Abstract
From the age of two years, an American child is more likely to consume a sugar-sweetened product than a fruit or vegetable on any given day—a troubling statistic, given that food preferences are established early in childhood, as well as the strong association between this dietary pattern and increased risk of developing a number of chronic diseases. Here, we review the ontogeny and biopsychology of sweet taste, highlighting how a biological drive to prefer sweetness at high concentrations during childhood, which would have conferred an advantage in environments of scarcity, now predisposes children to overconsume all that is sweet in a modern food system replete with added sugars. We review the power of sweet taste to blunt expressions of pain and mask bad tastes in foods as well as factors that predispose some to consume high-sugar diets, including experiential learning and taste preferences driven in part by genetics. Understanding children’s unique vulnerability to our current food environment, rich in both nutritive and nonnutritive sweeteners, is highlighted as a priority for future research to develop evidence-based strategies to help establish healthy dietary behaviors early in life.
Keywords: children, sweet taste, biology, psychology, nonnutritive sweeteners, taste genetics
Introduction
From the age of two years, an American is more likely to eat a manufactured sweet than a fruit or vegetable on a given day [1, 2]. By age three, most children consume at least one sugar-sweetened beverage daily [3]. More than 15% of children’s total caloric intake comes from added sugars, levels well above recommendations of 5–10% [4, 5]. In contrast, sweet-tasting fruits make up approximately 5% of calories [6–8]. Ultra-processed foods such as sugar-sweetened beverages, are major sources of added sugars in the diet [9, 10], and the higher the intake, the poorer the nutrient quality of the diet [11]. These dietary patterns are troubling since a diet established early in life that is low in whole fruits and high in energy-dense processed foods [2, 12] increases the risk of developing obesity, cardiovascular disease, hypertension, obesity-related cancers, and the most prevalent chronic disease of childhood, dental caries [13–20].
In this article, we review the ontogeny and biopsychology of sweet taste, a critical first step to address the modern patterns of food choices that are antithetical to health, and why it is so difficult to change bad habits and develop good ones. Two factors predispose humans toward obesogenic, high-sugar diets: (a) inborn, evolutionarily driven taste preferences that make individuals vulnerable to the modern food environment rich in sugar, and (b) lack of exposure to a variety of healthy foods during early childhood. Evolution has shaped the taste of foods children initially prefer or reject. In an environment with limited nutrients and abundant poisonous plants, sensory systems evolved to detect and prefer perceptions that specify crucial nutrients such as the once rare energy (carbohydrate)-rich plants that taste sweet [21, 22], while rejecting those that taste bitter [23]. By focusing on the biology and psychology of sweet taste, as well as the contextual learning about the sweetness of foods, we gain insights into why children as a group are more vulnerable to a food environment rich in sweetened processed foods and why some children are more vulnerable to this environment.
1. The Biology of Sweet Taste
In nature, carbohydrates are a source of energy often equated with sweetness, which is associated with powerful hedonic appeal. The emotions experienced upon tasting something sweet are complex processes mediated by sweet taste receptors in the periphery and by multiple brain substrates, which phylogenetically are remarkably well conserved [24]. Progress has been made in identifying the initial events underlying sweet taste recognition [25, 26] and the brain mechanisms underlying the strong hedonic responses experienced upon tasting something sweet [27, 28].
Sweetness as a sensation starts on the tongue. When sweet-tasting (nutritive and nonnutritive) ligands stimulate a receptor on taste cells, the resulting signal is conducted via G proteins [29], which activate pleasure-generating brain circuitry, where sweet taste perception and hedonics arise (reviewed in [30]). The hedonic “liking” and motivation “wanting” signals for a sweet taste are distinctly modulated through brain mesocorticolimbic circuitry involving the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum [28].
At the receptor level, two proteins, T1R2 and T1R3 (taste receptor family 1, proteins 2 and 3), combine to create a sweet taste receptor; their associated genes are TAS1R2 and TAS1R3. Among the G proteins, the one associated with intracellular sweet signaling is gustducin (Gα protein subunit), encoded by the GNAT3 gene [29, 31]. These receptor and transduction proteins are underexpressed in some people due to inborn genetic variation, and genotype explains, in part, differences in sweet taste sensitivity and preferences [32–38], at least among adults.
A recent study of 1,901 adolescent and young adult twins found that a common genetic factor accounted for 30% of the overall variance in their sensitivity to the sweet taste of two monosaccharides (glucose, fructose) and two nonnutritive sweeteners (neohesperidin dihydrochalcone, aspartame) [39]. While this twin study did not determine whether the common factor was the sweet taste receptor gene, other datareveal that (a) variation in the TAS1R3 subunit of the sweet taste receptor gene and (b) variation in the GNAT3 gene that encodes for the intracellular sweet transduction protein are related to sucrose detection thresholds (defined as the lowest concentration of a stimulus needed by a subject to detect its presence relative to water). Adults with the TAS1R3 CC genotype (rs1015443) or GNAT3 TT genotype (rs7792845) detected sugars at lower concentrations (were more sensitive) than those with the T or C allele, respectively [34–38]. Adults with the TAS1R3 CC genotype were more sweet sensitive and preferred lower levels of sweet than others [34]. Thus, at least for adults and older children, genotype explains why not all people perceive sweetness equally strongly [30].
Taste receptor proteins appear in many cell types including those with a secretory or other endocrine function like ductal and beta cells of the pancreas [40, 41] and enteroendocrine cells of the gut [42]. Likewise, some taste receptor cells in the oral cavity have an endocrine role in the body. These cells produce and secrete hormones such as serotonin, cholecystokinin, vasoactive intestinal peptide, glucagon-like peptide 1, ghrelin, and neuropeptide Y [43–46]. The effect of these molecules may be to modulate the function of their own or adjacent cells. Beyond these autocrine and paracrine effects, they may also enter the bloodstream, where they have hormonal effects [47]. The biological utility of these hormonal responses may be part of the cephalic-phase response, whereby the body prepares for incoming food, which is logical because the first contact of food with cells of the body occurs in the taste cells [47]. While most studies have made these endocrine observations in model organisms, mostly mice and rats, the close ties between the rodent and primate taste systems suggest these endocrine effects would be similar in humans. From a developmental perspective, early experience with excess sweet taste stimulation may prime the cephalic-phase effects and affect nutrient absorption from the gut in ways that could persist into adulthood.
2. The Biopsychology of Sweet Taste in Children
The study of the ontogeny of human sweet taste perception has been the focus of scientific investigations for many decades. Table 1 highlights the variety of scientific approaches, methodologies, and outcome measures that have been used to assess (a) sweet taste perception and preferences and (b) the consequence of tasting something sweet on behavioral responses in infants and children.
Table 1.
Age Group | Methodology and Outcomes | References |
---|---|---|
Preterm Infants | Increased strength and frequency of sucking of sucrose-sweetened nipple compared with unsweetened nipple | Maone et al., 1990 [48] |
Increased strength and frequency of sucking when tasting glucose-sweetened drops of solution compared with water. | Tatzer et al., 1985 [49] | |
Attenuated pain response (as evidenced by decreases in crying duration) during venipuncture procedure when tasting a sweet (sucrose or glucose) solution compared with water | Abad et al., 1996 [62]; Deshmukh and Udani, 2002 [63] | |
Decreased spontaneous crying relative to baseline when tasting sweet (sucrose or glucose) solution compared with water | Smith and Blass, 1996 [50] | |
Infants | Increased strength and frequency of sucking of a sucrose-sweetened compared with unsweetened nipple | Maone et al., 1990 [48] |
Attenuated pain response (as evidenced by decreases in crying incidence and duration) during and after painful procedures (e.g., immunization, heel lance, circumcision) when tasting a sweet (sucrose or glucose) solution compared with water | Harrison et al., [64]; Blass and Hoffmeyer, 1991 [60] | |
Decreased spontaneous crying relative to baseline when tasting a sweet (sucrose or glucose) solution compared with water | Smith and Blass, 1996 [50] | |
Increased sucking burst length and decreased sucking latency with a sucrose-sweetened compared with unsweetened nipple | Crook, 1978 [56]; Crook and Lipsitt, 1976 [57] | |
Increased sucking-like and mouthing responses, facial relaxation, smiling, and hand-mouth contact when tasting sweet (sucrose) solution compared with water | Rosenstein and Oster, 1988 [55]; Steiner et al, 2001 [53] | |
Decreased crying and increased hand-mouth contact when tasting sweet (sucrose) solution compared with water | Barr et al., 1999 [59] | |
Decreased crying and increased hand-mouth contact when tasting sweet (aspartame) solution compared with water | Barr et al., 1999 [59] | |
Increased intake of sweet solutions (sucrose, glucose, fructose, lactose) compared with water | Beauchamp and Moran, 1982 [108]; Desor et al., 1973 [52] | |
Children | Increased pain threshold during cold pressor test when tasting sweet (sucrose) solution compared with water | Miller et al, 1994 [67]; Pepino and Mennella, 2005 [69]; Mennella et al., 2010 [68] |
No effect on pain induced by needle-related procedures when tasting something sweet compared with water | Harrison et al., 2015 [70] | |
Concentration of sucrose most preferred higher among children than among adults, with the changeover to adult pattern occurring during mid-late adolescence | de Graaf and Zandstra, 1999 [82]; Mennella et al., 2005 [80]; Mennella et al., 2011 [79]; Mennella et al., 2014 [35]; Desor and Beauchamp, 1987 [78]; Desor et al., 1975 [83] | |
Concentration of sucrose most preferred higher during periods of growth | Coldwell et al., 2009 [90]; Mennella et al., 2014 [35] | |
Children learn through repeated exposure the most appropriate level of sweetness for a particular food | Beauchamp and Moran, 1984 [94]; Sullivan and Birch, 1990 [102]; Liem and de Graaf, 2004 [107] | |
Increased consumption of a sweetened version of a food or beverage (e.g., spaghetti, Kool-Aid, vegetables) compared with unsweetened counterpart | Filer, 1978 [95]; Beauchamp and Moran, 1984 [94]; Sharafi et al., 2013[97] | |
Sucrose masked the bitterness and increased liking of a range of bitter-tasting stimuli (urea, caffeine, denatonium benzoate, propylthiouracil, and quinine) | Mennella et al., 2015 [96] | |
Sucrose detection thresholds decreased with age and central obesity (the older the child or the more central obesity, the more sensitive the child was to sweet taste) | Joseph et al., 2016 [87] |
This review is not meant to be representative of all research conducted to date; rather, it highlights the breadth of methodologies employed and the convergence of findings.
The sensory pleasantness derived from tasting something sweet is inborn. Before birth, the ability to detect sweet tastes is functioning and interacting with systems controlling affect. Upon tasting something sweet, infants born preterm produced stronger and more frequent sucking responses [48, 49] and were less likely to exhibit spontaneous crying when compared to tasting water [50]. Within hours of birth, newborns can differentiate varying degrees of sweetness [51] and will ingest a greater volume of a solution that tastes sweeter [52]. When something sweet tasting is in the oral cavity, their faces relax and this relaxation is often accompanied by a smile [53–55] and they begin sucking more voraciously (i.e., longer sucking burst, decreased latency to suck) [56, 57]. That sweet taste per se promotes feeding is suggested by the finding that tasting either nutritive sweeteners (e.g., sucrose) or nonnutritive sweeteners (NNS; e.g., aspartame) elicits mouthing and sucking movements and hand-to-mouth contact [58–60], all of which are feeding-related behaviors.
Perhaps related to its effects on promoting ingestion, tasting something sweet blunts expressions of pain [61–64]. A small amount of a sweet solution placed on the tongue of a crying newborn exerts a rapid calming effect that persists for several minutes [58, 60] (see Stevens [61] for review). Because the NNS aspartame mimics the effects of sucrose [59, 65], and because the administration of sucrose by direct stomach loading is not effective [66], afferent signals from the mouth, rather than gastric or metabolic changes, appear to be responsible. Tasting sucrose, but not water, delayed 8- to 11-year-olds’ reporting of pain onset when undergoing the cold pressor test, a cold-induced pain stimulus test [67–69], but was not evident in adults [69]. However, the pain-reducing effect of sucrose in children is context dependent, since tasting something sweet was ineffective during needle-related procedural pain [70].
During the cold pressor test, the more a child liked the taste of sucrose, the better it increased pain tolerance. Of interest, the pain-reducing properties of sugars were attenuated among children who were overweight or at risk for overweight [69]. Whether this attenuation is due to impairment of hypothalamo-pituitary control and higher than normal beta-endorphin plasma levels, which have been observed in obese children [71], is not known. Nor do we know whether these children consumed carbohydrates and sweet-tasting foods chronically or in higher amounts, which could in turn modify opioid and dopamine receptor binding and thus the efficacy of its analgesic properties [72–77].
When it comes to sweet tastes, children live in different sensory worlds than adults. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have revealed that the preference for sweet taste remains heightened throughout childhood, declining to adult levels during mid-adolescence [33, 35, 68, 78–83]. Like adults [84, 85], children exhibit a wide range in their taste sensitivity to sweet (i.e., lowest concentration of sweetness that can be detected), and such variation is due, in part, to variation in taste receptor genes and measures of obesity [86, 87]. But it does not appear that children prefer higher levels of sweet taste because they are insensitive to its taste. What causes the age-related decline in sweet preference and consumption between adolescence and adulthood remains a mystery, but it has been observed in other mammals [88]. There is evidence that the heightened sweet preference may be linked to the growing child’s need for calories that begins during infancy and continues until the cessation of puberty [89]. Two studies found that levels of sweetness most preferred by children positively correlated with urinary concentrations of N-telopeptides of type I collagen [35, 90], a biomarker for bone resorption and growth [91], thus suggesting that the age-related decline in sucrose preferences may be related to the cessation of physical growth.
Children’s preference for sweet taste is evident not only in the level of sweetness they prefer in a liquid as measured in the laboratory but also in the foods they consume. The higher the level of sweetness in solution most preferred, the higher the sugar content of their favorite foods, including breakfast cereals [80, 92] and beverages [80, 93]. Children will drink more of a beverage (e.g., Kool-Aid [94]) and eat more of a food (e.g., spaghetti [95]) when it contains a higher amount of added sugar and thus tastes sweeter. Sweet taste drives consumption not just because children like its taste but because adding sugar masks the unpleasant tastes (e.g., bitter [96, 97]) inherent to a food or beverage or which develop during manufacturing and processing [98].
During childhood, children learn the rules of cuisine: what to eat, how to eat, when to eat, and how sweet a food is supposed to taste [99–104]. The sensation of sweetness is context dependent and can acquire meaning through associative learning [105, 106]. Through familiarization and repeated exposure, children develop a sense of what should, or should not, taste sweet [94, 102, 107]. Longitudinal studies revealed that babies who were routinely fed sweetened water (e.g., water sweetened with table sugar or honey) during the first months of life exhibited a greater preference for sweetened water when tested at 6 months and then again at 2–10 years of age, compared with children who had little or no experience with sweetened water as infants [81, 94, 108]. Perhaps feeding sugar water during infancy is an indication of the likelihood that sugar-sweetened beverages and other foods will be part of their family diet, or perhaps the children have been programmed to have heightened preferences for sweet tastes (see [109–111]).
In recent years, the food environment in which children live has changed, with increasing numbers of foods, beverages, and liquid medications geared for pediatric populations sweetened with NNS or blends of nutritive sweeteners and NNS [112], which contribute sweetness with few to no calories. Because of the paucity of research on how children respond to the taste of NNS, we expanded the study of sweet preferences in children to include the NNS sucralose and compared preference ratings between the two stimuli. The most preferred concentrations of sucrose and sucralose were determined separately among 6- to 14-year-olds via the Monell forced-choice, paired-comparison tracking procedure [79]. Sucrose solutions ranging in concentration from 90 to 1050 mM, and sucralose solutions ranging from 0.1 to 1.5 mM, were presented during two separate test sessions; concentration ranges were based on equated sweetness intensity of the two stimuli; psychophysical testing revealed sucralose is approximately 600 times sweeter than sucrose on a weight-by-weight basis [113]). All procedures were approved by the Office of Regulatory Affairs at the University of Pennsylvania, and informed consent was obtained from their mothers and informed assent from the children.
Children most preferred a 700 mM sucrose solution and a 1.03 mM sucralose solution. While sucralose is sweeter than sucrose on a weight-by-weight basis, from these data one cannot conclude that children like sucralose more than sucrose. To put this in perspective, this concentration of sucrose most preferred by these children is equivalent to approximately 14 teaspoons of sugar in 237 ml of water (i.e., an eight-ounce glass), nearly twice the sugar concentration of a typical cola. When the sweetness of sucralose is equated for the sweetness of sucrose [113], the level of sucralose most preferred is nearly identical (720 mM sucrose). That the level most preferred for these two taste stimuli are highly correlated [r(36)=0.70, p<0.001], suggests that, at least for sucrose and sucralose, children have a particular bliss point range for sweetness.
3. Concluding Remarks
The convergence of scientific evidence suggests that the ability to detect and prefer sweet taste is evident early in life and reflects both biology and experiential learning. Evolution has shaped the child’s response to sweetened foods and beverages, and our sensory systems evolved to detect and prefer the once rare calorie-rich foods that taste sweet [114]. While this “sweet” attraction may have served children well in a feast-or-famine setting, attracting them to mothers’ milk and then to energy-rich foods during periods of growth, today it makes them vulnerable to food environments abundant in processed foods rich in added sugars and lacking healthy sweet foods, such as fruits [33, 114, 115]. Indeed, the more fruits in a child’s diet, the less added sugar the child consumed [9, 116]. We hypothesize that in the current food environment, fruits are competing with “supernormal foods” [117]—that is, foods and beverages rich in added sugars [118] and/or NNS—which children may be especially vulnerable to overconsuming, given their proclivity for sweet taste. Once children become accustomed to a highly sweetened diet, they may find that the very tastes that their sensory systems evolved to be attracted to (e.g., fruits) don’t taste “sweet enough” [22]. This is supported by experimental studies in animal models showing that initially preferred sweet solutions became less preferred after rodents experienced more intense sweetness [88].
Given the increased prevalence of NNS in the food supply, including products intended for children (of which parents are often unaware [119]), how much added sugar is in the child’s diet may no longer be a good proxy for the diet’s overall level of “sweetness.” This changing food supply brings to light a number of questions regarding the impact of sweetness on the growing and developing child that are priorities for future research [120–122]. Because some taste receptors are located in the endocrine system of the gut [42], what are the consequences of exposure to NNS (sweet but few to no calories) in growing children [17, 123, 124]? What are the consequences of children learning to associate sweet taste with foods that typically are not sweet but, because of added sugars and/or NNS, have been processed to taste sweet? Since children prefer a more intense sweet sensation than do adults, are they are more likely to overconsume NNS? Given the current evidence suggesting that reducing overall intake of sweetness (both caloric sweeteners and NNS) may be a better strategy to lower rates of obesity than simply replacing the former with the latter [17], can children who have learned that foods should taste sweet relearn to like a particular food in its natural, unsweetened form?
Understanding the unique vulnerability of children to the modern food system is a critical first step for developing informed, evidence-based strategies to address what has become an issue of great public health importance, because many chronic diseases plaguing modern society derive in large part from poor food choices, dictated by our flavor preferences and the types of foods that are available and deemed appropriate for children. Increasing healthy dietary behaviors (and decreasing unhealthy habits), beginning in childhood, is recognized as the most important aspect of reducing an individual’s risk of lifestyle-related diseases [125].
Acknowledgments
This writing of this manuscript was supported by the National Institute of Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (NIDCD), National Institutes of Health (NIH) (grants R01 DC011287, P30 DC00117356) and by NIH postdoctoral training grants T32 DC100004-34 and F32 DC15172. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of NIDCD or NIH. The funding agencies had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation or contents of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest.
The authors have no conflict of interests to declare. Each is supported by funding provided by National Institutes of Health. Author JAM is supported by NIH grants R01 DC011287, R01 HD37119, and R01 HD072307; author NB is supported by T32 DC100004-34 and F32 DC15172; and author DRR is supported by R01 DC011287 and P30 DC00117356.
References
- 1.Siega-Riz AM, Kinlaw A, Deming DM, Reidy KC. New findings from the Feeding Infants and Toddlers Study 2008. Nestle Nutr Workshop Ser Pediatr Program. 2011;68:83–100. doi: 10.1159/000325667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Saavedra JM, Deming D, Dattilo A, Reidy K. Lessons from the feeding infants and toddlers study in North America: what children eat, and implications for obesity prevention. Ann Nutr Metab. 2013;62:27–36. doi: 10.1159/000351538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Nickelson J, Lawrence JC, Parton JM, Knowlden AP, McDermott RJ. What proportion of preschool-aged children consume sweetened beverages? J Sch Health. 2014;84:185–94. doi: 10.1111/josh.12136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.World Health Organization. [Accessed 18 Mar 2016];Sugars intake for adults and children. 2015 http://www.who.int/nutrition/publications/guidelines/sugars_intake/en/ [PubMed]
- 5.U.S. Department of Agriculture and U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2015–2020. 8. Washington D.C: Government Printing Office; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Reedy J, Krebs-Smith SM. Dietary sources of energy, solid fats, and added sugars among children and adolescents in the United States. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110:1477–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Krebs-Smith SM, Guenther PM, Subar AF, Kirkpatrick SI, Dodd KW. Americans do not meet federal dietary recommendations. J Nutr. 2010;140:1832–8. doi: 10.3945/jn.110.124826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ford CN, Slining MM, Popkin BM. Trends in dietary intake among US 2- to 6-year-old children, 1989–2008. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2013;113:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.08.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Drewnowski A, Rehm CD. Consumption of added sugars among US children and adults by food purchase location and food source. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;100:901–7. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.089458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Martinez Steele E, Baraldi LG, Louzada ML, Moubarac JC, Mozaffarian D, Monteiro CA. Ultra-processed foods and added sugars in the US diet: evidence from a nationally representative cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2016 doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Piernas C, Ng SW, Mendez MA, Gordon-Larsen P, Popkin BM. A dynamic panel model of the associations of sweetened beverage purchases with dietary quality and food-purchasing patterns. Am J Epidemiol. 2015;181:661–71. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dattilo AM, Birch L, Krebs NF, Lake A, Taveras EM, Saavedra JM. Need for early interventions in the prevention of pediatric overweight: a review and upcoming directions. J Obes. 2012;2012:1–18. doi: 10.1155/2012/123023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Welsh JA, Cunningham SA. The role of added sugars in pediatric obesity. Pediatr Clin North Am. 2011;58:1455–66. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2011.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Welsh JA, Sharma A, Cunningham SA, Vos MB. Consumption of added sugars and indicators of cardiovascular disease risk among US adolescents. Circulation. 2011;123:249–57. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.110.972166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Flynn MA, McNeil DA, Maloff B, Mutasingwa D, Wu M, Ford C, et al. Reducing obesity and related chronic disease risk in children and youth: a synthesis of evidence with ‘best practice’ recommendations. Obes Rev. 2006;7:7–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2006.00242.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Swinburn BA, Caterson I, Seidell JC, James WP. Diet, nutrition and the prevention of excess weight gain and obesity. Public Health Nutr. 2004;7:123–46. doi: 10.1079/phn2003585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Swithers SE. Artificial sweeteners are not the answer to childhood obesity. Appetite. 2015;93:85–90. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yang Q, Zhang Z, Gregg EW, Flanders WD, Merritt R, Hu FB. Added sugar intake and cardiovascular diseases mortality among US adults. JAMA Intern Med. 2014;174:516–24. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.American Academy of Pediatric Dentistry; American Academy of Pediatrics. Policy on early childhood caries (ECC): classifications, consequences, and preventive strategies. Pediatr Dent. 2008–2009;30:40–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Park S, Lin M, Onufrak S, Li R. Association of sugar-sweetened beverage intake during infancy with dental caries in 6-year-olds. Clin Nutr Res. 2015;4:9–17. doi: 10.7762/cnr.2015.4.1.9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Reed DR, Knaapila A. Genetics of taste and smell: poisons and pleasures. Prog Mol Biol Transl Scie. 2010;94:213–40. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-375003-7.00008-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ungar PS, Grine FE, Teaford MF. Dental microwear and diet of the Plio-Pleistocene hominin Paranthropus boisei. PLoS One. 2008;3:e2044. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Glendinning JI. Is the bitter rejection response always adaptive? Physiol Behav. 1994;56:1217–27. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90369-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;199:457–80. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Yarmolinsky DA, Zuker CS, Ryba NJ. Common sense about taste: from mammals to insects. Cell. 2009;139:234–44. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bachmanov AA, Bosak NP, Lin C, Matsumoto I, Ohmoto M, Reed DR, et al. Genetics of taste receptors. Curr Pharm Des. 2014;20:2669–83. doi: 10.2174/13816128113199990566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Berridge KC, Ho CY, Richard JM, DiFeliceantonio AG. The tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res. 2010;1350:43–64. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Building a neuroscience of pleasure and well-being. Psychol Well Being. 2011;1:1–3. doi: 10.1186/2211-1522-1-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Reed DR, Margolskee RF. Gustation genetics: sweet gustducin! Chem Senses. 2010;35:549–50. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Reed DR, McDaniel AH. The human sweet tooth. BMC Oral Health. 2006;6:S17. doi: 10.1186/1472-6831-6-S1-S17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.McLaughlin SK, McKinnon PJ, Margolskee RF. Gustducin is a taste-cell-specific G protein closely related to the transducins. Nature. 1992;357:563–9. doi: 10.1038/357563a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fushan AA, Simons CT, Slack JP, Manichaikul A, Drayna D. Allelic polymorphism within the TAS1R3 promoter is associated with human taste sensitivity to sucrose. Curr Biol. 2009;19:1288–93. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Mennella JA, Bobowski NK. The sweetness and bitterness of childhood: Insights from basic research on taste preferences. Physiol Behav. 2015;152:502–7. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Mennella JA, Finkbeiner S, Reed DR. The proof is in the pudding: children prefer lower fat but higher sugar than do mothers. Int J Obes (Lond) 2012;36:1285–91. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Mennella JA, Finkbeiner S, Lipchock SV, Hwang LD, Reed DR. Preferences for salty and sweet tastes are elevated and related to each other during childhood. PLoS One. 2014;9:e92201. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Eny KM, Wolever TM, Corey PN, El-Sohemy A. Genetic variation in TAS1R2 (Ile191Val) is associated with consumption of sugars in overweight and obese individuals in 2 distinct populations. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92:1501–10. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2010.29836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dias AG, Eny KM, Cockburn M, Chiu W, Nielsen DE, Duizer L, et al. Variation in the TAS1R2 gene, sweet taste perception and intake of sugars. J Nutrigenet Nutrigenomics. 2015;8:81–90. doi: 10.1159/000430886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Fushan AA, Simons CT, Slack JP, Drayna D. Association between common variation in genes encoding sweet taste signaling components and human sucrose perception. Chem Senses. 2010;35:579–92. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hwang LD, Zhu G, Breslin PA, Reed DR, Martin NG, Wright MJ. A common genetic influence on human intensity ratings of sugars and high-potency sweeteners. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2015;18:361–7. doi: 10.1017/thg.2015.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Taniguchi K. Expression of the sweet receptor protein, T1R3, in the human liver and pancreas. J Vet Med Sci. 2004;66:1311–4. doi: 10.1292/jvms.66.1311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kyriazis GA, Soundarapandian MM, Tyberg B. Sweet taste receptor signaling in beta cells mediates fructose-induced potentiation of glucose-stimulated insulin secretion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:E524–32. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115183109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Margolskee RF, Dyer J, Kokrashvili Z, Salmon KS, Ilegems E, Daly K, et al. T1R3 and gustducin in gut sense sugars to regulate expression of Na+-glucose cotransporter 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:15075–80. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706678104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Shen T, Kaya N, Zhao F-L, Lu S-G, Cao Y, Herness S. Co-expression patterns of the neuropeptides vasoactive intestinal peptide and cholecystokinin with the transduction molecules α-gustducin and T1R2 in rat taste receptor cells. Neuroscience. 2005;130:229–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Herness S, Zhao F-l. The neuropeptides CCK and NPY and the changing view of cell-to-cell communication in the taste bud. Physiol Behav. 2009;97:581–91. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Huang YA, Dando R, Roper SD. Autocrine and paracrine roles for ATP and serotonin in mouse taste buds. J Neurosci. 2009;29:13909–18. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2351-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Shin Y-K, Martin B, Kim W, White CM, Ji S, Sun Y, et al. Ghrelin is produced in taste cells and ghrelin receptor null mice show reduced taste responsivity to salty (NaCl) and sour (citric acid) tastants. PLoS One. 2010;5:e12729. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kokrashvili Z, Yee KK, Ilegems E, Iwatsuki K, Li Y, Mosinger B, et al. Endocrine taste cells. B J Nutr. 2014;111:S23–9. doi: 10.1017/S0007114513002262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Maone TR, Mattes RD, Bernbaum JC, Beauchamp GK. A new method for delivering a taste without fluids to preterm and term infants. Dev Psychobiol. 1990;13:179–91. doi: 10.1002/dev.420230208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tatzer E, Schubert MT, Timischl W, Simbruner G. Discrimination of taste and preference for sweet in premature babies. Early Hum Dev. 1985;12:23–30. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(85)90133-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Smith BA, Blass EM. Taste-mediated calming in premature, preterm, and full-term human infants. Dev Psychol. 1996;32:1084–9. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Desor JA, Maller O, Turner RE. Preference for sweet in humans: infants, children and adults. In: Weiffenbach JM, editor. Taste and development: the genesis of sweet preference. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office; 1977. pp. 161–72. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Desor JA, Maller O, Turner RE. Taste in acceptance of sugars by human infants. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1973;84:496–501. doi: 10.1037/h0034906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Steiner JE, Glaser D, Hawilo ME, Berridge KC. Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:53–74. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00051-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Steiner JE. Taste and development: the genesis of sweet preference. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 1977. Facial expressions of the neonate infant indicating the hedonics of food-related chemical stimuli. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rosenstein D, Oster H. Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Dev. 1988;59:1555–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Crook CK. Taste perception in the newborn infant. Infant Behav Dev. 1978;1:52–69. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Crook CK, Lipsitt LP. Neonatal nutritive sucking: effects of taste stimulation upon sucking rhythm and heart rate. Child Dev. 1976;47:518–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Barr RG, Quek VS, Cousineau D, Oberlander TF, Brian JA, Young SN. Effects of intra-oral sucrose on crying, mouthing and hand-mouth contact in newborn and six-week-old infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1994;36:608–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11898.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Barr RG, Pantel MS, Young SN, Wright JH, Hendricks LA, Gravel R. The response of crying newborns to sucrose: is it a “sweetness” effect? Physiol Behav. 1999;66:409–17. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00294-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Blass EM, Hoffmeyer LB. Sucrose as an analgesic for newborn infants. Pediatrics. 1991;87:215–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Stevens B, Yamada J, Ohlsson A. Sucrose for analgesia in newborn infants undergoing painful procedures. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD001069.pub3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Abad F, Diaz NM, Domenech E, Robayna M, Rico J. Oral sweet solution reduces pain-related behaviour in preterm infants. Acta Paediatr. 1996;85:854–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1996.tb14167.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Deshmukh LS, Udani RH. Analgesic effect of oral glucose in preterm infants during venipuncture--a double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. J Trop Pediatr. 2002;48:138–41. doi: 10.1093/tropej/48.3.138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Harrison D, Stevens B, Bueno M, Yamada J, Adams-Webber T, Beyene J, et al. Efficacy of sweet solutions for analgesia in infants between 1 and 12 months of age: a systematic review. Arch Dis Child. 2010;95:406–13. doi: 10.1136/adc.2009.174227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Bucher HU, Baumgartner R, Bucher N, Seiler M, Fauchere JC. Artificial sweetener reduces nociceptive reaction in term newborn infants. Early Hum Dev. 2000;59:51–60. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(00)00085-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ramenghi LA, Evans DJ, Levene MI. “Sucrose analgesia”: absorptive mechanism or taste perception? Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 1999;80:F146–7. doi: 10.1136/fn.80.2.f146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Miller A, Barr RG, Young SN. The cold pressor test in children: methodological aspects and the analgesic effect of intraoral sucrose. Pain. 1994;56:175–83. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)90092-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Lehmann-Castor SM, Yourshaw LM. Sweet preferences and analgesia during childhood: effects of family history of alcoholism and depression. Addiction. 2010;105:666–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02865.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Pepino MY, Mennella JA. Sucrose-induced analgesia is related to sweet preferences in children but not adults. Pain. 2005;119:210–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.09.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Harrison D, Yamada J, Adams-Webber T, Ohlsson A, Beyene J, Stevens B. Sweet tasting solutions for reduction of needle-related procedural pain in children aged one to 16 years. The Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015 doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008408.pub3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bernasconi S, Petraglia F, Iughetti L, Marcellini C, Lamborghini A, Facchinetti F, et al. Impaired beta-endorphin response to human corticotropin-releasing hormone in obese children. Acta Endocrinol (Copenh) 1988;119:7–10. doi: 10.1530/acta.0.1190007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Coy RT, Kanarek RB. Chronic sucrose intake reduces the antagonist effect of beta-funaltrexamine on morphine-induced antinociception in female but not in male rats. Nutr Neurosci. 2006;9:131–9. doi: 10.1080/10284150600895881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.D’Anci KE, Kanarek RB, Marks-Kaufman R. Beyond sweet taste: saccharin, sucrose, and polycose differ in their effects upon morphine-induced analgesia. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;56:341–5. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00227-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Levine AS, Kotz CM, Gosnell BA. Sugars and fats: the neurobiology of preference. J Nutr. 2003;133:831S–4S. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.3.831S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Levine AS, Kotz CM, Gosnell BA. Sugars: hedonic aspects, neuroregulation, and energy balance. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003;78:834S–42S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/78.4.834S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Colantuoni C, Rada P, McCarthy J, Patten C, Avena NM, Chadeayne A, et al. Evidence that intermittent, excessive sugar intake causes endogenous opioid dependence. Obes Res. 2002;10:478–88. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Rada P, Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly releases dopamine in the accumbens shell. Neuroscience. 2005;134:737–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Desor JA, Beauchamp GK. Longitudinal changes in sweet preferences in humans. Physiol Behav. 1987;39:639–41. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90166-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Mennella JA, Lukasewycz LD, Griffith JW, Beauchamp GK. Evaluation of the Monell forced-choice, paired-comparison tracking procedure for determining sweet taste preferences across the lifespan. Chem Senses. 2011;36:345–55. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Mennella JA, Pepino MY, Reed DR. Genetic and environmental determinants of bitter perception and sweet preferences. Pediatrics. 2005;115:e216–22. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-1582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Pepino MY, Mennella JA. Factors contributing to individual differences in sucrose preference. Chem Senses. 2005;30:i319–i320. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.de Graaf C, Zandstra EH. Sweetness intensity and pleasantness in children, adolescents, and adults. Physiol Behav. 1999;67:513–20. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(99)00090-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Desor JA, Greene LS, Maller O. Preferences for sweet and salty in 9- to 15-year-old and adult humans. Science. 1975;190:686–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1188365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Pepino MY, Finkbeiner S, Beauchamp GK, Mennella JA. Obese women have lower monosodium glutamate taste sensitivity and prefer higher concentrations than do normal-weight women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2010;18:959–65. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Pepino MY, Mennella JA. Effects of cigarette smoking and family history of alcoholism on sweet taste perception and food cravings in women. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:1891–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00519.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.James CE, Laing DG, Oram N. A comparison of the ability of 8–9-year-old children and adults to detect taste stimuli. Physiol Behav. 1997;62:193–7. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00030-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Joseph PV, Reed DR, Mennella JA. Individual differences among children in sucrose detection thresholds: Relationship with age, gender, and bitter taste genotype. Nurs Res. 2016;65:3–12. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0000000000000138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Bertino M, Wehmer F. Dietary influences on the development of sucrose acceptability in rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1981;14:19–28. doi: 10.1002/dev.420140104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Drewnowski A. Sensory control of energy density at different life stages. Proc Nutr Soc. 2000;59:239–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Coldwell SE, Oswald TK, Reed DR. A marker of growth differs between adolescents with high vs. low sugar preference. Physiol Behav. 2009;96:574–80. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bollen AM. A prospective longitudinal study of urinary excretion of a bone resorption marker in adolescents. Ann Hum Biol. 2000;27:199–211. doi: 10.1080/030144600282307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Liem DG, Mennella JA. Heightened sour preferences during childhood. Chem Senses. 2003;28:173–80. doi: 10.1093/chemse/28.2.173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Olson CM, Gemmill KP. Association of sweet preference and food selection among four to five year old children. Ecol Food Nutr. 1981;11:145–50. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Beauchamp GK, Moran M. Acceptance of sweet and salty tastes in 2-year-old children. Appetite. 1984;5:291–305. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(84)80002-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Filer LJ. Studies of taste preference in infancy and childhood. Pediatric Basics. 1978;12:5–9. [Google Scholar]
- 96.Mennella JA, Reed DR, Mathew PS, Roberts KM, Mansfield CJ. “A spoonful of sugar helps the medicine go down”: bitter masking by sucrose among children and adults. Chem Senses. 2015;40:17–25. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bju053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Sharafi M, Hayes JE, Duffy VB. Masking vegetable bitterness to improve palatability depends on vegetable type and taste phenotype. Chemosens Percept. 2013;6:8–19. doi: 10.1007/s12078-012-9137-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Kilcast D, den Ridder C, Narain C. Challenges to reducing sugar in foods. In: Weerasinghe D, editor. Sweetness and Sweeteners. Washington DC: American Chemical Society; 2008. pp. 481–91. [Google Scholar]
- 99.Mennella JA, Nicklaus S, Jagolino AL, Yourshaw LM. Variety is the spice of life: strategies for promoting fruit and vegetable acceptance during infancy. Physiol Behav. 2008;94:29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Gerrish CJ, Mennella JA. Flavor variety enhances food acceptance in formula-fed infants. Am J Clin Nutr. 2001;73:1080–5. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/73.6.1080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Forestell CA, Mennella JA. Early determinants of fruit and vegetable acceptance. Pediatrics. 2007;120:1247–54. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Sullivan SA, Birch LL. Pass the sugar, pass the salt: experience dictates preference. Dev Psychol. 1990;26:546–51. [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rozin P. The acquisition of food habits and preferences. In: Mattarazzo JD, Weiss SM, Herd JA, Miller NE, weiss SM, editors. Behavioral Health: A Handbook of Health Enhancement and Disease Prevention. New York, New York: John Wiley and Sons; 1984. pp. 590–607. [Google Scholar]
- 104.Rozin P, Vollmecke TA. Food likes and dislikes. Annu Rev Nutr. 1986;6:433–56. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nu.06.070186.002245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Beauchamp GK, Cowart BJ. Congenital and experiential factors in the development of human flavor preferences. Appetite. 1985;6:357–72. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(85)80004-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Sclafani A. Oral and postoral determinants of food reward. Physiol Behav. 2004;81:773–9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Liem DG, de Graaf C. Sweet and sour preferences in young children and adults: role of repeated exposure. Physiol Behav. 2004;83:421–9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.08.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Beauchamp GK, Moran M. Dietary experience and sweet taste preference in human infants. Appetite. 1982;3:139–52. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(82)80007-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Ong ZY, Muhlhausler BS. Maternal “junk-food” feeding of rat dams alters food choices and development of the mesolimbic reward pathway in the offspring. FASEB J. 2011;25:2167–79. doi: 10.1096/fj.10-178392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Murray SM, Tulloch AJ, Chen EY, Avena NM. Insights revealed by rodent models of sugar binge eating. CNS Spectr. 2015;20:530–6. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Bayol SA, Macharia R, Farrington SJ, Simbi BH, Stickland NC. Evidence that a maternal “junk food” diet during pregnancy and lactation can reduce muscle force in offspring. Eur J Nutr. 2009;48:62–5. doi: 10.1007/s00394-008-0760-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Mattes RD, Popkin BM. Nonnutritive sweetener consumption in humans: effects on appetite and food intake and their putative mechanisms. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89:1–14. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Wiet S, Beyts PK. Sensory characteristics of sucralose and other high intensity sweeteners. J Food Sci. 1992;57:1014–9. [Google Scholar]
- 114.Simmen B, Hladik CM. Sweet and bitter taste discrimination in primates: scaling effects across species. Folia Primatol (Basel) 1998;69:129–38. doi: 10.1159/000021575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Mennella JA. The sweet taste of childhood. In: Firestein S, Beauchamp GK, editors. The Senses: A comprehensive reference, Vol 4. Olfaction and Taste. San Diego, CA: Elsevier; 2008. pp. 183–8. [Google Scholar]
- 116.Wang J, Shang L, Light K, O’Loughlin J, Paradis G, Gray-Donald K. Associations between added sugar (solid vs. liquid) intakes, diet quality, and adiposity indicators in Canadian children. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2015;40:835–41. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2014-0447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Lenoir M, Serre F, Cantin L, Ahmed SH. Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward. PLoS One. 2007;2:e698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Fox MK, Gordon A, Nogales R, Wilson A. Availability and consumption of competitive foods in US public schools. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109:S57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2008.10.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Sylvetsky AC, Greenberg M, Zhao X, Rother KI. What parents think about giving nonnutritive sweteners to their children: A pilot study. Int J Pediatr. 2014 doi: 10.1155/2014/819872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Pepino MY. Metabolic effects of non-nutritive sweeteners. Physiol Behav. 2015;152:450–5. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Drewnowski A, Levine AS. Sugar and fat--from genes to culture. J Nutr. 2003;133:829S–30S. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.3.829S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Avena NM, Potenza MN, Gold MS. Why are we consuming so much sugar despite knowing too much can harm us? JAMA Intern Med. 2015;175:145–6. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.6968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Ren X, Ferreira JG, Zhou L, Shammah-Lagnado SJ, Yeckel CW, de Araujo IE. Nutrient selection in the absence of taste receptor signaling. J Neurosci. 2010;30:8012–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5749-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Brown RJ, Rother KI. Non-nutritive sweeteners and their role in the gastrointestinal tract. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97:2597–605. doi: 10.1210/jc.2012-1475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Rayner M, Scarborough P. The burden of food related ill health in the UK. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59:1054–7. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.036491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]