Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2016 Jan 16;171(1-3):176–181. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.01.017

Social Cognition over time in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis: findings from the NAPLS-2 cohort

Danijela Piskulic 1, Lu Liu 1, Kristin S Cadenhead 2, Tyrone D Cannon 3, Barbara A Cornblatt 4, Thomas H McGlashan 5, Diana O Perkins 6, Larry J Seidman 7, Ming T Tsuang 2,8, Elaine F Walker 9, Scott W Woods 5, Carrie E Bearden 10, Daniel H Mathalon 11,12, Jean Addington 1,*
PMCID: PMC5037438  NIHMSID: NIHMS752588  PMID: 26785807

Abstract

Deficits in social cognition are well established in schizophrenia and have been observed prior to the illness onset. Compared to healthy controls (HCs), individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis (CHR) are said to show deficits in social cognition similar to those observed in patients experiencing a first episode of psychosis. These deficits have been observed in several domains of social cognition, such as theory of mind (ToM), emotion perception and social perception. In the current study, the stability of three domains of social cognition (ToM, social perception and facial emotion perception) was assessed over time along and their association with both clinical symptoms and the later development of psychosis. Six hundred and seventy-five CHR individuals and 264 HC participants completed four tests of social cognition at baseline. Of those, 160 CHR and 155 HC participants completed assessments at all three time points (baseline, 1 year and 2 years) as part of their participation in the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study. The CHR group performed poorer on all tests of social cognition across all time points compared to HCs. Social cognition was not associated with attenuated positive symptoms at any time point in the study. CHR individuals who developed a psychotic disorder during the course of the study did not differ in social cognition compared to those who did not develop psychosis. This longitudinal study demonstrated mild to moderate, but persistent ToM and social perception impairments in those at CHR for psychosis compared to HCs.

Keywords: social cognition, theory of mind, social perception, emotion perception, clinical high risk, psychosis

1. Introduction

The NIMH Workshop of Social Cognition in Schizophrenia defines social cognition as a function that involves the perception, interpretation and processing of information that underlies social interactions. Because of the emphasis on a direct association with social behavior and a number of real world outcomes, social cognition has become one of the major areas of interest in schizophrenia (Pinkham et al., 2014). This is not accidental or surprising given overwhelming reports of poor social and role functioning in schizophrenia. The Social Cognition Psychometric Evaluation (SCOPE) study (Pinkham et al., 2014), which was designed to achieve a consensus on the key domains of social cognition in schizophrenia based on the expert advice, identified four major domains of social cognition: 1) theory of mind (ToM) or the ability to attribute beliefs and intentions to oneself and others; 2) emotion perception (both prosodic and facial) or the ability to recognize other people's feelings from either facial expressions or vocal inflections and use them to guide behaviors; 3) social perception and knowledge or the ability to judge and be aware of cues and rules that occur in social situations; and 4) attributional style or bias, which refers to an individual's tendency to attribute the cause of an event to either oneself, others or the environment. Deficits in social cognition are well evidenced in schizophrenia, both in the established illness (Penn et al., 2008) and prior to the illness onset (Barbato et al., 2015; Green et al., 2012) suggesting that they are relatively stable (Horan et al., 2012).

Recent progress in risk identification methodology has made it possible to identify individuals who are at clinical high risk of developing psychosis (CHR) based on clinical phenomenology, in particular sub-threshold psychotic symptoms (Addington and Heinssen, 2012). In the past decade, there has been a surge of studies examining social cognition in CHR populations compared to healthy controls (HCs) and patients with psychosis. Although the findings from these studies are mixed, the majority report quantifiable deficits in social cognition in CHR populations relative to healthy controls. Furthermore, the severity of those deficits is often similar to patients with psychotic disorders (Green et al., 2012; Thompson et al., 2011). Two recent meta-analyses of social cognition in CHR, reported deficits in all domains of social cognition (Lee et al., 2015; van Donkersgoed et al., 2015). The largest cumulative deficits have been observed in attributional bias and ToM, with somewhat smaller effects for emotion perception and social perception. The overall magnitude of social cognitive deficits in those at CHR fell between that of schizophrenia patients and their non-affected relatives (Lee et al., 2015). However, despite relatively consistent findings of social cognitive deficits in CHR samples, some reports support (Bora et al., 2008; Healey et al., 2013) and others deny (Lee et al., 2015; van Donkersgoed et al., 2015) whether social cognitive deficits predict conversion to psychosis.

Most studies that have examined social cognition in CHR to date have been based on small samples and have examined only one or two social cognitive domains at a time. The North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS 2) group recently published baseline data on social cognition and its association with symptoms in a large group of CHR participants assessing three different domains: ToM, social perception and facial emotion perception. At study entry, the CHR group showed deficits in all domains of social cognition compared to age and gender matched HCs. These deficits however, were not related to attenuated positive and negative symptom severity (Barbato et al., 2015). The aim of the current paper is to examine: first, the stability of social cognition over time; secondly, the cross-sectional correlations between social cognition and clinical symptoms at each time point; and thirdly to examine whether there are differences in social cognition between those who develop psychosis and those who do not in the NAPLS 2 sample.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were recruited as part of the multi-site NIMH funded NAPLS 2 that consisted of 764 CHR individuals (436 males, 328 females) and 280 HCs (141 males, 139 females) recruited across the eight NAPLS 2 sites. The majority, 743 CHR subjects, met the Criteria of Prodromal Syndromes (COPS) (McGlashan et al., 2010), however 21 CHR subjects were considered high risk due to presence of schizotypal features and age less than 18. Participants were excluded if they met criteria for any current or lifetime axis I psychotic disorder, IQ below 70 and past or current history of a clinically significant central nervous system disorder. Healthy controls were excluded if they had a first-degree relative with a current or past psychotic disorder. A more detailed description of recruitment procedures, ascertainment, and inclusion and exclusion criteria is provided elsewhere (Addington et al., 2015).

2.2. Measures

The Structured Interview for Psychosis-risk Syndrome (SIPS) (McGlashan et al., 2010) was used to determine whether an individual met COPS criteria. The Scale of Prodromal Symptoms (SOPS) consisting of 19 items in 4 symptom domains (i.e. positive, negative, general, and disorganized symptoms) was used to rate the severity of CHR symptoms.

Three well-established areas of social cognition were assessed in the current study; ToM, facial emotion perception and social perception, using validated measures (Pinkham et al., 2014). ToM was assessed using the Social Inference subscale of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (McDonald et al., 2003); facial emotion perception was assessed with the Penn Emotion Recognition task and the Penn Emotion Differentiation task (Gur et al., 2002); and social perception was assessed using the abbreviated version of the Relationship Across Domains (Sergi et al., 2009).

The Social Inference subscale of the TASIT includes 16 short video scenes, enriched with contextual cues, where actors are engaged in everyday conversations and use lies and sarcasm. In half of the vignettes the main speaker conveys a message that is contrary to what he or she believes (i.e., a lie), and in the other half the main speaker says something that is contrary to the actual meaning he or she wishes to convey (i.e., sarcasm). After each scene, participants answer questions about what the characters are thinking, doing, feeling and saying. Participants can answer “yes”, “no” or “don't know”. For each scene, the maximum score is four, yielding a maximum score of 64 as well as sub-scores for Lies and Sarcasm. The TASIT is an audiovisual measure with good psychometric properties and high ecological validity (McDonald et al., 2006) its efficacy in detecting ToM deficits has been proven with CHR individuals (Green et al., 2012).

To assess facial emotion perception, two well-established computerized tasks, the ER40 and the EDF40, were used. In these tasks, pictures representing facial expressions are shown in color. There are an equal number of male and female faces, and four races are represented (Caucasian, African-American, Asian and Hispanic). In the ER40, one face at a time is shown and participants have to choose the emotion that is represented from a list of five possibilities (anger, fear, neutral, happy and sad), shown on the right side of the screen. In the EDF40, two faces are shown and participants are asked to indicate which one shows an emotion (either happiness or sadness) more intensely. For the ER40 task, there is a total score ranging from 0 to 40, and individual sub-scores for happy, sad, angry, fearful and neutral facial expressions. For the EDF40 task, there is a total score ranging from 0 to 40, and two sub-scores for happy and sad facial expressions. Both of these tasks have been previously used with CHR individuals (Kohler et al., 2014).

The RAD is a measure of competence in relationship perception. We used the RAD-45 items, an abbreviated version of the RAD. The RAD-45 contains 15 vignettes each involving two characters whose interpersonal behaviors are consistent with one of the four relational models (Fiske, 2004). According to the relational model theory, people base their relationships on four implicit relationship models that regulate social behavior in several different domains of social life. Relationships conforming to the first model, named Communal Sharing, are based on the idea that the individuals have something in common and are equivalent and undifferentiated. The second model is called Authority Ranking and refers to relationships where there is a hierarchy between the members, which can be classified into “decision makers” and “followers”. The third model is called Equality Matching and is based on relationships involving a one-to-one distribution of efforts and resources between members. In the fourth model, called Market Pricing, relationships are based on ratios and rates, and members are focused on proportionality based on their contribution to a certain activity or business. In the RAD, each vignette is followed by three statements that describe interactions between the same two characters in different situations, with each statement representing one of the relational models. Participants are asked to use the information they have learned from the vignette to judge (answering yes/no) whether the behaviors described in each statement are likely to occur. Performance is measured as the total number of correct responses (ranging from 0-45) and four sub-scores, one for each relational model named above. The RAD has good psychometric properties and was specifically developed and validated to assess perception of relationships in individuals with schizophrenia based on evidence showing a link between poor use of relationship models and vulnerability to psychosis (Sergi et al., 2009).

2.3. Procedures

Both CHR individuals and HCs were recruited for the study, which was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of all eight NAPLS 2 sites. Written informed consent, including parental consent, was obtained from all adult participants and parents/guardians of minors. Clinical raters were experienced research clinicians. Gold standard post-training agreement on determining the prodromal diagnoses was excellent (kappa=0.90) (Addington et al., 2012). Social cognition assessments at all sites were conducted by trained raters. Data were collected at three time points: baseline, 1 year and 2 years.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS version 23 and SAS version 9.2. Between group differences on demographic variables were analyzed using Chi-square test and Student's t-test test. To accommodate for missing data at follow-up assessments and intra-participant correlation over time, a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) for repeated measures analysis was used to examine changes over time (baseline, 1 year and 2 years) and group differences for social cognitive domains (i.e. ToM, social perception and facial affect). Specifically, the variable for time of assessment (3 levels, time_0 for baseline, time_1 for 1 year and time_2 for 2 years) and the variable for group (two levels, group_0 for HCs and group_1 for CHR) and their interactions were included as fixed effects with participants modelled as random effects using an unstructured covariance matrix. Least Square Means (LS-Means) and their 95% confidence intervals (CI) were obtained from the mixed models with Tukey-Kramer's adjustment for multiple comparisons. Spearman rank-order correlation coefficient with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was used to examine cross-sectional correlations between social cognitive measures and clinical symptoms at each assessment point. Participants who received a diagnosis of a psychotic disorder during the course of the study were classified as converters. Group differences between converters and non-converters on baseline measures of social cognition were assessed using the Mann-Whitney U test (MWU) with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

3. Results

Six hundred and seventy-five CHR individuals (389 males and 286 females) and 264 HCs completed the social cognition assessments at baseline. Of those 675 CHR participants, 317 completed the one year follow-up, 188 completed the 2 year follow-up and 160 participants completed assessments at all three time points. Seventy-five CHR participants completed only the baseline assessment because they made the transition to psychosis within the first year. For the HCs, 115 participants completed all 3 assessments from 264 at baseline. The baseline characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 1. There were no differences in demographic characteristics or baseline social cognitive performance between participants that dropped out before the 24 month assessment and those that completed the study. Eighty-six CHR participants converted to psychosis during the two-year study period.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of the NAPLS 2 sample

Variable CHR n = 764 Controls n = 280 Test Statistic Effect Size
Mean (SD) t d
Age in years 18.50 (4.23) 19.73 (4.67) 3.86** 0.28
Years of education 11.28 (2.82) 12.68 (3.58) 5.90** 0.43
Frequency (%) X2 Cramer's V
Sex
Male 436 (57.1%) 141 (50.4%) 3.73* 0.06
Female 328 (42.9%) 139 (49.6%)
Race
First Nations 13 (1.7%) 4 (1.4%) 5.24 0.07
Asian 54 (7.1%) 30 (10.8%)
Black 118 (15.5%) 49 (17.5%)
Latin America/Middle East/White 478 (62.6%) 167 (59.6%)
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander 3 (0.4%) 1 (0.4%)
Interracial 97 (12.7%) 29 (10.4%)
Marital Status
Single never married 720 (95.0%) 266 (95.0%) 5.60 0.08
Othera 39 (5.0%) 14 (5.0%)
Currently working
Yes 189 (25.0%) 129 (46.0%) 42.84** 0.20
No 568 (75.0%) 151 (54.0%)
Currently enrolled as a student
Yes 625 (82.5%) 227 (81.1%) 0.27 0.02
No 133 (17.5%) 53 (18.9%)
*

p < 0.05

**

p< 0.01

a

Married, divorced, separated, widowed or cohabiting with a significant other

Results of the initial group comparisons on measures of social cognition across three time points are shown in Table 2. By appropriately modeling the intra-participant correlation over time, different variances across three time points and time-varying measures of social cognition, the results of the GLMM indicated that there were significant differences between the groups on all social cognitive measures at all time points. Furthermore, performance on all but one social cognitive test, the EDF40, improved over time in both groups (Table 3).

Table 2.

Generalized linear mixed model between HC and CHR group analysis for SC over time

Measure BL ΔM* (SE) 95% CI t d 1 year ΔM* (SE) 95% CI t d 2 years ΔM* (SE) t 95% CI d
TASIT total 2.45 (0.43) 1.22,3.67 5.71*** −0.42 2.16 (0.46) 0.85,3.47 4.71*** −0.34 2.33 (0.45) 5.17*** 1.04,3.61 −0.26
Lies 1.24 (0.26) 0.49,1.99 4.69*** −0.35 0.95 (0.28) 0.15,1.76 3.37* −0.28 1.23 (0.29) 4.15*** 0.38,2.07 −0.26
Sarcasm 1.21 (0.29) 0.39,2.02 424*** −0.36 1.22 (0.32) 0.30,2.14 3.79** −0.24 1.01 (0.34) 2.99* 0.04,1.97 −0.15
ER40 Total 0.87 (0.24) 0.17,1.57 3.56** −0.26 0.67 (0.28) −0.14,1.50 2.34 −0.17 0.61 (0.32) 1.92 −0.29,1.52 −0.16
Anger 0.02 (0.11) −0.27,0.32 0.22 −0.02 0.04 (0.12) −0.31,0.39 0.36 0.02 −0.07 (0.15) −0.5 −0.51,0.36 0.11
Fear 0.11 (0.09) −0.16,0.38 1.14 −0.08 0.14 (0.11) −0.17,0.456 1.29 −0.13 0.25 (0.12) 2.08 −0.09,0.58 −0.21
Happy 0.09 (0.05) −0.05,0.229 1.83 −0.14 0.16 (0.06) −0.00,0.32 2.84 0.24 0.00 (0.05) 0.06 −0.15,0.16 −0.02
Neutral 0.27 (0.11) −0.05,0.61 2.35 −0.20 0.18 (0.12) −0.18,0.54 1.46 −0.14 0.23 (0.15) 1.61 −0.18,0.65 −0.15
Sad 0.37 (0.09) 0.09,0.65 3.82** −0.27 0.11 (0.11) −0.20,0.43 1.04 −0.06 0.21 (0.13) 1.61 −0.16,0.58 −0.16
EDF40 Total 1.70 (0.42) 0.49,2.91 4.03*** −0.26 1.74 (0.51) 0.27,3.20 3.4** −0.23 0.51 (0.61) 0.84 −1.23,2.24 −0.05
Happy Total 0.99 (0.27) 0.22,1.76 3.68** −0.25 0.96 (0.34) −0.02,1.92 2.85 −0.25 0.05 (0.39) 0.14 −1.05,1.17 −0.17
Sad Total 0.71 (0.20) 0.14,1.29 3.25** −0.29 0.76 (0.24) 0.06,1.44 3.11* −0.26 0.47 (0.30) 1.55 −0.39,1.33 −0.11
RAD Total 2.29 (0.37) 1.23,3.35 6.19*** −0.44 2.39 (0.42) 1.18,3.60 5.63*** −0.39 2.32 (0.49) 4.78*** 0.94,3.71 −0.29
Communal 0.62 (0.14) 0.21,1.03 4.28*** −0.30 0.48 (0.16) 0.04,0.93 3.12* −0.23 0.67 (0.16) 4.18*** 0.21,1.12 −0.31
Market 0.19 (0.12) −0.16,0.53 1.52 −0.11 0.50 (0.14) 0.09,0.90 3.55** −0.28 0.45 (0.18) 2.47 −0.07,0.97 −0.23
Equality 0.54 (0.12) 0.19,0.89 4.39*** −0.32 0.42 (0.16) −0.02,0.87 2.71 −0.19 0.49 (0.19) 2.65 −0.04,1.03 −0.22
Authority 0.97 (0.15) 0.54,1.39 6.49*** −0.47 0.85 (0.18) 0.34,1.37 4.74*** −0.39 0.44 (0.20) 2.16 −0.14,1.02 −0.12
*

p < 0.05

**

p < 0.01

***

p < 0.001

CHR= clinical high group of psychosis; HC= healthy controls

ΔM*= least square means differences between groups; SE= standard error of the mean; d= Cohen's d

Table 3.

Generalized linear mixed model within group analysis for social cognition over time

CHR
Measure BL ΔM* (SE) BL M (SD) 1 year ΔM* (SE) 1 year M (SD) 2 years ΔM* (SE) 2 years M (SD)
TASIT total 52.30 (0.23) 52.3 (6.10) 54.3 (0.26)a*** 54.54 (5.64) 55.2 (0.27)a***b** 55.78 (5.10)
ER40 Total 32.79 (0.13) 32.79 (3.59) 33.48 (0.17)a** 33.51 (3.60) 34.05 (0.19)a***b* 34.12 (3.21)
EDF40 Total 24.29 (0.22) 24.29 (6.0) 24.36 (0.29) 24.62 (5.94) 24.66 (0.37) 24.71 (6.28)
RAD Total 31.63 (0.19) 31.68 (5.30) 32.96 (0.24)a*** 33.29 (5.32) 33.89 (0.29)a***b** 34.15 (5.62)
HC
Measure BL ΔM* (SE) BL M (SD) 1 year ΔM* (SE) 1 year M (SD) 2 years ΔM* (SE) 2 years M (SD)
TASIT total 54.80 (0.36) 54.80 (5.30) 56.4 (0.38)a*** 56.42 (4.50) 57.5 (0.36)a***b** 57.08 (4.45)
ER40 Total 33.67 (0.21) 33.67 (2.79) 34.16 (0.23) 34.09 (2.69) 34.67 (0.25)a** 34.61 (2.51)
EDF40 Total 25.99 (0.36) 25.97 (5.21) 26.09 (0.42) 26.19 (5.68) 25.17 (0.48) 25.38 (5.54)
RAD Total 33.93 (0.31) 33.95 (4.48) 35.35 (0.35)a*** 35.28 (4.27) 36.22 (0.39)a*** 35.67 (4.43)

Note: CHR= clinical high group of psychosis; HC= healthy controls

ΔM*= least square mean; SE= standard error of the mean; M= mean; SD= standard deviation

*

p<0.05

**

p<0.01

***

p<0.001

a

significantly different from baseline

b

significantly different from 1 year

Total scores on all measures of social cognition were inter-related across all three time-points in both study groups. Cross-sectional correlations between social cognition and the SOPS total positive, total negative, total disorganized and total generalized symptoms in the CHR group were assessed at all three time-points using Spearman rank-order correlation coefficient with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (Table 4). There were weak significant correlations between the social cognition measures and SOPS symptoms at baseline and the follow-up. However, after adjusting for multiple comparisons, there was only one negative correlation between ER40 and disorganization symptoms at baseline that survived the adjustment.

Table 4.

Correlations between measures of social cognition and SOPS symptoms in CHR participants

CHR
Measure Scale of Prodromal Symptoms
Positive Negative Disorganized General
Baseline
TASIT total .04 −.07 −.06 .07
RAD Total .01 .00 .04 .03
ER40 Total −.00 −.08* −.14*** −.04
EDF40 Total −.02 −.10** −.02 −.02
1 year
TASIT total −.02 −.05 −.01 .14*
RAD Total .07 .02 .11 .13*
ER40 Total −.12* −.04 −.13* −.01
EDF40 Total .08 −.01 .09 .13*
2 years
TASIT total .09 .00 .07 .01
RAD Total .08 .01 .07 .14
ER40 Total −.01 .02 .07 .06
EDF40 Total .07 −.21** .11 −.05

Values shown are correlation coefficients.

*

p < 0.05

**

p< 0.01

***

p< 0.003 following Bonferroni Correction at each time point.

TASIT=theory of mind task, RAD=social perception task, ER40 and EDF40= emotion perception tasks.

Results of the between group comparison on social cognitive tests at baseline for those who converted to psychosis and those who did not revealed no significant differences in medians between the converters and the non-converters on TASIT (53 vs. 54, U=21217.0, z= −1.0, p=0.29), RAD (31 vs. 32, U=21172.0, z=−0.44, p=0.65), ER40 (33 vs. 33, U=22047.5, z=−0.20, p=0.9) or EDF40 (24 vs. 25, U= 20884.0, z=−074, p=0.45). We further examined whether improvement occurred only for the non-converters. Since all conversions occurred before 2 years, we examined the change in social cognition from baseline to one year for the non-converters and converters using the GLMM (Table 5). Similar to the CHR group as a whole, the non-converters demonstrated improvement over time on all but one social cognitive test, EDF40. However, for the converters who converted after 1 year there was no improvement from baseline to 1 year on social cognition.

Table 5.

Generalized linear mixed model within group analysis for social cognition over time in converters and non-converters

Non-converters converters
Measure BL ΔM* (SE) 1 year ΔM* (SE) BL ΔM* (SE) 1 year ΔM* (SE)
TASIT total 52.50 (0.25) 54.40 (0.28)*** 51.07 (0.70) 53.43 (1.46)
ER40 Total 32.83 (0.14) 33.54 (0.18)*** 32.51 (0.40) 33.01 (0.94)
EDF40 Total 24.32 (0.25) 24.34 (0.31) 23.95 (0.68) 25.46 (1.57)
RAD Total 31.65 (0.22) 33.04 (0.27)*** 31.50 (0.61) 31.35 (1.35)

Note: ΔM*= least square mean; SE= standard error of the mean

***

p<0.001

4. Discussion

The CHR group performed poorer on all tests of social cognition across all time points compared to HCs even though magnitudes of group differences varied depending on the social cognitive domain and/or the assessment time point. Moderate differences between groups were noted in ToM and social perception at baseline and while these differences remained at follow-up assessments, the magnitude of differences decreased over time.

The observed deficit in ToM ability confirms previous evidence that individuals at CHR have difficulties with mental states attribution (Bora and Pantelis, 2013) and an earlier study using the TASIT, suggesting that CHR individuals show poor processing of counterfactual information (Green et al., 2012). The group differences in sarcasm detection suggest that impairment in processing counterfactual information starts early in the course of psychosis and may be considered as an indicator of vulnerability to psychosis. It may be that deficits in sarcasm detection impede social interaction and the establishment of peer-relationships, which can consequently adversely impact social functioning. However, given the reduction in effect sizes at follow-up these deficits may be less severe and less stable in CHR individuals compared to those with an established psychotic illness, with their performance in ToM intermediary to that of psychotic patients and HCs (Bora and Pantelis, 2013).

Although initially CHR individuals displayed poorer facial emotion perception compared to HCs, as previously reported (Addington et al., 2008; Amminger et al., 2012; Comparelli et al., 2013) these differences did not remain at follow-up. This may be due to practice effects that have been reported in relation to ER40 (Pinkham et al., 2015). Alternatively, there may be high variability when assessing facial emotion perception in youth as it has been suggested that facial emotion perception can vary significantly during the adolescent period due to continuous and non-linear development of the specific brain regions involved in facial emotion perception (Burnett et al., 2011). Previous studies have shown that CHR individuals had higher scores on facial emotion perception tasks compared to patients with schizophrenia but lower than HCs, without significantly differing from either group (Addington et al., 2008).

The observed impairment on social perception for the CHR group confirms findings from previous studies (Couture et al., 2008; Healey et al., 2013). The RAD specifically examines the understanding of social relationships. Poorer competence in relationship perception compared to HC participants observed in this study has previously been reported in CHR individuals (Green et al., 2012). Furthermore, inappropriate use of the relationship model, Authority Ranking, has been found to be associated with psychosis proneness (Allen et al., 2005), again fitting with our findings. It is important to note, however, that the RAD reportedly has weaker psychometric characteristics with pronounced floor effects in patients with schizophrenia (Pinkham et al., 2015), suggesting questionable utility of RAD as a repeated measure in clinical trials.

With one exception clinical symptoms were generally not associated with performance on measures of social cognition. Although significant, the strength of the association between facial affect recognition and disorganization symptoms at baseline was very low.

Although it has been suggested that CHR individuals who convert to psychosis were more prone to mislabeling neutral emotions as negative compared to their non-converter counterparts (even though face and voice emotion perception did not predict transition to psychosis) (Allott et al., 2014) and exhibited poorer performance in ToM (Bora and Pantelis, 2013; Healey et al., 2013) in this study there were no differences in social cognition between those who made the transition to psychosis and those who did not make the transition. Poorer performance on social cognition may be indicative of being potentially vulnerable to developing psychosis. As a group, those at CHR of psychosis tend to exhibit poorer social cognition relative to healthy controls but not as poor as those with an established illness. Furthermore, CHR participants who converted to psychosis did not display the same improvements in social cognition over time that were observed in the non-converters and the healthy controls. It is possible that this result is a function of the smaller sample size in this group relative to the non-converting CHR sample. Alternatively, it is possible that for those that do go on to develop psychosis the difficulties may persist and possibly worsen with the actual onset of psychosis.

The strengths of the current study lie in its longitudinal design, a large, well-defined sample and the assessment of three domains of social cognition. Limitations are that each domain of social cognition was assessed with a single measure, there was limited follow-up assessment for those who converted, and attrition at follow-up assessments.

In summary, the current longitudinal study demonstrated mild to moderate, but persistent ToM and social perception impairments in those at CHR for psychosis compared to HCs. Future research would benefit from exploring associations between distinct social cognitive profiles and functional and cognitive outcomes in CHR.

Acknowledgements

The Authors would like to acknowledge Dr. Monica Calkins, Dr. Christian Kohler and Dr. Ruben Gur for their valued assistance with the facial affect tasks.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Contributors

Dr. Piskulic and Ms. Liu undertook the statistical analysis, and Dr. Piskulic wrote the first draft of the manuscript. Dr. Addington was involved in writing of subsequent drafts of the manuscript. All of the authors listed were involved in study design and have contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

There are no conflicts of interest for any of the authors with respect to the data in this paper or for the study.

References

  1. Addington J, Cadenhead KS, Cornblatt BA, Mathalon DH, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW, Addington JA, Cannon TD. North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS 2): Overview and recruitment. Schizophrenia research. 2012;142(1-3):77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Addington J, Heinssen R. Prediction and prevention of psychosis in youth at clinical high risk. Annual review of clinical psychology. 2012;8:269–289. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Addington J, Liu L, Buchy L, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, Perkins DO, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW, Bearden CE, Mathalon DH, McGlashan TH. North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS 2): The Prodromal Symptoms. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 2015;203(5):328–335. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Addington J, Penn D, Woods SW, Addington D, Perkins DO. Facial affect recognition in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;192:67. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.039784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Allen N, Haslam N, Semedar A. Relationship Patterns Associated with Dimensions of Vulnerability to Psychopathology. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2005;29(6):733–746. [Google Scholar]
  6. Allott KA, Schafer MR, Thompson A, Nelson B, Bendall S, Bartholomeusz CF, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, Schlogelhofer M, Bechdolf A, Amminger GP. Emotion recognition as a predictor of transition to a psychotic disorder in ultra-high risk participants. Schizophrenia research. 2014;153(1-3):25–31. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Amminger GP, Schafer MR, Papageorgiou K, Klier CM, Schlogelhofer M, Mossaheb N, Werneck-Rohrer S, Nelson B, McGorry PD. Emotion recognition in individuals at clinical high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(5):1030–1039. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barbato M, Liu L, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW, Bearden CE, Mathalon DH, Heinssen R, Addington J. Theory of mind, emotion recognition and social perception in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis: Findings from the NAPLS-2 cohort. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition. 2015;2:133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2015.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bora E, Gokcen S, Kayahan B, Veznedaroglu B. Deficits of social-cognitive and social-perceptual aspects of theory of mind in remitted patients with schizophrenia: effect of residual symptoms. The Journal of nervous and mental disease. 2008;196(2):95. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318162a9e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bora E, Pantelis C. Theory of mind impairments in first-episode psychosis, individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis and in first-degree relatives of schizophrenia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia research. 2013;144(1–3):31–36. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Burnett S, Sebastian C, Cohen Kadosh K, Blakemore SJ. The social brain in adolescence: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioural studies. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2011;35(8):1654–1664. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Comparelli A, Corigliano V, De Carolis A, Mancinelli I, Trovini G, Ottavi G, Dehning J, Tatarelli R, Brugnoli R, Girardi P. Emotion recognition impairment is present early and is stable throughout the course of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia research. 2013;143(1):65–69. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Couture SM, Penn D, Addington J, Woods SW, Perkins DO. Assessment of social judgements and complex mental states in the early phases of psychosis. Schizophrenia research. 2008;100:237. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.12.484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Fiske AP. Relational model theory 2.0. In: Haslam N, editor. Relational Models Theory: A Conteporary Overview. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Mahwah, NJ.: 2004. [Google Scholar]
  15. Green MF, Bearden CE, Cannon TD, Fiske AP, Hellemann GS, Horan WP, Kee K, Kern RS, Lee J, Sergi MJ, Subotnik KL, Sugar CA, Ventura J, Yee CM, Nuechterlein KH. Social Cognition in Schizophrenia, Part 1: Performance Across Phase of Illness. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(4):854–864. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gur RC, Sara R, Hagendoorn M, Marom O, Hughett P, Macy L, Turner T, Bajcsy R, Posner A, Gur RE. A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J Neurosci Methods. 2002;115(2):137–143. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(02)00006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Healey K, Penn DL, Perkins D, Woods SW, Addington J. Theory of Mind and Social Judgments in People at Clinical High Risk of Psychosis. Schizophrenia research. 2013;150(0):498–504. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Horan WP, Green MF, Degroot M, Fiske A, Hellemann G, Kee K, Kern RS, Lee J, Sergi MJ, Subotnik KL, Sugar CA, Ventura J, Nuechterlein KH. Social Cognition in Schizophrenia, Part 2: 12-Month Stability and Prediction of Functional Outcome in First-Episode Patients. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(4):865–872. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kohler CG, Richard JA, Brensinger CM, Borgmann-Winter KE, Conroy CG, Moberg PJ, Gur RC, Gur RE, Calkins ME. Facial emotion perception differs in young persons at genetic and clinical high-risk for psychosis. Psychiatry research. 2014;216(2):206–212. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lee TY, Hong SB, Shin NY, Kwon JS. Social cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia research. 2015;164(1-3):28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. McDonald S, Bornhofen C, Shum D, Long E, Saunders C, Neulinger K. Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): a clinical test of social perception. Disability and rehabilitation. 2006;28(24):1529–1542. doi: 10.1080/09638280600646185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. McDonald S, Flanagan S, Rollins J, Kinch J. TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation. 2003;18(3):219–238. doi: 10.1097/00001199-200305000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. McGlashan T, Walsh B, Woods S. The Psychosis-Risk Syndrome: Handbook for Diagnosis and Follow-up. Oxford University Press; New York: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  24. Penn DL, Sanna LJ, Roberts DL. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an overview. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(3):408. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Buck B, Healey K, Harvey PD. The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: results of the expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(4):813–823. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Harvey PD. Social Cognition Psychometric Evaluation: Results of the Initial Psychometric Study. Schizophr Bull. 2015 doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Sergi MJ, Fiske AP, Horan WP, Kern RS, Kee KS, Subotnik KL, Nuechterlein KH, Green MF. Development of a measure of relationship perception in schizophrenia. Psychiatry research. 2009;166(1):54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Thompson AD, Bartholomeusz C, Yung AR. Social cognition deficits and the 'ultra high risk' for psychosis population: a review of literature. Early intervention in psychiatry. 2011;5(3):192–202. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2011.00275.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. van Donkersgoed RJ, Wunderink L, Nieboer R, Aleman A, Pijnenborg GH. Social Cognition in Individuals at Ultra-High Risk for Psychosis: A Meta-Analysis. PloS one. 2015;10(10):e0141075. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES