Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2016 Aug 23;5:e15292. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15292

Revealing the neural fingerprints of a missing hand

Sanne Kikkert 1,2,, James Kolasinski 1,3,, Saad Jbabdi 1, Irene Tracey 1,4, Christian F Beckmann 1,2,5, Heidi Johansen-Berg 1, Tamar R Makin 1,*
Editor: Klaas Enno Stephan6
PMCID: PMC5040556  PMID: 27552053

Abstract

The hand area of the primary somatosensory cortex contains detailed finger topography, thought to be shaped and maintained by daily life experience. Here we utilise phantom sensations and ultra high-field neuroimaging to uncover preserved, though latent, representation of amputees’ missing hand. We show that representation of the missing hand’s individual fingers persists in the primary somatosensory cortex even decades after arm amputation. By demonstrating stable topography despite amputation, our finding questions the extent to which continued sensory input is necessary to maintain organisation in sensory cortex, thereby reopening the question what happens to a cortical territory once its main input is lost. The discovery of persistent digit topography of amputees’ missing hand could be exploited for the development of intuitive and fine-grained control of neuroprosthetics, requiring neural signals of individual digits.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.001

Research Organism: Human

eLife digest

The brain has a remarkable ability to adapt to changes in circumstances. But what happens to the brain when it loses a key source of input, for example, following the amputation of a limb? A region of the brain known as primary somatosensory cortex processes sensory inputs from all over the body. The more sensitive an area of the body is, the more fine-grained its representation is in the cortex. For example, the hand is represented with a highly detailed map, with each finger represented seperately.

The brain is thought to require ongoing sensory signals from the body to maintain these detailed representations in the cortex. Indeed, textbooks typically state that the brain will ‘overwrite’ its representation of a body part if input from that area no longer arrives. According to this view, people who have lost a hand should show little or no activity in the area of primary somatosensory cortex that used to represent it.

However, many people who have had a limb amputated continue to experience vivid sensations of the missing limb long after its loss. When asked to move their so-called ‘phantom’ limb, these individuals report being able to feel the movement. Kikkert, Kolasinski et al. now show, using advanced imaging techniques, that the brains of individuals with phantom hands continue to represent the missing hand several decades after its loss. Indeed, asking the subjects to move individual fingers of their phantom hand activates fine-grained representations of those fingers, similar to those seen in two-handed controls.

By showing that the brain ‘remembers’ an amputated hand, Kikkert, Kolasinski et al. demonstrate that ongoing sensory input is not required to maintain representations of the body in somatosensory cortex. This, in turn, offers new hope for developing prosthetic limbs that are under direct brain control. If the brain continues to represent individual fingers many years after their loss, it should be possible to exploit those pathways to achieve intuitive fine-grained control of artificial fingers.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.002

Introduction

The hand area of the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) contains detailed digit maps, with physically adjacent digits represented next to each other. Using high-field neuroimaging, it is now possible to identify these characteristic digit maps in humans, with high inter- and intra-subject reliability (Ejaz et al., 2015; Kolasinski et al., 2016). Digit topography is characterised in neuroimaging by two main principles: digit selectivity (Kolasinski et al., 2016) and inter-digit overlap (Ejaz et al., 2015). These maps are thought to be shaped and maintained by daily life experience: digits used more frequently together in daily life benefit from increased representational overlap (Ejaz et al., 2015), and following single digit amputation remaining digits’ topography changes (Merzenich et al., 1984).

Amputees commonly experience lingering sensations from their amputated body part (Flor and Nikolajsen, 2006). Phantom sensations are not necessarily painful, and are best described as a vivid sensation of the missing hand as if it is still present (Flor and Nikolajsen, 2006). When instructed to move their phantom hand, amputees report detailed kinaesthetic sensations regarding the extent of movement afforded by different phantom digits. Phantom movements are known to evoke signals in the sensorimotor system (Makin et al., 2013aReilly et al., 2006; Raffin et al., 2012b), previously attributed to abnormal processing caused by the amputation (e.g. aberrant inputs (Makin et al., 2013a), peripheral reorganisation (Reilly et al., 2006)). Here we interrogate the information content underlying activity elicited by phantom hand movements. If this information content is unchanged despite amputation, then activity patterns should show characteristic S1 digit topography.

To study phantom digit topography, we used ultra high-field 7 tesla neuroimaging in two individuals who lost their left hand several decades ago (25 and 31 years post amputation) and eleven right-handed controls. Both amputees reported exceptionally vivid kinaesthetic phantom sensations during individual digit movements (see Figure 1—source data 1, 2, for clinical and demographic details). Amputees and controls were visually cued to execute individual phantom digit movements (left hand digits in controls). Importantly, phantom movements are distinguishable from imagined movements. This is supported by empirical evidence demonstrating that phantom limb movements elicit both central and peripheral motor signals, that are different from those found during imagined movements (Makin et al., 2013aReilly et al., 2006Raffin et al., 2012b, 2012a). To ensure adequate task performance, amputees were asked to demonstrate to the experimenter outside the scanner the extent of volitional movement carried out in each of their phantom digits during the task, by mirroring the phantom movements with their intact hand.

To capture the first principle of topography, we employed a technique designed to identify digit preference in S1 (travelling wave design, see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for design details; see Figure 1—figure supplement 2 for other brain areas activated by phantom hand movements). The resulting gradients of digit preference (hereafter digit maps) are presented in Figure 1. As indicated by the black arrows in the example control participants (Figure 1A), a characteristic digit map shows a gradient of digit preference, progressing from thumb (red, laterally) to little finger (pink, medially). Similarly, both amputees showed a clear gradient of digit preference in the central sulcus and postcentral gyrus (Figure 1B; see Figure 1—figure supplement 3 for intact hand maps). Qualitatively, the position, digit order, and extent of the missing hand maps were similar to those observed in controls. Analysis of spatial correspondence of 'same' versus 'different' digit clusters between two halves of the dataset further confirmed that while reduced (compared to controls), digit selectivity in amputees was consistent (Figure 1D; see Materials and methods for further details).

Figure 1. Missing hand maps revealed in amputees during phantom digits movement.

Black arrows indicate preference for digits 1–5: thumb (red); index (yellow); middle (green); ring (blue) and little finger (purple) in two-handed controls (A, C) and amputees (B). Participants performed single digit flexion and extension movements with their non-dominant (controls) or phantom hand (amputees) in a travelling wave paradigm. Qualitatively similar digit topographies were found in each amputee and the controls. White arrows indicate the central sulcus. A = anterior; P = posterior. Multiple comparisons were adjusted using false discovery rate (FDR). (D) Maps’ intra-individual split-half consistency, assessed using the Dice overlap coefficient. On average, 'same'-digit selective clusters (dark bars) showed greater consistency than 'different'-digit clusters (light bars) in amputees and controls. Amputees showed lower split-half consistency for 'same'-digit clusters (averaged across digits) compared to controls (95% confidence intervals (CI) = 0.43–0.68, as assessed using a bootstrap approach). However, amputees’ 'same'-digit clusters were more consistent than 'different'-digit clusters in controls (i.e. fell outside the CI of 'different'-digit clusters split-half consistency in controls), indicating that although reduced, the digit maps of the amputees were consistent.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.003

Figure 1—source data 1. Amputee demographic and clinical details.
To measure phantom sensations, as well as other demographic and clinical details of potential relevance to the studied missing hand representation, amputees completed a range of questionnaires (e.g. amputation details, prosthesis usage etc.). Amputees rated intensities of phantom/stump pain and non-painful phantom sensations (vividness), using a 0 – 100 scale, as follows: (i) intensity of worst pain/vivid sensation experienced during the last week (or in a typical week involving such sensations); (ii) intensity of phantom pain on average over the last week (or in a typical week if last week was atypical); and (iii) current intensity/vividness of phantom pain and sensations, during the scanning day. Phantom limb pain magnitude was calculated by dividing worst pain intensity by pain frequency (1- all the time; 2- daily; 3- weekly). An analogous measure was obtained for vividness of non-painful phantom sensations. This approach represented the chronic aspect of the phantom pain/sensation as it combines frequency and intensity (Makin et al., 2013a). We also asked the amputees whether they experienced telescoping, a common phenomenon where the perceived length of the phantom limb is changed (Flor and Nikolajsen, 2006). Amp. = amputation; Amp. level = amputation level measured in percentages: (residual arm length/intact arm length) x 100. Intact arm length is measured from shoulder to fingertips, where 41% is equivalent to the level of the elbow and 75% is equivalent to the level of the wrist; PLS = phantom limb sensations; mag. = magnitude; scan = score of sensation phantom limb vividness/pain intensity on scanning day (scale 0 – 100); PLP = phantom limb pain; ave. = score of average phantom limb pain in a typical week (scale 0 – 100); prosthetics usage = prosthetic limb usage (frequency): 0- never, 1- rarely, 2- occasionally, 3- daily, 4- more than 4 hours a day, 5- more than 8 hours a day; Y. = yes; N. = no.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.15292.004
Figure 1—source data 2. Phantom digits movement vividness, difficulty and quality.
Amputees were asked to rate how vivid the phantom movement felt (PDS = phantom digit sensation; scale 0 – 100; ranging from unable to feel movement to movement is felt as vividly as the sensation of movement elicited by the intact hand digits). In addition, amputees were asked to rate how difficult it was to perform each phantom digit movement (diff. = difficulty; scale 0 – 100; ranging from easy to impossible). Finally, participants were asked to rate the quality and extent of movement afforded by each phantom digit. Amputees’ answers were standardised as follows: 1- full range of movements (i.e. equivalent to movements with the homologous digit on the intact hand). 2– near to full range of movement (i.e. similar to movement of the intact hand, though reduced). 3– noticeably less range of movement compared to the intact hand (i.e. partial finger flexion and extension movement).
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.15292.005

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Travelling wave task and analysis.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

A travelling wave paradigm, involving digit movements in a set sequence, was used to capture digit preference and the smooth progression of adjacent digit representations in primary somatosensory cortex. (A) The travelling wave task involved 8 repeated cycles of continuous individual digit movements. Each 45 s cycle consisted of five 9 s blocks dedicated to each digit (as exemplified by the coloured squares), with each run cycling through either D1-D2-D3-D4-D5 (forward, top) or D5-D4-D3-D2-D1 (backward, bottom). Each of the forward and backward runs was repeated twice. The numbers 1–5 and colours correspond to the five digits, as visualised in the right side of the panel. (B) Preprocessed BOLD time courses from individual voxels (the example time course of one voxel is shown in red) were cross-correlated against a reference model. The model corresponded to a single-digit movement (9 s ‘on’, 36 s ‘off’- the period of movement of all other digits), and was systematically shifted in time to model activity throughout a full cycle (30 shifts, corresponding to the 30 lags of the 45 s cycle). The figure shows three different model lags (1, 12 and 24), resulting in varying cross-correlation outcomes. Lag 24 was the optimal model lag for this preprocessed BOLD time course. (C) The standardised r-values (Fisher’s r-to-z, y-axis) resulting from the cross-correlation were plotted as a function of lag (x-axis) for each individual voxel in each run. In the left panel, cross-correlation values from the example voxel shown in B are plotted for each of the four runs. The optimal fit for the voxel is demonstrated by the peak cross-correlation (digit 1, in the left panel; digit 2 in the right panel, taken from separate voxels). Lags associated with the same digits were averaged, first within and then across runs. A winner-take-all approach was used to determine digit preference, based on the highest average r-value, resulting in five separate digit maps. False discovery rate (FDR) was applied for each of these single digit maps to adjust the threshold based on the criterion q (FDR) < 0.05. (D) Each of the five digits was assigned a colour code, as demonstrated on the left. The five digit maps were projected to the cortical surface, to display digit preference maps. Note that no masking was applied when presenting the resulting maps. Within the resulting five-digit map a gradient of progression across digits was visualised, based on the voxel-wise r-values for each of the 30 lags. The resulting map was located in the central sulcus and postcentral gyrus, corresponding to the primary somatosensory cortex, around the anatomical hand knob.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Brain areas activated by phantom hand movement.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Contrast between phantom (amputees; A) or left (controls; B) hand movements and baseline (rest) in the block design, projected on cortical surfaces of the contralateral hemisphere. Analysis was performed using a standard 2-level general linear model. Amputees and controls showed activation in primary somatosensory and motor cortex (S1 and M1, respectively; left panel), as well as in the supplementary motor area (SMA, right panel). Note that more inferior areas (e.g. secondary somatosensory cortex) were not covered by the partial field of view utilised for the ultra high-field acquisition. White arrows indicate the central sulcus. A = anterior; P = posterior. Displayed activations were thresholded at Z > 2. These results show that phantom hand movements activate a network of brain areas normally associated with hand movement.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Intact hand maps in amputees.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

In order to visualise standard topography in the amputees’ somatosensory cortex, topographic mapping was also carried out for the intact hand. For this purpose, task demands were adjusted to optimally activate the somatosensory cortex. Participants were provided with tactile feedback via a MRI-compatible button-box (manufactured in house), placed on the participant’s torso for the duration of the scan. Participants pressed the button box with the relevant digit using a travelling wave paradigm (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Since this task did not involve any difficulty for the amputees, as it only consisted of moving digits of the intact hand, all participants were paced to perform button press movements with individual digits at a set (1 Hz) frequency, as indicated by the flashing of the corresponding circle at the specified rate. A forward and backward sequence was employed in separate runs. Each of the forward and backward runs was repeated twice, for a total duration of 25 min and 40 s. All other procedures and analysis were as described in the main analysis for the missing hand digit maps (Figure 1). The black arrows indicate preferences for digits 1–5: thumb (red); index (yellow); middle (green); ring (blue) and little finger (purple) for the intact hand in amputees (A) and two handed controls (B). White arrows indicate the central sulcus. A = anterior; P = posterior. Multiple comparisons were adjusted using false discovery rate (FDR). (C) Intra-individual split-half consistency was assessed using the Dice overlap coefficient for the digit maps. 'Same'-digit selective clusters (dark) showed significantly greater consistency than 'different'-digit clusters (light) in amputees and controls. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. Note that the organisation, extent and consistency of the digit maps qualitatively resembled the left (missing) hand maps (obtained using self-paced digit flexion and extension) found in both controls and amputees (Figure 1).

A hallmark of a functional sensorimotor system is the distinct representation of the two hands. To further confirm the existence of the missing hand map independently of intact hand contributions (Makin et al., 2013b), we designed an opposing bimanual travelling wave task. Participants performed paired movements with digits of the phantom and intact hands in reverse sequences (i.e. one hand moved from digit 1 to digit 5, while the other simultaneously moved from digit 5 to digit 1; Figure 2A). Both amputees showed maps in the hemisphere contralateral to the intact hand, dissociated from a second map in the missing hand hemisphere (Figure 2B,C). The latter map corresponded to the original (unimanual movements, Figure 1) missing hand map, as confirmed by higher spatial correspondence for 'same' (average Dice overlap coefficient: 0.49, 0.40 for above and below elbow amputees) versus 'different' (0.06, 0.04) digit clusters across the two (unimanual and bimanual) missing hand datasets (Figure 2E). This provides a replication for the preserved missing hand map. This result also demonstrates the independence of the missing hand topography from the representation of the intact hand. Our group and others previously showed that following unilateral arm amputation, the intact hand becomes over-represented in the missing hand territory (Makin et al., 2013bPhilip and Frey, 2014; Raffin et al., 2016). Our results therefore confirm that the digit map in the missing hand territory is not driven by an emerging representation of the intact hand. Note, however, that our results do not exclude the possibility for reorganisation in the missing hand territory. As demonstrated in Figure 2—figure supplement 1, this study’s participants showed over-representation of their intact hand in the missing hand territory, indicating that the preserved missing hand topography can co-occur with remapping of body parts.

Figure 2. Missing hand maps are replicated during dissociated bimanual hand movements.

(A) In the bimanual task, individuals performed paired digit movements with both hands, resulting in reversed digit cycles between the hands. The colour code indicates the cycle lag as shown in (A). Symmetrical colour gradients across the two hemispheres would reveal the missing hand map as dependent on intact hand movements (B: case 1). Colour gradient reversal across hemispheres indicates dissociated hand representations (B: case 2), as was seen in both amputees and controls (C, D respectively, left two panels). Maps contralateral to the missing hand (left hand in controls) resembled the unimanual task maps (Figure 1), both qualitatively (C, D, right panel) and quantitatively, using the Dice overlap coefficient (E). The digit maps were adjusted for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate (FDR). Other annotations are as in Figure 1.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.009

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Over-representation of the intact hand in the missing hand territory.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

To investigate whether the observed missing hand maps (Figure 1, Figure 4) were preserved despite reorganisation of displaced inputs (as reported previously [Makin et al., 2013b]), we quantified intact hand representation in the territory of the missing hand in the amputees participating in the current study. For this purpose, we employed an independent dataset, acquired using standard field neuroimaging (acquisition parameters were identical to those described for the standard field imaging in the materials and methods section, with the exception of the number of acquisition volumes). (A) Participants were cued to perform movements with their intact hand (simultaneous flexion and extension of all five digits), or dominant hand in controls (n = 15), as part of a block-design protocol, used to identify body-part representations (see [Makin et al., 2013b] for details of a similar protocol). This movement condition was contrasted against a baseline (rest) period and standardised (z) beta values during intact/dominant hand movements were extracted from the peak voxel within a postcentral gyrus (PCG) mask in the contralateral hemisphere. To identify the estimated position of the missing hand territory, this peak voxel was mirror flipped on the axial plane, and the standardised beta value for intact hand activity was extracted from this ROI in the ipsilateral hemisphere. (B) To assess asymmetry in intact hand activity across the two hemispheres, the following inter-hemispheric asymmetry index was calculated for each of the three amputees and 15 controls individually: (contralateral hemisphere – ipsilateral hemisphere) / (contralateral hemisphere + ipsilateral hemisphere). Higher values of this index indicate that the activity pattern for the intact hand was greater in the contralateral, compared to the ipsilateral, sensorimotor hand area, as found was in the control participants. Lower values in this index represent less asymmetry, resulting from (relatively) higher activity in the ipsilateral hand area during intact hand movements. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. The above elbow and brachial plexus amputees showed significantly lower asymmetry than controls (one-tailed Crawford and Howell t-test [Corballis, 2009]; t(14) = −1.84, p = 0.04; and t(14) = −1.96, p = 0.04 respectively). The below elbow amputee showed a high asymmetry value, similar to controls (one-tailed Crawford and Howell t-test (Corballis, 2009); t(14) = 1.26, p = 0.11). This indicates that two of the tested amputees show over-representation of their intact hand in the missing hand territory, as previously reported (Makin et al., 2013b). These results suggest that the missing hand maps can be preserved while reorganisation of displaced inputs from the intact hand has taken place.

While the travelling wave maps demonstrate digit preference, they provide little information about the second principle of digit topography: inter-digit overlap. To study inter-digit overlap, we used pairwise digit representational similarity of multivoxel patterns (Diedrichsen et al., 2011). This approach fully reveals the intricate overlap pattern across all digits (Ejaz et al., 2015), as shown for controls in Figure 3B,D. In general, both amputees showed greater average overlap across digits (0.55 and 0.54 for above and below elbow amputees respectively), compared to controls (95% confidence intervals (CI) = 0.24–0.41). However, when examining the inter-digit overlap pattern in the missing hand map, both amputees demonstrated typical patterns (Figure 3A,C; see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for intact hand). This was reflected in high correlation between each amputee’s inter-digit overlap pattern and the controls, as assessed using a bootstrapping approach (average rs = 0.61 and rs = 0.78, 95% CI = 0.25–0.89 and 95% CI = 0.62–0.90 for above and below elbow amputees respectively). These average amputee-to-controls Spearman correlation values fell well within the controls-to-controls Spearman correlation range (95% CI = 0.35–0.95; Figure 3C), providing further evidence for characteristic missing hand representation decades after amputation.

Figure 3. Amputees show characteristic inter-digit overlap patterns during phantom digit movements.

Inter-digit representational similarity of multivoxel patterns underlying the maps shown in Figure 1, derived from a block-design paradigm in amputees (A) and controls (B, D). Similarity (or overlap) is decreased between non-neighbouring digits and tends to increase between digits 3–5, as shown in the controls’ averaged matrix. (C) Positive distribution of inter-subject correlations, between controls’ inter-digit overlap patterns. The grey area indicates the 95% confidence interval (CI) for controls. Pink and blue lines indicate average amputee-to-controls correlations for above and below elbow amputees respectively. Pink and blue shaded areas indicate the corresponding 95% CIs. Both amputee-to-controls correlation averages fell within the normal controls-to-controls correlation range, suggesting that the amputees exhibited a characteristic pattern of inter-digit overlap.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.011

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Intact hand inter-digit overlap in amputees.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

In order to visualise standard topography in the amputees’ somatosensory cortex, inter-digit representational similarity was assessed for multivoxel patterns underlying the right (intact) hand maps shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 3 in amputees (intact hand, A) and controls (dominant hand, B). To estimate inter-digit representational similarity for the intact hand, the same blocked design as described in the materials and methods section missing hand inter-digit overlap was used. Tactile feedback (from the button box) was incorporated into the task to optimally activate the somatosensory cortex. Participants were instructed to perform single digit button presses (using an MRI-compatible button-box manufactured in house) with their right intact (amputees) or right dominant (controls) hand at a 1 Hz frequency. To cue the participant which digit should be moved, the circle corresponding to this digit flashed at the specified 1 Hz rate. Digits 1–5: thumb; index; middle; ring and little finger.

Finally, we also studied missing hand representation in a third amputee (31 years since amputation) whose cause of amputation involved a brachial plexus avulsion, abolishing communication between the residual arm and the central nervous system. Due to MRI safety limitations 3 tesla neuroimaging was used (see Figure 4B for quality comparisons to ultra high-field digit maps). Using the bimanual task specified above (Figure 4A) we identified digit preference for the missing hand in the postcentral gyrus and central sulcus (Figure 4C). We also identified a typical inter-digit overlap pattern (Figure 4D), as confirmed in comparison with the control population described in Figure 3 (average Spearman correlation with controls = 0.76, with 95% CI = 0.30–0.92). This result provides further evidence for the existence of preserved digit topography in the absence of peripheral input.

Figure 4. Topography is preserved despite the absence of peripheral input.

Figure 4.

To rule out the potential contribution of peripheral inputs from the injured nerve to preservation of the phantom hand map, we tested an upper-limb amputee suffering from brachial plexus avulsion injury with vivid kinaesthetic phantom digit sensations. This injury involves the tearing of the nerve from its attachment at the spinal cord, abolishing afferent inputs and efferent outputs to the residual limb. 3 tesla MRI was used here due to safety restrictions, meaning that standard field digit preference maps were acquired. The bimanual travelling wave task (A, Figure 2) elicited maps in the controls with reversed colour gradients (B - left 2 panels). In comparison to the ultra high-field left hand maps (right panel), the standard field left hand maps were cruder (e.g. less digit specificity, B - middle panel). (C) The brachial plexus amputee showed two maps with reversed colour gradients, comparable to those found in controls. The map in the missing hand hemisphere showed preference for digits in primary somatosensory cortex. (D) Inter-digit representational similarity was assessed for multivoxel patterns underlying the missing hand map. The correlation value of the brachial plexus amputee’s inter-digit overlap pattern with the controls exemplified in Figure 3 fell within the normal controls-to-controls correlation range. Together, these results suggest that preserved representation of the missing hand existed independently of peripheral inputs. Other annotations are as in Figure 1.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.013

Together we show that, although the missing hand maps were weaker and noisier than the maps found in controls, the functional digit layout of S1 prevails following arm amputation. Digit topography, previously thought to depend on experience (Ejaz et al., 2015; Merzenich et al., 1984), was detectable despite decades without organised peripheral inputs associated with normal hand function. Our findings call for a reassessment of the role of sensory input in regulating brain organisation and plasticity. By demonstrating characteristic topography of the missing hand decades after arm amputation, our findings reopen the question of what happens to a cortical territory once its main inputs are removed.

Textbooks teach us that the cortical territory previously assigned to processing the now lost input is invaded by new representations. For example, following arm amputation, the missing hand territory in S1 is taken over by representations of other body parts (e.g. the neighbouring representation of the face in monkeys [Pons et al., 1991], or the intact hand in humans [Makin et al., 2013b]). Conversely, recent research in the visual cortex suggests that reorganisation in the adult brain may be restricted. For example following macular degeneration, the functional representation of the intact visual field was unchanged ([Baseler et al., 2011], see [Smirnakis et al., 2005] for similar results in non-human primates). Common to all these previous studies aiming to characterise reorganisation or the lack thereof, is that the authors probed the cortical neighbours of the area previously responsible for processing the lost input. While this approach is suitable for documenting shifted representation of the cortical neighbours, it leaves unexplored the possibility that the original function of the region deprived of sensory input may be preserved, though latent. Amputees experiencing phantom sensations provide a unique model to study what happens to the deprived cortical territory itself during sensory input loss. Here we show that reorganisation in the missing hand territory following input loss does not abolish the original functional layout in sensory cortex.

How can our finding of preserved S1 topography of a missing hand be allied with the wealth of evidence showing cortical reorganisation in S1 following sensory input loss? (e.g. amputation and spinal cord injury [Merzenich et al., 1984; Pons et al., 1991; Jain et al., 2008]; see Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for reorganisation in the current study’s volunteers cohort). Already in their seal work Merzenich et al. suggested that reorganisation following sensory input loss does not exclude the possibility for simultaneous preservation of the original function of that region (Merzenich et al., 1984). Accordingly, recent structural and functional evidence shows that the capacity for S1 reorganisation is more limited than initially thought, and that instead the functional changes previously observed in S1 following input loss could be attributed to reorganisation in sub-cortical areas in the afferent pathway, principally the brainstem (Jain et al., 1998; Kambi et al., 2014; Chand and Jain, 2015). In other words, previous findings of massive cortical reorganisation in S1 reflect reorganisation of inputs, rather than reorganisation within S1 itself. This recent evident nicely complements our own finding of SI reorganisation, overlaid on preserved structure and function.

Which inputs could contribute to the maintenance of the missing hand topography? The variability in the level and nature of amputations in this study’s cohort allows us to consider the potential contribution of the peripheral nervous system in the preservation of missing hand topography. In the below elbow amputee, some forearm muscles normally controlling hand movements are spared and therefore proprioceptive inputs relating to phantom hand movements likely persist (Nyström and Hagbarth, 1981). In the above elbow amputee, these inputs would be absent, though ectopic firing from the injured nerve (Nyström and Hagbarth, 1981) or intact dorsal root ganglia could preserve some afferent inputs (as previously shown following peripheral nerve injury [Nordin et al., 1984], see also [Vaso et al., 2014] for related findings). However, in the amputee suffering from brachial plexus avulsion injury, the dorsal root ganglia are damaged, meaning that no such peripheral input should be available. Given the observation of preserved topography in all three cases, it is highly probable that the preserved missing hand maps are not maintained by peripheral input, but rather are driven by processing in the central nervous system itself.

What neural signals may be triggering the brain activations subserving the missing hand maps? Given the relatively unimpaired motor system in amputees, it is possible that these representations are driven by motor (efferent) information. The motor system is thought to provide information about its descending commands to the sensory system, by means of efference copy. When efference signals reach the sensory areas, they evoke activity in those areas. The pattern of this corollary discharge could resemble that of the sensory feedback to be expected from the movement (London and Miller, 2013). While predictive signals are fundamental components for current theories of motor control (Franklin and Wolpert, 2011), surprisingly little empirical evidence exists to demonstrate efferent signals in the primates’ S1 hand representation independently of afferent processing (London and Miller, 2013). Our evidence for digit topography in S1 despite the physical absence of a hand suggests the involvement of non-afferent processing in S1. The persistence of efference signals from the motor system could contribute to the maintenance of preserved information content in SI despite afferent input loss.

Our findings are based on the unique phenomenology of phantom sensations, and as such all tested amputees reported experiencing exceptionally vivid phantom sensations, allowing them to voluntarily move each of their phantom fingers. An open question remains whether a relationship exists between the experience of phantom sensations and preserved missing hand topography. Our previous findings, showing that phantom hand movements activate the missing hand territory in individuals experiencing varying levels of phantom sensation vividness (Makin et al., 2013a; 2015) might indicate that this is a general phenomenon. However, we note that these previous studies do not provide information on whether the topographic features underlying the phantom-evoked activity were preserved. Further research is needed to determine whether the preservation of missing hand topography depends on (or gives rise to) the experience of phantom sensations, or whether our finding reflects a fundamental organising principle of the brain that is independent of experience.

The notion of brain reorganisation has posed an unexpected obstacle to recent technological developments of human-technology fusion, i.e. neuroprosthetics. Neuroprosthetics allow patients with sensorimotor impairments (e.g. amputation, paralysis) to directly interface with a robotic limb using cortical signals of the hand. However, the relevant information necessary for intuitive neuroprosthesis control may not be attainable once input is lost and functional reorganisation occurs. By demonstrating persistence of topography despite input loss, our finding could be exploited to develop intuitive fine-grained control of neuroprosthetics (Bensmaia and Miller, 2014) (e.g. requiring representation of individual digits) in disabled populations.

Materials and methods

Participants

The main inclusion criteria for amputees in the study were: (a) unilateral upper limb amputation; (b) vivid kinaesthetic sensations during voluntary movements of each of the five digits of the phantom hand (based on self-report); (c) compatibility with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) safety guidelines. We initially screened 63 unilateral upper-limb amputees, of which 22 met the initial criteria for MRI safety. Eight amputees met our 2nd criterion for vividness of kinaesthetic sensations while volitionally moving each of their phantom digits. Only two of these candidates were approved for undergoing ultra high-field (7 tesla) MRI, based on institute’s standard operating procedures for MRI safety. (Ultra high-field (7T) imaging is a newly emerging technique, and as such most clinical implants (e.g. surgical clips) have not been approved for safety in these devices. Therefore, according to current local guidelines it is unsafe to scan individuals who have previously undergone major surgery (e.g. amputees) without a conclusive surgical history.) These amputees were invited to take part in the main study (see Figure 1—source data 1, 2, for demographic and clinical details). This involved studying movements of the phantom and intact hands, across two scanning sessions (see Supplementary file 1). Two additional inclusion criteria were used for a further investigation of peripheral contributions to the missing hand digit topography: (d) brachial plexus avulsion injury; (e) abolishment of afferent inputs and efferent outputs (based on behavioural and electromyography (EMG) testing, see below for details). Three amputees who met criteria a, b and c also met criterion d. Only one of these candidates also met criterion e and was therefore invited to participate in the additional study (see Figure 1—source data 1, 2). Because this individual was not approved to undergo ultra high-field MRI, he was tested in a single session at standard field MRI. All three amputees included in the study were amputated on their left arm due to a trauma and were right-hand dominant prior to the amputation (based on self report).

In addition, a group of thirteen two-handed control participants with a dominant right hand were recruited to take part in the study (mean age ± s.e.m. = 43 ± 3; three females). All control participants performed tasks with their left (non-dominant) hand as a control for the phantom digit movements, in a single session. A subset of six age- and sex-matched control participants (mean age ± s.e.m. = 50 ± 14; all male) also completed tasks involving right (dominant) hand movements. One of these participants was subsequently excluded from the study due to an atypical functional digit layout of both hands with a reversed representation of the thumb and index finger. Another participant was excluded due to excessive head motion inside the scanner (greater than the functional voxel dimension (1.2 mm) of absolute mean displacement), leading to visible spin history artefact. The resulting subset of four control participants (45 ± 13; all male: C1 – C4) also completed a standard field (3 tesla) version of the study. Participants in this group therefore completed three sessions (two ultra high-field sessions and one standard field session; see Supplementary file 1). Ethical approval was granted by the NHS National Research Ethics service (10/H0707/29) and written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the study.

An additional standard field imaging dataset of 15 control participants and the three amputees specified above was also utilised in the current study. The purpose of this analysis was to explore inter-hemispheric (a)symmetry of the intact hand. The pertinent details relating to this dataset are highlighted in Figure 2—figure supplement 1, and will not be detailed further in the materials and methods section.

Behavioural testing

To investigate topographic mapping in the absence of peripheral input, we screened participants with a brachial plexus avulsion injury. This injury involves the tearing of the nerve from its attachment at the spinal cord, abolishing afferent inputs and efferent outputs to the residual limb. To determine the extent of the brachial plexus avulsion injury, we conducted behavioural testing to measure stump sensitivity. Stimuli ranging from light touch to painful pinpricks were applied on the triceps of the residual arm (stump) and a corresponding site on the intact arm. Participants were blindfolded and asked to detect the stimuli, which were presented at varying inter-stimulus intervals in the following order: air puff, light brush, cotton swab stroke, PinPrick probes (MRC systems) with weights: 8 mN, 64 mN and 512 mN. Each stimulus was presented several times. These stimuli are routinely used for qualitative sensoric testing (QST), a technique used to determine sensitivity to touch and pain (Rolke et al., 2006).

To assess stump muscle activity we used surface electromyography (EMG). We targeted the biceps and triceps of the residual arm, known to show muscle activity during phantom hand movements in amputees (Raffin et al., 2012a).

All three amputees were able to detect each of the stimuli when presented on their intact arm. Only one of the three tested amputees reporting to suffer from brachial plexus avulsion injury was unable to detect any of the stimuli on the residual arm. This participant also did not reveal a change in the EMG channels induced by cued phantom hand movements. This amputee therefore met our requirement for abolishment of afferent inputs and efferent outputs and was included in the standard field (3T) study.

MRI tasks

Digit representation was probed using a visually cued active (motor) task. In an intact sensorimotor system, movement recruits a combination of peripheral receptors encoding a range of somatosensory modalities (e.g. surface and deeper mechanoreceptors; proprioceptors), as well as efference information from the motor system. Using an active task, we have previously shown high consistency of primary somatosensory (S1) digit topography across multiple scanning sessions (Kolasinski et al., 2016).

The present study involved functional MRI (fMRI) paradigms designed to identify the two principles of topographic mapping of the hand (Graziano and Aflalo, 2007), as previously shown using neuroimaging: digit selectivity (Kolasinski et al., 2016) and inter-digit overlap (Ejaz et al., 2015). (1) To identify voxels showing digit preference (digit maps) a travelling wave approach was employed (also known as phase-encoding fMRI) (Wandell et al., 2007). This approach has previously been well-validated for sensory body mapping (Kolasinski et al., 2016; Mancini et al., 2012; Orlov et al., 2010; Sanchez-Panchuelo et al., 2010; Sereno and Huang, 2006; Zeharia et al., 2015). Whereas the travelling wave approach provides detailed information regarding digit preference, the winner-takes-all analysis (see below) makes this approach insensitive to overlapping cortical digit representations. Moreover, as the paradigm involves continuous cycles of digit movements with no interruption of baseline periods, regions activated by multiple digits will be difficult to identify using a standard GLM (Besle et al., 2013). (2) To identify the extent of overlap between representations of individual digits (inter-digit representational similarity) a block design was therefore also employed. Below we describe the experimental design and analysis in further detail.

Participants were presented with five white circles, corresponding to the five digits, shown on a visual display projected into the scanner bore. To cue the participant which digit should be moved, the circle corresponding to this digit changed (i.e. in colour or by flashing). Participants were instructed to perform individual digit movements with either their intact or phantom hand. Volitional (and not necessarily painful) movement of a phantom hand elicits both central (Makin et al., 2013a; Raffin et al., 2012bMakin et al., 2015Reilly and Sirigu, 2008) and peripheral motor signals (Reilly et al., 2006Raffin et al., 2012a; Reilly and Sirigu, 2008) that are different from those found during imagined movements. It was clearly stated to the amputees that they were required to perform actual movements with their phantom digits (i.e. try and move the digits of the missing hand), rather than motor imagery. To ensure good understanding of these instructions, the amputees were asked to demonstrate to the experimenter outside the scanner the extent of volitional movement carried out in each of their phantom digits, by mirroring each movement onto their intact hand. For the below elbow amputee, stump muscles were palpated by the experimenter outside the scanner to verify that actual movements were executed during movement of the phantom digits.

To compare between the amputees and controls, the phantom (left) hand was matched to the non-dominant (left) hand of control participants, and the intact (right) hand was matched to the (right) dominant hand of controls. Below we describe the parameters used for the main (ultra high-field) study. Adjusted parameters used for the standard field control experiment are mentioned when relevant.

Missing hand unimanual map

The traveling wave paradigm, as detailed in Figure 1—figure supplement 1, involved unimanual digit movements in a set sequence. The task consisted of blocks of 9 s in which the participant was instructed to move one digit. Each digit block was followed by a subsequent block of a neighbouring digit, and repeated as follows: The forward sequence cycled through the movement blocks for digits: D1-D2-D3-D4-D5. The backward sequence cycled through the movement blocks in a reverse of the forward sequence (D5-D4-D3-D2-D1, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). A sequence was repeated 8 times per run, with a duration of 6 min and 25 s. A forward and backward sequence was employed in separate runs. Each of the forward and backward runs was repeated twice, with a total duration of 25 min and 40 s.

When one of the white circles on the screen turned red, participants performed self-paced flexion and extension phantom (or left hand) digit movements with the digit corresponding to that circle, for the duration of the block (indicated by the presence of the red circle). The leftmost circle corresponded to D5 and the rightmost circle corresponded to D1 of the phantom (or left) hand. Amputees were instructed to move their phantom digits at a comfortable pace. Controls were instructed to perform slow movements, roughly corresponding to the pace and range of movement reported by the amputees (3-4 s per flexion and extension movement). Participants practiced the movements extensively with an experimenter outside the scanner prior to the scan.

Missing hand inter-digit overlap

To assess inter-digit representational similarity of the missing hand, a block design was also employed. This task involved individual digit movement blocks for each of the five digits, as well as a no movement (rest) condition. Each of these 6 conditions was repeated 7 times in a counterbalanced order and each block lasted 12 s, with a total duration of 8 min and 24 s. Participants were instructed to perform self-paced individual digit movements when the circle corresponding to this digit changed in colour, as described above. All five circles remaining white, and a brief flash of the word ‘Rest’ indicated the rest condition.

Bimanual digit map

To replicate the missing hand maps using a separate dataset and additionally test whether topographic representation of the missing hand in amputees was independent of the intact hand, we asked our participants to perform a bimanual travelling wave task. Participants were provided with the visual display of five white circles (as described above) and were instructed to engage both hands in paired simultaneous digit movements when the relevant circle turned red, resulting in reversed digit movement cycles between the two hands. The leftmost circle corresponded to D5 of the left hand and D1 of the right hand; the second left circle corresponded to D4 of the left hand and D2 of the right hand; etc. (Figure 2A). A forward and backward sequence was used in separate runs. Each of the forward and backward runs was repeated twice, for a total duration of 25 min and 40 s.

MRI acquisition

Ultra high-field

Ultra high-field fMRI data was acquired using a Siemens 7 tesla (7T) Magnetom system with a 32-channel head coil. Task fMRI data was acquired using a limited field of view (FOV), with 19–22 true axial slices centred on the anatomical location of the hand knob (Yousry et al., 1997) in the central sulcus bilaterally. The following acquisition parameters were used: sequence: multislice gradient echo EPI, TR: 1500 ms, TE: 25 ms, flip angle: 90°, GRAPPA factor: 2. The spatial resolution was 1.2 mm isotropic.

To improve image registration, a whole brain and a partial field of view single volume high-saturation EPI image were acquired with the same slice positioning as the task fMRI. Anatomical T1-weighed scans, used for surface projection, were acquired using a 3 tesla (3T) system when available. For control participants 1, 5, 6, and 11 a 7T whole-brain T1-weighted image was acquired.

Standard field

Standard field MRI images were acquired using a 3T Verio MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil. A multiband T2*-weighted pulse sequence with an acceleration factor of 6 was used (Moeller et al., 2010; Uğurbil et al., 2013). This provided the opportunity to acquire data with increased spatial (2 mm isotropic) and temporal (TR: 1300 ms) resolution than available with standard EPI sequences. The following acquisition parameters were used: TE: 40 ms; flip angle: 66°, 72 transversal slices. A high-saturation first volume of each acquired multiband run was collected for registration purposes. Field-maps were acquired for field unwarping.

MRI analysis

MRI analysis was implemented using tools from FSL and Connectome Workbench software (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl; http://www.humanconnectome.org) (Jenkinson et al., 2012; Woolrich et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2004) in combination with in house scripts developed using Matlab (version 8.4, R2014b). Cortical surface reconstructions, used for visualisation of the fMRI results, were produced using FreeSurfer (http://freesurfer.net) (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 2001).

MRI preprocessing

Common pre-processing steps for fMRI data were applied to each individual run in native (three dimensional, 3D) space, using FSL’s Expert Analysis Tool FEAT (v6.00; fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki). The following steps were included: Motion correction using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002), brain extraction using automated brain extraction tool BET (Smith, 2002), spatial smoothing using a 1.5 mm FWHM (full width at half maximum) Gaussian kernel for the ultra high-field scans and 2 mm FWHM for the standard field scans, and high pass temporal filtering with a cut-off of 100 s. All BOLD EPI data were assessed for excessive motion using motion estimate outputs from MCFLIRT: ultra high-field functional data from one participant exhibited greater than 1.2 mm (functional voxel size) of absolute mean displacement and was excluded from all further analysis.

Image registration

Ultra high-field

Image registration was accomplished using FLIRT (FMRIB’s linear image registration tool) (Jenkinson et al., 2002; Jenkinson and Smith, 2001). To ensure good registration of the partial-FOV functional data to the anatomical image, image registration was carried out in individual, visually inspected, steps.

First, the task fMRI data from each run was registered to a partial-FOV single volume high-saturation EPI image, acquired at the first session. This partial-FOV image was registered to a whole brain FOV single volume high-saturation EPI image. The whole-brain image was then registered to the T1-weighted image; initially using the mutual information cost function (6 degrees of freedom), and then optimised using boundary-based registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009) (6 degrees of freedom, FMRIB's Automated Segmentation Tool (FAST) for white matter segmentation, no search). For two participants (C4 and C11), manual alignment was used to register the single volume partial-FOV high-saturation EPI image to the structural white matter and pial surfaces using blink comparison as implemented in Freeview.

Standard field

Functional and anatomical images were aligned using a similar pipeline as described above. Each individual run was first co-registered to the high-saturation first volume of the acquired multiband run, and then to the T1-weighted image. In addition, individual field-maps and field-map based unwarping of the multiband images were included to reduce spatial distortions and additionally improve co-registration.

Travelling wave analysis

The travelling wave runs were analysed for each individual participant in native (3D) space, using a cross-correlation approach previously applied in retinotopy (Engel et al., 1997). The approach is based on continuous presentation of stimuli in a set cycle that are expected to result in neighbouring cortical representations (e.g. rotating wedge in the visual field; sequential digit stimulation in a set cycle). It is designed to capture voxels showing increased response to one condition, above and beyond all other conditions (in our case, preference for a specific digit). Beyond preference, this technique also provides a powerful tool for capturing the smooth progression of adjacent representations that are typical for topographic maps. It is therefore considered a preferable technique for capturing topographic representations (Wandell et al., 2007). With respect to somatotopy, the travelling wave approach (or the homologous phase-encoding approach) has previously been validated against blocked and event-related fMRI paradigms (Kolasinski et al., 2016; Orlov et al., 2010; Sanchez-Panchuelo et al., 2010; Besle et al., 2013).

We have recently utilized the traveling wave approach, in combination with ultra high-field fMRI to demonstrate highly reproducible maps of individual digits in S1 (Kolasinski et al., 2016). In the current study, we closely followed these previously validated experimental procedures, as described in Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Participants moved individual digits in a set cycle (see the fMRI tasks section above). A reference model was generated using a gamma-HRF convolved boxcar function, while taking into account the hemodynamic delay. The model was constructed using a 9 s ‘on’ (the duration of a single digit movement) and 36 s ‘off’ (the period of movement of all other digits), accounting for a single 45 s cycle (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). This cycle was repeated 8 times to reflect the full run duration. The reference model was systematically shifted in time to model activity throughout the full movement cycle. Because the runs were acquired using a TR of 1.5 s, the model was shifted 30 times by 1 lag to account for the full 45 s cycle of movement.

For each individual voxel, each of the 30 reference models was correlated with the preprocessed BOLD signal time course to estimate cross-correlation values. The resulting r-values were standardised using the Fisher’s r-to-z transformation. By plotting these standardised r-values as a function of the lag, tuning curves can be created, and the optimal fit for each voxel can be inferred (Figure 1—figure supplement 1C).

The travelling wave approach uses a set cycle, and as such could be susceptible to order-related biases resulting from the sluggish hemodynamic response. For this reason, the order of the cycle was varied between forward (D1-D2-D3-D4-D5) and backward (D5-D4-D3-D2-D1) in different runs (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). To average across the four runs, lags were initially assigned to each of the five digits (six lags per digit). Within each voxel, the r-values corresponding to each digit were averaged, resulting in five r-values, corresponding to each of the digits for a given run. The digit-specific r-values were then averaged across the forward and backward runs on a voxel-by-voxel basis. A winner-take-all approach was applied to produce maps in which each voxel was assigned exclusively to one individual digit, providing us with digit specificity.

To visualise the gradient of progression across digits, lag-specific maps were also produced. For each backward run, the resulting r-values from the cross-correlation analysis were standardised and time-reversed. Forward and backward runs were averaged, on a lag-by-lag basis. To construct a gradient map, a winner-take-all approach was used across all 30 lags, in which each voxel was assigned exclusively to one individual lag.

Cortical surface projections were constructed from T1-weighted images. The digit and gradient maps were registered to structural space and projected to two-dimensional surface space using a cortical ribbon mapping method. To account for multiple comparisons, thresholding was implemented on the surface using the false discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini, 1995), calculated for each digit individually. This approach doesn't take into consideration the neighbourhood relationships of the voxels, and therefore doesn't force cluster patterns. The thresholded maps were set at a false detection criterion of q<0.05 based on the native (3D) values. The FDR thresholded digit-specific clusters were overlaid into a single hand map. Within the resulting five-digit map, we used a 30-lag colour code to visualise the gradient of progression across digits (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D).

For the standard field digit maps, the same procedures were used, with the following exceptions. Due to the difference in temporal resolution (TR = 1.3 s), each digit block lasted 9.1 s, resulting in slightly longer movement cycles (45.5 s). 35 HRF reference models were constructed (compatible with 35 lags), and a more conservative FDR criterion (q<0.01) was selected, due to a reduced signal/noise ratio.

Split-half consistency

To quantify consistency in digit preference, as identified using the travelling wave task, we split the data used to construct the digit preference maps, and compared the spatial correspondence between digit selective clusters. Split-half consistency of the digit-specific clusters was calculated using the Dice coefficient (Kolasinski et al., 2016; Dice, 1945). The Dice coefficient varies from 0 (no spatial correspondence between digit representations) to 1 (perfect spatial correspondence between digit representations). Where A and B are the areas of two digit representations, the Dice Coefficient is expressed as:

2×|AB||A|+|B|

Each digit’s winner-take-all map was minimally thresholded on the cortical surface (Z>2). Spatial correspondence was calculated in S1 (as defined by FreeSurfer) between each possible digit pair across the split-halves of the unimanual digit maps (Figure 1, Figure 1—figure supplement 3). The first forward and backward runs were combined to provide the first digit-specific clusters, and the second forward and backward runs were combined to form the second digit-specific clusters. By doing so, we established a split-half consistency measure for the travelling wave maps of the phantom (or left) hand and intact (or right) hand separately. One control participant was discarded from the subsequent analysis, as his mean spatial correspondence values (across all possible digit combinations) fell outside two standard deviations from the control group mean.

A benchmark for digit selectivity is greater split-half spatial correspondence across 'same' versus 'different' digits. Mean 'same'-digit values (across all five 'same'-digit pairs) were greater than mean 'different'-digit values (across all twenty 'different'-digit pairs) in each of the participants. The 'same'-digit spatial correspondence values in the two amputees (0.30 for the above and below elbow amputees, average across all 'same'-digit pairings) were smaller than those observed in the control participants (0.57 on average across all control participants; Figure 1D), suggesting lower consistency of digit selectivity in amputees. To further assess digit selectivity in the amputees, the 'different'-digit distribution of Dice spatial correspondence values was estimated using a bootstrapping procedure (5000 iterations) for each individual participant. 'Same'-digit values were averaged for each participant, and compared against the 95% confidence interval (CI) on a subject-by-subject basis. For all participants (amputees and controls), the 'same'-digit value fell well outside the range of spatial correspondence seen between 'different' digits. This indicates that the consistency shown for digit-selective clusters was greater than expected by chance in all participants. Next, the mean 'same'-digit values in the two amputees were compared to the 'different'-digit spatial correspondence distribution of each of the controls. 'Same'-digit spatial correspondence values in both amputees fell outside the 'different'-digit spatial correspondence distribution of each of the controls (CI: 0.03–0.08 on average). This was also confirmed by comparing the mean spatial correspondence between 'same'-digit values for each of the amputees and 'different'-digit values of the controls (two-tailed Crawford and Howell t-test [Corballis, 2009]; t(9) = 14.24, p<0.001 and t(9) = 14.31, p<0.001 for above and below elbow amputees respectively). This indicated that although reduced, the digit maps of the amputees were consistent, even relative to controls.

In addition we aimed to compare between the unimanual and bimanual digit maps within the missing hand hemisphere (Figure 2E), providing us with an estimate of consistency when accounting for potential contributions of the intact hand. The 'different'-digit distribution of Dice spatial correspondence values between the unimanual and bimanual maps was estimated using a bootstrapping procedure (5000 iterations) for each individual participant. 'Same'-digit spatial correspondence values (mean across all 'same' digits) in both amputees (0.49 and 0.40 for above and below elbow amputees respectively) fell outside the 'different'-digit spatial correspondence distribution. This was observed both compared to their own 'different'-digit distribution (CI: 0.03–10 for both above and below elbow amputees) and each of the controls’ 'different'-digit distributions (CI: 0.03–0.13 on average). This was also confirmed by comparing the mean spatial correspondence between 'same'-digit values for each of the amputees and 'different'-digit values of the controls (two-tailed Crawford and Howell t-test [Corballis, 2009]; t(3) = 29.14, p<0.001 and t(3) = 22.57, p<0.001 for above and below elbow amputees respectively).

Inter-digit representational similarity analysis

We used an fMRI pattern component approach to identify the extent to which representations of the different digits overlapped with each other, while considering multivoxel patterns underlying the S1 digit maps. Voxels underlying the digit preference map surrounding the anatomical hand knob were combined to form a region of interest (ROI) in native space. The multivoxel pattern-component model is described in detail in (Diedrichsen et al., 2011) and implemented in http://www.icn.ucl.ac.uk/motorcontrol/imaging/multivariate_analysis.html. This method allows deriving unbiased estimates of the true correlations between the underlying activation patterns for each condition.

In brief, a generative model is created that assumes the observed patterns are associated with a set of underlying pattern components that relate to the different experimental conditions or noise. Activity patterns are decomposed within the ROI into a common (noise) component that is shared between all trials and one specific component for each of the five digits. By estimating the variability (or strength) of the similarity between the latter components, the inter-digit representational overlap in the ROI is revealed. Figure 3 and Figure 3—figure supplement 1 (Figure 4D for standard field) show the resulting matrices, demonstrating the level of representational similarity (or overlap) between each digit pair. The topographic inter-digit pattern is characterised by increased overlap between neighbouring digits. Furthermore, digits showing increased synergies in daily life (e.g. digits 3 and 4) tend to show greater representational overlap than digits that are more independent during hand function (e.g. digits 1 and 2) (Ejaz et al., 2015). The resulting inter-digit representational overlap pattern therefore provides a detailed 'fingerprint' of hand representation. We initially assessed overall (mean) overlap within the matrix of each participant, by averaging all unique inter-digit similarity r-values (across the 10 cells of the matrix, while excluding the diagonal). On average, the amputees showed higher inter-digit overlap (0.55 and 0.54 for above and below elbow amputees, respectively) than the control group (CI: 0.24–0.41). To investigate whether the missing hand inter-digit overlap pattern was comparable to normal hand representation, as found in controls, we generated the distribution (and 95% confidence intervals) of correlation across the control population. For this purpose, inter-digit overlap patterns (i.e. the cells in the similarity matrices) were correlated across pairs of controls, using a bootstrapping approach (10,000 iterations) and a Spearman test. One control participant was discarded from the subsequent analysis, as his mean Spearman correlation value (with all other controls) fell outside two standard deviations from the control group mean. (Note that the exclusion of the outlier participant did not affect the outcome of this analysis.) Next, the same procedure was carried out between each amputee and each of the controls’ inter-digit overlap patterns. By plotting the average amputee-control Spearman rho values and confidence intervals against the confidence interval of the controls-to-controls correlation distribution, we were able to show that the amputees’ inter-digit representational overlap pattern fell within the normal range of the controls. There was no significant difference between the amputees-to-controls average Spearman correlation and the control-to-control correlations also when using more conventional statistics (two-tailed Crawford and Howell t-test [Corballis, 2009]; t(9) = −1.10, p=0.30 and t(9) = 0.31, p=0.76 for above and below elbow amputees respectively).

Because the pattern component approach implemented here is minimally affected by differences in noise amplitude resulting from different scanners and acquisition parameters (Diedrichsen et al., 2011), we repeated the same analysis, using the brachial plexus amputees’ data and the control participants’ data (ultra high-field MRI). The average correlation of this amputee’s inter-digit overlap pattern with each of the controls’ inter-digit overlap patterns (0.76), fell within the controls-to-controls correlation range. There was no significant difference between the amputee-to-controls average Spearman correlation and the control-to-control correlations (two-tailed Crawford and Howell t-test [Corballis, 2009]; t(9) = 0.14, p=0.89)

We also calculated a Bayesian t-test to compare between the controls-to-controls distribution and each of the amputee-to-controls distributions using JASP (Morey; Love; Rouder et al., 2009). The Cauchy prior width was set at 0.707 (default). Based on the well accepted criterion of Bayes factor smaller than 1/3 (Wetzels et al., 2011; Dienes et al., 2014) our findings support the null hypothesis (amputees’ inter-digit similarity pattern is not different from controls) for the below elbow and brachial plexus amputee (Bayes factor = 0.23 and 0.28 respectively). For the above elbow amputee the evidence was inconclusive (Bayes factor = 2.94). Note that this criterion for Bayesian factor is considered as moderate (though positive) evidence (Kass et al., 1995).

Acknowledgements

The study was funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society. SK is supported by the UK Medical Research Council and Merton College, Oxford. JK holds a Stevenson Junior Research Fellowship at University College, Oxford. SJ is supported by the UK Medical Research Council (MR/L009013/1). CFB is supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO-Vidi 864-12-003) and gratefully acknowledges funding from the Wellcome Trust UK Strategic Award (098369/Z/12/Z). IT is supported by the following: Wellcome Trust Strategic Award and NIHR Oxford Biomedical Research centre. HJB is a Wellcome Trust Principal Research Fellow (110027/Z/15/Z). TRM holds a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship jointly funded by the Wellcome Trust and the Royal Society (104128/Z/14/Z). We thank our participants for taking part in the study. We thank Devin Terhune and Naveed Ejaz for advice on analysis and Tim Vogels, Paul Matthews, Jody Culham, Tim Behrens and Holly Bridge for comments on the manuscript.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Merton College, University of Oxford Graduate School Studentship to Sanne Kikkert.

  • Medical Research Council Graduate School Studentship to Sanne Kikkert.

  • University College, Oxford Stevenson Junior Research Fellowship to James Kolasinski.

  • Medical Research Council MR/L009013/1 to Saad Jbabdi.

  • Wellcome Trust Strategic Award to Irene Tracey.

  • NIHR Oxford Biomedical Research Centre to Irene Tracey.

  • Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek NWO-Vidi 864-12-003 to Christian F Beckmann.

  • Wellcome Trust UK Strategic Award, 098369/Z/12/Z to Christian F Beckmann.

  • Wellcome Trust Principal Research Fellow, 110027/Z/15/Z to Heidi Johansen-Berg.

  • Royal Society Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, 104128/Z/14/Z to Tamar R Makin.

  • Wellcome Trust Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, 104128/Z/14/Z to Tamar R Makin.

Additional information

Competing interests

HJ-B: Reviewing editor, eLife.

The other authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

SK, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

JK, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

SJ, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

IT, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

CFB, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

HJ-B, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

TRM, Conception and design, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article.

Ethics

Human subjects: Ethical approval was granted by the NHS National Research Ethics service (10/H0707/29) and written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to the study.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Runs acquired for each participant. .

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.014

elife-15292-supp1.docx (54KB, docx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.15292.014

References

  1. Baseler HA, Gouws A, Haak KV, Racey C, Crossland MD, Tufail A, Rubin GS, Cornelissen FW, Morland AB. Large-scale remapping of visual cortex is absent in adult humans with macular degeneration. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:649–655. doi: 10.1038/nn.2793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. 1995;57:289–300. doi: 10.2307/2346101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bensmaia SJ, Miller LE. Restoring sensorimotor function through intracortical interfaces: progress and looming challenges. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2014;15:313–325. doi: 10.1038/nrn3724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Besle J, Sánchez-Panchuelo RM, Bowtell R, Francis S, Schluppeck D. Single-subject fMRI mapping at 7 T of the representation of fingertips in S1: a comparison of event-related and phase-encoding designs. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;109:2293–2305. doi: 10.1152/jn.00499.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chand P, Jain N. Intracortical and thalamocortical connections of the hand and face representations in somatosensory area 3b of macaque monkeys and effects of chronic spinal cord injuries. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:13475–13486. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2069-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Corballis MC. Comparing a single case with a control sample: refinements and extensions. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:2687–2689. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. NeuroImage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Dice LR. Measures of the amount of ecologic association between species. Ecology. 1945;26:297–302. doi: 10.2307/1932409. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Diedrichsen J, Ridgway GR, Friston KJ, Wiestler T. Comparing the similarity and spatial structure of neural representations: a pattern-component model. NeuroImage. 2011;55:1665–1678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dienes Z. Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:1–17. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ejaz N, Hamada M, Diedrichsen J. Hand use predicts the structure of representations in sensorimotor cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2015;18:1034–1040. doi: 10.1038/nn.4038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Engel SA, Glover GH, Wandell BA. Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cerebral Cortex. 1997;7:181–192. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fischl B, Liu A, Dale AM. Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. 2001;20:70–80. doi: 10.1109/42.906426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Flor H, Nikolajsen L, Staehelin Jensen T. Phantom limb pain: a case of maladaptive CNS plasticity? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7:873–881. doi: 10.1038/nrn1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Franklin DW, Wolpert DM. Computational mechanisms of sensorimotor control. Neuron. 2011;72:425–442. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Graziano MS, Aflalo TN. Mapping behavioral repertoire onto the cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:239–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage. 2009;48:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Jain N, Catania KC, Kaas JH. A histologically visible representation of the fingers and palm in primate area 3b and its immutability following long-term deafferentations. Cerebral Cortex. 1998;8:227–236. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.3.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Jain N, Qi HX, Collins CE, Kaas JH. Large-scale reorganization in the somatosensory cortex and thalamus after sensory loss in macaque monkeys. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:11042–11060. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2334-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage. 2002;17:825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical Image Analysis. 2001;5:143–156. doi: 10.1016/S1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kambi N, Halder P, Rajan R, Arora V, Chand P, Arora M, Jain N. Large-scale reorganization of the somatosensory cortex following spinal cord injuries is due to brainstem plasticity. Nature Communications. 2014;5:e15292. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kass RE, Raftery AE, Factors B. Bayes Factors. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1995;90:773–795. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kolasinski J, Makin TR, Jbabdi S, Clare S, Stagg CJ, Johansen-Berg H. Investigating the stability of fine-grain digit somatotopy in individual human participants. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:1113–1127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1742-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. London BM, Miller LE. Responses of somatosensory area 2 neurons to actively and passively generated limb movements. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2013;109:1505–1513. doi: 10.1152/jn.00372.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Love J, Selker R, Marsman M, Jamil T, Dropmann D, Verhagen AJ. JASP. v 0.7.1 Computer Software; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  28. Makin TR, Cramer AO, Scholz J, Hahamy A, Henderson Slater D, Tracey I, Johansen-Berg H. Deprivation-related and use-dependent plasticity go hand in hand. eLife. 2013b;2:1–15. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Makin TR, Scholz J, Filippini N, Henderson Slater D, Tracey I, Johansen-Berg H. Phantom pain is associated with preserved structure and function in the former hand area. Nature Communications. 2013a;4:e15292. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Makin TR, Scholz J, Henderson Slater D, Johansen-Berg H, Tracey I. Reassessing cortical reorganization in the primary sensorimotor cortex following arm amputation. Brain. 2015;138:2140–2146. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mancini F, Haggard P, Iannetti GD, Longo MR, Sereno MI. Fine-grained nociceptive maps in primary somatosensory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:17155–17162. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3059-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Merzenich MM, Nelson RJ, Stryker MP, Cynader MS, Schoppmann A, Zook JM. Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1984;224:591–605. doi: 10.1002/cne.902240408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Moeller S, Yacoub E, Olman CA, Auerbach E, Strupp J, Harel N, Uğurbil K. Multiband multislice GE-EPI at 7 tesla, with 16-fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial and temporal whole-brain fMRI. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2010;63:1144–1153. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Morey RD, Rouder JN. Bayes Factor. v 0.9.11-3 Computer software; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  35. Nordin M, Nyström B, Wallin U, Hagbarth KE. Ectopic sensory discharges and paresthesiae in patients with disorders of peripheral nerves, dorsal roots and dorsal columns. Pain. 1984;20:231–245. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(84)90013-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nyström B, Hagbarth KE. Microelectrode recordings from transected nerves in amputees with phantom limb pain. Neuroscience Letters. 1981;27:211–216. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(81)90270-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Orlov T, Makin TR, Zohary E. Topographic representation of the human body in the occipitotemporal cortex. Neuron. 2010;68:586–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Philip BA, Frey SH. Compensatory changes accompanying chronic forced use of the nondominant hand by unilateral amputees. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:3622–3631. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3770-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pons TP, Garraghty PE, Ommaya AK, Kaas JH, Taub E, Mishkin M. Massive cortical reorganization after sensory deafferentation in adult macaques. Science. 1991;252:1857–1860. doi: 10.1126/science.1843843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raffin E, Giraux P, Reilly KT. The moving phantom: motor execution or motor imagery? Cortex. 2012a;48:746–757. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Raffin E, Mattout J, Reilly KT, Giraux P. Disentangling motor execution from motor imagery with the phantom limb. Brain. 2012b;135:582–595. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Raffin E, Richard N, Giraux P, Reilly KT. Primary motor cortex changes after amputation correlate with phantom limb pain and the ability to move the phantom limb. NeuroImage. 2016;130:134–144. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Reilly KT, Mercier C, Schieber MH, Sirigu A. Persistent hand motor commands in the amputees' brain. Brain. 2006;129:2211–2223. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Reilly KT, Sirigu A. The motor cortex and its role in phantom limb phenomena. The Neuroscientist. 2008;14:195–202. doi: 10.1177/1073858407309466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rolke R, Magerl W, Campbell KA, Schalber C, Caspari S, Birklein F, Treede RD. Quantitative sensory testing: a comprehensive protocol for clinical trials. European Journal of Pain. 2006;10:77–88. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rouder JN, Speckman PL, Sun D, Morey RD, Iverson G. Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2009;16:225–237. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sanchez-Panchuelo RM, Francis S, Bowtell R, Schluppeck D. Mapping human somatosensory cortex in individual subjects with 7T functional MRI. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;103:2544–2556. doi: 10.1152/jn.01017.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Sereno MI, Huang RS. A human parietal face area contains aligned head-centered visual and tactile maps. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:1337–1343. doi: 10.1038/nn1777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Smirnakis SM, Brewer AA, Schmid MC, Tolias AS, Schüz A, Augath M, Inhoffen W, Wandell BA, Logothetis NK. Lack of long-term cortical reorganization after macaque retinal lesions. Nature. 2005;435:300–307. doi: 10.1038/nature03495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23 Suppl 1:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Human Brain Mapping. 2002;17:143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Uğurbil K, Xu J, Auerbach EJ, Moeller S, Vu AT, Duarte-Carvajalino JM, Lenglet C, Wu X, Schmitter S, Van de Moortele PF, Strupp J, Sapiro G, De Martino F, Wang D, Harel N, Garwood M, Chen L, Feinberg DA, Smith SM, Miller KL, Sotiropoulos SN, Jbabdi S, Andersson JL, Behrens TE, Glasser MF, Van Essen DC, Yacoub E, WU-Minn HCP Consortium Pushing spatial and temporal resolution for functional and diffusion MRI in the human connectome project. NeuroImage. 2013;80:80–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Vaso A, Adahan HM, Gjika A, Zahaj S, Zhurda T, Vyshka G, Devor M. Peripheral nervous system origin of phantom limb pain. Pain. 2014;155:1384–1391. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA. Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron. 2007;56:366–383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Wetzels R, Matzke D, Lee MD, Rouder JN, Iverson GJ, Wagenmakers EJ. Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: An empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6:291–298. doi: 10.1177/1745691611406923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T, Beckmann C, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. NeuroImage. 2009;45:S173–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Yousry TA, Schmid UD, Alkadhi H, Schmidt D, Peraud A, Buettner A, Winkler P. Localization of the motor hand area to a knob on the precentral gyrus. A new landmark. Brain. 1997;120:141–157. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.1.141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Zeharia N, Hertz U, Flash T, Amedi A. New whole-body sensory-motor gradients revealed using phase-locked analysis and verified using multivoxel pattern analysis and functional connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:2845–2859. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4246-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2016 Aug 23;5:e15292. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15292.015

Decision letter

Editor: Klaas Enno Stephan1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Revealing the neural fingerprints of a missing hand" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Sabine Kastner as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in the review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Klaas Enno Stephan (Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1) and Brigitte Röder (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary of manuscript:

This paper revisits the question to what degree sensory representations in cortex are plastic following the loss of certain sensory inputs. Two amputees who lost their hands were studied with 7 Tesla fMRI, using a paradigm that cued phantom digit movements. An additional amputee with brachial plexus avulsion was investigated using 3 Tesla fMRI. Healthy controls served as comparison. The authors report that a representation of the missing hand's fingers can be detected in the primary somatosensory cortex of amputees, even though their loss of limb dates back several decades.

Summary of reviews:

All three reviewers were positively impressed by the paper and thought that it contributes some very interesting findings would usefully inform our understanding of plasticity of sensory representations in cortex. They agreed that the experimental design and the statistical analyses were of excellent quality. However, they also had a number of comments and suggestions which would need to be addressed in a revision of your paper.

The policy of the journal is to provide you with a single set of comments which reflect the consensus view amongst reviewers. These comments can be found below and are divided into essential or Major Issues, which must be addressed convincingly, and Minor Issues. We hope that you will find these comments helpful to further improve the paper.

Essential revisions:

1) All reviewers felt that the conclusions were too strong – both considering the strength of the statistical results and with regard to their neuroscientific framing – and should be toned down and nuanced. In brief, the reviewers think it would be appropriate to conclude that hand representations are still detectable decades after amputation but the impression should be avoided that amputees cannot be distinguished from controls. This is detailed by the next two points.

2) With regard to statistics, the results of the analyses are more mixed and less decisive than the conclusions suggest. Concerning digit selectivity, there is a qualitative correspondence of selectivity maps between amputees and controls, but the selectivity is significantly reduced in amputees. Concerning the second part of the analysis, interdigit overlap, some of the tests also indicate significant differences between amputees and controls. Furthermore, the results of the Bayesian analysis are relatively weak; this should be acknowledged more clearly. Two of the Bayes factors reported are in favour of the authors' hypothesis of no difference between groups, but are just very slightly above the conventional threshold for "positive evidence" (BF <0.33 or >3, according to the Kass & Raftery classification); a third Bayes factor reported goes against their hypothesis, but marginally fails to reach the positive evidence threshold. Notably, in Bayesian statistics, the positive evidence threshold is not usually considered sufficient to disambiguate hypotheses decisively; for this, one would typically require a Bayes factor of at least 20.

3) Concerning the neuroscientific aspects, the reviewers thought that the historical framing and interpretation of the findings should be approved. They point out that, had these findings been published 30 years ago, they would not have been regarded as surprising. It was not before the work of M. Merzenich in 1980 that evidence became available against the "hard-wired dogma" of adult brains. However, this has not been fully replaced with a "soft-wired dogma" since then; rather, it is widely recognised that both aspects coexist. While it has become clear that the brain has considerable capacity for functional reorganisation and plasticity even in adult life (for example, concerning somatotopy, electrophysiological work has demonstrated that neurons are partially able to acquire new receptive fields after deafferenting a digit etc.), there are hardly claims or data suggesting a full loss of digit somatotopy. By contrast, some authors have claimed that individual digit topography can be detected by touching the face of some amputees. In brief, reorganization and preserved digit somatotopy are not mutually exclusive, but reflect a balance of stability and plasticity in the CNS. The reviewers think that this is exactly what the statistical results reflect (see point 2 above) and find that a more balanced interpretation and discussion is necessary. The present conclusions (including the abstract) oversimplify and are worded too strongly.

4) All patients selected reported "vivid kinaesthetic sensations". This raises the question whether those amputees reporting vivid sensations of their digits are those who maintain somatotopy, while the others do not. In other words, the present results may be specific for a particular subgroup of amputees; this should be discussed.

5) How well did the patients manage to move individual digits, and how can this be assessed, given that this movement was imaginary?

6) The complexity of the possible neurophysiological processes that underlie the fMRI results requires more consideration in an extended discussion, specifically with regard to the possible pathways that might have generated the brain activity observed here. The following three comments may be useful in this regard:

a) Equally as interesting as the preservation of the deafferented glabrous finger map in area 3b and 1, per se, is fact that these maps could be selectively locally activated, presumably by higher level postcentral somatosensory areas and/or by precentral motor areas. Understanding the pathway of activation is further challenged by the differences in the 3 cases presented here. In the below-elbow case, it is likely that portions of the extensor digitorum communis (finger raising) and flexor digitorum profundus (finger curling) muscles as well as their afferent and efferent innervation remain and were activated during the 'imagined' movements, which could probably be better described as 'executed' (muscle) movements. In this case, one might imagine the involvement of M-I to S-I connections, along with all the complexities of the difference between muscles located in the arm versus glabrous skin on the underside of the fingers. But in the above-elbow case, those finger muscles in the arm are gone; although dorsal root ganglion cells formerly innervating glabrous finger skin as well as motorneurons formerly innervating those muscles are likely to have persisted above the injury, and may have made ectopic connections. Finally, the brachial plexus avulsion case would likely have involved damage to the dorsal root ganglia and the spinal cord itself. In that case, the most peripheral source of abnormal neural activity could only have been neurons in the dorsal horn itself.

b) An important piece of information to keep in mind when interpreting these results is that there are several levels of map before we reach the cortex: dorsal root ganglion -> dorsal column nuclei -> ventrobasal nucleus -> S-I, and dorsal root ganglion -> dorsal column nuclei -> ventrolateral nucleus -> M-I cortex. Recent experiments on non-human primates with chronic lesions of the dorsal column subserving the arm have suggested that our initial expectations for sites of plastiticy may not have been correct (for example, P Chand and N Jain (2015) Intracortical and thalamocortical connections of the hand and face representations in somatosensory area 3b of macaque monkeys and effects of chronic spinal cord injuries. Journal of Neuroscience 35:13475-13486.) That study showed that despite the electrophysiological presence of large-scale somatotopic reorganization in 3b, surprisingly, there was no evidence for any marked increase in cortico-cortical connections across the hand-face border in 3b, or any evidence of altered map connections from the hand and face parts of the ventrobasal nucleus to the cortex. So the large-scale expansion of the chin representation into the deafferented hand representation in 3b may actually instead have been driven by the reorganization of the dorsal column nuclei. In the context of the present paper, this suggests that reorganized inputs to cortical maps should strongly be distinguished from the intrinsic organization of the cortical maps themselves, which contrary to expectations, may turn out to be less plastic than more peripheral stations."

c) Another point perhaps worth a sentence in the Discussion is that the experiment performed with the below-the-elbow amputee – having them contract their finger muscles without generating an stimulation of the (missing) glabrous skin surface of the finger is not straightforwardly possible with an intact limb since any activation of muscles that move the fingers will generate some direct somatosensory stimulation, even with the hand held up in the air. However, it might be possible to test this in an intact limb by anesthetizing an intact hand with a wrist block to see if cortical region previously thought to be mainly driven by stimulation of the glabrous surface of the fingers can be directly driven by efference copy (or other) signals.

d) The fact that phantom limb sensations can arise almost immediately during brachial blocks (Gentili ME, Verton C, Kinirons B, Bonnet F. (2002) Clinical perception of phantom limb sensation in patients with brachial plexus block. Eur J Anaesthesiol 19:105-108 also suggests that some 'plasticity' is immediate.

eLife. 2016 Aug 23;5:e15292. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15292.016

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) All reviewers felt that the conclusions were too strong – both considering the strength of the statistical results and with regard to their neuroscientific framing – and should be toned down and nuanced. In brief, the reviewers think it would be appropriate to conclude that hand representations are still detectable decades after amputation but the impression should be avoided that amputees cannot be distinguished from controls. This is detailed by the next two points.

We take the reviewers point that the persistent representation of the missing hand was not entirely matched to the controls’ topography in all tests. For example, results of our analysis indeed suggest that digit selectivity in amputees was weaker than in controls. In the revised manuscript we have reworded the description of our results throughout the text to better reflect our main findings as well as their limitations (please see below for key examples from the Abstract, Impact statement and Discussion). Furthermore, in the revised Discussion we also highlight the extent to which digit representation differs in amputees and controls.

Revised text in the Abstract:

“We show that representation of the missing hand’s individual fingers persists in the primary somatosensory cortex even decades after arm amputation. By demonstrating stable topography despite amputation, our finding questions the extent to which continued sensory input is necessary to maintain organisation in sensory cortex, thereby reopening the question what happens to a cortical territory once its main input is lost.”

Revised text in the Impact statement:

“We show that the functional finger layout of the primary somatosensory cortex, previously thought to depend on experience, remains distinct despite the physical absence of fingers for several decades.”

Revised text in the Discussion of the Main text:

“Together we show that, although the missing hand maps were weaker and noisier than the maps found in controls, the functional digit layout of S1 prevails following arm amputation. Digit topography, previously thought to depend on experience (Ejez, Hamada and Diedrichsen, 2015; Merzenich et al., 1984), was detectable despite decades without organised peripheral inputs associated with normal hand function.”

2) With regard to statistics, the results of the analyses are more mixed and less decisive than the conclusions suggest. Concerning digit selectivity, there is a qualitative correspondence of selectivity maps between amputees and controls, but the selectivity is significantly reduced in amputees. Concerning the second part of the analysis, interdigit overlap, some of the tests also indicate significant differences between amputees and controls. Furthermore, the results of the Bayesian analysis are relatively weak; this should be acknowledged more clearly. Two of the Bayes factors reported are in favour of the authors' hypothesis of no difference between groups, but are just very slightly above the conventional threshold for "positive evidence" (BF <0.33 or >3, according to the Kass & Raftery classification); a third Bayes factor reported goes against their hypothesis, but marginally fails to reach the positive evidence threshold. Notably, in Bayesian statistics, the positive evidence threshold is not usually considered sufficient to disambiguate hypotheses decisively; for this, one would typically require a Bayes factor of at least 20.

As indicated in our response to point 1 above, we accept the reviewers’ reservation. We revised the manuscript to better reflect the outcomes of our various analyses, including those showing differences between the amputees and controls. Rather than stating that the amputees show identical topography as controls, we now state that topography remains detectable in amputees.

For revised text in the Abstract, Impact statement and Discussion of the Main text, see response to Reviewers’ point 1.

The interpretation of the results of our Bayesian analysis has been based on the paper by Zoltan Dienes “Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results” (Dienes, 2014). According to this paper, a Bayes Factor (BF) under 0.33 provides coherent evidence to support the null hypothesis. This criterion is consistent with the results found in two of the amputees. A BF between 0.33 and 3 should be interpreted as ambiguous evidence (neither confirming or rejecting the null hypothesis), as found in the third amputee. This BF criterion is currently used as a standard in the field. For example, in a key paper by Wetzels et al. (Wetzels et al., 2011), a BF greater than 3 (or lower than 0.33) is considered substantial evidence for (or against) the hypothesis. In this paper, a BF of 3 is shown to correspond to a p value of 0.01. In the guidelines by Kass and Raftery (Kass and Raftery, 1995), where a criterion of BF > 20 is suggested as strong evidence for the alternative hypothesis, a BF greater than 3 is still considered positive evidence (thus validating out interpretation for BF < 0.33). Nevertheless, to correspond to the most stringent criteria, we interpret our findings as “moderate evidence” in favour of the null hypothesis in the revised manuscript.

Revised text in the Materials and methods:

“We also calculated a Bayesian t-test to compare between the controls-to-controls distribution and each of the amputee-to-controls distributions using JASP (Love et al; Morey and Rouder; Rouder et al. 2009). The Cauchy prior width was set at 0.707 (default). Based on the well accepted criterion of Bayes factor smaller than 1/3 (56,57) our findings support the null hypothesis (amputees’ inter-digit similarity pattern is not different from controls) for the below elbow and brachial plexus amputee (Bayes factor = 0.23 and 0.28 respectively). For the above elbow amputee the evidence was inconclusive (Bayes factor = 2.94). Note that this criterion for Bayesian factor is considered as moderate (though positive) evidence (Kass and Raffery, 1995).”

3) Concerning the neuroscientific aspects, the reviewers thought that the historical framing and interpretation of the findings should be approved. They point out that, had these findings been published 30 years ago, they would not have been regarded as surprising. It was not before the work of M. Merzenich in 1980 that evidence became available against the "hard-wired dogma" of adult brains. However, this has not been fully replaced with a "soft-wired dogma" since then; rather, it is widely recognised that both aspects coexist. While it has become clear that the brain has considerable capacity for functional reorganisation and plasticity even in adult life (for example, concerning somatotopy, electrophysiological work has demonstrated that neurons are partially able to acquire new receptive fields after deafferenting a digit etc.), there are hardly claims or data suggesting a full loss of digit somatotopy. By contrast, some authors have claimed that individual digit topography can be detected by touching the face of some amputees. In brief, reorganization and preserved digit somatotopy are not mutually exclusive, but reflect a balance of stability and plasticity in the CNS. The reviewers think that this is exactly what the statistical results reflect (see point 2 above) and find that a more balanced interpretation and discussion is necessary. The present conclusions (including the abstract) oversimplify and are worded too strongly.

We fully agree that our results demonstrate that a delicate balance exists between soft- and hard-wiring. We also agree that this view has been prevalent in the field. For example, in the original digit reorganisation paper by Merzenich et al. (Merzenich et al., 1984), the authors suggested that phantom sensations most likely arise from a preserved functional representation of the missing digit – therefore not excluding the potential for persistent organisation co-occurring with reorganisation. However, evidence to support this co-existence of preserved organisation and reorganisation are only now beginning to emerge (see point 7 for further discussion of how the visual community resolves between the hard- and soft-wired dogmas; see also point 6b for further discussion of how reorganisation and preserved representation can co-occur following sensory input loss). We believe that the discrepancy between the theoretical framework, embracing the co-occurrence of preservation and plasticity, and the mainstream evidence in favour of plasticity, is due to methodological limitations. Once the main input is lost to a brain area, scientists resort to studying representations of its cortical neighbours to best characterise the freed-up territory (e.g. adjacent digits, arm or face in primates following digit or arm deafferentation (Merzenich et al., 1984; Pons et al., 1991; Kambi et al., 2014)(1); neighbouring whiskers in rodents following whisker removal (2,3); or peripheral visual representation in humans with foveal visual loss (Baseler et al., 2011)). This approach leaves unexplored the possibility that the original function of the region may be preserved, though latent. Our experimental approach therefore provides a new perspective on the capacity for the brain to maintain hard-wired representation despite a drastic change in inputs. We revised the discussion of the manuscript to better reflect the existing theoretical framework. For example, we discuss previous literature in the visual field suggesting limits of plasticity in the adult human brain (see also point 7) and explain how our research advances on these findings.

Revised text in the Abstract:

“By demonstrating stable topography despite amputation, our finding questions the extent to which continued sensory input is necessary to maintain organisation in sensory cortex, thereby reopening the question what happens to a cortical territory once its main input is lost.”

Revised text in the Discussion of the Main text:

“Conversely, recent research in the visual cortex suggests that reorganisation in the adult brain may be restricted. For example following macular degeneration, the functional representation of the intact visual field was unchanged ((Baseler et al., 2011), see (Smirnakis et al., 2005) for similar results in non-human primates). Common to all these previous studies aiming to characterise reorganisation or the lack thereof, is that the authors probed the cortical neighbours of the area previously responsible for processing the lost input. While this approach is suitable for documenting shifted representation of the cortical neighbours, it leaves unexplored the possibility that the original function of the region deprived of sensory input may be preserved, though latent. Amputees experiencing phantom sensations provide a unique model to study what happens to the deprived cortical territory itself during sensory input loss.”

Further, we fully agree with the reviewers that reorganisation and preserved digit topography are not mutually exclusive. In fact, we believe that the main strength of our results is revealing the latent hard-wired finger maps, despite reorganisation. Two of the three amputees tested in the current study revealed overrepresentation of the intact hand in the missing hand territory, compatible with reorganisation (Figure 2—figure supplement 1; see also point 12). In the revised manuscript we further emphasise and interpret the co-occurrence of reorganisation and preserved missing hand topography in the main text and the Discussion.

Revised text in the Main text:

“This result also demonstrates the independence of the missing hand topography from the representation of the intact hand. Our group and others previously showed that following unilateral arm amputation, the intact hand becomes over-represented in the missing hand territory (Makin et al., 2013; Philip and Frey, 2014; Raffin et al., 2016). Our results therefore confirm that the digit map in the missing hand territory is not driven by an emerging representation of the intact hand. Note, however, that our results do not exclude the possibility for reorganisation in the missing hand territory. As demonstrated in Figure 2—figure supplement 1, this study’s participants showed over-representation of their intact hand in the missing hand territory, indicating that the preserved missing hand topography can co-occur with remapping of body parts.”

Revised text in the Discussion of the Main text:

“How can our finding of preserved S1 topography of a missing hand be allied with the wealth of evidence showing cortical reorganisation in S1 following sensory input loss? (e.g. amputation and spinal cord injury (Merzenich et al., 1984; Pons et al., 1991; Jain et al. 2008); see Figure 1—figure supplement 2 for reorganisation in the current study’s volunteers cohort). Already in their seminal work Merzenich et al. suggested that reorganisation following sensory input loss does not exclude the possibility for simultaneous preservation of the original function of that region (Merzenich et al., 1984). Accordingly, recent structural and functional evidence shows that the capacity for S1 reorganisation is more limited than initially thought, and that instead the functional changes previously observed in S1 following input loss could be attributed to reorganisation in sub-cortical areas in the afferent pathway, principally the brainstem (Jain, Catania and Kaas, 1998; Kambi et al., 2014; Chand and Jain, 2015). In other words, previous findings of massive cortical reorganisation in S1 reflect reorganisation of inputs, rather than reorganisation within S1 itself. This recent evident nicely complements our own finding of SI reorganisation, overlaid on preserved structure and function.”

We decided not to discuss the case studies reporting facial referred sensations (4,5) as potential evidence in favour of the co-occurrence of reorganisation and preserved missing hand representation. This is due to limitations in the original experimental designs (e.g. the studies were not systematically conducted, did not involve control conditions and were not double blind), as well as the qualitative report of the findings (e.g. no statistical quantification was carried out). Indeed, further studies that avoided some of these methodological pitfalls showed that referred sensations could be triggered by touch applied on body parts that are not cortical neighbours of the hand area (e.g. feet, chest and neck), as well as body parts that are contralateral to the missing hand (6, 7). As such, we felt there is insufficient evidence to link referred sensations and SI organisation and/or reorganisation. Nevertheless, we would be happy to discuss these studies should the editor or reviewers feel that this information should be added to the manuscript.

4) All patients selected reported "vivid kinaesthetic sensations". This raises the question whether those amputees reporting vivid sensations of their digits are those who maintain somatotopy, while the others do not. In other words, the present results may be specific for a particular subgroup of amputees; this should be discussed.

We agree that this question warrants further discussion. Unfortunately, the methods used in this study critically rely on the ability to move individual phantom digits in order to trigger the phantom hand representation. We therefore only recruited amputees with strong kinaesthetic sensation of each of the phantom digits. It would have been impossible to run the current study with individuals experiencing no (or partial) phantom hand sensations, as this would violate some of the assumptions underlying the travelling wave analysis. However, we can draw from our previously published findings conducted in a standard field (3 tesla) scanner (using a procedure that allowed us to scan individuals with varying degrees of phantom sensations (Makin et al., 2013; Makin et al., 2015). We showed that 16 of 17 individuals were able to activate the missing hand territory when moving (or attempting to move) their phantom hand. Although these studies were not suited to resolve whether this activity reflected topographic organisation, they hint that persistent representation may be the rule, rather than the exception. Nevertheless, it is still possible that the features of the preserved topography (e.g. overlap across digits, digit preferences) may depend upon (or drive) phantom sensations. In the revised manuscript we highlight our previous finding and discuss the potential causal link between phantom sensations and preserved topography. We are currently running a new study to tackle this important research question and hope that we will be able to shed more light on this interesting question in the future.

Revised text in the Discussion of the Main text:

“Our findings are based on the unique phenomenology of phantom sensations, and as such all tested amputees reported experiencing exceptionally vivid phantom sensations, allowing them to voluntarily move each of their phantom fingers. An open question remains whether a relationship exists between the experience of phantom sensations and preserved missing hand topography. Our previous findings, showing that phantom hand movements activate the missing hand territory in individuals experiencing varying levels of phantom sensation vividness (Makin et al., 2013; Makin et al., 2015) might indicate that this is a general phenomenon. However, we note that these previous studies do not provide information on whether the topographic features underlying the phantom-evoked activity were preserved. Further research is needed to determine whether the preservation of missing hand topography depends on (or gives rise to) the experience of phantom sensations, or whether our finding reflects a fundamental organising principle of the brain that is independent of experience.”

5) How well did the patients manage to move individual digits, and how can this be assessed, given that this movement was imaginary?

First, we would like to clarify the nature of phantom hand movements, with respect to motor imagery. It has previously been shown that phantom movements differ from imagined movement (including imagined movement of the phantom) both in terms of peripheral signals (i.e. electromyography, (Raffin, Giraux and Reilly, 2012)) and central signals (Raffin et al., 2012). In our study we instructed participants to move their phantom digits and ensured that our participants correctly interpreted these instructions (see also below). We believe this dissociation between motor execution and motor imagery to be essential for the interpretation of our findings and therefore clarify this important point in the revised manuscript.

Revised text in the Main text:

“Importantly, phantom movements are distinguishable from imagined movements. This is supported by empirical evidence demonstrating that phantom limb movements elicit both central and peripheral motor signals, that are different from those found during imagined movements (Makin et al., 2013; Reilly et al., 2006; Raffin et all, 2012).”

To ensure the amputees understood the task and were able to perform it well, we asked them to demonstrate (outside the scanner) the extent of volitional movement afforded with each of their phantom digits, by mirroring the phantom movements onto their intact hand. In addition, for the below elbow amputee, the experimenter palpated the stump muscles outside the scanner to verify that actual phantom movements were executed during the task. This procedure is described in detail in the Materials and methods, MRI tasks section. In the revised manuscript we also clarify this in the main text.

Revised text in the Main text:

“To ensure adequate task performance, amputees were asked to demonstrate to the experimenter outside the scanner the extent of volitional movement carried out in each of their phantom digits during the task, by mirroring the phantom movements with their intact hand.”

We also assessed the amputees vividness and difficulty to perform movements with the individual digits of the phantom hand using subjective ratings (Figure 1 —figure supplement 2). In addition to this, we asked the amputees to describe the quality and extent of movement afforded in each of the phantom digits. In the revised manuscript, this information has been added to Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

6) The complexity of the possible neurophysiological processes that underlie the fMRI results requires more consideration in an extended discussion, specifically with regard to the possible pathways that might have generated the brain activity observed here. The following three comments may be useful in this regard:

We would like to thank the reviewers for these interesting, thought provoking, thoughtful and thorough comments. We greatly enjoyed corresponding with them, and believe that the revisions made based on these comments have greatly improved our Discussion.

a) Equally as interesting as the preservation of the deafferented glabrous finger map in area 3b and 1, per se, is fact that these maps could be selectively locally activated, presumably by higher level postcentral somatosensory areas and/or by precentral motor areas. Understanding the pathway of activation is further challenged by the differences in the 3 cases presented here. In the below-elbow case, it is likely that portions of the extensor digitorum communis (finger raising) and flexor digitorum profundus (finger curling) muscles as well as their afferent and efferent innervation remain and were activated during the 'imagined' movements, which could probably be better described as 'executed' (muscle) movements. In this case, one might imagine the involvement of M-I to S-I connections, along with all the complexities of the difference between muscles located in the arm versus glabrous skin on the underside of the fingers. But in the above-elbow case, those finger muscles in the arm are gone; although dorsal root ganglion cells formerly innervating glabrous finger skin as well as motorneurons formerly innervating those muscles are likely to have persisted above the injury, and may have made ectopic connections. Finally, the brachial plexus avulsion case would likely have involved damage to the dorsal root ganglia and the spinal cord itself. In that case, the most peripheral source of abnormal neural activity could only have been neurons in the dorsal horn itself.

The reviewers’ conveyed train of thought is entirely compatible with ours, and we are pleased to include this in the manuscript. In the revised manuscript we included a paragraph explaining the three cases presented in the manuscript and the potential mechanism driving the observed brain activity, given the differences between the three amputation cases.

Revised text in the Discussion of the Main text:

“Which inputs could contribute to the maintenance of the missing hand topography? The variability in the level and nature of amputations in this study’s cohort allows us to consider the potential contribution of the peripheral nervous system in the preservation of missing hand topography. In the below elbow amputee, some forearm muscles normally controlling hand movements are spared and therefore proprioceptive inputs relating to phantom hand movements likely persist (Nystrom and Hagbarth, 1981). In the above elbow amputee, these inputs would be absent, though ectopic firing from the injured nerve (Nystrom and Hagbarty, 1981) or intact dorsal root ganglia could preserve some afferent inputs (as previously shown following peripheral nerve injury (Nordin et al., 1984), see also (Vaso et al., 2014) for related findings). However, in the amputee suffering from brachial plexus avulsion injury, the dorsal root ganglia are damaged, meaning that no such peripheral input should be available. Given the observation of preserved topography in all three cases, it is highly probable that the preserved missing hand maps are not maintained by peripheral input, but rather are driven by processing in the central nervous system itself.”

b) An important piece of information to keep in mind when interpreting these results is that there are several levels of map before we reach the cortex: dorsal root ganglion -> dorsal column nuclei -> ventrobasal nucleus -> S-I, and dorsal root ganglion -> dorsal column nuclei -> ventrolateral nucleus -> M-I cortex. Recent experiments on non-human primates with chronic lesions of the dorsal column subserving the arm have suggested that our initial expectations for sites of plastiticy may not have been correct (for example, P Chand and N Jain (2015) Intracortical and thalamocortical connections of the hand and face representations in somatosensory area 3b of macaque monkeys and effects of chronic spinal cord injuries. Journal of Neuroscience 35:13475-13486.) That study showed that despite the electrophysiological presence of large-scale somatotopic reorganization in 3b, surprisingly, there was no evidence for any marked increase in cortico-cortical connections across the hand-face border in 3b, or any evidence of altered map connections from the hand and face parts of the ventrobasal nucleus to the cortex. So the large-scale expansion of the chin representation into the deafferented hand representation in 3b may actually instead have been driven by the reorganization of the dorsal column nuclei. In the context of the present paper, this suggests that reorganized inputs to cortical maps should strongly be distinguished from the intrinsic organization of the cortical maps themselves, which contrary to expectations, may turn out to be less plastic than more peripheral stations."

We agree that the manuscript could benefit from a mechanistic interpretation, and that the latest evidence for brainstem plasticity potentially provides a compelling framework to resolve our own findings with previous work on reorganisation. Specifically, our results are compatible with the notion that reorganisation may occur at subcortical areas, leaving the inherent organisation of SI relatively intact. We have added a paragraph in the Discussion of the revised manuscript explaining how to put together previous reorganisation studies and our result of preserved functional organisation in SI.

For revised text in the Discussion of the Main text, see response to reviewers’ point 3.

c) Another point perhaps worth a sentence in the Discussion is that the experiment performed with the below-the-elbow amputee – having them contract their finger muscles without generating an stimulation of the (missing) glabrous skin surface of the finger is not straightforwardly possible with an intact limb since any activation of muscles that move the fingers will generate some direct somatosensory stimulation, even with the hand held up in the air. However, it might be possible to test this in an intact limb by anesthetizing an intact hand with a wrist block to see if cortical region previously thought to be mainly driven by stimulation of the glabrous surface of the fingers can be directly driven by efference copy (or other) signals.

To rephrase the reviewers’ comment, it remains to be determined what the neural mechanism driving activation of the preserved missing hand maps is. Key to understanding this is the difference between imagery and actual phantom movements, as highlighted before in point 5. Indeed, while the sensory input is profoundly impacted following amputation, motor signals persist. Our amputees therefore provide a unique model to examine representation in the brain, without any sensory inputs. Since we show digit topography in primary somatosensory cortex in the physical absence of a hand, we believe our finding demonstrates the potential involvement of efference copies in driving these maps. We believe that the missing hand topography elicited in our study is the consequence of corollary discharge: information about descending commands that the motor system provides to the sensory system. In the revised manuscript we elaborate further on the potential role of efference copies in driving the missing hand maps (see also point 13).

Revised text in the Discussion of the Main text:

“What neural signals may be triggering the brain activations subserving the missing hand maps? Given the relatively unimpaired motor system in amputees, it is possible that these representations are driven by motor (efferent) information. The motor system is thought to provide information about its descending commands to the sensory system, by means of efference copy. When efference signals reach the sensory areas, they evoke activity in those areas. The pattern of this corollary discharge could resemble that of the sensory feedback to be expected from the movement (London and Miller, 2013). While predictive signals are fundamental components for current theories of motor control (Franklin and Wolpert, 2011), surprisingly little empirical evidence exists to demonstrate efferent signals in the primates’ S1 hand representation independently of afferent processing (London and Miller, 2013). Our evidence for digit topography in S1 despite the physical absence of a hand suggests the involvement of non-afferent processing in S1. The persistence of efference signals from the motor system could contribute to the maintenance of preserved structure and function in SI despite afferent input loss.”

Applying a sensory nerve block to an intact limb would indeed allow further investigation regarding the role of efference signals in eliciting detailed representation in primary somatosensory cortex. In the revised manuscript we did not elaborate on this interesting issue, as it seems to exceed the focus of the paper.

d) The fact that phantom limb sensations can arise almost immediately during brachial blocks (Gentili ME, Verton C, Kinirons B, Bonnet F. (2002) Clinical perception of phantom limb sensation in patients with brachial plexus block. Eur J Anaesthesiol 19:105-108 also suggests that some 'plasticity' is immediate.

We thank the reviewers for pointing us to this reference (8). We read the paper with great interest, but unfortunately we have to conclude that the authors are misusing the term “phantom sensations”. In the paper, the authors define phantom sensation as the inability to correctly describe the position of a limb following axillary block. This definition is very different from the way phantom sensations are described by amputees. Amputees describe a phantom sensation as the vivid experience that their missing limb is still present, often coinciding with the ability to move the phantom limb. Crucial to this point, amputees are able to describe the exact position of the phantom limb. As such we would prefer not to speculate whether phantom sensations could be induced by nerve blocks.

In our lab we use pharmacological nerve block in healthy controls and we can confirm that the experience that is reported following pharmacological nerve block is different from reports of amputees’ phantom sensations. This is consistent with microneurography studies showing persistent activity in the amputated nerve in amputees (Nystrom and Hagbarth, 1981), which would presumably be abolished during nerve block. In the revised manuscript we did not elaborate on this interesting issue, as it seems to exceed the focus of the paper.

References:

1. Flor H, Elbert T, Knecht S, Wienbruch C, Pantev C, Birbaumer N, et al. Phantom-limb pain as a perceptual correlate of cortical reorganization following arm amputation. Nature. 1995 Jun 8;375(6531):482–4.

2. Feldman DE, Brecht M. Map plasticity in somatosensory cortex. Science. 2005 Nov 4;310(5749):810–5.

3. Margolis DJ, Lütcke H, Schulz K, Haiss F, Weber B, Kügler S, et al. Reorganization of cortical population activity imaged throughout long-term sensory deprivation. Nature Neuroscience. Nature Publishing Group; 2012 Oct 21;15(11):1539–46.

4. Ramachandran VS, Stewart M, Rogers-Ramachandran DC. Perceptual correlates of massive cortical reorganization. Neuroreport. 1992 Jun 30;3(7):583–6.

5. Ramachandran VS. Behavioral and magnetoencephalographic correlates of plasticity in the adult human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1993 Nov 15;90(22):10413–20.

6. Knecht S, Henningsen H, Elbert T, Flor H, Höhling C, Pantev C, et al. Reorganizational and perceptional changes after amputation. Brain. 1996 Aug;119 (Pt 4):1213–9.

7. Grüsser SM, Mühlnickel W, Schaefer M, Villringer K, Christmann C, Koeppe C, et al. Remote activation of referred phantom sensation and cortical reorganization in human upper extremity amputees. Exp Brain Res. 2004 Jan;154(1):97–102.

8. Gentili ME, Verton C, Kinirons B, Bonnet F. Clinical perception of phantom limb sensation in patients with brachial plexus block. EJA. 2006 Aug 16;19(02):105.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Amputee demographic and clinical details.

    To measure phantom sensations, as well as other demographic and clinical details of potential relevance to the studied missing hand representation, amputees completed a range of questionnaires (e.g. amputation details, prosthesis usage etc.). Amputees rated intensities of phantom/stump pain and non-painful phantom sensations (vividness), using a 0 – 100 scale, as follows: (i) intensity of worst pain/vivid sensation experienced during the last week (or in a typical week involving such sensations); (ii) intensity of phantom pain on average over the last week (or in a typical week if last week was atypical); and (iii) current intensity/vividness of phantom pain and sensations, during the scanning day. Phantom limb pain magnitude was calculated by dividing worst pain intensity by pain frequency (1- all the time; 2- daily; 3- weekly). An analogous measure was obtained for vividness of non-painful phantom sensations. This approach represented the chronic aspect of the phantom pain/sensation as it combines frequency and intensity (Makin et al., 2013a). We also asked the amputees whether they experienced telescoping, a common phenomenon where the perceived length of the phantom limb is changed (Flor and Nikolajsen, 2006). Amp. = amputation; Amp. level = amputation level measured in percentages: (residual arm length/intact arm length) x 100. Intact arm length is measured from shoulder to fingertips, where 41% is equivalent to the level of the elbow and 75% is equivalent to the level of the wrist; PLS = phantom limb sensations; mag. = magnitude; scan = score of sensation phantom limb vividness/pain intensity on scanning day (scale 0 – 100); PLP = phantom limb pain; ave. = score of average phantom limb pain in a typical week (scale 0 – 100); prosthetics usage = prosthetic limb usage (frequency): 0- never, 1- rarely, 2- occasionally, 3- daily, 4- more than 4 hours a day, 5- more than 8 hours a day; Y. = yes; N. = no.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.004

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.15292.004
    Figure 1—source data 2. Phantom digits movement vividness, difficulty and quality.

    Amputees were asked to rate how vivid the phantom movement felt (PDS = phantom digit sensation; scale 0 – 100; ranging from unable to feel movement to movement is felt as vividly as the sensation of movement elicited by the intact hand digits). In addition, amputees were asked to rate how difficult it was to perform each phantom digit movement (diff. = difficulty; scale 0 – 100; ranging from easy to impossible). Finally, participants were asked to rate the quality and extent of movement afforded by each phantom digit. Amputees’ answers were standardised as follows: 1- full range of movements (i.e. equivalent to movements with the homologous digit on the intact hand). 2– near to full range of movement (i.e. similar to movement of the intact hand, though reduced). 3– noticeably less range of movement compared to the intact hand (i.e. partial finger flexion and extension movement).

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.005

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.15292.005
    Supplementary file 1. Runs acquired for each participant. .

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.15292.014

    elife-15292-supp1.docx (54KB, docx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.15292.014

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES