Skip to main content
Preventive Medicine Reports logoLink to Preventive Medicine Reports
. 2016 Sep 7;4:507–511. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2016.09.004

Participation in the special supplemental nutrition program for women, infants, and children is not associated with early childhood socioemotional development: Results from a longitudinal cohort study

Abigail Arons d, Corneliu Bolbocean c, Nicole R Bush a, Frances A Tylavsky b, Kaja Z LeWinn e,
PMCID: PMC5040633  PMID: 27688993

Abstract

Socioemotional development in early childhood has long-term impacts on health status and social outcomes, and racial and socioeconomic disparities in socioemotional skills emerge early in life. The Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) is an early childhood nutrition intervention with the potential to ameliorate these disparities. Our objective was to assess the impact of WIC on early socioemotional development in a longitudinal study. We examined the association between WIC participation and scores on the Brief Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) in 327 predominantly African American mother–child dyads who were participants in the longitudinal Conditions Affecting Neurocognitive Development in Early Life (CANDLE) Study (Memphis, TN). To account for selection bias, we used within-child fixed effects to model the variability in each child's BITSEA scores over two measurement occasions (ages 12 and 24 months). Final models were adjusted for time-varying characteristics including child age, maternal stress, mental health, child abuse potential, marital status, and food stamp participation. In fully adjusted models, we found no statistically significant effect of WIC on change in socioemotional development (β = 0.22 [SD = 0.39] and β = − 0.58 [SD = 0.79] for BITSEA Competence and Problem subdomains, respectively). Using rigorous methods and a longitudinal study design, we found no significant association between WIC and socioemotional development in a high needs population. This finding suggests that early childhood interventions that more specifically target socioemotional development are necessary if we are to reduce racial disparities in socioemotional skills and prevent poor social and health outcomes across the life course.

Keywords: Socioemotional, WIC, Early childhood, Policy, Disparities

Highlights

  • The effect of WIC on early childhood socioemotional development is largely unknown.

  • We study this association in a well-characterized, longitudinal cohort of mother–child dyads.

  • We adjust for stable characteristics with fixed effects and add covariates.

  • We find no association between WIC and childhood socioemotional development.

1. Introduction

Socioemotional development in early childhood has been shown to have long-term impacts on health status and social outcomes, including disability, premature mortality, and adult socioeconomic status (Power et al., 2013). This far-reaching influence is especially important because racial and socioeconomic disparities in socioemotional development emerge early in life, with low-income and minority children typically experiencing poorer outcomes. For instance, an analysis of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) data found significant socioemotional disparities by income and race by 9 months of age, and that these disparities continue to widen with age (Halle et al., 2009). Analysis of data from the Conditions Affecting Neurocognitive Development and Learning in Early Childhood (CANDLE) study (a subsample of which was used for the current study) found similar racial disparities in socioemotional development at 12 months, with African American children more likely to have behavioral problems than Caucasian children (Palmer et al., 2013). Because these disparities appear early in life and have long-term consequences for many health outcomes, improving childhood socioemotional development is a key target for interventions to reduce health and social disparities across the life course.

The Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) is one program that may improve socioemotional development and has been evaluated extensively over four decades. Early studies were fraught with issues related to self-selection, as a large proportion of people eligible for WIC do not participate in the program (Colman et al., 2012). Still, recent evidence that accounts for selection bias using statistical techniques such as propensity score matching and fixed effects suggests that WIC participation improves a variety of health outcomes, including low birth weight (Foster et al., 2010) and childhood anemia, (Currie, 2009) and reduces racial disparities in several health outcomes such as infant mortality (Khanani et al., 2010).

However, the impact of WIC on socioemotional and cognitive outcomes is relatively understudied, as most prior WIC studies focus on indices of physical health. Notably, a recent rigorous study found positive WIC effects on cognitive development (Jackson, 2015). However, in comprehensive literature reviews conducted for WIC studies from 1972 to 2012 (Hamilton and Lin, 2004, Colman et al., 2012) only 3 of 153 studies examined WIC's impact on socioemotional outcomes (Rush et al., 1988, Kowaleski-Jones and Duncan, 2000, Rivera, 2008). None of these studies found significant effects of WIC on socioemotional development. Two of the three studies used rigorous methods to account for selection bias; however, neither of these more rigorous studies used a measure of socioemotional development that assesses both adaptive and maladaptive domains of behavior and is predictive of future outcomes.

In a large, predominantly African American population with known racial disparities in socioemotional outcomes (Karabekiroglu et al., 2010), we examined the association between WIC participation and socioemotional development. We improve on prior studies by 1) using a predictive measure of socioemotional development that covers a broad array of socioemotional domains, 2) drawing on a longitudinal dataset in which the social environments of children are well characterized, allowing for greater adjustment for potential confounders and 3) rigorously accounting for selection bias by using a within-child fixed effects modeling approach. By applying these more rigorous methods to a high-needs, predominantly African American sample, we aimed to clarify whether WIC is a potentially effective intervention for improving early childhood socioemotional development and reducing racial disparities in that domain.

2. Methods

The Urban Child Institute's Conditions Affecting Neurocognitive Development and Learning in Early Childhood (CANDLE) study is a longitudinal cohort study of 1503 mother–child dyads in Shelby County, Tennessee recruited between 2006 and 2011. The study has been described previously (Palmer et al., 2013). We used Medicaid participation as a proxy for WIC eligibility, as Medicaid confers WIC eligibility and Medicaid take-up is higher than WIC take-up (Bitler and Currie, 2005). For instance, in 2006–2009 during the time of the CANDLE study, Tennessee's WIC take-up rate was 52–53% of eligible people enrolling, (Betson et al., 2011) versus a child Medicaid take-up rate in Tennessee of 86–91% in 2008–2010 (Kenney et al., 2012). Using this method, our analytic subset included 327 mother–child dyads who reported participating in Medicaid at ages 12 months and 24 months, meaning the entire subsample of families was eligible for WIC at both time points.

The WIC participation variable was collected in the Food Supplement Information questionnaire, a CANDLE-specific questionnaire administered at the 12 month clinic visit and the 24 month home visit, in which participants were asked “Is your CANDLE child now receiving benefits from the WIC program?”

We measured socioemotional development with the Brief Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (BITSEA), which was assessed during clinic visits by a licensed psychologist or an advanced graduate student when children were 12 and 24 months of age. The BITSEA is a 42-item questionnaire used to screen infants and toddlers for socioemotional competence and behavioral problems. The BITSEA has been found to be reliable and valid for children of diverse demographic backgrounds ages 12 months to 36 months (Karabekiroglu et al., 2010) and is predictive of future socioemotional development (Briggs-Gowan and Carter, 2008). Responses are aggregated into two index scores, the Problem Total for socioemotional and behavior problems (i.e. aggression, anxiety, maladaptive behaviors) and the Competence score for socioemotional competency (i.e. empathy, prosocial peer relations). Falling in the lowest 25th percentile for the Problem score indicates possible socioemotional problems, and falling below the 15th percentile for the Competence score indicates delays in this subdomain.

The CANDLE study richly characterized additional aspects of the maternal and child environment, and we were able to include a number of time-varying child and family covariates in our analyses that may have confounded our results. These characteristics include child age, maternal education, income, maternal stress as measured by the Parenting Stress Index, maternal mental health as measured by the Brief Symptom Inventory, child abuse as measured by the Child Abuse Potential Inventory, maternal marital status, and food stamp participation.

2.1. Statistical analyses

The primary statistical analysis employed in this study was within-child fixed effects, with separate models assessing the effect of WIC participation on a child's BITSEA Competence score and BITSEA Problem score. Because ordinary least squares regression and similar methods fail to account for selection bias, fixed effects have been used in several recent WIC studies. These models can compare within-child data over time to control for time invariant characteristics that impact WIC participation (Colman et al., 2012). We used within-child fixed effects to capitalize on the longitudinal nature of our study data. These analyses explicitly model the variability in BITSEA scores within each child over our two measurement occasions, at age 12 months and age 24 months.

The model employed was: Yit = β1  Fit + Xit  γ + αi + εit, where i is the mother–child dyad identifier, t is 12 or 24 months, Y is BITSEA score, F is a binary variable for WIC participation, X is a vector of time-varying, dyad-specific covariates, and α represents time-invariant dyad-specific covariates. The design controls for unobserved child characteristics, and for time-invariant family and environmental background attributes. In the adjusted model, we also include the time-varying covariates mentioned above. To further investigate the effect of WIC, we created two subsamples that included only African Americans and only those who received WIC prenatally. Each of these subsamples included the vast majority of our participants (88% and 91% of the analytic sample, respectively). Using the same modeling strategy, we also examined whether WIC was associated with BITSEA as a dichotomous variable (cutoff met or not) using logistic fixed effects models. We also ran random effects models adjusting for the same covariates as above. All statistical analyses were conducted using STATA 13 (Stata Corp., College Station, TX).

3. Results

The study sample was majority African American, with a majority of mothers reporting unmarried/non-cohabitating marital status, a high school education, and prenatal WIC participation (Table 1). We further subdivided the sample into groups depending on their WIC participation: 41% had WIC at both time points, 28% had WIC only at age 12 months, 7% had WIC only at 24 months, and 24% had no WIC at either time point. One way ANOVA tests showed significant differences (p < 0.05) among these subgroups across some time invariant and time varying characteristics (Appendix A).

Table 1.

Characteristics of the CANDLE study (Memphis, TN) analytic sample (N = 327 dyads).

Time invariant characteristics N %
Child gender
Female 163 49.9
Male 164 50.2



Race/ethnicity
African American 286 87.5
White 28 8.5
Other 13 4.0
Prenatal WIC Participation 300 91.7



Time varying characteristics Child age 12 months
Child age 24 months
N % N %
Maternal marital status
Married 38 11.6 43 13.2
Living with partner 70 21.4 61 18.7
Single/divorced/widowed/separated 219 67.0 222 68.0



Maternal education
Less than high school 30 9.2 23 7.0
High school/GED 221 67.6 205 62.7
Technical school 38 11.6 44 13.5
College degree 33 10.1 47 14.4
Graduate degree 5 1.5 8 2.5



Income
Less than $5000 74 24.7 70 22.4
$5000–$9999 56 18.7 55 17.6
$10,000–$14,999 48 16.0 49 15.7
$15,000–$19,999 35 11.7 39 12.5
$20,000–$24,999 40 13.3 44 14.1
$25,000–$34,999 29 9.7 32 10.2
$35,000–$44,999 11 3.7 15 4.8
$45,000–$54,999 4 1.3 5 1.6
More than $55,000 3 1.0 4 1.3



WIC participation 226 69.1 155 47.4



High parenting stress (% reporting high) 45 13.8 52 16.1



Mean SD Mean SD
BITSEA score
Competence raw total score 15.42 3.19 17.73 2.80
Problem raw total score 11.12 6.23 11.38 6.77



Child abuse potential inventory total 105.76 80.18 100.86 80.05



Brief symptom inventory total (maternal mental health) 48.43 10.81 46.75 11.52

We found a significant association between WIC participation and continuous BITSEA Competence in the unadjusted model; however, the addition of covariates eliminated this association (Table 2). We failed to show a significant association between WIC participation and continuous BITSEA Problem scores (Table 2), and BITSEA Problem and Competence cut-off scores (not shown). The covariate with the largest significant association with BITSEA scores was age. These results were consistent when we examined only African Americans, and only those with prenatal WIC exposure. Random effects models showed similar results to the fixed effects approach.

Table 2.

Association between WIC participation and continuous BITSEA Socioemotional Competence and Problem scores in the CANDLE study (Memphis, TN). (N = 327 dyads).

Full Sample, Unadjusted Full Sample Adjusted African Americans only, Adjusted Prenatal WIC Participants only, Adjusted
BITSEA Competence
WIC effect − 1.63 (0.38) 0.22 (0.39) 0.43 (0.42) 0.27 (0.40)
Observations 655 606 532 555
R (Halle et al., 2009) 0.05 0.36 0.38 0.38



BITSEA Problem
WIC effect − 0.33 (0.67) − 0.58 (0.79) − 0.18 (0.80) − 0.60 (0.83)
Observations 655 606 532 555
R (Halle et al., 2009) 0.00 0.13 0.15 0.14

Note: All estimates are fixed effects, with standard errors in parentheses. All estimates (except “Unadjusted”) are adjusted for child age, income, maternal stress and mental health, maternal marital status, maternal education, food stamp participation and child abuse-potential scores. BITSEA stands for Brief Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment.

p < 0.05.

4. Discussion

The goal of the current study was to examine the association between WIC participation and socioemotional development in a high needs population for which there are known racial disparities in socioemotional skills. We used a fixed effects modeling approach that capitalized on a richly characterized, longitudinal sample of mothers and their children to more rigorously test whether WIC predicted change in a validated measure of socioemotional development. To further reduce the effects of selection bias, we used a sample that was WIC-eligible throughout the study, and examined differences between four WIC participation subgroups: WIC at both time points, WIC only at age 12 months, WIC only at age 24 months and no WIC at either time point. Income, gender, and prenatal WIC exposure were statistically different between these subgroups. We explicitly controlled for income as a time varying characteristic and relied on the fixed effects approach to control for all time-invariant variables. We further investigated the effect of WIC by running our final model in a subsample with prenatal exposure to WIC. While our initial unadjusted model found a statistically significant negative effect, in our fully adjusted models we failed to reject the null hypothesis of no effect. In other words, the effect of WIC participation was not statistically significantly related to changes in Competence or Problem behaviors between 12 and 24-months of age as measured by BITSEA scores. These findings contribute to a conflicting evidence base that has shown a positive effect, negative effect, and/or no effect of WIC participation on socioemotional development (Rush et al., 1988, Kowaleski-Jones and Duncan, 2000, Rivera, 2008). The debate on the effectiveness of WIC on socioemotional development is yet to be settled.

There are many hypothesized mechanisms through which WIC participation may influence socioemotional development. WIC participation improves child nutrition (Whaley et al., 2012), which itself might have a direct impact on socioemotional development through nutrient effects on brain development and improved health (Bellisle, 2004, Gibson and Green, 2002). WIC also provides referrals to health, welfare and social services, which has been shown to attenuate stress-related child health risks (Black et al., 2012). In addition, WIC participation may reduce food insecurity (Laraia et al., 2006, Salmons, 2013) which may have a separate effect on child attachment and cognition (Herman et al., 2004). In the current study, potential mediators such as maternal stress were not predictive of socioemotional competence in our sample (data not presented), precluding further examination of maternal stress as a mediating factor. We did not test other potential mediators, as we found no main effect of WIC on socioemotional development in adjusted models. Critically, our finding suggests that WIC alone is not sufficient to reduce disparities in socioemotional development if it does not improve these outcomes in high-risk African American children, and that additional interventions more targeted towards such outcomes are necessary.

The main limitation of this study is the potential for residual bias due to self-selection into WIC not accounted for by the fixed-effects analysis. Furthermore, there may additional sources of bias, and unmeasured time-varying factors such as neighborhood effects (Rossin-Slater, 2013) that may be related to both socioemotional development and the decision to participate in WIC for which we have not accounted. The only way to definitely remove all bias would be through a randomized trial, which would not be practically or ethically possible for WIC. Given the observational design of our study, we took advantage of a very rich, longitudinal dataset with detailed characterization of our participants' social environments, and employed fixed effects models to reduce potential sources of bias and generate more accurate estimates of the true WIC effect.

5. Conclusions

WIC is a nutrition policy intervention with wide-ranging preventive benefits for health and development; however, we find no statistically significant effect of WIC on socioemotional development in a primarily African American, low-income population. Additional research examining the differential effect of prenatal versus childhood WIC participation or a repeated analysis at older ages could show a benefit from WIC participation. However, the current study adds more conclusive evidence that WIC does not improve this important child outcome. Early childhood interventions that specifically target socioemotional development are necessary if we are to reduce racial disparities in socioemotional skills and prevent poor social and health outcomes across the life course.

Conflict of interest

No authors declare a conflict of interest.

Transparency document

Transparency document.

mmc1.pdf (1.2MB, pdf)

Acknowledgments

The CANDLE study (Director F.T.) was funded by the Urban Child Institute, Memphis, TN. Funding also provided by NIMH (K01MH097978 to K. Z. L.) and the UCSF Pathways Explore program (A. A.).

Footnotes

The Transparency document associated to this article can be found, in the online version.

Appendix A. Full demographic table across subgroups.

Time invariant characteristics WIC at both times (n = 133)
WIC at 12 months, no WIC at 24 months (n = 93)
No WIC at 12 months, WIC at 24 months (n = 22)
No WIC at either time (n = 79)
N % N % N % N %
Child Gender
 Female 78 58.7 36 38.7 10 45.5 39 49.4
 Male 55 41.4 57 61.3 12 54.6 40 50.6
Race/Ethnicity
 African American 121 91.0 84 90.3 19 86.4 62 78.5
 White 8 6.0 6 6.5 1 4.6 13 16.5
 Other 4 3.0 3 3.2 2 9.1 4 5.1
Prenatal WIC Participation 130 97.7 87 93.6 22 100 61 77.2



Time varying characteristics Child age
12 months
24 months
12 months
24 months
12 months
24 months
12 months
24 months
N % N % N % N % N % N % N % N %
Maternal marital status
 Married 14 10.5 16 13.2 10 10.8 13 14 0 0 0 0 14 17.7 14 18
 Living with partner 31 23.3 24 18.7 16 17.2 14 15.1 7 31.8 8 36.4 16 20.3 15 19.2
 Single/divorced/widowed/separated 88 66.2 113 85 67 72 66 71 15 68.2 14 63.6 49 62 49 62.8
Maternal education
 Less than high school 13 9.8 11 8.3 11 11.8 7 7.5 1 4.6 1 4.6 5 6.3 4 5.1
 High School/GED 91 68.4 83 62.4 59 63.4 60 64.5 18 81.8 17 77.3 53 67.1 45 57
 Technical school 16 12 18 13.5 9 9.7 9 9.7 1 4.6 0 0 12 15.2 17 21.5
 College degree 12 9.02 19 14.3 10 10.8 13 14 2 9.1 4 18.2 9 11.4 11 13.9
 Graduate degree 1 0.8 2 1.5 4 4.3 4 4.3 0 0 0 0 0 0 2 2.5
Income
 Less than $5000 34 27.9 34 26.8 23 27.4 23 25.8 8 36.4 2 9.5 9 12.5 11 14.5
 $5000–$9999 24 19.7 21 16.5 16 19.0 20 22.5 4 18.2 5 23.8 12 16.7 9 11.8
 $10,000–$14,999 15 12.3 12 9.4 11 13.1 18 20.2 6 27.3 5 23.8 16 22.2 14 18.4
 $15,000–$19,999 16 13.1 17 13.4 10 11.9 7 7.9 0 0.0 4 19.0 9 12.5 11 14.5
 $20,000–$24,999 15 12.3 19 15.0 13 15.5 11 12.4 1 4.5 3 14.3 11 15.3 11 14.5
 $25,000–$34,999 15 12.3 18 14.2 5 6.0 6 6.7 2 9.1 1 4.8 7 9.7 7 9.2
 $35,000–$44,999 2 1.6 4 3.1 4 4.8 2 2.2 0 0.0 1 4.8 5 6.9 8 10.5
 $45,000–$54,999 0 0.0 1 0.8 2 2.4 2 2.2 0 0.0 0 0.0 2 2.8 2 2.6
 More than $55,000 1 0.8 1 0.8 0 0.0 0 0.0 1 4.5 0 0.0 1 1.4 3 3.9
High parenting stress (% reporting high) 15 11.3 23 17.6 18 19.6 13 14 2 9.1 2 9.1 10 12.7 14 18



Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
BITSEA score
 Competence raw total score 15.53 3.42 17.64 2.6 15.04 3.09 17.63 2.85 15.32 3.01 17.23 3.56 15.7 2.96 17.99 2.85
 Problem raw total score 11.07 5.76 12.63 7.24 11.55 6.5 11.45 7.35 10.73 5.72 8.59 4.7 10.8 6.86 9.95 5.14
Child abuse potential inventory total 107.7 75.1 109.0 81.3 112.7 82.4 100.2 75.8 97.05 89.2 101 89.4 96.66 83.7 88.18 80.0
Brief symptom inventory (maternal mental health) 49.07 10.3 47.92 11.0 48.61 11.2 44.97 11.1 45.55 11.0 47.23 12.5 47.96 11.2 46.78 12.4

Indicates p > 0.05 among groups.

References

  1. Bellisle F. Effects of diet on behaviour and cognition in children. Br. J. Nutr. 2004;92(S2):S227–S232. doi: 10.1079/bjn20041171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Betson D., Martinez-Schiferl M., Giannarelli L., Zedlewski S. U.S. Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service, Office of Research and Analysis; 2011. National and State-level Estimates of Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) Eligibles and Program Reach, 2000–2009. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bitler M.P., Currie J. Does WIC work? The effects of WIC on pregnancy and birth outcomes. J. Policy Anal. Manage. 2005;24(1):73–91. doi: 10.1002/pam.20070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Black M.M., Quigg A.M., Cook J. WIC participation and attenuation of stress-related child health risks of household food insecurity and caregiver depressive symptoms. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2012;166(5):444–451. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Briggs-Gowan M.J., Carter A.S. Social–emotional screening status in early childhood predicts elementary school outcomes. Pediatrics. 2008;121(5):957–962. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-1948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Colman S., Nichols-Barrer I.P., Redline J.E., Devaney B.L., Ansell S.V., Joyce T. 2012. Effects of the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC): A Review of Recent Research. (Mathematica Policy Research) [Google Scholar]
  7. Currie J. Policy interventions to address child health disparities: moving beyond health insurance. Pediatrics. 2009;124(Supplement 3):S246–S254. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-1100M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Foster E.M., Jiang M., Gibson-Davis C.M. The effect of the WIC program on the health of newborns. Health Serv. Res. 2010;45(4):1083–1104. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6773.2010.01115.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gibson E.L., Green M.W. Nutritional influences on cognitive function: mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutr. Res. Rev. 2002;15(01):169–206. doi: 10.1079/NRR200131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Halle T., Forry N., Hair E. Child Trends; Washington, DC: 2009. Disparities in Early Learning and Development: Lessons From the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  11. Hamilton W.L., Lin B.H. United States Department of Agriculture, Economic Research Service; 2004. Effects of Food Assistance and Nutrition Programs on Nutrition and Health: Volume 3, Literature Review (No. 33863) [Google Scholar]
  12. Herman D.R., Harrison G.G., Afifi A.A., Jenks E. The effect of the WIC program on food security status of pregnant, first-time participants. Fam. Econ. Nutr. Rev. 2004;16(1):21. [Google Scholar]
  13. Jackson M.I. Early childhood WIC participation, cognitive development and academic achievement. Soc. Sci. Med. 2015;126:145–153. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.12.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Karabekiroglu K., Briggs-Gowan M.J., Carter A.S., Rodopman-Arman A., Akbas S. The clinical validity and reliability of the Brief Infant–Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) Infant Behav. Dev. 2010;33(4):503–509. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kenney G., Lynch V., Huntress M., Haley J., Anderson N. Urban Institute and Robert Wood Johnson Foundation; 2012. Medicaid/CHIP Participation Among Children and Parents. [Google Scholar]
  16. Khanani I., Elam J., Hearn R., Jones C., Maseru N. The impact of prenatal WIC participation on infant mortality and racial disparities. Am. J. Public Health. 2010;100(S1):S204–S209. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.168922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kowaleski-Jones L., Duncan G.J. 2000. Effects of Participation in the WIC Food Assistance Program on Children's Health and Development: Evidence from NLSY Children. (Discussion Paper) [Google Scholar]
  18. Laraia B.A., Siega-Riz A.M., Gundersen C., Dole N. Psychosocial factors and socioeconomic indicators are associated with household food insecurity among pregnant women. J. Nutr. 2006;136(1):177–182. doi: 10.1093/jn/136.1.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Palmer F.B., Anand K.J., Graff J.C. Early adversity, socioemotional development, and stress in urban 1-year-old children. J. Pediatr. 2013;163(6):1733–1739. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2013.08.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Power C., Kuh D., Morton S. From developmental origins of adult disease to life course research on adult disease and aging: insights from birth cohort studies. Public Health. 2013;34(1):7. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031912-114423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Rivera A.C. 2008. Impact and Process Evaluation of Prenatal WIC on Maternal and Infant Outcomes. (ProQuest) [Google Scholar]
  22. Rossin-Slater M. WIC in your neighborhood: new evidence on the impacts of geographic access to clinics. J. Public Econ. 2013;102:51–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2013.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Rush D., Leighton J., Sloan N.L. The national WIC evaluation: evaluation of the special supplemental food program for women, infants, and children. VI. Study of infants and children. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1988;48(2):484–511. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/48.2.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Salmons T.F. 2013. Evaluating the Special Supplemental Program for Women, Infants, and Children: An Examination of Participation and Programmatic Effectiveness Among Children. [Google Scholar]
  25. Whaley S.E., Ritchie L.D., Spector P., Gomez J. Revised WIC food package improves diets of WIC families. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2012;44(3):204–209. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2011.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Transparency document.

mmc1.pdf (1.2MB, pdf)

Articles from Preventive Medicine Reports are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES