sub-type-specific promoters: tph-1 (serotonergic neurons) (Cunningham et al., 2012), glr-1 (command interneurons) (Zheng et al., 1999), che-2 (chemosensory neurons) (Gray et al., 2005), ttx-3 (AIY) (Chalasani et al., 2007), npr-9(AIB) (Piggott et al., 2011), tdc-1 (RIM) (Cunningham et al., 2012) and rig-3 (AVA) (Marvin et al., 2013). Reconstitution of AMPK (aak-2) using the glr-1, tdc-1, ttx-3, npr-9 promoters was sufficient to partially rescue the defective distal exploration typical of aak-2 mutants suggesting that AMPK functions within these neurons to phosphorylate targets involved in regulating these behavioural responses. Conversely, introducing AMPK (aak-2) in the sensory neurons, the serotonergic neurons, or the command interneurons involved in triggering reversals failed to correct the defect (Figure 2A). As AMPK expression within the GLR-1-expressing neurons partially rescued the locomotory defect of aak-2 mutants, it is very likely that AMPK may be independently required in the GLR-1-expressing neurons AIB and RIM, although this does not exclude a role for aak-2 in other GLR-1-expressing neurons.

Figure 2. AMPK acts in the AIB and AIY interneurons to integrate chemosensory signals and trigger distal exploratory behaviour in starved animals.

(A) Targeted expression of aak-2 within the GLR-1-expressing neurons [Pglr-1::aak-2], RIM neurons [Ptdc-1::aak-2], AIB interneurons [Pnpr-9::aak-2] and AIY interneurons [Pttx-3::aak-2], but not chemosensory neurons [Pche-2::aak-2], serotonergic neurons [Ptph-1::aak-2] or AVA neurons [Prig-3::aak-2] partially restores the distal exploratory defect in starved aak-2 mutants by affecting both forward locomotion (i) and reversal frequency (ii) (n>20), (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001). (B) Simultaneous rescue of aak-2 in the AIB and AIY using [Pnpr-9::aak-2][Pttx-3::aak-2] transgenes is sufficient to completely restore the defective exploratory behaviour typical of starved aak-2 mutants (n>20), (one-way ANOVA ***p<0.0001). (C) Starved worms were placed 1.5 cm away from a spot of food (fresh OP50) and the time required to encounter the food was monitored for the worms that found food within 16 minutes. aak-2 mutants display defective food detection indicated by increased time spent to find food and this defect can be rescued by specific expression of aak-2 throughout the nervous system using [Punc-119::aak-2] transgene (n>10). (D) Targeted expression of aak-2 within the AIB and AIY interneurons, but not the chemosensory neurons rescue the defective chemosensation of aak-2 mutants toward IAA (n>300). Error bars in (C,D) represent ± SEM (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.009

Figure 2—source data 1. AMPK reconstitution in different neurons to restore the defective distal exploratory behaviour and defective chemotaxis typical of aak-2 mutants.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.010

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Neural circuits engaged in the regulation of locomotory behaviour in C. elegans.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Schematic showing the proposed circuitry that regulates locomotory behaviour upon food removal. Green and red colos show depolarized and hyperpolarized neurons, respectively. This circuitry consists of AWC sensory neurons and AIB and AIY interneurons. Food or odour removal results in AWC depolarization. Upon its depolarization, AWC releases glutamate at AWC/AIB and AWC/AIY synapses. The released glutamate then eventually acts on AMPA-type glutamate receptor GLR-1 in the AIB or glutamate-gated chloride channel GLC-3 in the AIY resulting in their depolarization and hyperpolarization, respectively. This neuronal pattern ultimately promotes reversals and turns resulting in shorter runs and increased local exploration (Chalasani et al., 2007). (B) Once AWC neuronal activity reduces, it however results in a reduction in the AIB neuronal activity and AIY depolarization that collectively leads to suppression of reversals and turns and inducing longer runs and distal exploration (Chalasani et al., 2007; Gordus et al., 2015). (C) Simplified wiring diagram showing the synaptic connection between AWC sensory neurons and three AIB, RIM and AVA interneurons in a circuit linking AWC to reversal behaviour. RIM, AIB and AVA participate in the overall network states. Once RIM is dominated, the circuit output is toward more probabilistic and upon domination of AIB the circuit output is more reliable (Model proposed by Gordus et al., 2015).

The AIY and AIB interneurons receive synapses from the AWC sensory neurons and act in parallel to suppress and enhance reversals, respectively, in response to food availability (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A,B) (Chalasani et al., 2007). The AIB interneurons relay their signals to the RIM and AVA, which in turn send their outputs to the muscles (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C) (Gordus et al., 2015). As an AMPK expression within the AIY, AIB and RIM partially restored the defective distal exploration of starved aak-2 mutants, it is likely that AMPK functions in multiple neurons in a parallel circuit to regulate locomotory behaviour in response to starvation.

When we expressed aak-2 in both the AIY and the AIB interneurons, we completely restored the defective distal exploration of aak-2 mutants, while expression of aak-2 within the RIM and the AIB interneurons, or in RIM and AIY, did not improve the distal exploration defect of starved aak-2 mutants beyond the rescue observed in AMPK mutant animals expressing aak-2 within the AIB or the AIY interneurons alone (Figure 2B). The RIM neurons form chemical and electrical synapses with AIB and AIY, respectively (White et al., 1986). Of note, it has been shown that chemical synapses from the RIM neurons enhance the variability in response to odour in the AWC-AIB-RIM olfactory circuit (Gordus et al., 2015). Therefore, aak-2 expression within the RIM neuron might influence this variability, whereby the expression of aak-2 within the AIB and RIM neurons biases the circuit towards either a more probabilistic or reliable output. Taken together, our data highlight a critical role of AMPK in the AIY and AIB interneurons to regulate the appropriate transition from local to distal exploration in animals subjected to acute starvation.

AMPK functions within the AIB and AIY interneurons to integrate sensory signals in response to starvation

AMPK mutants demonstrate reduced forward movement accompanied by an increased frequency of reversals. If these defects disrupt distal exploratory foraging behaviour then starved aak-2 mutants could also be defective in finding food at a distance. We therefore placed starved animals on plates with a point food source (E. coli) and monitored the time required for each animal to encounter the bacteria. We noted that aak-2 mutants were substantially less efficient than wild type controls in their ability to track to the food source (Figure 2C). However, from this assay alone, we were unable to distinguish if this defect is a consequence of a deficiency in their ability to sense food or to integrate the detection of nutritional signals to appropriately modify locomotory behaviour since we also observed a significant defect in the ability of AMPK mutants to chemotaxis toward the attractant isoamyl alcohol (IAA) (Figure 2D). However, since the expression of aak-2 within the sensory neurons failed to restore the defective distal exploratory behaviour in aak-2 mutants, while this defect was fully rescued by driving aak-2 expression specifically within the AIB and AIY interneurons, we conclude that AMPK is critical for the integration of sensory cues and not with chemosensation per se (Figure 2D). Taken together, our results indicate that AMPK is required in the AIB and AIY interneurons to trigger appropriate exploratory behaviour in response to acute starvation.

AMPK functions upstream of mgl-1, glr-1 and eat-4 to modulate distal exploration

To position how and where AMPK functions within the AIY and AIB interneurons to modulate the transition between local and distal exploration, we performed optogenetic experiments to manipulate AIB and AIY activity in starved aak-2 mutants (Kocabas et al., 2012). ChR2-mediated depolarization of the AIY interneurons in aak-2 mutant animals resulted in a significant decrease in reversal frequency that was persistent throughout the illumination time. In contrast, AIY activation in WT animals slightly altered the forward locomotion and reversal rate which is consistent with previous studies demonstrating the activation of the AIY interneurons in starved animals (Gray et al., 2005; Flavell et al., 2013) (Figure 3A, Figure 4—figure supplement 2A).

Figure 3. AMPK regulates glutamatergic signalling.

(A,B) ChR2-mediated activation of AIY interneurons (A), or ARCH -mediated silencing of AIB interneurons (B) in starved aak-2 mutants grown in the presence of all-trans retinal (ATR) induces distal exploration by suppressing reversals and inducing forward locomotion (n>15), Error bars represent ± SEM (two-way ANOVA *p<0.05, ***p<0.0001). (C,D,E) Compromised eat-4, glr-1 or mgl-1 function rescues the defective exploratory behaviour of starved aak-2 mutants by inducing forward locomotion (i) and suppressing reversals (ii) (n>20), (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001). In the box and whisker plots (C,D,E) the central line is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.012

Figure 3—source data 1. Optogenetics and epistatic analysis.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.013
Figure 3—source data 2. The analysis of locomotory behaviour of potential AMPK phospho targets.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.014
Figure 3—source data 3. Potential AMPK phosphorylation targets expressed within the AIB and AIY interneurons.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.015

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. glr-1, eat-4 and mgl-1 are epistatic to aak-2.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) sid-1 [Punc-119::sid-1+ Pmyo-2::mCherry] or aak-2; sid-1 [Punc-119::sid-1+ Pmyo-2::mCherry] young adult animals grown on HT-115 E. coli clones expressing double stranded RNAi targeting various RNAi clones were starved for 1 hr and their locomotory behaviour was assessed (n>10). Among all the potential phosphorylation targets of AMPK within the AIB and AIY interneurons, glr-1, eat-4 and mgl-1 displayed epistatic relationship with aak-2 in both forward (i) and backward locomotion (ii). (B) Depletion of nmr-1, avr-14 and mgl-2 in aak-2 background did not result in any additive or epistatic relationship with aak-2 (n>10). Error bars represent SEM (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001).

In a complementary experiment, we assessed the behaviour of starved aak-2 mutants following inactivation of AIB induced by the light-gated chloride pump archaerhodopsin-3 (Arch) (Kocabas et al., 2012). The Arch-induced hyperpolarization of AIB was sufficient to suppress reversals in the aak-2 mutants, while also promoting normal distal exploratory behaviour during starvation. In contrast, Arch-induced AIB inactivation in WT animals altered their locomotory behaviour slightly which can be explained by inactivation of AIB during acute bouts of starvation as the locomotory behaviour of starved animals has been shown to remain unchanged upon AIB ablation (Gray et al., 2005) (Figure 3B). We interpret these observations to indicate that although AMPK activity is required within the AIB and AIY interneurons for appropriate distal exploration in response to acute starvation, it does so without negatively impacting normal neurotransmission, neuronal development or connectivity within the interneurons.

Having identified the interneurons in which aak-2 is required to suppress reversals, promote forward locomotion, and eventually trigger distal exploration, we then set out to identify AMPK targets within these interneurons that mediate its effects in response to starvation. Using bioinformatic tools we scanned the C. elegans proteome for proteins that possessed consensus AMPK phosphorylation sites and were known to be expressed in the AIB or AIY interneurons, providing us with a list of potential neuronal phosphorylation targets of AMPK (Figure 3—source data 3).

Among the candidates that we identified through our bioinformatic analysis GLR-1, EAT-4, and MGL-1 emerged as highly predicted phosphorylation targets of AMPK (Figure 3C,D,E, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). eat-4 encodes an ortholog of a mammalian brain-specific sodium-dependent inorganic phosphate co-transporter I (BNPI), and is required for glutamatergic neurotransmission in the AIB (Lee et al., 1999Piggott et al., 2011). Interestingly, we found that eat-4 is epistatic to AMPK function during the response to acute starvation; single eat-4 mutants behave like starved WT animals by decreasing reversal frequency in contrast to the aak-2 mutants that exhibit an increased number of reversals. The aak-2; eat-4 double mutants behave more like starved WT (or eat-4) animals than aak-2 mutants (Figure 3C) indicating that eat-4 acts downstream of, or in parallel to AMPK, in the genetic pathway controlling this starvation-induced behaviour.

The AMPA-type GluR GLR-1 is expressed in both the command interneurons and in some motor neurons, and is implicated in memory formation and the behavioural responses to light nose touch and to sensory cues such as food (Hart et al., 1995; Rose et al., 2003; Chalasani et al., 2007). Moreover, GLR-1 function is required for glutamatergic activity in the AIB (Chalasani et al., 2007). Similar to eat-4, mutations in glr-1 suppress the defective distal exploratory behaviour of AMPK mutants suggesting that like eat-4, glr-1 is epistatic to aak-2 (Figure 3D). Taken together, these results indicate that blocking the glutamatergic inputs to the AIB through the elimination of glr-1, or blocking its glutamatergic neurotransmission by disrupting eat-4, can compensate for the behavioural defects in starved aak-2 mutants. Our data are therefore consistent with aak-2 acting upstream of, or in parallel to these two glutamatergic effectors to modulate neuronal activity in the AIB interneuron during periods of acute starvation.

As initially determined in mammals, the metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluRs) also have an important neuromodulatory role in glutamatergic transmission within the C. elegans nervous system (Dillon et al., 2006, 2015). mgl-1 encodes one of the three metabotropic glutamate receptors in C. elegans and is expressed throughout the AIY interneurons. Based on sequence analysis, MGL-1 is a group II metabotropic glutamate receptor that acts as both pre- and postsynaptic detectors of glutamate to reduce neuronal excitation at least partly by inhibition of adenylyl cyclase activity (Dillon et al., 2015; Niswender and Conn, 2010). Previous studies have identified mgl-1 as an essential component for the appropriate regulation of fat accumulation (Greer et al., 2008) and the starvation responses mediated by AIY (Kang and Avery, 2009). Our genetic analyses indicate that mutations in mgl-1 could also significantly, but not completely, suppress the prolonged local exploration typical of aak-2 mutants whereby a compromise of mgl-1 in the aak-2 background results in reduced reversals and increased distal runs (Figure 3E). These results suggest that AMPK may negatively regulate MGL-1 activity in the AIY in response to starvation.

Our data are consistent with AMPK acting as a potential modulator of glutamatergic inputs in both the AIB and the AIY, but it cannot distinguish whether AMPK affects glutamatergic inputs in a subset of glutamatergic neurons (i.e.,the AIB and AIY) or in all glutamatergic neurons. Furthermore, if AMPK does have a specific role in the AIB and the AIY, then what determines its functionality in this subset of AMPK-sensitive neurons? To test if AMPK affects other mediators of glutamatergic inputs we assessed whether AMPK compromise could modify the phenotypes associated with: nmr-1, which encodes a NMDA-type ionotropic glutamate receptor subunit; the glutamate-gated chloride channels avr-14 and mgl-2 which encodes a group I metabotropic glutamate receptor (Chalasani et al., 2007; Piggott et al., 2011; Dillon et al., 2015). Double mutant combinations with aak-2 did not modify any of the aak-2 phenotypes we tested (Figure 3—figure supplement 1B) suggesting that aak-2 does not affect glutamatergic transmission generally, but rather it seems to only affect a subset of glutamatergic neurons critical for starvation-dependent functions and/or behaviours.

Parallel and opposing function of AMPK in the modulation of AIB and AIY outputs

Given the epistatic relationship between aak-2 and genes involved in glutamatergic signalling we wanted to clarify if AMPK regulates the AIB and AIY neural activity through these targets. We monitored calcium transients using the genetically-encoded calcium sensor G-CaMP individually in both interneurons as a proxy for neuronal activity (Figure 4A,B,C,D,E,F). We observed a decrease in AIY activity in starved aak-2 mutants that was rescued by AIY-specific aak-2 expression, consistent with aak-2 regulating AIY activity/response during starvation (Figure 4A,B,C,G, Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). Previous studies have shown that calcium spikes in the AIY interneurons correlate positively with forward runs and negatively with the initiation of reversals with gradual increases preceding forward run initiation (Flavell et al., 2013; Luo et al., 2014). Our results suggest that the decreased AIY activity in starved aak-2 mutants at least partially contributes to their defective distal exploratory behaviour. To further investigate whether the increased reversal rate correlates with decreased AIY activity in starved aak-2 mutants, we monitored the calcium levels in the AIY interneuron of freely behaving animals. The AIY calcium levels were consistently higher upon the termination of reversals, and during the subsequent forward runs with gradual increase preceding forward run initiation suggesting that the calcium peaks in the AIY correlate with the suppression of reversals and the observed acute increase in forward speed (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A). Furthermore, consistent with our previous results, we found that starved aak-2 mutants display increased reversal frequency and shorter durations of forward locomotion, both of which correlated with decreased calcium influx and associated AIY activity (Figure 4—figure supplement 2C,E,F).

Figure 4. AMPK regulates AIB and AIY neuronal activity.

(A,B,C,D,E,F) A sample ΔF/Fb plot showing spontaneous changes in the AIY and AIB neuronal activity in starved individuals during the course of a 65 and 120 s imaging window, respectively. These plots are the basis of the data in G, H, I and J. The heatmaps show normalized G-CaMP1 and G-CaMP3 traces in AIY and AIB, respectively in multiple animals. (G) The average total integrated fluorescence intensity (ΔF/Fb) over the course of a 65 s window. AIY neuronal activity is reduced in starved aak-2 mutants and this reduction in the AIY neuronal activity is restored by targeted expression of aak-2 within the AIY interneurons using the AIY-specific [Pttx-3::aak-2] transgene (n>10). (H) Removal of mgl-1 in aak-2 mutants rescues the reduced AIY neuronal activity observed in starved aak-2 mutants (n>10). (I) The average total integrated fluorescence intensity (ΔF/Fb) in AIB is increased upon removal of aak-2 in starved animals, and this increase is reversed by expression of aak-2 within the AIB using the AIB-specific [Pnpr-9::aak-2] transgene (n>10). (J) Removal of glr-1, but not eat-4, restores the increased AIB neuronal activity in starved aak-2 mutants (n>10). Error bars represent ± SEM (one-way ANOVA ***p<0.0001).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.017

Figure 4—source data 1. Calcium imaging in the AIB and AIY interneurons in WT, aak-2, mgl-1, glr-1 and eat-4 and double mutants.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.018
Figure 4—source data 2. Calcium imaging in freely WT and aak-2 behaving animals.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.019

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Heat maps showing AIY and AIB spontaneous neuronal activity in starved animals.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A,B) Starved animals were placed on 10% agarose pads and the AIY and AIB neuronal activity was measured for 65 and 120 s respectively (n>10). G-CaMP imaging was performed on a Zeiss microscope. Images were captured by AxioVision software at 3Hz for AIY and 2Hz for AIB and were analyzed by imageJ. Baseline F (Fb) was measured as the global minima for 5–10 frames over the duration of each time-lapse sequence and the ΔF/Fb results were normalized between 0 and 1 for each animal.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. AIY and AIB spontaneous neuronal activity in starved freely behaving animals.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A,B) A sample normalized ΔF/Fb plot showing spontaneous changes in the AIY and AIB neuronal activity in starved individuals during the course of a 90 s imaging window. AIY calcium levels decreases at the onset of reversals and reach the maximum levels at the onset of forward locomotion and it remains consistently high during forward runs (n>6) (A). AIB calcium level increases during reversals and it gradually decreases and return to baseline values with the resumption of forward movement (n>6) (B). (C,D) aak-2 mutants display higher reversal frequency and decreased duration of forward runs which is correlated with increased AIB calcium levels and decreased AIY neuronal activity (n>6). (E,F) The decreased forward locomotion rate and increased reversal frequency in aak-2 mutants are consistently correlated by increased AIB neuronal activity and decreased AIY activity (n>6). Error bars represent ± SEM (two-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001).

The epistatic relationship between mgl-1 and aak-2 in distal exploration was further corroborated by the total integrated fluorescence intensity demonstrated by aak-2; mgl-1 animals. These readings resemble both starved WT and/or mgl-1 mutants, reinforcing our data indicating that mgl-1 is epistatic to aak-2 function in the AIY interneuron during acute starvation, where MGL-1 potentially acts as a postsynaptic receptor in the AIY (Figure 4H—figure supplement 1A). AIY-ablated animals are defective in transitioning between local and distal exploration (Gray et al., 2005), these results reveal an additional, albeit critical, role of AMPK within the AIY interneurons to modulate AIY neuronal activity autonomously, thus contributing to the behavioural transition associated with the decision to explore more distal environments when animals are subjected to starvation.

Unlike the AIY interneurons, activity in AIB increased in starved aak-2 mutants and could be rescued by AIB-specific aak-2 expression (Figure 4D,E,F,I). Calcium peaks in the AIB have been shown to correlate with reversal frequency (Piggott et al., 2011; Gordus et al., 2015), but during our recording the animals are restrained therefore we presume that each calcium peak is representative of a reversal attempt. Given the increased calcium levels in the AIB interneurons in starved aak-2 mutants, our data indirectly suggest that the increased reversal frequency and the abnormally prolonged local exploration is at least partly a consequence of increased AIB neuronal activity. To further examine if the increased AIB activity in starved aak-2 mutants correlates with their increased reversal frequency, we examined the behaviour of freely moving animals during acute starvation. The calcium peaks in the AIB were well correlated with reversal frequency, further suggesting that the increased reversal frequency in starved aak-2 mutants is at least partially due to the increased AIB activity (Figure 4—figure supplement 2B,D,G,H).

The genetic requirement for aak-2 in the AIB interneurons to modulate neuronal activity, along with its epistatic relationship with eat-4 and glr-1 in the modulation of locomotory behaviour, are consistent with at least two models: During acute starvation aak-2 might regulate excitatory synaptic inputs onto the AIB interneurons through modulation of glr-1 activity, or aak-2 might affect glutamatergic neurotransmission from the AIB interneurons through, or in parallel with eat-4. To distinguish between these possibilities, we monitored calcium transients in aak-2; glr-1 and aak-2; [Pnpr-9::eat-4(RNAi)] double mutants. Of note, we observed that although blocking the AIB neurotransmission in aak-2; [Pnpr-9::eat-4 (RNAi)] animals resulted in normal distal exploratory behaviour during starvation (Figure 3C), the total integrated signal was significantly higher compared to both WT and [Pnpr-9::eat-4 (RNAi)] animals (Figure 4J, Figure 4—figure supplement 1B). This suggests that aak-2 acts independently of eat-4 to modulate neural activity in the AIB.

Taken together, our genetic analyses indicate that AMPK is required within the AIY and the AIB interneurons to modulate their neuronal activity through its direct or indirect regulation of the glutamatergic receptors GLR-1 and MGL-1 during starvation-induced exploratory behaviour.

AMPK controls GLR-1 abundance in the neuronal postsynaptic membranes

Increased GLR-1-mediated glutamatergic inputs have been shown to bias the locomotory circuit toward reversals (Zheng et al., 1999). Consistent with this, overexpression of GLR-1 specifically within the AIB interneurons increases reversal frequency and turning rate in the absence of food (Chalasani et al., 2007). Because the abundance of GLR-1 within the AIB influences reversal behaviour, we tested whether the increased reversal frequency in aak-2 mutants is a consequence of changes in GLR-1 levels. We quantified glr-1 mRNA levels in WT and aak-2 mutants under well-fed and starved conditions and we did not detect any differences (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A). However, we did notice that total GLR-1 protein levels in starved WT animals were reduced and this effect was less pronounced in starved aak-2 mutants (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B) suggesting that AMPK may modulate GLR-1 protein levels in starved animals either at the level of synthesis or stability.

Studies have demonstrated that disruption of GLR-1 endocytosis results in increased GLR-1 abundance in the postsynaptic elements which results in increased synaptic strength and reversal frequency (Burbea et al., 2002; Juo et al., 2007). Given the epistatic relationship between aak-2 and glr-1 we next asked if the increased reversal frequency in starved aak-2 mutants occurs as a result of decreased GLR-1 turnover linked to a change in its post-translational regulation due to the absence of AMPK. To examine this possibility, we used a previously described chimaeric receptor tagged with the green fluorescent protein (GLR-1::GFP) that localizes to discrete punctate structures and that can be used to visualize central glutamatergic synapses in living animals (Burbea et al., 2002). We measured the density of GLR-1::GFP-containing puncta in the AIB neuronal process in a strain that expresses GLR-1::GFP driven by the AIB-specific promoter npr-9. Notably, we observed that the GLR-1 levels in the AIB neuronal process in the nerve ring where the AIB interneurons form their synapses (Figure 5A) was significantly greater in aak-2 mutants than WT animals during starvation (Figure 5B,C). Moreover, this difference could be rescued by introducing AMPK specifically within the AIB interneurons (Figure 5C), highlighting the importance of AMPK in modulating GLR-1 abundance within the AIB interneurons.

Figure 5. AMPK regulates GLR-1 abundance in the AIB neuronal process and the ventral nerve cord.

(A) Schematic representation showing AIB interneurons that extend their processes into the nerve ring. (B,C) Representative images of [Pnpr-9::glr-1::gfp] in the AIB neuronal processes in starved WT and aak-2 mutants (B). GLR-1::GFP level is significantly increased in the AIB neuronal processes in the aak-2 mutants compared to WT animals under starvation condition. Targeted expression of aak-2 within the AIB interneurons rescues the increased GLR-1::GFP level in starved aak-2 mutants (C). At least 3 [Pnpr-9::glr-1::gfp] transgenic lines were separately examined for this experiment (n>20), Error bars represent ± SEM (one-way ANOVA ***p<0.0001). Scale bars are 10 μm. (D,E,F) Representative images of [Pglr-1::glr-1::gfp] in the ventral nerve cord (D). GLR-1::GFP level (E) and puncta width (F) are significantly increased in the ventral nerve cord of starved aak-2 mutants compared to WT animals. (n>20), Error bars represent ± SEM (Student's 2-tailed t test ***p<0.0001). Scale bars are 10 μm.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.022

Figure 5—source data 1. Measurement of GLR-1 abundance in the AIB neuronal process and ventral nerve cord of WT and aak-2 mutants.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.023
Figure 5—source data 2. The extent of GLR-1 and EAT-4 colocalization in WT and aak-2 mutants.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.024

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. AMPK regulates GLR-1 abundance by affecting steady state protein levels.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Relative messenger RNA levels were analyzed with semi-quantitative RT-PCR in well-fed and starved animals. glr-1 mRNA levels were similar in well-fed and starved WT and aak-2 mutants. (B) Western blot analysis of GLR-1 levels in Well-fed and starved WT and aak-2 mutants expressing GLR-1::GFP using GFP antibody. GLR-1 protein levels decrease upon starvation in WT animals and this is less pronounced in aak-2 mutants. (C,D,E) Co-localization (arrows) of EAT-4 with GLR-1 is shown in the merged images. GLR-1 puncta were closely opposed to EAT-4 puncta in both WT (C) and aak-2 mutants (D) (n>7), Error bars represent ± SEM (Student's two-tailed t test). (F) A schematic showing the 3 AMPK phosphorylation sites in the cytoplasmic tail of the GLR-1 protein two of which are conserved between C. elegans and rat glutamate receptor GluR1.

Our data suggest that AMPK regulates the GLR-1 abundance in the AIB interneurons to potentially modulate synaptic inputs and consequently affect reversal behaviour. But aak-2 mutants have additional behavioural defects that may also manifest due to inappropriate glutamatergic signalling (Cunningham et al., 2012). Therefore, to further investigate if the effect of AMPK on GLR-1 abundance is limited to the AIB interneurons or alternatively AMPK is globally required for the modulation of GLR-1 steady state levels throughout the nervous system, we measured the density of GLR-1::GFP-containing puncta in the anterior region of the ventral nerve cord between the RIG neuron cell bodies, and in the vulva using the [Pglr-1::glr-1::gfp] transgene (Burbea et al., 2002). Interestingly, we observed a significant increase in the GLR-1 levels and the size (diameter) of GLR-1-expressing puncta in starved aak-2 mutant animals compared to starved WT controls (Figure 5D,E,F). Furthermore, we noted that the GLR-1::GFP signal in aak-2 mutants overlapped with that of EAT-4 which marks the majority of glutamatergic synapses (in Figure 5—figure supplement 1C,D,E). To summarize, the removal of aak-2 results in an increase in GLR-1 abundance in starved animals and a large fraction of the GLR-1::GFP puncta seen in the mutants correspond to postsynaptic elements.

As AIB interneurons form their synapses in the nerve ring, the increased GLR-1 abundance in the ventral nerve cord of aak-2 mutants must be a consequence of AMPK compromise in additional GLR-1-expressing neurons. Since the quantification of fluorescent intensity and size/width of the puncta are two determinants of total receptor abundance at each synapse, these results collectively suggest that GLR-1 levels at the postsynaptic elements are globally changed in aak-2 mutants. Therefore, AMPK compromise is associated with increased steady state GLR-1 protein levels and GLR-1::GFP fluorescence, and that most of the GLR-1 receptors may be degraded in an AMPK-dependent manner, or alternatively, that GLR-1 synthesis may also be affected in starved aak-2 mutants.

AMPK acts with UNC-11/AP180 to regulate GLR-1 levels within the postsynaptic elements and consequently modulate synaptic strength

Mammalian AMPA receptors are removed from postsynaptic membranes by clathrin-mediated endocytosis (Carroll et al., 1999; Man et al., 2000). In C. elegans, the ubiquitylation of conserved cytoplasmic lysine residues on GLR-1 constitutes an endocytic signal to remove the receptors from postsynaptic elements which subsequently targets them for degradation; a process that is mediated by the unc-11/AP180 clathrin adaptin protein (Burbea et al., 2002). To further investigate if AMPK modulates GLR-1 abundance by affecting endocytosis in response to starvation, we measured GLR-1 abundance in unc-11 mutants that are unable to internalize GLR-1. The GLR-1 levels in aak-2; unc-11 double mutants were indistinguishable from those found in either unc-11 single mutants (Figure 6A,B,C). Similarly, the increased GLR-1 levels and size/width of GLR-1 puncta measured in the aak-2 mutants was not further enhanced when all four cytoplasmic lysine residues required for GLR-1 ubiquitylation and endocytosis were mutated to arginine [GLR-1(4KR)::GFP], further suggesting that aak-2 acts in a linear pathway with an unc-11/clathrin-mediated mechanism to regulate GLR-1 endocytosis (Figure 6A,B,C).

Figure 6. AMPK directly regulates GLR-1 abundance through endocytic pathway.

Figure 6.

(A) Representative images of [Pglr-1::glr-1::gfp] in the ventral nerve cord in starved WT, aak-2, unc-11 and aak-2; unc-11 mutants. Scale bars are 10 μm. (B,C) Similar to starved aak-2 mutants, GLR-1::GFP level (B) and puncta width (C) are increased upon removal of unc-11 or mutation of four lysines required for GLR-1 ubiquitination and endocytosis [Pglr-1::glr-1(4KR)::gfp]. Increased GLR-1::GFP level and enhanced puncta width are not further enhanced upon depletion of aak-2 in GLR-1 endocytosis defective mutants (n>25), Error bars represent ± SEM (one-way ANOVA ***p<0.0001). (D) Mutating AMPK phosphorylation sites S907 and S924 to non-phosphorylable alanine residues present in the cytoplasmic domain of GLR-1 (S907A, S924A) results in increased reversal frequency compounded with reduced forward locomotion and this defect can be rescued by introducing eat-4 mutations (n>10), (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001). In the box and whisker plots the central line is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points. (E) Expression of the non-phosphorylable variant of [Pnpr-9::glr-1(S907A, S924A)::gfp] results in increased GLR-1::GFP level in the AIB neuronal process and it is not further increased upon disruption of endocytosis in unc-11 mutants. At least 3 transgenic lines expressing non-phosphorylable variant of GLR-1 (S907A, S924A) were separately examined for this experiment (n>15), Error bars represent ± SEM (one-way ANOVA ***p<0.0001).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.026

Figure 6—source data 1. Analysis of GLR-1 abundance in endocytosis defective mutants.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.027

Activation of AMPK during periods of starvation could result in the regulation of GLR-1 abundance by its direct phosphorylation through its consensus phosphorylation sites. To determine the importance of these sites we used the [Pnpr-9::glr-1::gfp] fusion protein as a template to generate a GLR-1::GFP variant with non-phosphorylable AMPK sites. The cytoplasmic domain of GluR has already been described as an important phosphoregulatory target, mostly by kinases such as PKA and PKC (Roche et al., 1996). Since two conserved AMPK phosphorylation sites were identified within the cytoplasmic domain of GLR-1 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1F), we mutated both S907 and S924 to non-phosphorylable alanine residues to evaluate how these sites contribute to the stability of GLR-1 within the AIB neuronal process during periods of starvation. Wild type animals bearing the compound (S907A, S924A) variant transgene demonstrated defective exploratory behaviour that was comparable to aak-2 mutants (Figure 6D). To further examine if the GLR-1 receptors that accumulate in AMPK mutants act as functional synaptic receptors we examined the locomotory behaviour of eat-4; glr-1 double mutants expressing the non-phosphorylable variant (S907A, S924A). We noticed that the increased reversal frequency rate accompanied by reduced forward locomotion in animals expressing the (S907A, S924A) variant was largely suppressed in the absence of eat-4 indicating that the compromise of AMPK signalling results in an increased abundance of functional synaptic GLR-1 receptors (Figure 6D). Furthermore, the GLR-1 (S907A, S924A) variant accumulated in the AIB neuronal process and this accumulation was not enhanced upon removal of unc-11 (Figure 6E), further suggesting that AMPK regulates GLR-1 abundance directly by phosphorylating the protein to mediate UNC-11/AP180-dependent GLR-1 endocytosis in response to starvation.

Elevated glutamatergic transmission within the command interneurons is associated with a hyper-reversal phenotype and a dramatic decrease in the duration of forward movement (Zheng et al., 1999). It would therefore be intuitive that an increase in synaptic strength, or input, would result in a consequential increase in reversal frequency. Although we were unable to directly examine the role of AMPK in the regulation of synaptic strength, taken together, our data suggest that AMPK modulates synaptic strength by regulating GLR-1 abundance in the postsynaptic puncta to bias the behavioural readout in response to starvation.

AMPK modulates MGL-1 in the mRNA and protein levels in the AIY interneurons to modify distal exploratory behaviour in response to starvation

The results of our epistasis analysis between mgl-1 and aak-2 are consistent with mgl-1 acting downstream of aak-2 to regulate the transition from local to distal exploration in parallel with glr-1 in response to starvation. Therefore, to further explore the mechanism by which AMPK regulates MGL-1 activity in AIY, we determined the fluorescence intensity of a [Pmgl-1::mgl-1::GFP] translational fusion protein in both well-fed and starved animals. Starved animals all showed reduced MGL-1 levels, which was completely reversed in aak-2 mutants (Figure 7A,B, Figure 7—figure supplement 1A). Moreover, expressing aak-2 specifically within the AIY interneurons was sufficient to restore MGL-1 levels to those of starved WT animals suggesting that AMPK activity modulates MGL-1 abundance in the AIY in response to starvation.

Figure 7. AMPK regulates the abundance of MGL-1 levels, while direct phosphorylation may affect MGL-1 function.

(A,B) Starvation reduces [MGL-1::GFP] levels significantly in the AIY [Pttx-3::RFP] of WT animals and to a lesser extent in aak-2 mutants (AIY neuron is shown in the circle). Increased [MGL-1::GFP] levels in starved aak-2 mutants can be reversed by targeted expression of aak-2 in the AIY interneurons using [Pttx-3::aak-2] transgene (n>15), Error bars represent ± SEM (two-way ANOVA ***p<0.0001). Scale bars are 20 μm. (C) Mutating the consensus AMPK phosphorylation site at serine 234 to a non-phosphorylable alanine in MGL-1 (S234A) resulted in a reduced forward locomotion rate that is not further reduced in aak-2; mgl-1 (S234A) double mutants (i) (n>10), (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001). In the box and whisker plots the central line is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.028

Figure 7—source data 1. Analysis of MGL-1 abundance in WT and aak-2 mutants.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.029

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. AMPK regulates MGL-1 by modulating its steady state mRNA levels while also affecting MGL-1 function through direct phosphorylation.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) [MGL-1::GFP] levels are unchanged in well fed WT and aak-2 mutants in the AIY interneurons (shown in the circle) indicated by [TTX-3::RFP]. Scale bars represent 20 μm. (B,C) Relative messenger RNA levels were analyzed with semi-quantitative RT-PCR in well-fed and starved animals. mgl-1 mRNA levels (normalized to WT) are significantly higher in starved aak-2 mutants compared to starved WT animals. Error bars represent ± SEM (two-way ANOVA *p<0.05). (D) A schematic showing the conserved AMPK phosphorylation site in C. elegans MGL-1 and human GRM2 and GRM3 group II metabotropic glutamate receptors. (E) Normalized [MGL-1::GFP] levels are unchanged upon mutating serine 234 in mgl-1 (n>10), Error bars represent ± SEM (one-way ANOVA ***p<0.0001).

To determine how AMPK affected the MGL-1 abundance in the AIY we first measured the mRNA levels in well-fed and starved animals using semi quantitative RT-PCR, while in parallel we also monitored glc-3 levels which are expressed in the AIY where it acts as a glutamate-gated chloride channel required for local search behaviour (Chalasani et al., 2007). Although we did not observe any change in the levels of glc-3 mRNA in well-fed or starved animals, we did observe an slight increase in mgl-1 mRNA levels in well-fed and a more significant increase in starved aak-2 mutants (Figure 7—figure supplement 1B,C) suggesting that AMPK regulates MGL-1 levels by either blocking its transcription or affecting the stability of mgl-1 transcripts. The former possibility is less plausible given the short time delay that occurs between the response to food deprivation and the consequent behavioural changes. Since we did not detect any significant change in the mgl-1 mRNA levels in WT animals, it is likely that AMPK controls the abundance of mgl-1 transcripts in the AIY and possibly other mgl-1-expressing neurons in response to starvation, typical of its function in adapting to energy stress. These results however do not exclude the possible role of AMPK in regulating MGL-1 protein levels directly through phosphorylation, as has been described for some ion channels (Lang and Föller, 2014), although this would be in addition to its observed impact on mgl-1 transcript abundance.

To further investigate this possibility, we mutated the conserved potential AMPK phosphorylation site in MGL-1 (Figure 7—figure supplement 1D) to examine if AMPK also regulates MGL-1 through direct phosphorylation. We noted that mutating serine 234 to alanine (S234A) resulted in a modest, but nevertheless significant reduction in forward locomotion (Figure 7C), although this mutation did not result in increased MGL-1 levels (Figure 7—figure supplement 1E). This suggests that AMPK regulates mgl-1 activity by impinging on both mRNA and protein function. The AMPK-mediated phosphorylation of serine 234 in MGL-1 may be required for the modulation of distal exploratory behaviour independent of its regulation of MGL-1 levels. We currently cannot exclude that this post-translational modification could affect some aspect of MGL-1 receptor function. Taken together, our results are consistent with a role for AMPK in modulating mgl-1 function in the AIY interneurons whereby mgl-1 activity is attenuated in response to starvation-induced AMPK signalling resulting in increased AIY activity and the consequent suppression of reversals.

Our results so far suggest that increased GLR-1 and MGL-1 abundance in the AIB and AIY interneurons, respectively, cause the defective distal exploration in starved aak-2 animals. To further test whether indeed this may be the case we overexpressed GLR-1 and MGL-1 in the AIB and AIY interneurons, respectively, in WT animals to verify if this misexpression could recapitulate the defective distal exploration of starved aak-2 mutants. The overexpression of MGL-1 in the AIY resulted in a phenotype that was quite similar to that seen in starved aak-2 mutants whereas overexpression of GLR-1 in the AIB resulted in a more modest, but nevertheless significant, defect in transition between local to distal exploration. This difference can be explained by the fact that the overexpressed GLR-1 is most likely still targeted for endocytosis and degradation while AMPK is present and active, while in aak-2 mutants there is a defect in GLR-1 endocytosis allowing it to accumulate to functionally critical levels in the puncta. Nevertheless, the overexpression of either of these transgenes results in a behavioural defect which phenocopies that of starved aak-2 mutants (Figure 8—figure supplement 1).

Discussion

In most free-living organisms energy stress is often accompanied by characteristic compensatory behaviours that are triggered to increase the probability of encountering a food source (Wang et al., 2005; Gray et al., 2005; Dietrich et al., 2015). The spectrum of these responses however varies dramatically and naturally reflects both the environment and the physiology of the organism. Humans are characteristically hypersensitive and/or rapidly irritable during periods of acute food deprivation providing a familiar example of how a collection of neural circuits become reproducibly activated or blocked in response to this specific acute stress, culminating in highly predictable behavioural responses. These behavioural changes are achieved in part through synaptic plasticity, which is required for adaptation to varying circumstances in an experience-dependent manner (Bito, 2010; Collingridge et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2012). During our work we have taken advantage of a common physiological response to an acute environmental stress in order to delineate the genetic pathway, the corresponding neuronal circuitry, and the biochemical targets that drive adaptive exploratory behaviours in response to starvation in C. elegans.

AMPK function within the AWC-AIB-AIY circuitry is required for modification of distal exploratory behaviour in response to acute starvation

We and others found that the disruption of AMPK signalling resulted in marked changes in exploratory behaviour partly by reduced forward locomotion (Lee et al., 2008; Cunningham et al., 2014). However, we also noticed that starved aak-2 animals execute frequent reversals, which might contribute significantly to their defective distal exploration given the antagonistic relationship between the well-characterized forward and backward locomotory circuits. We were able to improve forward locomotion by the targeted expression of aak-2 in the AIB and AIY interneurons, while consequently reducing the abnormally frequent reversal behaviour typical of AMPK mutants, consistent with the involvement of AIB and AIY in regulating both reversal frequency and the duration of forward movement (Tsalik and Hobert, 2003; Wakabayashi et al., 2004). Notably, unlike aak-2 expression in the sensory neurons, its expression within the AIB and AIY interneurons also improved the chemotaxis defect of aak-2 mutants suggesting that AMPK may be critical for the integration of sensory signals received by AIB and AIY interneurons.

A recent study showed that aak-2 compromise mimics the effect of elevated serotonin on movement (Cunningham et al., 2014). The AIB and AIY interneurons synapse directly or indirectly onto the command interneurons to control whether C. elegans moves forward or backward (Gray et al., 2005; Chalasani et al., 2007; Piggott et al., 2011; Chen et al., 2013). Moreover, these neurons have been identified as potential targets of serotonergic signalling to mediate the effects of serotonin on locomotion (Harris et al., 2009). It is likely therefore that aak-2 acts within the AIB and AIY interneurons to modulate the effects of serotonergic signalling on the resulting movement rather than functioning directly within the serotonergic neurons per se.

AMPK acts as a molecular trigger in the AWC-AIB-AIY circuitry to modify their neuronal activity and the resulting behavioural outputs

The very short time delay that occurs between the response to the lack of food and the observed behaviours makes it unlikely that the effects of AMPK within the AIB and AIY interneurons are mediated by transcription. Alternatively, AMPK has been demonstrated to act directly upon ion channels and gap junctions (Hallows et al., 2000; Lang and Föller, 2014). Our calcium imaging data are consistent with a model whereby starvation-induced AMPK activation leads to the inhibition of neuronal activity within the AIB, while promoting activity in the AIY interneuron, thereby altering behavioural outputs in a coordinated manner.

How then can we account for the AMPK-dependent starvation-induced changes in calcium levels within the AIB and AIY interneurons and the effects on their respective neural activity? Neuronal activity results from the interplay between synaptic excitation and inhibition. In the brain, excitation is mainly achieved through glutamatergic transmission (Bito, 2010; Collingridge et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2012). Remarkably, our results delineate a novel role for AMPK in the modulation of neuronal activity in response to starvation through the simultaneous regulation of glutamatergic inputs mediated by two different receptors: GluR/GLR-1 in the AIB and MGL-1 in the AIY. Of note, unlike mgl-1 null mutations that resulted in the partial rescue of locomotory behaviour of aak-2 mutants, we noticed a complete rescue upon removal of eat-4 or glr-1. As eat-4 is the only identified vesicular glutamate transporter in C. elegans, inhibition of eat-4 is expected to result in a complete inactivation of both mgl-1- and glr-1-dependent outputs that are both potentially dependent on glutamate release. In the case of glr-1, AIY may require either direct or indirect synaptic inputs from an additional glr-1-expressing neuron(s), or alternatively may signal to a glr-1-expressing neuron(s) so that eliminating glr-1 not only results in reduced AIB activity, but also directly or indirectly blocks AIY synaptic inputs or outputs.

AMPK coordinates glutamatergic signals through distinct mechanisms

In addition to the plethora of processes that are regulated downstream of AMPK activation, AMPK has been shown to modulate synaptic aging (Samuel et al., 2014). In mice, starvation increases the activity of the AgRP neurons by affecting their firing rate, ultimately inducing intense food-seeking behaviour and increased feeding (Dietrich et al., 2015). The enhanced firing rate of the AgRP neurons has been shown to be largely dependent on glutamatergic inputs mediated by glutamatergic ionotropic AMPA and NMDA receptors (Bito, 2010; Liu et al., 2012). AMPK modulates this food intake behaviour through the regulation of glutamate release in both mice and in C. elegans, although the precise mechanism has not been elucidated in either case (Yang et al., 2011Cunningham et al., 2012).

Our data demonstrate that AMPK (aak-2) regulates the GLR-1 and MGL-1 abundance postsynaptically. The AWC is sensitive to food availability and is inhibited during starvation, resulting in the inactivation of AIB and the activation of AIY (Gray et al., 2005; Chalasani et al., 2007). Our data demonstrate that the loss of AMPK signalling results in an increase in AIB neuronal activity and a concomitant decrease in AIY activity. This may be rationalized by the fact that C. elegans neurons release neurotransmitter in a graded fashion. Like vertebrate rod and cone photoreceptors which show tonic activity in the dark, and are hyperpolarized or depolarized by light, and removal of light respectively, AWC olfactory neurons have been shown to display basal activity in the absence of odour, but are inhibited or stimulated by odour or odour removal, respectively (Chalasani et al., 2007). Therefore, the increased GLR-1 and MGL-1 abundance in the AIB and AIY interneurons could potentially result in responses of longer duration and increased strength upon tonic neurotransmitter release at rest, or upon activation of AWC consequently increasing reversal frequency and shortening runs (Figure 8).

Figure 8. AMPK coordinates neuronal activity of the AIB and AIY interneurons by regulating their glutamatergic synaptic inputs in response to acute starvation.

The schematic showing the proposed role of AMPK in the AIB and AIY interneurons for regulation of appropriate transition between local to distal exploration during starvation conditions. Green and red colos show depolarized and hyperpolarized neurons, respectively. AMPK regulates GLR-1-mediated synaptic inputs into AIB interneurons by directly phosphorylating GLR-1 and subsequently modulating its endocytosis and potentially its degradation leading to decreased AIB activity in starved animals. AMPK also regulates the MGL-1-dependent synaptic inputs into the AIY interneurons, not only through its effects on mgl-1 mRNA levels but also by direct phosphorylation of MGL-1 protein. These functions of AMPK within the AIB and AIY interneurons collectively result in the transition between local exploration that occurs in well fed animals to distal exploration that occurs in starved animals, thus allowing them to explore their environment more extensively for energy resources. Legends to figure supplements

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.031

Figure 8—source data 1. glr-1 and mgl-1 over-expression data.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.032

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Overexpression of glr-1 and mgl-1 in the AIB and AIY, respectively results in the similar defect in distal locomotory behaviour as aak-2 mutants.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

Overexpression (OE) of glr-1 or mgl-1 in the AIB and AIY interneurons, respectively results in decreased forward locomotion rate and increased reversal frequency similar to starved aak-2 mutants (n>15), (one-way ANOVA *p<0.05, **p<0.001, ***p<0.0001). In box and whisker plots, the central line is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th and 75th percentiles, and the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points.

In C. elegans an increase in glutamatergic inputs mediated by GLR-1 results in an enhanced reversal frequency making this simple behaviour a faithful determinant of increased synaptic strength (Zheng et al., 1999; Burbea et al., 2002; Chalasani et al., 2007). We provide several lines of evidence that demonstrate that AMPK regulates GLR-1 abundance, by affecting its ubiquitylation status during conditions of acute starvation. First, the total GLR-1 level is decreased upon starvation. Second, the amplitude and size/width of GLR-1 puncta that are two determinants of GLR-1 abundance are increased in the AIB neuronal process and in the ventral nerve cord in aak-2 mutants. Third, this elevated GLR-1 accumulation cannot be further enhanced upon the disruption of the various processes required to remove GLR-1 from the membrane suggesting that AMPK and the ubiquitin-mediated internalization of GLR-1 may therefore function in a linear genetic pathway. Fourth, mutating the AMPK phosphorylation sites in GLR-1 results in increased GLR-1 abundance and defective distal exploratory behaviour. Finally, this defect is comparable to that observed in aak-2 mutants, while crossing the GLR-1 (S907A, S924A) variant into the aak-2 mutant background did not further exacerbate the distal exploratory behavioural defect. Therefore, consistent with its well-described role in the mouse where it regulates glutamatergic neurotransmission (Yang et al., 2011), AMPK is required to regulate the abundance of GLR-1 in specific glutamatergic neurons. In doing so it modulates glutamatergic inputs and synaptic strength to elicit appropriate behavioural outcomes in response to starvation.

AMPA receptors are either inserted or removed from postsynaptic membranes in an activity-dependent manner leading to the potentiation or depression of synaptic transmission, respectively (Carroll et al., 1999). Our findings might be further extended to implicate AMPK in the control of synaptic plasticity that underlies learning and memory as the appropriate regulation of glutamate receptor abundance is critical for long-term memory through habituation in C. elegans (Rose et al., 2003). Any increase in GluR abundance, and hence signalling, that arises in AMPK-compromised animals is very likely to result in irregular plasticity and abnormal behavioural outcomes, consistent with what is observed in starved AMPK mutants.

In addition to its regulation of GLR-1 abundance in the AIB, we show that AMPK also regulates glutamatergic inputs by regulating MGL-1 in the AIY. A key function of group II mGlu receptors is to modulate neuronal excitability and synaptic transmission at least partly by inhibiting adenylate cyclase (Dillon et al., 2006; Niswender and Conn, 2010). Group II mGlu receptors are required in both cognitive and emotional processes, and have been linked to various neuropsychiatric conditions, including anxiety, stress-related disorders, schizophrenia and substance misuse (Niswender and Conn, 2010). In mammals, activation of mGluR II induces long term depression at GABAergic synapses in the cochlear nucleus magnocellularis neurons where they have been shown to modulate synaptic plasticity (Lu, 2007).

In C. elegans, mgl-1 regulates a systemic starvation response and fat accumulation (Greer et al., 2008; Kang et al., 2009). Consistent with the inhibitory role of group II mGlu receptors in the vertebrate nervous system, mgl-1 was recently demonstrated to coordinate environmental sensory cues to modulate the activity of the pharyngeal neural network (Dillon et al., 2015). Although some studies have indicated that metabotropic glutamate receptors can be phosphoregulated by kinases such as PKC (Kim et al., 2005; Ko et al., 2012), our work has uncovered an AMPK-mediated modulation of the MGL-1 in response to acute food deprivation in C. elegans. This effect is at least partly achieved by influencing the regulation of mgl-1 mRNA levels in starved animals. Considering the brief delay between stimulus and response it is most likely that AMPK mediates its effect on mgl-1 transcript levels through the phosphorylation of key regulators of mgl-1 mRNA stability as described previously (Yun et al., 2005). Therefore, in addition to its well-described role in the regulation of ionic channels by affecting their degradation, AMPK may regulate its downstream neuronal effectors by affecting mRNA stability to regulate protein levels.

In summary, our findings suggest a simple model, in which AMPK coordinately regulates the activity of the AIB and AIY interneurons by modulating their glutamatergic synaptic inputs through two distinct mechanisms. AMPK modulates glutamatergic inputs in both interneurons by impinging on AMPA-type glutamate receptor GLR-1 and metabotropic glutamate receptor MGL-1 which consequently regulate distinct downstream behavioural outputs. The most salient feature of our study is our demonstration that AMPK acts as a molecular switch in the nervous system that is most likely not limited to the circuitries engaged in the regulation of feeding and locomotory behaviours. Given the conserved AMPK phosphorylation sites in GLR-1 and MGL-1, we speculate that AMPK regulates the activity of neural circuits in various organisms at least partly by affecting these two receptor types to modulate a wide range of neuronal outputs that respond to physiological or developmental contexts involving various stresses. Alternatively, its starvation-inducible or pharmacological activation could provide a highly effective, non-invasive means of modifying behavioural outputs.

Materials and methods

Strains

Caenorhabditis elegans strains were maintained under standard conditions (Brenner, 1974). The Bristol isolate (N2) was used as wild type. The following alleles were used in this study: aak-1(tm1944), aak-2(ok524), eat-4(ky5), glr-1 (n2461), lite-1(ce314), unc-11(e48), mgl-1(tm1811), osm-6(p811), avr-14(ad1305), nmr-1(ak4), mgl-2(tm355), sid-1(pk3321). Please see Supplementary file 1 for the complete list of strains used in this study.

Plasmid constructs and transgenes

Genomic aak-2 and mgl-1 and their 2 kb and 3 kb upstream sequences were amplified and cloned upstream of GFP to generate [Paak-2::aak-2::gfp] and [Pmgl-1::mgl-1::gfp]. unc-119, unc-54, glr-1, tph-1, rig-3, ttx-3, npr-9, che-2 promoters were amplified from genomic DNA and were cloned upstream of aak-2 cDNA (Narbonne and Roy, 2009) to drive aak-2 expression globally throughout the nervous system, body wall muscle and in different neuronal subtypes. GLR-1::GFP was amplified from nuIs24 strain and was inserted downstream of npr-9 promoter to generate [Pnpr-9::glr-1::gfp]. The resulting constructs were injected at 10–50 ng/μl and either Punc-122::gfp, Pelt-2::gfp, Pmyo-2::gfp or Punc-122::dsred were used as the co-injection marker. To generate the GLR-1 variants including (S907A) and (S924A), PCR-mediated site-directed mutagenesis was performed using Gene-Tailor site-directed mutagenesis (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) on [Pnpr-9::glr-1::gfp] and transgenic lines were generated after microinjecting plasmids at 50 ng/μl.

We used CRISPR/Cas9 to mutagenize the AMPK phosphorylation site serine 234 to alanine in mgl-1 as described previously (Paix et al., 2014).

Quantitative analysis of exploratory behaviour

Well-fed adult animals were scored for exploratory behaviour following 2 hr starvation as described previously (Gray et al., 2005; Calhoun et al., 2015). For our time course experiment mid-L4 animals were scored for exploratory behaviour either immediately after removal from food or following longer durations off food. To minimize the behavioural modifications caused by variability in their environment, animals were grown on the similar size and amount of bacterial patches (Calhoun et al., 2015). All body bends and reversals were scored by eye, by an investigator blind to the genotype of the animals.

L4 animals of sid-1(pk3321); uIs69 [pCFJ90 (pmyo-2::mCherry) + unc-119p::sid-1] and aak-2(ok524); sid-1(pk3321); uIs69 [pCFJ90 (pmyo-2::mCherry) + unc-119p::sid-1] which are nervous system RNAi hypersensitive were transferred to the RNAi plates seeded with overnight cultures of HT-115 E. coli clones expressing double stranded RNAi targeting glr-1, glr-2, eat-4, mod-1, ser-2, inx-7, unc-7, inx-19, gar-2, gcy-1, flp-1, mgl-1, npr-11. The exploratory behaviour of the resultant progeny was monitored upon reaching to adulthood.

Locomotory behaviour in the presence of food

The basal and enhanced slowing response was measured by transferring well-fed animals to plates with no bacteria, and transferring them to an assay plate with a thin layer of bacteria immediately or after 30 min off food as described previously (Sawin et al., 2000).

Chemotactic analysis

Chemotaxis assays towards a bacterial lawn or isoamyl alcohol (IAA) were performed on 10 cm nematode growth medium (NGM) plates incubated at room temperature (22°C) overnight as previously described. Animal behaviour was scored by eye, by an investigator blind to the genotype of the animal. 10 and 300 animals were used for each chemotaxis assay, either toward the bacterial lawn and IAA, respectively (Hart, 2006).

Optogenetics

C. C.elegans grown on NGM plates supplied with 5 µM all-trans retinal (ATR) were starved for 2 hr and their exploratory behaviour was monitored on retinal-free NGM plates. A 1 min pulse of blue (480 nm) or yellow light (540 nm) was delivered from an Arc lamp (EXFO) by a 10× objective (Zeiss M2Bio) to the head of an individual animal to activate ChR2 or Arch, respectively as described previously (Piggott et al., 2011; Kocabas et al., 2012). In order to eliminate intrinsic phototaxis responses, we used lite-1 mutants that are defective in UV light phototaxis. lite-1 mutants did not show any obvious defect in the locomotory behaviour in our assays. To depolarize the AIY interneurons or hyperpolarize the AIB interneurons, we used previously described strains expressing channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) or archaerhodopsin-3 (Arch), respectively (Kocabas et al., 2012).

In vivo imaging

For the calcium imaging adult transgenic animals expressing [Pnpr-9::G-CaMP3+ Pnpr-9::DsRed2B] (Piggott et al., 2011) or [Pttx-3::G-CaMP1+ unc-122::GFP] (Chalasani et al., 2007) were transferred to a 10% agarose pad following 2 hr starvation. A coverslip was then applied and the calcium transients were monitored in the cell body or neuronal process of the AIB and AIY interneurons, respectively as previously described (Lemieux et al., 2015). Briefly, to measure calcium transients in the AIB interneurons fluorescence images (green channel: G-CaMP3, red channel: DsRed2B) were acquired sequentially over 120 s window at a frequency of 2 Hz through a 63X objective on a Zeiss microscope. To measure calcium transients in the AIY interneurons fluorescence images (Green channel) were acquired from the neuronal process in the nerve ring over a 65 s window at a frequency of 3 Hz through a 63X objective. The resulting time-point series obtained from the fluorescence images for the AIB and AIY were then analyzed using ImageJ (RRID:SCR_001935).

To calculate the baseline values (Fb) averages of global minima for 5–10 frames over the duration of each time-lapse sequence were obtained and the difference between a given each time point’s fluorescence and Fb (ΔF) was normalized by dividing ΔF to Fb to plot ΔF/Fb for each individual. In our analyses, we report the total integrated signal of all the observed peaks during the 65 s or 120 s recording window that consists of an unambiguously defined initiation of a spontaneous transient in the neural process or cell body for the AIY and AIB, respectively. To measure the total integrated fluorescence the ΔF/F for each time point were summed over each time-lapse sequence to obtain a measurement for each time series sequence as described previously (Lemieux et al., 2015).

Calcium imaging in freely behaving animals was performed on a Zeiss microscope equipped with a Hamamatsu camera as previously described (Luo et al., 2014; Flavell et al., 2013). To prevent behavioural responses to blue light, the imaging was performed in the lite-1 background (Flavell et al., 2013). Animals were placed on 10% agarose pads to slow down their locomotion and the pad was sealed in a small chamber to prevent evaporation. The elapsed time from placing animals on the agarose pads to initiation of imaging was <5 min. Fluorescence time-lapse imaging was performed over 90 s at a frequency of 2 Hz using a 10x objective by an experimenter blind to the genotype. The images were processed as described before (Lemieux et al., 2015; Luo et al., 2014).

Microscopy

All images were acquired using a Zeiss AX10 microscope equipped with a Hamamatsu camera as previously described (Burbea et al., 2002). Starved GLR-1::GFP-expressing strains were immobilized with 10 mM levamisole. Images were captured and processed using AxioVision software. Maximum intensity projections of Z series stacks were obtained. GLR-1 levels were calculated using imageJ and normalized relative to WT. Puncta widths were estimated as the peak width at half-maximal amplitudes as described previously (Burbea et al., 2002). At least three transgenic lines were examined for these experiments and all the imaging and subsequent quantification was performed by an experimenter who was blind to the genotype.

The extent of signal overlaps between GLR-1 and the presynaptic marker EAT-4 was indicated by imaging transgenic animals expressing GLR-1::GFP and EAT-4::mCherry. Maximum intensity projections of sequential images from fluorescent green and red filters were obtained. The local maximum in each punctum was used to determine the spatial position of the GFP and mCherry peaks. The percentage of GLR-1 puncta co-localizing with a EAT-4 punctum was assessed by the fraction of GLR-1 puncta peaks that were less than 1 μm (or the width of an average punctum) from a presynaptic peak as previously described (Burbea et al., 2002).

For quantification of MGL-1 abundance in the AIY, the fluorescence intensity was measured in at least 3 transgenic lines expressing [Pmgl-1::mgl-1::gfp] which were starved for two hours. The exposure time was adjusted in the well-fed wild-type background and images were acquired by an experimenter who was blind to the genotype. Pixel intensity was measured in ImageJ (NIH) by calculating the mean pixel intensity for the entire region of interest.

Western blot

To compare GLR-1 levels, proteins were extracted from well-fed and starved WT and aak-2 mutants by sonication in lysis buffer (50 mM Hepes pH7.5, 150 mM NaCl, 10% glycerol, 1% Triton X-100, 1.5 mM MgCl2, 1 mM EDTA and protease inhibitors). Protein concentration was then determined by NanoDrop 2000c spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific, Waltham, MA). A similar amount of protein for each condition was then subjected to 8% SDS-PAGE and then transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane (Bio-Rad, Canada) and blotted by anti-GFP antibody (RRID:AB_162553) as described previously (Xie and Roy, 2015).

Semi-qRT-PCR

RNA extraction was performed in well-fed and starved animals as described previously (Lu and Roy. Similar quantities of RNA from each sample were used to generate comparable amplicon levels for each gene tested. PCR was performed using gene-specific primers for each query gene for 15 cycles with ProtoScript M-MuLV Taq RT-PCR Kit (NEB, E6400S).

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Cori Bargmann, Sreecanth Chalasani and Joshua Kaplan for reagents, along with Michael Hendricks for both reagents and very helpful discussion. We are grateful to our fellow lab members, and I am particularly indebted to my colleagues Francois Fagotto and Monique Zetka for their support. We also thank Mohammad R Raeesi N for his help with image processing and analysis and also the Caenorhabditis Genetic Center for C. elegans strains. This work was supported by funds awarded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • Canadian Institutes of Health Research MOP 84486 to Richard Roy.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

MA, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

RR, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Additional files

Supplemenmtary file 1. Complete list of strains used in this study.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.034

elife-16349-fig1.xlsx (11.3KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.034

References

  1. Aponte Y, Atasoy D, Sternson SM. AGRP neurons are sufficient to orchestrate feeding behavior rapidly and without training. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:351–355. doi: 10.1038/nn.2739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ashrafi K. Mapping out starvation responses. Cell Metabolism. 2006;3:235–236. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2006.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Balthasar N, Dalgaard LT, Lee CE, Yu J, Funahashi H, Williams T, Ferreira M, Tang V, McGovern RA, Kenny CD, Christiansen LM, Edelstein E, Choi B, Boss O, Aschkenasi C, Zhang CY, Mountjoy K, Kishi T, Elmquist JK, Lowell BB. Divergence of melanocortin pathways in the control of food intake and energy expenditure. Cell. 2005;123:493–505. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2005.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Belgardt BF, Okamura T, Brüning JC. Hormone and glucose signalling in POMC and AgRP neurons. Journal of Physiology. 2009;587:5305–5314. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.179192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bewick GA, Gardiner JV, Dhillo WS, Kent AS, White NE, Webster Z, Ghatei MA, Bloom SR. Post-embryonic ablation of AgRP neurons in mice leads to a lean, hypophagic phenotype. FASEB Journal . 2005;19:1680–1682. doi: 10.1096/fj.04-3434fje. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bito H. The chemical biology of synapses and neuronal circuits. Nature Chemical Biology. 2010;6:560–563. doi: 10.1038/nchembio.408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brenner S. The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 1974;77:71–94. doi: 10.1093/genetics/77.1.71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burbea M, Dreier L, Dittman JS, Grunwald ME, Kaplan JM. Ubiquitin and AP180 regulate the abundance of GLR-1 glutamate receptors at postsynaptic elements in C. elegans. Neuron. 2002;35:107–120. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00749-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Calhoun AJ, Tong A, Pokala N, Fitzpatrick JA, Sharpee TO, Chalasani SH. Neural mechanisms for evaluating environmental variability in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuron. 2015;86:428–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Carroll RC, Lissin DV, von Zastrow M, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC. Rapid redistribution of glutamate receptors contributes to long-term depression in hippocampal cultures. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:454–460. doi: 10.1038/8123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chalasani SH, Chronis N, Tsunozaki M, Gray JM, Ramot D, Goodman MB, Bargmann CI. Dissecting a circuit for olfactory behaviour in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature. 2007;450:63–70. doi: 10.1038/nature06292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chalfie M, Sulston JE, White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, Brenner S. The neural circuit for touch sensitivity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Journal of Neuroscience. 1985;5:956–964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.05-04-00956.1985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chen Z, Hendricks M, Cornils A, Maier W, Alcedo J, Zhang Y. Two insulin-like peptides antagonistically regulate aversive olfactory learning in C. elegans. Neuron. 2013;77:572–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Collingridge GL, Peineau S, Howland JG, Wang YT. Long-term depression in the CNS. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:459–473. doi: 10.1038/nrn2867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cone RD. Anatomy and regulation of the central melanocortin system. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:571–578. doi: 10.1038/nn1455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cowley MA, Smart JL, Rubinstein M, Cerdán MG, Diano S, Horvath TL, Cone RD, Low MJ. Leptin activates anorexigenic POMC neurons through a neural network in the arcuate nucleus. Nature. 2001;411:480–484. doi: 10.1038/35078085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cunningham KA, Bouagnon AD, Barros AG, Lin L, Malard L, Romano-Silva MA, Ashrafi K. Loss of a neural AMP-activated kinase mimics the effects of elevated serotonin on fat, movement, and hormonal secretions. PLoS Genetics. 2014;10:e1004394. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1004394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cunningham KA, Hua Z, Srinivasan S, Liu J, Lee BH, Edwards RH, Ashrafi K. AMP-activated kinase links serotonergic signaling to glutamate release for regulation of feeding behavior in C. elegans. Cell Metabolism. 2012;16:113–121. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2012.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dietrich MO, Horvath TL. Synaptic plasticity of feeding circuits: hormones and hysteresis. Cell. 2011;146:863–865. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.08.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dietrich MO, Zimmer MR, Bober J, Horvath TL. Hypothalamic Agrp neurons drive stereotypic behaviors beyond feeding. Cell. 2015;160:1222–1232. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dillon J, Franks CJ, Murray C, Edwards RJ, Calahorro F, Ishihara T, Katsura I, Holden-Dye L, O'Connor V. Metabotropic glutamate receptors: Modulators of context-dependent feeding behaviour in C. elegans. The Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2015;290:15052–15065. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M114.606608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dillon J, Hopper NA, Holden-Dye L, O'Connor V. Molecular characterization of the metabotropic glutamate receptor family in Caenorhabditis elegans. Biochemical Society Transactions. 2006;34:942–948. doi: 10.1042/BST0340942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Flavell SW, Pokala N, Macosko EZ, Albrecht DR, Larsch J, Bargmann CI. Serotonin and the neuropeptide PDF initiate and extend opposing behavioral states in C. elegans. Cell. 2013;154:1023–1035. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gordus A, Pokala N, Levy S, Flavell SW, Bargmann CI. Feedback from network states generates variability in a probabilistic olfactory circuit. Cell. 2015;161:215–227. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gray JM, Hill JJ, Bargmann CI. A circuit for navigation in Caenorhabditis elegans. PNAS. 2005;102:3184–3191. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409009101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gray JV, Petsko GA, Johnston GC, Ringe D, Singer RA, Werner-Washburne M. "Sleeping beauty": quiescence in Saccharomyces cerevisiae. Microbiology and Molecular Biology Reviews. 2004;68:187–206. doi: 10.1128/MMBR.68.2.187-206.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Greer ER, Pérez CL, Van Gilst MR, Lee BH, Ashrafi K. Neural and molecular dissection of a C. elegans sensory circuit that regulates fat and feeding. Cell Metabolism. 2008;8:118–131. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2008.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hallows KR, Raghuram V, Kemp BE, Witters LA, Foskett JK. Inhibition of cystic fibrosis transmembrane conductance regulator by novel interaction with the metabolic sensor AMP-activated protein kinase. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2000;105:1711–1721. doi: 10.1172/JCI9622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hardie DG, Ashford ML. AMPK: regulating energy balance at the cellular and whole body levels. Physiology. 2014;29:99–107. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00050.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hardie DG, Ross FA, Hawley SA. AMPK: a nutrient and energy sensor that maintains energy homeostasis. Nature Reviews Molecular Cell Biology. 2012;13:251–262. doi: 10.1038/nrm3311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hardie DG. AMPK: a key regulator of energy balance in the single cell and the whole organism. International Journal of Obesity. 2008;32:S7–S12. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Harris GP, Hapiak VM, Wragg RT, Miller SB, Hughes LJ, Hobson RJ, Steven R, Bamber B, Komuniecki RW. Three distinct amine receptors operating at different levels within the locomotory circuit are each essential for the serotonergic modulation of chemosensation in Caenorhabditis elegans. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:1446–1456. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4585-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hart A. Behavior. In: The C. elegans Research Community, editor. WormBook. 2006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hart AC, Sims S, Kaplan JM. Synaptic code for sensory modalities revealed by C. elegans GLR-1 glutamate receptor. Nature. 1995;378:82–85. doi: 10.1038/378082a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hendricks M. Neuroecology: tuning foraging strategies to environmental variability. Current Biology. 2015;25:R498–R500. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Juo P, Harbaugh T, Garriga G, Kaplan JM. CDK-5 regulates the abundance of GLR-1 glutamate receptors in the ventral cord of Caenorhabditis elegans. Molecular Biology of the Cell. 2007;18:3883–3893. doi: 10.1091/mbc.E06-09-0818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kang C, Avery L. Systemic regulation of starvation response in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genes & Development. 2009;23:12–17. doi: 10.1101/gad.1723409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kim CH, Braud S, Isaac JT, Roche KW. Protein kinase C phosphorylation of the metabotropic glutamate receptor mGluR5 on Serine 839 regulates Ca2+ oscillations. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2005;280:25409–25415. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M502644200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ko SJ, Isozaki K, Kim I, Lee JH, Cho HJ, Sohn SY, Oh SR, Park S, Kim DG, Kim CH, Roche KW. PKC phosphorylation regulates mGluR5 trafficking by enhancing binding of Siah-1A. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:16391–16401. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1964-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kocabas A, Shen CH, Guo ZV, Ramanathan S. Controlling interneuron activity in Caenorhabditis elegans to evoke chemotactic behaviour. Nature. 2012;490:273–277. doi: 10.1038/nature11431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kola B. Role of AMP-activated protein kinase in the control of appetite. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2008;20:942–951. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01745.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lang F, Föller M. Regulation of ion channels and transporters by AMP-activated kinase (AMPK) Channels. 2014;8:20–28. doi: 10.4161/chan.27423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lee H, Cho JS, Lambacher N, Lee J, Lee SJ, Lee TH, Gartner A, Koo HS. The Caenorhabditis elegans AMP-activated protein kinase AAK-2 is phosphorylated by LKB1 and is required for resistance to oxidative stress and for normal motility and foraging behavior. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2008;283:14988–14993. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M709115200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lee RY, Sawin ER, Chalfie M, Horvitz HR, Avery L. EAT-4, a homolog of a mammalian sodium-dependent inorganic phosphate cotransporter, is necessary for glutamatergic neurotransmission in caenorhabditis elegans.  Journal of Neuroscience . 1999;19:159–167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00159.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lemieux GA, Cunningham KA, Lin L, Mayer F, Werb Z, Ashrafi K. Kynurenic acid is a nutritional cue that enables behavioral plasticity. Cell. 2015;160:119–131. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.12.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lim CT, Kola B, Korbonits M. AMPK as a mediator of hormonal signalling. Journal of Molecular Endocrinology. 2010;44:87–97. doi: 10.1677/JME-09-0063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Liu T, Kong D, Shah BP, Ye C, Koda S, Saunders A, Ding JB, Yang Z, Sabatini BL, Lowell BB. Fasting activation of AgRP neurons requires NMDA receptors and involves spinogenesis and increased excitatory tone. Neuron. 2012;73:511–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lu Y, Roy R. Centrosome/Cell cycle uncoupling and elimination in the endoreduplicating intestinal cells of C. elegans. PLoS One. 2015;9:e110958. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.011095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lu Y. Endogenous mGluR activity suppresses GABAergic transmission in avian cochlear nucleus magnocellularis neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2007;97:1018–1029. doi: 10.1152/jn.00883.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Luo L, Wen Q, Ren J, Hendricks M, Gershow M, Qin Y, Greenwood J, Soucy ER, Klein M, Smith-Parker HK, Calvo AC, Colón-Ramos DA, Samuel AD, Zhang Y. Dynamic encoding of perception, memory, and movement in a C. elegans chemotaxis circuit. Neuron. 2014;82:1115–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. López M, Varela L, Vázquez MJ, Rodríguez-Cuenca S, González CR, Velagapudi VR, Morgan DA, Schoenmakers E, Agassandian K, Lage R, Martínez de Morentin PB, Tovar S, Nogueiras R, Carling D, Lelliott C, Gallego R, Oresic M, Chatterjee K, Saha AK, Rahmouni K, Diéguez C, Vidal-Puig A. Hypothalamic AMPK and fatty acid metabolism mediate thyroid regulation of energy balance. Nature Medicine. 2010;16:1001–1008. doi: 10.1038/nm.2207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Man HY, Lin JW, Ju WH, Ahmadian G, Liu L, Becker LE, Sheng M, Wang YT. Regulation of AMPA receptor-mediated synaptic transmission by clathrin-dependent receptor internalization. Neuron. 2000;25:649–662. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81067-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Marvin JS, Borghuis BG, Tian L, Cichon J, Harnett MT, Akerboom J, Gordus A, Renninger SL, Chen TW, Bargmann CI, Orger MB, Schreiter ER, Demb JB, Gan WB, Hires SA, Looger LL. An optimized fluorescent probe for visualizing glutamate neurotransmission. Nature Methods. 2013;10:162–170. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Minokoshi Y, Alquier T, Furukawa N, Kim YB, Lee A, Xue B, Mu J, Foufelle F, Ferré P, Birnbaum MJ, Stuck BJ, Kahn BB. AMP-kinase regulates food intake by responding to hormonal and nutrient signals in the hypothalamus. Nature. 2004;428:569–574. doi: 10.1038/nature02440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Narbonne P, Roy R. Regulation of germline stem cell proliferation downstream of nutrient sensing. Cell Division. 2006;1:29–38. doi: 10.1186/1747-1028-1-29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Narbonne P, Roy R. Caenorhabditis elegans dauers need LKB1/AMPK to ration lipid reserves and ensure long-term survival. Nature. 2009;457:210–214. doi: 10.1038/nature07536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Niswender CM, Conn PJ. Metabotropic glutamate receptors: physiology, pharmacology, and disease. Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology. 2010;50:295–322. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.011008.145533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Paix A, Wang Y, Smith HE, Lee CY, Calidas D, Lu T, Smith J, Schmidt H, Krause MW, Seydoux G. Scalable and versatile genome editing using linear DNAs with microhomology to Cas9 Sites in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2014;198:1347–1356. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.170423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Piggott BJ, Liu J, Feng Z, Wescott SA, Xu XZ. The neural circuits and synaptic mechanisms underlying motor initiation in C. elegans. Cell. 2011;147:922–933. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.08.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Roche KW, O'Brien RJ, Mammen AL, Bernhardt J, Huganir RL. Characterization of multiple phosphorylation sites on the AMPA receptor GluR1 subunit. Neuron. 1996;16:1179–1188. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80144-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rose JK, Kaun KR, Chen SH, Rankin CH. GLR-1, a non-NMDA glutamate receptor homolog, is critical for long-term memory in Caenorhabditis elegans.  Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:9595–9599. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-29-09595.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Samuel MA, Voinescu PE, Lilley BN, de Cabo R, Foretz M, Viollet B, Pawlyk B, Sandberg MA, Vavvas DG, Sanes JR. LKB1 and AMPK regulate synaptic remodeling in old age. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1190–1197. doi: 10.1038/nn.3772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sawin ER, Ranganathan R, Horvitz HR. C. elegans locomotory rate is modulated by the environment through a dopaminergic pathway and by experience through a serotonergic pathway. Neuron. 2000;26:619–631. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81199-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Schneeberger M, Claret M. Recent insights into the role of hypothalamic AMPK signaling cascade upon metabolic control. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2012;6:185. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sternson SM, Nicholas Betley J, Cao ZF. Neural circuits and motivational processes for hunger. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2013;23:353–360. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Tsalik EL, Hobert O. Functional mapping of neurons that control locomotory behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. Journal of Neurobiology. 2003;56:178–197. doi: 10.1002/neu.10245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wakabayashi T, Kitagawa I, Shingai R. Neurons regulating the duration of forward locomotion in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuroscience Research. 2004;50:103–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2004.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wang T, Hung CC, Randall DJ. The comparative physiology of food deprivation: from feast to famine. Annual Review of Physiology.  2005;68:223–251. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.68.040104.105739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, Brenner S. The structure of the ventral nerve cord of Caenorhabditis elegans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1976;275:327–348. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1976.0086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, Brenner S. The structure of the nervous system of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 1986;314:1–340. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1986.0056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wicks SR, Roehrig CJ, Rankin CH. A dynamic network simulation of the nematode tap withdrawal circuit: predictions concerning synaptic function using behavioral criteria. Journal of Neuroscience . 1996;16:4017–4031. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-12-04017.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Xie M, Roy R. The causative gene in chanarian dorfman syndrome regulates lipid droplet homeostasis in C. elegans. PLoS Genetics. 2015;11:e1005284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1005284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Yang Y, Atasoy D, Su HH, Sternson SM. Hunger states switch a flip-flop memory circuit via a synaptic AMPK-dependent positive feedback loop. Cell. 2011;146:992–1003. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.07.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Yun H, Lee M, Kim SS, Ha J. Glucose deprivation increases mRNA stability of vascular endothelial growth factor through activation of AMP-activated protein kinase in DU145 prostate carcinoma. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2005;280:9963–9972. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M412994200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Zheng Y, Brockie PJ, Mellem JE, Madsen DM, Maricq AV. Neuronal control of locomotion in C. elegans is modified by a dominant mutation in the GLR-1 ionotropic glutamate receptor. Neuron. 1999;24:347–361. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80849-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2016 Sep 19;5:e16349. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16349.035

Decision letter

Editor: Kang Shen1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "AMPK acts as a molecular trigger to coordinate glutamatergic signals and adaptive behaviours during acute starvation" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and K VijayRaghavan as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

In this paper, Ahmadi and Roy address how the AMP-activated protein kinase (AMPK, aak-2) regulates behavioral response to acute starvation. Because AMPK monitors the status of cellular energy, the findings provide a mechanistic link between physiological and behavioral responses to stress of food-deprivation. The study has identified the key interneurons in a locomotory circuit as the functional targets of the AMPK aak-2 in response to starvation. It has also found that aak-2 regulates the stability of two glutamatergic receptors in the interneurons to generate starvation-induced behavioral changes. The study employs multiple experimental approaches to thoroughly support the conclusions.

The following concerns should be addressed:

1) Figure 1B and Figure 1—figure supplement 1B. While it seems clear that the aak-2 mutant displays a defect in suppressing reversals when starved, the defects of aak-2 in locomotory speed do not seem to be specific to starvation. In Figure 1—figure supplement 1B well-fed aak-2 also moves more slowly than well-fed wild-type animals. Because aak-2 is also possibly expressed in motor neurons and some pre-motor neurons, aak-2 may regulate speed under the normal condition. The effect of aak-2 on starvation-induced decrease in reversal rate is a parameter that is more specific to starvation, which should be the focus for behavioral characterization.

Along the same line, in Figure 1, the authors showed that starving the animals for 10 mins is already causing a behavioral phenotype. Are aak-2 animals with no starvation different from wild type? In other words, are the aak-2 mutant phenotype caused by developmental deficits?

Can overexpression of GLR-1 in AIB or MGL-1 in AIY mimic the effect of aak-2?

2) It seems that the calcium imaging results were collected from animals that were immobilized without any sensory stimulation. AIB activity is known to link with reversals, therefore, it is reasonable to assume that the observed and quantified spontaneous activity in AIB represent "attempts" of reversals. However, it is not clear what the spontaneous activity measured in AIY represents. Some clarification is needed.

Can one measure the activity of these neurons during animal movements?

Along the same line, one question is how the imaging trials were selected since it is just spontaneous activity being measured, was the experimenter blinded to the genotype of the animal?

3) There are some concerns about the reagents and the way the receptor imaging experiments are done. Why are the Figure 6 experiments not done in AIB?

Are the strains used to quantify GLR-1 abundance in AIB or MGL1 in AIY stable integrated transgenes? One problem with this type of imaging experiments is the overexpression effect of transgenes.

eLife. 2016 Sep 19;5:e16349. doi: 10.7554/eLife.16349.036

Author response


The following concerns should be addressed:

1) Figure 1B and Figure 1—figure supplement 1B. While it seems clear that the aak-2 mutant displays a defect in suppressing reversals when starved, the defects of aak-2 in locomotory speed do not seem to be specific to starvation. In Figure 1—figure supplement 1B well-fed aak-2 also moves more slowly than well-fed wild-type animals. Because aak-2 is also possibly expressed in motor neurons and some pre-motor neurons, aak-2 may regulate speed under the normal condition. The effect of aak-2 on starvation-induced decrease in reversal rate is a parameter that is more specific to starvation, which should be the focus for behavioral characterization.

Based on our analysis aak-2 mutants do not show any difference in their rate of forward or backward locomotion when assayed in the presence of food; a situation where AMPK is most likely inactive (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). Similarly, aak-2 animals that were previously well-fed or starved display a comparable rate of forward locomotion similarly-treated wild type animals when evaluated on an abundant food source after a short period of time (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B, C). To further examine if aak-2 mutants display a general dampening of locomotory speed we examined the forward locomotion rate (body bends) of aak-2 mutants upon exposure to a gentle touch applied to the posterior (just before the anus). We found that aak-2 mutants show similar locomotory speed compared to WT animals following this touch stimulus suggesting that they do not display any defect in locomotory speed (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D and subsection “Neuronal AMPK signalling triggers distal exploratory behaviour in starved animals”, fourth paragraph). Therefore, it is likely that the basal AMPK activity regulates the observed change in speed specifically in response to food depletion in well-fed animals, while it has a more pronounced effect on behaviour when activated upon prolonged starvation. Since the increased reversal frequency affects the duration of forward movement (Burbea M, Dreier L., 2002; Tsalik and Hobert, 2003 and our Figure 4—figure supplement 2E, G), we believe that the forward locomotion rate in starved aak-2 animals is very likely to be also affected by the aak-2-associated defect in suppressing reversals. This can potentially explain the reason for greater difference in forward locomotion between aak-2 and WT animals upon 2 hours of starvation compared to 10 minutes off food. Because of the antagonistic relationship between forward and backward locomotion, we always try to include the results for both forward and backward locomotion throughout our study. We pointed this out in the third paragraph of the aforementioned subsection in our description of the data presented in the first figure (Figure 1B). We show that the effect of AMPK on locomotory behaviour is largely confined to starved animals, while AMPK also regulates reversal frequency.

Along the same line, in Figure 1, the authors showed that starving the animals for 10 mins is already causing a behavioral phenotype. Are aak-2 animals with no starvation different from wild type? In other words, are the aak-2 mutant phenotype caused by developmental deficits?

We examined locomotory behaviour in the presence of food and in response to a mechanical stimulus in the posterior region and we conclude that AMPK mutants are not impaired in any apparent way. aak-2 animals do display a reduced forward rate of locomotion in the absence of food, but not in the presence of a thin layer of bacteria (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). As previously mentioned, to examine if aak-2 mutants display a decrease in locomotory speed we monitored the locomotory behaviour of aak-2 mutants upon exposure to a gentle touch applied to the posterior end of their body. In our opinion this serves as a good control since it is a stimulus that is unrelated to the starvation-sensing circuit. We found that aak-2 mutants show no difference in locomotory speed compared to WT animals under these conditions (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D and subsection “Neuronal AMPK signalling triggers distal exploratory behaviour in starved animals”, fourth paragraph), thus suggesting that the locomotory phenotypes we describe in our study are unlikely to be a consequence of any more general developmental defect.

In addition, when aak-2(RNAi) was performed in late L2 stage larvae, well after the majority of motor neurons are generated, we noted a modest but significant reduction in forward locomotion rate upon removal from food (data not shown) that was compounded with an increase in reversal frequency when the animals were starved (Figure 1—figure supplement 2 and in the aforementioned paragraph) suggesting that the reduced locomotory speed caused by AMPK loss of function in starved animals is unlikely to be a consequence of a more general developmental defect that impinges on motor function. This is further corroborated by our optogenetic results where we show that although AIY activation or AIB inactivation in starved WT animals (when AIB is likely inactive and AIY is likely active (Gray et al., 2005)), do not change their locomotory behaviour, while optogenetically activating AIY or inactivating AIB in aak-2 mutants ameliorates both forward and backward locomotion. Therefore, taken altogether it is unlikely that the reduced locomotory speed in aak-2 mutants is a consequence of a more general developmental deficit. Rather, it is most probably a consequence of reduced AIY activity and increased AIB activity in starved aak-2 mutants.

Can overexpression of GLR-1 in AIB or MGL-1 in AIY mimic the effect of aak-2?

This is a great suggestion that could further support our conclusions. We did the experiment and the data have been added to the revised manuscript as Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Previous studies have shown that overexpression of GLR-1 in the AIB interneurons causes an increase in reversal rate in the absence of food(Chalasani et al., 2007), while a group in Japan has recently reported that mgl-1 mutants display a reduced reversal rate. Their data indicate that the expression levels of MGL-1 in the AIY interneurons are critical for appropriate chemotaxis toward DA and increased MGL-1 levels result in abnormal choice between the repellant and attractant (Suehiro and Mitani, International C. elegans Conference 2015). These results are consistent with our data that demonstrate the accumulation of GLR-1 and MGL-1 in the AIB and AIY interneurons of starved AMPK mutants, respectively. Furthermore, these changes in abundance correlate with changes in the forward and backward locomotion rate and aberrant chemotaxis. In our study, using various approaches, we highlight that the appropriate suppression of reversals that would normally occur during starvation is impaired in aak-2 mutants and is potentially caused by the increased abundance of GLR-1 and/or MGL-1. To determine whether the increased abundance of these receptors can cause the same phenotype as aak-2 mutants, we examined the locomotory behaviour of starved animals that overexpress GLR-1 in the AIB, or MGL-1 in the AIY, or in animals that overexpress both transgenes. As we have shown in Figure 8—figure supplement 1, overexpressing MGL-1 in the AIY resulted in a phenotype that was quite similar to that seen in starved aak-2 mutants. The overexpression of MGL-1 in the AIY interneurons was more effective in phenocopying the locomotory defects of starved aak-2 mutants than the effect of overexpression of GLR-1 in the AIB, which resulted in a modest, but nevertheless significant, defect in transition between local to distal exploration. This can be explained by the fact that overexpressed GLR-1 is most likely still targeted for endocytosis and degradation while AMPK is present and active. In aak-2 mutants there is a defect in GLR-1 endocytosis allowing it to accumulate in puncta. However, when it is overexpressed there is some accumulation but AMPK is still present to modulate the excess accumulation of the receptor. Nevertheless, overexpression of either of these, or both transgenes results in a similar behavioural defect as that which we report for aak-2 mutants. We have added these data to the manuscript as Figure 8—figure supplement 1, and we describe our interpretations in the last paragraph of the subsection “AMPK modulates MGL-1 in the mRNA and protein levels in the AIY interneurons to modify distal exploratory behaviour in response to starvation”.

2) It seems that the calcium imaging results were collected from animals that were immobilized without any sensory stimulation. AIB activity is known to link with reversals, therefore, it is reasonable to assume that the observed and quantified spontaneous activity in AIB represent "attempts" of reversals. However, it is not clear what the spontaneous activity measured in AIY represents. Some clarification is needed.

Two previous studies which measured the calcium levels in the AIY in freely moving animals had shown that the calcium peaks observed in the AIY correlate with termination of reversals and onset and duration of forward runs with gradual increases preceding forward run initiation. This also coincides with point where the locomotory speed is maximal (Flavell et al., 2013; Luo et al., 2014). This information is added to the new text file (subsection “Parallel and opposing function of AMPK in the modulation of AIB and AIY outputs”, first paragraph).

To further investigate if the defective distal exploration of aak-2 mutants is a consequence of decreased AIY activity and increased AIB activity in starved aak-2 mutants and to correlate the locomotory activity with changes in the AIB and AIY neuronal activity, we performed calcium imaging in the AIY and AIB in freely moving WT and aak-2 animals during starvation. Consistent with previous studies we noted a correlation between the increased Ca2+ influxes in the AIY with termination of reversals which remains high during forward runs. Moreover, not surprisingly, the frequency and intensity of Ca2+ influxes/neuronal activity in the AIB increases during reversals. These data further corroborate our findings presented in the original manuscript that showed that the defective transition from local to distal exploration during starvation is a consequence of both decreased AIY activity and increased AIB activity. These new data have been added to this revised manuscript as Figure 4—figure supplement 2, while we describe this correlation in the text (subsection “Parallel and opposing function of AMPK in the modulation of AIB and AIY outputs”, first and third paragraphs).

Can one measure the activity of these neurons during animal movements?

To examine the correlation between neural activity in AIB and AIY and the locomotory behaviours we describe, we performed calcium imaging in freely moving WT and aak-2 animals placed on 10% agarose pads as a means to reduce their movement. These data have been included in the revised manuscript as Figure 4—figure supplement 2 this correlation is described in the text (subsection “Parallel and opposing function of AMPK in the modulation of AIB and AIY outputs”, first and third paragraphs).

Along the same line, one question is how the imaging trials were selected since it is just spontaneous activity being measured, was the experimenter blinded to the genotype of the animal?

Most of our calcium imaging studies were performed in immobilized animals and the experimenter was indeed blind to the genotype of animals. Moreover, every time that this experiment was performed, all the animals placed on the agarose pad were examined unless the animals were unhealthy or moved out of field of view.

3) There are some concerns about the reagents and the way the receptor imaging experiments are done. Why are the Figure 6 experiments not done in AIB?

In Figure 6A,B,C we investigated if AMPK might regulate GLR-1 abundance in the nervous system through effects on the endocytic pathway using two different approaches: first, by using unc-11 mutants which disrupt the AP2-adaptin complex and endocytosis or secondly, using a GLR-1 transgenic variant in which the critical lysine residues that are targeted by ubiquitylation and which trigger endocytosis are mutated to alanine. Since we showed that AMPK plays an important role in the modulation of GLR-1 abundance in the ventral nerve cord by modulating its endocytosis, we wanted to determine if AMPK might regulate GLR-1 abundance specifically in the AIB neuronal puncta in starved animals through similar mechanisms. To test this we used a non-phosphorylable GLR-1 variant that was expressed specifically in the AIB and found that AMPK indeed regulates GLR-1 abundance in the neuronal process through its effects on the endocytic pathway. Although we discussed the results of our analysis of aak-2; unc-11, which we initially performed as a control but did not include it in the final figure, we have now incorporated this result into the final figure and we specifically clarify that this experiment was performed in the AIB, and not the ventral nerve cord, both in the text and in the legend for Figure 6E.

Are the strains used to quantify GLR-1 abundance in AIB or MGL1 in AIY stable integrated transgenes? One problem with this type of imaging experiments is the overexpression effect of transgenes.

In these experiments, all the imaging and subsequent quantifications were performed by an experimenter blind to the genotypes.

In the case of MGL-1 we introduced GFP into the C terminus of mgl-1 using CRISPR technology. Therefore the GFP signal was initially quantified in the strains that express a single copy of the MGL-1::GFP from endogenous mgl-1 promoter sequence. However, since mgl-1 is expressed in the AIY interneurons at much lower levels compared to other mgl-1-expressing neurons (data not shown), this signal was very faint and was easily bleached, we repeated and confirmed these results in strains that express MGL-1::GFP in extra chromosomal arrays in order to obtain better quality images and eliminate the problem of rapid photobleaching. To ensure that our observations of the multi-copy transgene are not a consequence of differential expression of the array, 3 independent MGL-1::GFP-expressing strains were examined by an experimenter who was blind to the genotypes of the strains in each case. The results obtained with all three transgenic lines were consistent with our observations made with the CRISPR-introduced allele. We therefore strongly believe that the signal we observe with the extrachromosomal transgenes recapitulates the endogenous situation and indicates that MGL-1 levels vary between starved WT and aak-2 mutant animals.

Since we had to assess the GLR-1::GFP signal in the neuronal process of AIB, a single copy transgene was not sufficient to generate a strong and quantifiable signal for our experiments. Therefore, as was the case with the MGL-1::GFP, we generated multiple transgenic lines which were scored by an experimenter who was blind to the genotypes while the images were being acquired and also during subsequent analysis. We obtained reproducible results from all of our transgenic lines that were consistent with GLR-1 being regulated by AMPK in the AIB neuronal process. Moreover, since GLR-1 abundance in the AIB neuronal process was clearly elevated in unc-11 animals, and which was not further enhanced upon the removal of aak-2, we conclude that AMPK must be part of a linear pathway that contributes to the endocytic regulation of GLR-1 abundance. Consistent with this possibility GLR-1 variants that lacked the consensus AMPK phosphorylation sites in GLR-1 accumulated in a manner similar to unc-11 mutants or in aak-2; unc-11 doubles. Because we can quantify differences in the levels of GLR-1 and its variants in the various genotypes we have tested, it seems unlikely that the transgenic expression levels contribute significantly to the regulation that we observe. We therefore contend that the AMPK-dependent differences in GLR-1 abundance that we observe in the AIB neuronal process in starved animals are not the result of the increased GLR-1 contributed by the extrachromosomal, multicopy transgenes, but rather due to the regulation of the protein by AMPK and additional regulators of the endocytic degradation pathway.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Locomotory behaviour in AMPK mutant animals.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.004

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.004
    Figure 1—source data 2. Food and stimuli-related behaviours in AMPK mutants.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.005

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.005
    Figure 1—source data 3. Locomotory behaviour upon depletion of aak-2 with 1535 RNAi.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.006

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.006
    Figure 2—source data 1. AMPK reconstitution in different neurons to restore the defective distal exploratory behaviour and defective chemotaxis typical of aak-2 mutants.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.010

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.010
    Figure 3—source data 1. Optogenetics and epistatic analysis.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.013

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.013
    Figure 3—source data 2. The analysis of locomotory behaviour of potential AMPK phospho targets.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.014

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.014
    Figure 3—source data 3. Potential AMPK phosphorylation targets expressed within the AIB and AIY interneurons.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.015

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.015
    Figure 4—source data 1. Calcium imaging in the AIB and AIY interneurons in WT, aak-2, mgl-1, glr-1 and eat-4 and double mutants.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.018

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.018
    Figure 4—source data 2. Calcium imaging in freely WT and aak-2 behaving animals.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.019

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.019
    Figure 5—source data 1. Measurement of GLR-1 abundance in the AIB neuronal process and ventral nerve cord of WT and aak-2 mutants.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.023

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.023
    Figure 5—source data 2. The extent of GLR-1 and EAT-4 colocalization in WT and aak-2 mutants.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.024

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.024
    Figure 6—source data 1. Analysis of GLR-1 abundance in endocytosis defective mutants.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.027

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.027
    Figure 7—source data 1. Analysis of MGL-1 abundance in WT and aak-2 mutants.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.029

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.029
    Figure 8—source data 1. glr-1 and mgl-1 over-expression data.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.032

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.032
    Supplemenmtary file 1. Complete list of strains used in this study.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16349.034

    elife-16349-fig1.xlsx (11.3KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.16349.034

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES