Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2016 Sep 6;113(38):10696–10701. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1612278113

Episodic specificity induction impacts activity in a core brain network during construction of imagined future experiences

Kevin P Madore a,1, Karl K Szpunar b, Donna Rose Addis c, Daniel L Schacter a,1
PMCID: PMC5035866  PMID: 27601666

Significance

Recent behavioral studies using an episodic specificity induction—training in recollecting details of past experiences—have suggested a role for episodic memory in imagining future events, solving problems, and thinking creatively. The present fMRI study examines the brain regions impacted by the specificity induction. The experiment shows that receiving a specificity induction led to increased activity in key brain regions previously implicated in detailed event construction, including the hippocampus and inferior parietal lobule, when participants imagined future events. These results provide insights into the influence of episodic memory beyond simple remembering, and may help to guide potential applications for individuals from populations characterized by overgeneralized memory and imagination, such as healthy aging and clinical depression.

Keywords: episodic specificity induction, imagination, hippocampus, core network, fMRI

Abstract

Recent behavioral work suggests that an episodic specificity induction—brief training in recollecting the details of a past experience—enhances performance on subsequent tasks that rely on episodic retrieval, including imagining future experiences, solving open-ended problems, and thinking creatively. Despite these far-reaching behavioral effects, nothing is known about the neural processes impacted by an episodic specificity induction. Related neuroimaging work has linked episodic retrieval with a core network of brain regions that supports imagining future experiences. We tested the hypothesis that key structures in this network are influenced by the specificity induction. Participants received the specificity induction or one of two control inductions and then generated future events and semantic object comparisons during fMRI scanning. After receiving the specificity induction compared with the control, participants exhibited significantly more activity in several core network regions during the construction of imagined events over object comparisons, including the left anterior hippocampus, right inferior parietal lobule, right posterior cingulate cortex, and right ventral precuneus. Induction-related differences in the episodic detail of imagined events significantly modulated induction-related differences in the construction of imagined events in the left anterior hippocampus and right inferior parietal lobule. Resting-state functional connectivity analyses with hippocampal and inferior parietal lobule seed regions and the rest of the brain also revealed significantly stronger core network coupling following the specificity induction compared with the control. These findings provide evidence that an episodic specificity induction selectively targets episodic processes that are commonly linked to key core network regions, including the hippocampus.


Numerous recent studies have revealed striking overlap in the neural and cognitive processes that support remembering past experiences and imagining future experiences or novel scenes (reviewed in refs. 1, 2). According to the constructive episodic simulation hypothesis (3), similarities between remembering and imagining reflect to a large extent the contributions of episodic memory to both processes (4). However, some evidence indicates that these similarities can also reflect the influence of nonepisodic processes, such as descriptive ability or narrative style, that influence remembering and imagining (5).

We recently developed an experimental approach to distinguishing episodic and nonepisodic influences on remembering and imagining that we refer to as an episodic specificity induction: brief training in recollecting episodic details of recent experiences (reviewed in ref. 6). After receiving an episodic specificity induction (vs. a control induction), participants subsequently remembered past and imagined future experiences with increased episodic but not semantic detail, and the specificity induction had no effect on details generated during tasks that do not draw on episodic memory, such as describing a picture (7) or defining and comparing words (8). We have also shown that the specificity induction boosts performance on such tasks as means-end problem solving (9, 10) and divergent creative thinking (11) that have also been linked previously to episodic memory. Based on these results, we have proposed that the specificity induction biases participants to adopt a specific retrieval orientation—i.e., to focus on episodic details related to places, people, or actions—and that this heightened focus on episodic details impacts performance on tasks that involve constructing mental events or scenes containing details like those emphasized during the specificity induction (6).

Although our previous work has examined the cognitive processes impacted by the specificity induction, our characterization of those processes, together with previous research concerning the neural underpinnings of remembering and imagining, leads to predictions regarding the neural processes that should be influenced by the induction. Previous studies have indicated that remembering and imagining rely on a common core network of brain regions (12, 13) that overlaps substantially with the default network (1417). According to a recent meta-analysis (13), this core network includes regions within all of the key segments of the default network, including its medial temporal lobe (MTL) subsystem (hippocampus, parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortex, inferior parietal lobe, ventromedial prefrontal cortex), which has been linked with the construction of imagined events or scenes, and its dorsomedial prefrontal subsystem (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, lateral temporal cortex), which has been linked with social components of events (15).

In light of our behavioral characterization of the specificity induction, we hypothesize and test here that (i) this induction should impact primarily structures within the MTL subsystem, in particular those regions—the hippocampus and inferior parietal lobule—that have been linked previously to detailed episodic retrieval and to imagining of specific (vs. general) future events (1820). Moreover, our behavioral characterization of the specificity induction as affecting primarily participants’ retrieval orientation when they construct mental events or scenes leads us to predict that (ii) these effects will be observed mainly during the initial construction of an event. We adopted a construction-elaboration paradigm used in previous studies of remembering and imagining (21) to test this hypothesis, whereby an initial phase of event construction is distinguished from later event elaboration. To maximize our power to detect possibly subtle effects of the specificity induction, we replaced the remembering condition with additional imagining trials. We also hypothesized that (iii) induction-related differences in the episodic detail of imagined events from a postscan interview would modulate induction-related differences in neural activity in the construction of imagined events during scanning in the hippocampus and inferior parietal lobule. Resting-state functional connectivity analyses with hippocampal and inferior parietal lobule seed regions and the rest of the brain were also performed to test whether (iv) stronger coupling between these regions and other core network areas would be observed following the specificity relative to control induction.

To address our predictions, participants completed a within-subjects fMRI paradigm in one session (Fig. S1). In each segment in the scanner, participants (i) viewed one of two short videos, completed a short filler task, and then received the episodic specificity induction or one of two control inductions; (ii) viewed 36 object cues after receiving an induction and, for each cue, generated an imagined event or an object size sentence and definitions (i.e., the main tasks); and (iii) completed a resting-state scan. Different stimuli were used in each segment for the induction and main tasks (counterbalanced across participants). For each of the main task scanning trials, we collected reaction time to construct, and detail and engagement ratings. Following scanning, participants verbally generated their thoughts for each main task cue and completed additional ratings. A similar approach was tested in a behavioral pilot in which induction effects were observed (8).

Fig. S1.

Fig. S1.

Participants completed a within-subjects, event-related design in a single fMRI scanning session. (Top) Scanning procedure. In segment one of scanning, participants viewed a short video followed by a brief filler task, and then received a control induction (focused on general impressions of the video or math problems) or the episodic specificity induction (focused on specific features of the video). After the induction phase, participants completed three runs of functional neuroimaging for the main tasks in which they viewed 36 object cues (12 per run) and generated an imagined event related to the cue or a semantic object comparison and definitions related to the cue. Following these runs of scanning, participants completed one resting-state scan, and then a brief filler task before beginning segment two (involving whichever video and induction were not received in the first segment, and new main task cues). (Middle) Sample trial cycle during the three fMRI task runs. Participants viewed a screen for 20 s with the phrase “imagine” or “objects” followed by the instruction “near future event” or “size and define” followed by an object cue (e.g., CLOCK). Participants pressed a button when they had initially constructed an imagined event or size sentence for the object cue, and then elaborated on the contents of the imagined event or semantic definitions for the objects from the size sentence until the trial was over. Two ratings for 4 s each for detail and task engagement then appeared, followed by an odd/even jittered baseline task for 4, 6, or 8 s. (Bottom) Postscan procedure. After completing the two segments in the scanner, participants again viewed the cues they had seen for the main tasks outside the scanner, verbally stated what they had thought about for each one, and completed additional ratings to ensure task compliance. Individual difference questionnaires for creativity, personality, and memory were also collected (which are not included in this report). Participant responses to the cues were audio-recorded. After the scanning session and postscan, the responses were transcribed and scored by raters blind to hypothesis and induction for different details.

Results

Main Task Results.

Imagining future events.

Behavioral variables collected in the scanner (reaction time and subjective ratings for detail and engagement; Table S1) did not vary as a function of induction (F1,31 ≤ 1.82; P values ≥ 0.19). Critically, generative responses (Table S2) collected in the postscan interview indicated that participants generated significantly more total details for imagined events—but not object comparisons—that followed the specificity induction compared with the control (F1,31 = 8.87; P = 0.006; ηp2 = 0.22); critically, this increase in total details for imagined events was driven by a selective and significant boost in the production of episodic details—but not semantic details—following the specificity induction relative to the control (F1,31 = 24.12; P < 0.001; ηp2 = 0.44). No induction-related differences were exhibited for any type of detail generated on object comparisons (F1,31 ≤ 1.05; P values ≥ 0.31). Results of the postscan ratings appear in Table S3.

Table S1.

fMRI main task reaction times and ratings

Rating Control induction Specificity induction
Imagine construction, s 5.74 (3.04) 5.63 (2.81)
Object construction, s 7.75 (2.57) 7.48 (2.32)
Imagine detail rating 3.70 (0.61) 3.75 (0.52)
Object detail rating 3.49 (0.65) 3.53 (0.63)
Imagine on-task (out of 18) 17.50 (1.32) 17.41 (1.50)
Object on-task (out of 18) 17.66 (0.60) 17.72 (0.58)

Values are presented as mean (SD). Detail ratings range from 1 (least) to 5 (most). No significant differences emerged in output variables as a function of induction. As in previous work (52), objects took longer to construct than imagined events. They were also rated as less detailed.

Table S2.

Postscan details generated

Detail Control induction Specificity induction
Imagine total details 13.63 (7.12) 14.64 (7.49)
Object total details 12.91 (7.16) 12.17 (5.31)
Imagine episodic details 11.62 (5.58) 13.06 (5.98)
Imagine semantic details 2.00 (2.14) 1.58 (1.96)
Object main details 12.10 (7.01) 11.49 (5.25)
Object extraneous details 0.81 (0.96) 0.68 (0.96)

Values are presented as mean (SD). Consistent with previous behavioral work (8), imagined events following the specificity induction contained significantly more total details than those generated following the control. This increase was driven by a selective and significant increase in episodic details, but not semantic details. There was no difference in any sort of detail on object comparisons as a function of induction. There were also no differences in details generated across each task irrespective of induction. It should be noted that total time and total word count for task responses generated during the postscan likewise did not vary as a function of induction.

Table S3.

Postscan ratings

Rating Control induction Specificity induction
Imagine difficulty 2.02 (0.52) 1.92 (0.48)
Object difficulty 2.52 (0.62) 2.53 (0.53)
Imagine similarity 2.76 (0.70) 2.88 (0.74)
Imagine plausibility 3.45 (0.73) 3.54 (0.69)
Object familiarity 3.92 (0.47) 3.93 (0.53)

Values are presented as mean (SD). Scores on the Likert scale range from 1 (least) to 5 (most). No significant differences emerged in output variables as a function of induction. Object trials were rated as more difficult than imagine trials. Overall, imagined events were plausible and not very similar to past experiences, and object comparisons involved familiar enough objects.

Following both inductions, participants exhibited significant and broad core network activation for imagining events over object comparisons during the construction phase and elaboration phase (P < 0.001, uncorrected and k ≥ 65 voxels, yielding a corrected threshold of P < 0.05; Fig. 1 and Table S4). These findings replicate previous work (13, 19, 21) and indicate that participants were completing the main tasks in the scanner as expected.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Main task results for (A) constructing and (B) elaborating on imagined events (relative to the semantic object task) following the control induction and following the specificity induction at the threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 65 voxels, yielding a corrected threshold of P < 0.05). This pattern of findings closely parallels that of the core network, which overlaps with the default network (1317).

Table S4.

Regions with peak activation in main task analyses during imagine construction > object construction and imagine elaboration > object elaboration for each induction

Brain region x y z Z-score
Imagine construction > object construction for control induction
 Left medial prefrontal cortex/anterior cingulate cortex −4 54 −2 6.84
 Left parahippocampal gyrus −24 −34 −14 6.37
 Left posterior cingulate cortex −4 −56 16 5.89
 Right middle temporal gyrus 54 −4 −16 5.67
 Right parahippocampal gyrus 26 −32 −14 5.31
 Left middle temporal gyrus −64 −6 −14 5.06
 Right superior temporal gyrus 52 −58 18 4.44
Imagine construction > object construction for specificity induction
 Left medial prefrontal cortex −2 50 −6 7.52
 Right superior temporal gyrus 52 −60 22 6.69
 Left inferior temporal gyrus −60 −6 −18 5.88
 Left angular gyrus −46 −74 32 5.18
 Left temporal pole −48 18 −30 5.03
 Right precentral gyrus 40 −12 52 4.63
 Left cerebellum −6 −52 −42 4.47
 Right middle frontal gyrus 48 36 −2 4.34
 Left cerebellum −18 −84 −40 4.32
 Left superior frontal gyrus −20 34 46 3.96
Imagine elaboration > object elaboration for control induction
 Left medial prefrontal cortex −6 20 −16 6.26
 Left posterior cingulate cortex −2 −54 18 5.04
 Right middle occipital gyrus 46 −64 28 4.76
 Right middle temporal gyrus 58 −4 −18 4.60
 Right middle temporal pole 44 24 −34 4.51
 Left parahippocampal gyrus −24 −36 −14 4.46
 Left angular gyrus −42 −72 30 4.42
 Left middle temporal gyrus −60 −8 −14 4.10
 Left inferior orbitofrontal cortex −38 28 −16 3.71
Imagine elaboration > object elaboration for specificity induction
 Left medial prefrontal cortex −10 46 −16 6.33
 Left middle temporal gyrus −66 −14 −16 5.93
 Right middle temporal gyrus 62 −48 6 4.48
 Left cerebellum −28 −78 −34 4.38
 Right paracentral lobule 4 −34 74 4.19
 Left angular gyrus −48 −74 34 4.09

For each cluster of activation, the MNI coordinates of the maximally activated (i.e., peak) voxel are reported at the threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 65 voxels, yielding in a corrected threshold of P < 0.05).

Critically, participants exhibited significantly greater activation in several core network regions following the specificity induction compared with the control for constructing imagined events over object comparisons (P < 0.001, uncorrected and k ≥ 65 voxels, yielding a corrected threshold of P < 0.05; Fig. 2 and Table S5). These included several core network regions (13): the left anterior hippocampus, right inferior parietal lobule, right posterior cingulate cortex, and right ventral precuneus. To further characterize the results, descriptive plots for percent signal change in these regions and the others that emerged are presented in Fig. 2. Note that error bars are not plotted as a result of potential noise, and significance tests were not run on these data (22, 23).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

MTL subsystem regions (and other regions within and outside of the core network) with stronger recruitment for constructing imagined events (relative to the semantic object task) following the specificity induction compared with the control at the threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 65 voxels, yielding a corrected threshold of P < 0.05). The y axis of each chart depicts percent signal change (extracted from the region’s peak voxel); the red bars depict imagine construction and the blue bars depict object construction. L, left; R, right.

Table S5.

Regions with peak activation in main task analyses for specificity induction > control induction during the construction phase of imagined events > object comparisons

Brain region x y z Z-score
Right inferior parietal lobule 38 −32 36 4.82
Right thalamus 12 −6 10 4.17
Left anterior hippocampus −34 −16 −12 3.93
Right precentral gyrus 38 −10 46 3.91
Right posterior cingulate cortex 6 −34 24 3.76
Right supplementary motor area 2 −16 72 3.67
Right ventral precuneus 14 −58 46 3.65
Left caudate −20 14 16 3.62

For each cluster of activation, the MNI coordinates of the maximally activated (i.e., peak) voxel are reported at the threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 65 voxels, yielding in a corrected threshold of P < 0.05).

To further link the key induction-related behavioral and brain results, the detail scores obtained from the postscan interview were entered as a modulator of interest during the construction phase of imagined events and object comparisons in the scanner. For the detail index, episodic details on the imagine task and on-topic, factual information on the objects task were used. Critically, induction-related differences in detail were significantly related to induction-related differences in neural activity during the construction of imagined events over object comparisons following the specificity induction compared with the control; these parametric modulation effects were evident in the left anterior hippocampus, right inferior parietal lobule, and right ventral precuneus, as well as the right anterior hippocampus and other regions (P < 0.005, uncorrected and k ≥ 10 voxels; further details regarding thresholding are provided in Materials and Methods). This analysis (Fig. S2 and Table S6) indicates that the key induction-related behavioral effect (i.e., greater episodic details for imagined events following specificity vs. control) modulated the key induction-related neural effect (i.e., greater activation in the left anterior hippocampus and right inferior parietal lobule for imagined events following the specificity vs. control).

Fig. S2.

Fig. S2.

MTL subsystem regions (and the precuneus) with stronger recruitment for constructing imagined events (relative to the semantic object task) with higher detail following the specificity induction compared with the control at the threshold of P < 0.005, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 10 voxels).

Table S6.

Regions with peak activation in parametric modulation analysis (of detail) for specificity induction > control induction during the construction phase

Brain region x y z Z-score
Left middle temporal gyrus −44 −64 6 3.83
Right fusiform gyrus 42 −50 −16 3.78
Right precentral gyrus 32 −20 40 3.53
Right ventral precuneus 4 −56 46 3.49
Right cerebellum 22 −34 −24 3.43
Right cuneus 26 −62 20 3.33
Left cerebellum −6 −50 −32 3.25
Left insula −40 −10 12 3.23
Left caudate −10 2 20 3.19
Left superior temporal pole −38 18 −24 3.05
Right superior temporal gyrus 60 −34 14 3.03
Right anterior hippocampus 30 −20 −16 3.00
Right caudate 14 2 12 2.88
Right inferior parietal lobule (posterior) 36 −76 44 2.86
Left anterior hippocampus −36 −26 −14 2.83

For each cluster of activation, the MNI coordinates of the maximally activated (i.e., peak) voxel are reported at the threshold of P < 0.005, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 10 voxels).

The induction-related results were selective to the construction phase; no significant activations in any direction were evident for elaboration. There were also no significant activations for the opposite contrasts of task and induction.

Resting-state analyses.

To more fully characterize the influence of the induction manipulation, we examined its effects on subsequent resting-state connectivity of key core network regions that emerged from the main task analyses: the left anterior hippocampus (xyz, −34, −16, −12) and right inferior parietal lobule (xyz, 38, −32, 36). Following the specificity induction relative to the control (Fig. 3 and Table S7), the left anterior hippocampal seed served to significantly boost connectivity with the right parahippocampal gyrus, and the right inferior parietal lobule seed served to significantly boost connectivity with the left parahippocampal gyrus, left superior medial frontal gyrus, and left anterior cingulate cortex (P < 0.001, uncorrected and k ≥ 38 voxels, yielding a corrected threshold of P < 0.05). These results suggest short-term, functional reorganization in the core network as a function of induction. No activations survived the opposite induction contrast.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Resting-state functional connectivity results following the specificity compared with the control induction for (A) a left anterior hippocampal seed region (extracted from a peak voxel xyz of −34, −16, −12) and (B) a right inferior parietal lobule seed region (extracted from a peak voxel xyz of 38, −32, 36) and the rest of the brain at a threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 38 voxels, yielding a corrected threshold of P < 0.05).

Table S7.

Regions with peak activation in resting-state functional connectivity analyses for specificity induction > control induction

Brain region x y z t-score
Specificity induction > control induction for left anterior hippocampal seed
 Right parahippocampal gyrus 20 −4 −26 3.84
 Right midbrain 8 −26 −10 3.79
 Right cerebellum 16 −48 −60 3.77
 Left intraparietal sulcus −28 −52 24 3.74
Specificity induction > control induction for right inferior parietal lobule seed
 Left parahippocampal gyrus −32 −2 −26 4.43
 Left anterior cingulate cortex −20 −32 20 4.19
 Left inferior temporal gyrus −62 −42 −22 4.16
 Left thalamus −2 −4 4 4.08
 Left postcentral gyrus −48 −18 24 3.93
 Left anterior cingulate cortex −12 38 22 3.92
 Left postcentral gyrus −52 −16 34 3.88
 Left superior frontal gyrus −8 62 42 3.84
 Left superior medial frontal gyrus −6 32 56 3.82

For each cluster of activation, the MNI coordinates of the maximally activated (i.e., peak) voxel are reported at the threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected (with an extent threshold of 38 voxels, yielding in a corrected threshold of P < 0.05).

Discussion

In the present fMRI study, we established the neural signature of an episodic specificity induction for imagining future events. Previous research has suggested that a core network of brain regions comes online when individuals remember past and imagine future events (13), and that this network can be segmented into a MTL subsystem linked to the construction of events or scenes and a dorsomedial prefrontal subsystem linked to the social and self-referential components of these events or scenes (15). We found that participants did indeed activate the core network when generating imagined future events over semantic object comparisons after receiving the specificity and control inductions. Critically, and as hypothesized, receiving the specificity induction compared with either control also led to significantly increased activity in key regions of the MTL subsystem of the core network, including the hippocampus and inferior parietal lobule, when generating future events relative to object comparisons. Postscan responses suggested that the specificity induction was operating as expected in the scanner: significantly more episodic but not semantic details were generated for imagined events following the specificity induction compared with the control, with no differences in any type of detail for object comparisons. This pattern of behavioral results replicates and extends previous work (8), and confirms that differential neural patterns of activity were linked to the experimental manipulation participants received. Further support for a behavior–brain link was established via a parametric modulation analysis, which indicated that induction-related differences in episodic detail in imagined events from the postscan interview significantly modulated induction-related neural activity in the left anterior hippocampus and right inferior parietal lobule (and other regions) during imagine trials in the scanner. This latter finding should be taken as preliminary, however, as it did not emerge with more conservative statistical thresholds (corrected for multiple comparisons; Materials and Methods).

The finding that neural induction effects were limited to the construction phase of imagining future events and did not extend to elaboration is also in line with our hypotheses. We have previously suggested (6) that the specificity induction leads individuals to focus on episodic details related to places, people, and actions of an event or scene and thus targets the process of retrieval orientation—a goal-directed strategy for retrieving an episode in a more or less specific way when presented with a cue (24). The neural induction effects we observed in the MTL subsystem during construction—but not elaboration—suggest that the specificity induction may help participants to adopt a specific retrieval orientation that is used on later tasks that also require participants to construct a mental event or scene that contains details like those emphasized during the induction. This account of the data can also be situated under the theoretical framework of an event model, which is composed, in part, of elements of episodic memory that are bounded in space and sequential time involving physical and figural entities (25). The induction, by facilitating a specific retrieval mode, may help individuals to internally trigger the construction or assembly of a mental event model that is filled with specific details. This notion of construction in an event model also fits with the recent idea that the hippocampus supports the encoding and retrieval of temporal sequences that constitute an event (2628).

In support of this view, we found increased activity in the left anterior hippocampus during the construction phase of imagination following the specificity induction relative to the control. This finding converges with evidence suggesting that the anterior hippocampus supports the relational processing of elements of an encoded memory at retrieval (29, 30), as well as the flexible recombination of previously learned elements into a novel representation (31). Evidence has also indicated that the anterior hippocampus tracks the content (vs. the temporal ordering) of imagined events (32) and the specificity (vs. abstractness) of imagined events (18, 19) and autobiographical plans (33). The constructive episodic simulation hypothesis (3) posits that imagining future events involves extracting and recombining elements of previous memories into a novel scenario, and that these cognitive processes are in part dependent on the hippocampus. Under this framework, the induction may lead to increased anterior hippocampal activity when participants imagine future events by ramping up processes involved in the extraction and relational recombination of elements of previous memories into a novel scenario.

Nonetheless, we are cautious in interpreting too heavily the precise location of increased induction-related activity within the hippocampus. Several factors can influence the location of hippocampal activity (reviewed in ref. 34), and work from the spatial cognition domain on the anterior–posterior hippocampal axis suggests that the anterior hippocampus supports coarse-grained (vs. fine-grained) representations, at least those that are spatial (refs. 35, 36; reviewed in ref. 37). The anterior hippocampus has also been associated with the encoding of novel simulations into memory (38). However, if the induction simply helped participants to encode novel representations into memory, we would have expected to observe increases in details generated in the postscan for cues involving imagined events and object definitions following the specificity induction, but we found effects only for imagined events. Future work should continue to identify subregions of the hippocampus that map onto subcomponents of imagining events or scenes by using high-resolution fMRI (discussed in ref. 39), as well as the role of lateralization in the hippocampus and other brain regions during simulation (40).

We also found increased induction-related neural activity in the right inferior parietal lobule when participants constructed imagined future events. Like the anterior hippocampus, the inferior parietal lobule has been implicated in studies in which participants imagine events in more specific (vs. more general) detail (19), particularly during the construction phase (21). The inferior parietal lobule is also part of the MTL subsystem that is thought to track with episodic memory, event imagination, and scene content (15), and activity in this region has been associated with the successful retrieval and integration of perceptual details from memory (20). In a related topic, we found evidence that activity in the right ventral precuneus increased following the specificity induction relative to the control for imagined events. The inferior parietal lobule and ventral precuneus have recently been linked to mental orientation in space, time, and person (41). We have also previously found that the right ventral precuneus exhibits increased activity during repeated future simulations (e.g., ref. 42), but it is unclear whether these changes are specifically related to changes in event detail.

In addition, we found preliminary evidence suggesting that the specificity induction may impact the processing of contextual scene details and more self-relevant, social details into a novel simulation. Following the specificity induction compared with the control, resting-state functional connectivity analyses showed stronger coupling between the left anterior hippocampal seed and a key region linked with scene processing (i.e., right parahippocampal gyrus; refs. 4244) and between the right inferior parietal lobule seed and scene (i.e., left parahippocampal gyrus) and social regions (i.e., left superior medial frontal gyrus; refs. 15, 42), as well as the left anterior cingulate cortex. The anterior cingulate cortex is part of the frontoparietal control network (45) that has been associated with emotional processing and executive functions including cognitive control. However, because resting-state analyses involve measuring neural activity in the absence of task demands, we are cautious about interpreting these induction-related findings too heavily. Because the induction affected neural functioning during task performance that immediately preceded the resting-state scans, it is unclear whether the short-term reorganization of functional networks in the absence of task demands is a result of (i) the induction manipulation or (ii) the specific neural processing that emerged as a result of the induction during the main tasks. Although the specific processing that emerged as a result of the induction could plausibly have the same effect as the induction itself, future work should investigate this issue more systematically by having participants perform the resting-state scans immediately after receiving an induction.

Another caveat of the present study is that we did not obtain differences in detail ratings in the scanner as a function of induction. Participants could have plausibly rated their simulations as more detailed in the scanner following the specificity induction compared with the control. Nonetheless, previous work has suggested that subjective rating scales may not be the most sensitive measure of episodic detail. Studies have found that subjective ratings of detail and vividness for episodic simulations are higher in older adults than in young adults or are similarly rated (4649), yet objective scoring measures routinely show that older adults produce fewer episodic details in their narratives compared with young adults (46, 47). Despite the complexities associated with measuring event detail, this outcome allows us to interpret the data patterns knowing that the imagined events after both inductions were, at least subjectively, matched on features that can contribute to the behavioral and neural expressions of simulation.

Taken together, the results suggest that the cognitive processes that are isolated and enhanced via the episodic specificity induction behaviorally are linked to key neural regions in the core network implicated in remembering and imagining events, including the hippocampus. Future work should continue to investigate the contributions of episodic memory, from behavioral and neural perspectives, to cognitive tasks that could involve episodic elements of past experiences, such as imagination, problem-solving, and creativity. These findings may also help to guide interventions for individuals from populations characterized by overgeneralized memory and imagination, such as healthy aging (49) and clinical depression (5052), that have been shown to benefit from specificity inductions in previous behavioral research (7, 9, 53).

Materials and Methods

Participants.

Thirty-two young adults (mean age, 21.0 y; SD, 2.38; 20 female) participated in the study, recruited via advertisements at universities in Boston, MA. All participants were right-handed and fluent in English and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and no history of neurological or psychological impairment. They all gave written informed consent and were treated in a manner approved by Harvard University’s ethics committee. An additional seven participants were excluded for excessive movement or task noncompliance.

Induction Materials and Procedure.

An overview of the scanning and postscan design is provided in Fig. S1. To begin each of the two main segments in the scanner, participants received an induction after viewing a ∼2-min video of a man and woman performing kitchen activities and completing a 2-min number judgment filler task on a computer screen. Participants viewed the computer screen via a mirror attached to the scanner head coil and scanner-compatible headphones. All participants received the episodic specificity induction in one of the two segments; for the other segment, half of the participants were randomly assigned to receive the impressions control induction and half received the math control induction. Participants were randomly assigned to receive the specificity induction first or one of the two control inductions first, and induction order was counterbalanced across participants. Participants were in the scanner for this portion of the study but were not scanned, heard induction questions over a loud speaker, and responded out loud. Inductions with interviews were audio-recorded (ref. 7 includes full scripts); all inductions took an average of 5 min and did not differ significantly in length.

The episodic specificity induction consisted of a question set based on the cognitive interview (54), a forensic protocol that boosts accurate details associated with eyewitness events. Participants were told that they were the chief expert about the video, and then responded to three mental imagery probes to report everything about the video’s setting, people, and actions as specifically and in as much detail as possible. Open-ended follow-up questions were used to probe generated details. Information on the control inductions is provided in SI Materials and Methods.

fMRI Materials and Procedure.

In each of the two segments following the induction, participants completed four runs of functional neuroimaging: three task runs during which they viewed 36 total object cues of the main tasks in an event-related design and one resting-state run. Three practice trials of each main task were completed to ensure understanding.

Main Tasks.

Each main task run was 7 m, 34 s in duration and began and ended with 16 s of fixation. Within each run, six imagined event trials and six object comparison trials were randomly presented with the construction-elaboration paradigm for 20 s each (19, 21). Following each trial, two ratings appeared (4 s each), and then a rest period during which a basic odd/even number judgment task was performed (mean, 6 s; jittered at 4 s, 6 s, or 8 s). Participants made responses during the main tasks via an MR-compatible five-button response box in their right hand.

Eighteen total imagined event trials were included per segment. For each trial, the word “imagine” appeared on one line of the screen, followed by the instruction “near future event” on the next line, followed by the cue in capital letters on the third line. As in previous work (8, 19, 21), participants were instructed to silently generate a novel future event or scenario that could happen to them within the next few years in as much detail as possible that was somehow related to the cue, plausible, new, viewed from a field perspective, and specific to one time and place. By using the construction-elaboration paradigm, participants were instructed that they should press their thumb when they had constructed each imagined event, and, after pressing their thumb, should elaborate or fill in all of the details of the event until the trial was over. At the end of each trial, the screen changed and participants rated (i) how detailed the mental image of their imagination was (from 1 to 5, with 1 indicating no/few details and least vivid to 5 indicating many details and most vivid) and (ii) whether they stayed engaged on task (either 1 indicating yes or 2 indicating no). Eighteen total object comparison trials were also included per segment and matched with imagined events for task structure and response mode (SI Materials and Methods). Although both main tasks required generative search and retrieval (55), only the imagine task required generating episodic content.

Resting State.

In each of the two segments following the main task runs, participants completed a resting-state scan for 7 m, 13 s in which they viewed a black background with a white fixation cross.

Postscan Interview.

Immediately after scanning, participants completed a postscan interview (19, 21). Participants viewed each object cue from the scanner (in the same order to reduce cognitive load) and verbally reported whatever they had thought about (without adding new details). Each trial was completed in a self-paced manner without input or probing from the experimenter, and, following each trial, participants completed four ratings for imagined events and two for object comparisons (SI Materials and Methods provides information on additional ratings). Pilot testing before the study commenced (n = 2) showed that participants could describe what they had silently generated.

Participants’ actual verbal reports for imagined events and object comparisons were audio-recorded for later transcription and scoring with the autobiographical interview procedure (56). For imagined events, bits of information contained in these verbal reports were segmented. Each detail was classified as either episodic or semantic to the main event described. Episodic details included the who, what, where, and when elements of the central event specific in time and place; semantic details included factual information, off-topic and repetitive information, and commentary. For object comparisons, bits of information were also segmented and scored into detail subcategories (as in ref. 8). The main measure of interest was elements of the central object definitions that were on-task and meaningful; extraneous details included elements of the reports that were off-topic, repetitive, not meaningful, or commentary. Two independent raters blind to all experimental hypotheses and the induction conditions scored the verbal reports after completing an interrater reliability assessment of 20 trials of imagined events and object comparisons from the pilot subjects not included in the main study. Reliability was high across scored measures (Cronbach’s standardized α ≥ 0.90). Additional information on scoring is provided in SI Materials and Methods.

fMRI Acquisition, Preprocessing, and Analysis Parameters.

Main task approach.

Scanning and preprocessing parameters for the main tasks are provided in SI Materials and Methods. Preprocessed data were statistically analyzed by using the general linear model (examples of this approach are provided in refs. 19, 21). Each participant’s blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response for (i) construction and (ii) elaboration were modeled separately for each imagined event trial and each object comparison trial by using SPM12’s canonical hemodynamic response function (hrf) with first-level fixed-effects models. One first-level model was created for the control induction runs and one for the specificity induction runs. The hrf for construction (i.e., regressors for imagine and object construction) was applied 2 s after cue onset, and the hrf for elaboration (i.e., regressors for imagine and object elaboration) was applied 2 s after the participant made a button press [mean elaboration (jittered) = 8.65 s across tasks). The entire 20-s duration of each trial was not modeled to reduce contamination effects. The BOLD response for the rating phase of each trial was also modeled at the rating onset (i.e., regressors for imagine and object rating), and subject-specific movement parameters for each run were added as covariates of no interest.

To examine whether participants displayed typical neural patterns of performance on the imagined event and object comparison tasks and to test for any induction-related effects, we computed contrasts for (i) imagine construction > object construction and (ii) imagine elaboration > object elaboration. At the second level, we entered the contrast images into random-effects one-sample t tests for each induction separately for (i) construction and (ii) elaboration to ensure that typical neural patterns of core network recruitment were observed after each induction and phase (13, 19, 21). Critically, at the second level, we also entered contrast images into random-effects paired t tests whereby each pair of scans included the respective control induction contrast image and specificity induction contrast image for each participant separately for (i) construction and (ii) elaboration. An interaction effect was also computed (SI Materials and Methods).

The significance threshold and minimum cluster size (P < 0.001, uncorrected and k ≥ 65 voxels), equivalent to P < 0.05 corrected for multiple comparisons, was determined via Analysis of Functional NeuroImages’ (AFNI) 3dClustSim program (in June 2015) by using a Monte Carlo simulation (10,000 iterations) within the 3D whole-brain search volume (179,380 2-mm3 voxels) to estimate the overall probability of false positives (as in refs. 39, 57). To minimize the possibility of false positives with cluster thresholding in functional neuroimaging analyses (58), we used a version of the 3dClustSim program that is free from technical problems uncovered in previous versions, and that incorporated the correct smoothing value (i.e., derived from the group residual mean-square images) with a conservative cluster-defining threshold (i.e., P < 0.001 vs. P < 0.01).

Next, we performed a parametric modulation analysis in SPM by including regressors in the first-level models outlined earlier for control and specificity runs separately (as in ref. 18). Although we used a cognitive experimental manipulation—a feature of the methodological design that should pinpoint in a systematic way the impact of the behavioral induction on neural performance—we took this additional step to relate behavioral and neural data. We entered, trial-by-trial, a detail score for each imagined event and object comparison obtained in the postscan interview as a covariate of interest for each respective imagine construction and object construction trial (i.e., regressors for imagine detail and object detail). We focused on the behavioral detail index and the construction phase fMRI data because results indicated induction-related effects on these key outputs. The detail score covariate was modeled linearly, represented the orthogonal contribution of detail in the absence of any other covariates, and was mean-centered according to SPM algorithms. We contrasted the modulatory effects of imagine detail covariate > object detail covariate during the construction phase at the first level. At the second level, we entered these first-level contrast images into a random-effects paired t test whereby each pair of scans included the respective control induction contrast image and specificity induction contrast image for each participant. This analysis allowed us to identify which regions during construction showed differential activity following the specificity induction compared with the control as modulated by an index of detail for imagined events over object comparisons.

A significance threshold of P < 0.005, uncorrected with an extent threshold of 10 contiguously activated voxels (2 mm3) was applied for whole-brain testing of the parametric modulation (the same or similar thresholds were used for parametric modulation analyses in refs. 18, 59, 60). Although the results of this particular analysis did not survive more stringent corrected thresholds, we included it as preliminary induction-related evidence of a behavior–brain link (a theoretical and quantitative justification of the threshold is provided in ref. 61).

Resting-state approach.

For the resting-state scans, we performed a series of preprocessing steps (including global signal regression) on the raw data followed by a series of functional connectivity-specific preprocessing steps (SI Materials and Methods). For the analyses (based on refs. 62, 63), seed regions in the hippocampus and inferior parietal lobule (i.e., a 6-mm sphere centered on the region’s peak voxel) were selected on the basis of results from the main task analyses and in line with a priori hypotheses. To create whole-brain correlation images, the averaged time series across all voxels comprising a seed region of interest (ROI) was used as the variable of interest with the time series corresponding to each voxel across the brain via Pearson’s product moment correlation. Comparisons of connectivity strength with seed regions across specificity and control inductions were made by using a pairwise t test in AFNI. All statistical analyses of correlation data were performed on Fisher z-transformations (64), which are approximately normally distributed. Results involve those voxels that survived a statistical threshold of P < 0.001, uncorrected with an extent threshold of 38 contiguously activated voxels applied for whole-brain testing (search volume of 266,816 2-mm3 voxels) using 3dClustSim and equivalent to a significance threshold of P < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons. Note that the cluster extent required to achieve a corrected α of 0.05 with a voxelwise threshold of P < 0.001 here was smaller than the extent required in the main task analysis as a result of differences in EPI acquisition and the smoothness of the data.

Visualization and localization steps for the main tasks and resting-state analyses are provided in SI Materials and Methods. All data and materials are available upon request.

SI Materials and Methods

Induction Phase and Main Tasks.

Control inductions.

To parallel the structure of the specificity induction, the impressions control induction was a question set that required reflecting back on and speaking about the viewed video, but not retrieving specific episodic details. Participants were asked about their global impressions of the video, and then stated their general opinions about other aspects of the video’s setting, people, and actions as elicited via a question bank. The other control induction, the math control, was completing basic math problems. Because the impressions control focuses on global opinions and could bias individuals toward adopting a gist-like retrieval orientation on subsequent tasks, we used the math control as a neutral baseline. Although previous work has revealed a boost in episodic detail after the specificity induction irrespective of which control induction is the comparison (7, 8, 11), we included both to ensure the generality of any observed effects. No mental imagery probes were used in either control induction.

Cues.

The object cues (as listed later) were high in concreteness (mean = 6.88, SD = 0.16), “imageability” (mean = 5.84, SD = 0.34), and Thorndike–Lorge frequency (mean = 1.65, SD = 0.27) as normed in ref. 65. They were drawn from previous work on imagined events (19, 21). The cues were divided into 4 lists of 18 cues each, and did not differ in respect to these characteristics; the order of lists was fully counterbalanced across induction sequence and control induction used, and participants were randomly assigned to one of the orders (as in refs. 19, 21). None of the cues were related to the content of the induction videos.

List of cues.

The cues (from ref. 65) used in the main tasks for imagined events and object comparisons were as follows: apple, beaver, bird, book, bottle, bowl, butter, butterfly, candy, car, claw, clock, coffee, coin, corn, cotton, diamond, doll, dollar, dress, elephant, engine, fireplace, flag, fork, fox, frog, fur, hammer, horse, instrument, iron, jelly, lemon, letter, lime, lobster, meat, microscope, nail, newspaper, palace, peach, pencil, pepper, photograph, piano, pipe, pole, potato, rattle, rock, salad, ship, shoe, slipper, snake, stain, star, strawberry, string, sugar, tablespoon, ticket, toast, tool, tower, toy, tree, truck, umbrella, and wine.

Object comparison task.

Eighteen total trials were included per segment (as with imagined events). For each trial, the word “objects” appeared followed by the instruction “size and define” followed by the cue in capital letters. As in previous work (8, 19, 21, 52), participants were instructed to silently generate two objects that were somehow factually related to each object cue and put them in a sentence focused on the physical sizes of the objects from largest to smallest (i.e., “X is larger than Y is larger than Z”). Participants were told that the object cue itself could be first, second, or last in the size sentence. By using the construction-elaboration paradigm as in the imagined event task, participants were told to press their thumb when they had constructed the size sentence, and to then elaborate on a semantic definition of each object until the trial was over. Participants were told to generate everything they could for the object definitions in as much detail as possible, including typical functions, attributes, and characteristics of the objects. Participants were instructed to not think about the objects in relation to themselves or their own lives, but to focus on generating objects and definitions as if they came from a dictionary or Wikipedia page. Following each trial, participants rated (i) how detailed they thought their definitions were (from 1 indicating no/few details and least vivid to 5 indicating many details and most vivid) and (ii) whether they were engaged on task (1 indicating yes or 2 indicating no). This task was used as the comparison for imagined events because it requires searching for, integrating, and generating information related to a given cue, but does not involve elements of episodic experience for completion (further details are provided in refs. 19, 21).

Postscan ratings.

Participants completed additional phenomenological ratings for imagined events and object comparisons following their verbal generation of content for each cue. For imagined events, participants rated (i) how difficult it was to imagine the event in the scanner, (ii) how similar the imagined event was to an experience from their own life, (iii) how plausible the event was to occur (all from 1 indicating least to 5 indicating most), and (iv) whether the event was imagined within the next few years (yes or no). For object comparisons, participants rated (i) how difficult it was to complete the task in the scanner and (ii) how familiar the objects were to them (both from 1 indicating least to 5 indicating most). These ratings were included to ensure that any performance differences across the inductions would not be the result of these particular features of generated content. As predicted, ratings on the two tasks did not differ as a function of induction.

Postscan scoring.

Because the critical detail indices for imagined events and object comparisons were not necessarily equated, and to ensure that any related effects of the specificity induction do not depend on the scoring criteria used, we computed a total detail score and adopted this score as our main dependent variable (8). We also examined the details split separately as in our previous work on the induction (7, 8, 11) and in the standard autobiographical interview (56) to confirm the induction operated as expected.

fMRI main task acquisition.

Neuroimaging data were collected via a 3-T Siemens Magnetom Tim Trio MRI scanner with a 32-channel head coil. Anatomical images were collected via a T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient multiecho sequence [176 sagittal slices; repetition time (TR) = 2,530 ms; echo times (TEs) = 1.64, 3.50, 5.36, and 7.22 ms; flip angle = 7°; 1-mm3 voxels; field of view (FoV) = 256 mm]. All BOLD data were collected via a T2*-weighted multiband EPI pulse sequence that used multiband radiofrequency (RF) pulses and simultaneous multislice acquisition (66, 67). For the six (three per segment) BOLD main task runs, the EPI parameters were as follows: 69 interleaved axial-oblique slices, TR = 2,000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 80°, 2-mm3 nominal voxels, 6/8 partial Fourier, FoV = 216 mm, SMS = 3. For the two (one per segment) BOLD resting-state runs, the EPI parameters were as follows: 72 interleaved axial-oblique slices, TR = 755 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 58°, 2-mm3 nominal voxels, 6/8 partial Fourier, FoV = 216 mm, SMS = 8. The SMS/EPI acquisitions used a modified version of the Siemens WIP 770A.

fMRI main task preprocessing.

Imaging data were preprocessed by using SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, United Kingdom) to account for noise and artifacts from scanning. The first four functional images were discarded to account for T1-saturation effects. Slice-timing correction, realignment, spatial normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template (resampled at 2-mm3 voxels), and spatial smoothing [using an 8-mm full-width half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel] were also completed.

fMRI main task analyses.

On average, 15 imagined event and 17 object comparison trials per induction were analyzed (out of 18). Type of control induction and order of induction manipulation did not affect behavioral or neural outcomes.

fMRI main task interaction effect.

For the main task analyses, we also computed an interaction effect of imagine construction + object elaboration > object construction + imagine elaboration at the first level for each participant for control runs and for specificity runs separately. At the second level, we entered these contrast images into a random-effects paired t test with pairs corresponding to the specificity induction image and control induction image for each participant. The same statistical thresholding (P < 0.001, k ≥ 65 voxels) was used as in the main task analyses. A significant pattern of activation was observed in the right inferior parietal lobule (xyz, 36, −30, 36) for this contrast, indicating that following the specificity induction compared with the control participants exhibited greater activity in this region for imagined events over object comparisons during the construction phase selectively. No significant activity emerged for the opposite task or induction contrast.

fMRI main task visualization and localization.

Peak coordinates of active regions are reported in MNI space and were localized with xjView 8 (www.alivelearn.net/xjview). The same parameters were used for the parametric modulation and resting-state analyses. Percent signal change was extracted from activations of interest for the imagined event and object comparison conditions for the induction-related, main-task contrasts by using the MarsBaR SPM toolbox (68).

Resting State Acquisition and Preprocessing.

The acquisition parameters are listed earlier. For preprocessing, a series of steps were first performed on the raw data. To ensure stabilization of the BOLD signal, the first four time points of each functional run were removed. A rigid-body correction within and across runs accounted for head motion (FSL 4.1.7; FMRIB). In addition, a nonlinear registration of the functional data to a T2*-weighted MNI template (SPM2; Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, United Kingdom) yielded images resampled at 2-mm3 voxels. Next, a series of functional connectivity-specific preprocessing steps were carried out (based on refs. 62, 63). Specifically, data within each session were first concatenated and spatially smoothed by using a 6-mm FWHM kernel. These images were then temporally filtered (low-pass) to retain frequencies below 0.08 Hz. A series of nuisance regressors reflecting spurious noise or systematic variance associated with nonneural sources were created and removed by using partial regression. These nuisance regressors included the six parameters computed from the rigid-body motion correction and their derivatives, the averaged signal within the lateral ventricles, an ROI within the deep white matter, and an ROI comprising the whole brain (i.e., global signal regression). The first temporal derivative of each regressor was also included to account for temporal shifts in the BOLD signal. It should be noted that the merits of using global signal regression have been debated (an alternative is provided in ref. 69, and an example of justification for the adopted technique is provided in ref. 70).

Acknowledgments

We thank Haley Dodds and Samantha F. Schoenberger for assistance with behavioral transcriptions and scoring, Stephanie McMains and Harvard University’s Center for Brain Science staff for assistance with neuroimaging design and analyses, Himanshu Bhat and Thomas Benner of Siemens Healthcare for the simultaneous multislice (SMS)/echoplanar imaging (EPI) sequence, and Steven Cauley of Massachusetts General Hospital for modifications that enabled implementation of our protocols in a routine session. This research was funded by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH060941 (to D.L.S.), the Sackler Scholar Programme in Psychobiology (K.P.M.), and Rutherford Discovery Fellowship RDF-10-UOA-024 (to D.R.A.).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1612278113/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Schacter DL, et al. The future of memory: Remembering, imagining, and the brain. Neuron. 2012;76(4):677–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mullally SL, Maguire EA. Memory, imagination, and predicting the future: A common brain mechanism? Neuroscientist. 2014;20(3):220–234. doi: 10.1177/1073858413495091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schacter DL, Addis DR. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007;362(1481):773–786. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Tulving E. Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gaesser B, Sacchetti DC, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Characterizing age-related changes in remembering the past and imagining the future. Psychol Aging. 2011;26(1):80–84. doi: 10.1037/a0021054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schacter DL, Madore KP. Remembering the past and imagining the future: Identifying and enhancing the contribution of episodic memory. Mem Stud. 2016;9:245–255. doi: 10.1177/1750698016645230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Madore KP, Gaesser B, Schacter DL. Constructive episodic simulation: Dissociable effects of a specificity induction on remembering, imagining, and describing in young and older adults. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2014;40(3):609–622. doi: 10.1037/a0034885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Madore KP, Schacter DL. Remembering the past and imagining the future: Selective effects of an episodic specificity induction on detail generation. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2016;69(2):285–298. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.999097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Madore KP, Schacter DL. An episodic specificity induction enhances means-end problem solving in young and older adults. Psychol Aging. 2014;29(4):913–924. doi: 10.1037/a0038209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jing HG, Madore KP, Schacter DL. Worrying about the future: An episodic specificity induction impacts problem solving, reappraisal, and well-being. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2016;145(4):402–418. doi: 10.1037/xge0000142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Madore KP, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Creativity and memory: Effects of an episodic specificity induction on divergent thinking. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(9):1461–1468. doi: 10.1177/0956797615591863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL. Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(9):657–661. doi: 10.1038/nrn2213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Benoit RG, Schacter DL. Specifying the core network supporting episodic simulation and episodic memory by activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia. 2015;75:450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65(4):550–562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Andrews-Hanna JR, Saxe R, Yarkoni T. Contributions of episodic retrieval and mentalizing to autobiographical thought: Evidence from functional neuroimaging, resting-state connectivity, and fMRI meta-analyses. Neuroimage. 2014;91:324–335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Yeo BT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Addis DR, Schacter DL. Constructive episodic simulation: Temporal distance and detail of past and future events modulate hippocampal engagement. Hippocampus. 2008;18(2):227–237. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Addis DR, Cheng T, Roberts RP, Schacter DL. Hippocampal contributions to the episodic simulation of specific and general future events. Hippocampus. 2011;21(10):1045–1052. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Guerin SA, Robbins CA, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Interactions between visual attention and episodic retrieval: Dissociable contributions of parietal regions during gist-based false recognition. Neuron. 2012;75(6):1122–1134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL. Remembering the past and imagining the future: Common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(7):1363–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kriegeskorte N, Simmons WK, Bellgowan PSF, Baker CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: The dangers of double dipping. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(5):535–540. doi: 10.1038/nn.2303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Vul E, Harris C, Winkielman P, Pashler H. Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2009;4(3):274–290. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Morcom AM, Rugg MD. Retrieval orientation and the control of recollection: An fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24(12):2372–2384. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Radvansky GA, Zacks JM. Event Cognition. Oxford Univ Press; New York: 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Eichenbaum H. Memory on time. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(2):81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Davachi L, DuBrow S. How the hippocampus preserves order: The role of prediction and context. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(2):92–99. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ranganath C, Hsieh LT. The hippocampus: A special place for time. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2016;1369(1):93–110. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Giovanello KS, Schnyer D, Verfaellie M. Distinct hippocampal regions make unique contributions to relational memory. Hippocampus. 2009;19(2):111–117. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nielson DM, Smith TA, Sreekumar V, Dennis S, Sederberg PB. Human hippocampus represents space and time during retrieval of real-world memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(35):11078–11083. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507104112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Preston AR, Shrager Y, Dudukovic NM, Gabrieli JDE. Hippocampal contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory. Hippocampus. 2004;14(2):148–152. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.D’Argembeau A, Jeunehomme O, Majerus S, Bastin C, Salmon E. The neural basis of temporal order processing in past and future thought. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015;27(1):185–197. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Spreng RN, Gerlach KD, Turner GR, Schacter DL. Autobiographical planning and the brain: Activation and its modulation by qualitative features. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015;27(11):2147–2157. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Viard A, Desgranges B, Eustache F, Piolino P. Factors affecting medial temporal lobe engagement for past and future episodic events: An ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Cogn. 2012;80(1):111–125. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zeidman P, Lutti A, Maguire EA. Investigating the functions of subregions within anterior hippocampus. Cortex. 2015;73:240–256. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Zeidman P, Mullally SL, Maguire EA. Constructing, perceiving, and maintaining scenes: Hippocampal activity and connectivity. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(10):3836–3855. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Poppenk J, Evensmoen HR, Moscovitch M, Nadel L. Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(5):230–240. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Martin VC, Schacter DL, Corballis MC, Addis DR. A role for the hippocampus in encoding simulations of future events. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(33):13858–13863. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105816108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Addis DR, Schacter DL. The hippocampus and imagining the future: Where do we stand? Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;5:173. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Arzy S, Molnar-Szakacs I, Blanke O. Self in time: Imagined self-location influences neural activity related to mental time travel. J Neurosci. 2008;28(25):6502–6507. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5712-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Peer M, Salomon R, Goldberg I, Blanke O, Arzy S. Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(35):11072–11077. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504242112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Szpunar KK, St Jacques PL, Robbins CA, Wig GS, Schacter DL. Repetition-related reductions in neural activity reveal component processes of mental simulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(5):712–722. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Maguire EA. Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J Neurosci. 2007;27(52):14365–14374. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4549-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Benoit RG, Szpunar KK, Schacter DL. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex supports affective future simulation by integrating distributed knowledge. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111(46):16550–16555. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1419274111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100(6):3328–3342. doi: 10.1152/jn.90355.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Cole SN, Morrison CM, Conway MA. Episodic future thinking: Linking neuropsychological performance with episodic detail in young and old adults. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 2013;66(9):1687–1706. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.758157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Addis DR, Musicaro R, Pan L, Schacter DL. Episodic simulation of past and future events in older adults: Evidence from an experimental recombination task. Psychol Aging. 2010;25(2):369–376. doi: 10.1037/a0017280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Addis DR, Roberts RP, Schacter DL. Age-related neural changes in autobiographical remembering and imagining. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(13):3656–3669. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL. Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychol Sci. 2008;19(1):33–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02043.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Williams JMG, et al. The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Mem Cognit. 1996;24(1):116–125. doi: 10.3758/bf03197278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.MacLeod AK, Conway C. Well-being and positive future thinking for the self versus others. Cogn Emotion. 2007;21:1114–1124. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hach S, Tippett LJ, Addis DR. Neural changes associated with the generation of specific past and future events in depression. Neuropsychologia. 2014;65:41–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Neshat-Doost HT, et al. Enhancing autobiographical memory specificity through cognitive training: An intervention for depression translated from basic science. Clin Psychol Sci. 2013;1:84–92. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Fisher RP, Geiselman RE. Memory-Enhancing Techniques for Investigative Interviewing: The Cognitive Interview. Charles C. Thomas Books; Springfield, IL: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Conway MA, Pleydell-Pearce CW. The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychol Rev. 2000;107(2):261–288. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychol Aging. 2002;17(4):677–689. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gaesser B, Spreng RN, McLelland VC, Addis DR, Schacter DL. Imagining the future: Evidence for a hippocampal contribution to constructive processing. Hippocampus. 2013;23(12):1150–1161. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113(28):7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Holland AC, Addis DR, Kensinger EA. The neural correlates of specific versus general autobiographical memory construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(12):3164–3177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.De Brigard F, Addis DR, Ford JH, Schacter DL, Giovanello KS. Remembering what could have happened: Neural correlates of episodic counterfactual thinking. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(12):2401–2414. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lieberman MD, Cunningham WA. Type I and type II error concerns in fMRI research: Re-balancing the scale. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4(4):423–428. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med. 1995;34(4):537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Van Dijk KR, et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: Theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol. 2010;103(1):297–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Zar JH. Biostatistical Analysis. 3rd Ed Prentice Hall; Upper Saddle River, NJ: 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Clark JM, Paivio A. Extensions of the Paivio, Yuille, and Madigan (1968) norms. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2004;36(3):371–383. doi: 10.3758/bf03195584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Moeller S, et al. Multiband multislice GE-EPI at 7 tesla, with 16-fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial and temporal whole-brain fMRI. Magn Reson Med. 2010;63(5):1144–1153. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Setsompop K, et al. Blipped-controlled aliasing in parallel imaging for simultaneous multislice echo planar imaging with reduced g-factor penalty. Magn Reson Med. 2012;67(5):1210–1224. doi: 10.1002/mrm.23097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Brett M, Anton JL, Valbregue R, Poline JB. 2002. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract]. Presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, June 10–16, 2002, Sendai, Japan [CD-ROM]. Neuroimage 16:2.
  • 69.Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: Are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage. 2009;44(3):893–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fox MD, Zhang D, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME. The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J Neurophysiol. 2009;101(6):3270–3283. doi: 10.1152/jn.90777.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES