Abstract
Large-scale brain signals exhibit rich intermittent patterning, reflecting the fact that the cortex actively eschews fixed points in favor of itinerant wandering with frequent state transitions. Fluctuations in endogenous cortical activity occur at multiple time scales and index a dynamic repertoire of network states that are continuously explored, even in the absence of external sensory inputs. Here, we quantified such moment-to-moment brain signal variability at rest in a large, cross-sectional sample of children ranging in age from seven to eleven years. Our findings revealed a monotonic rise in the complexity of electroencephalogram (EEG) signals as measured by sample entropy, from the youngest to the oldest age cohort, across a range of time scales and spatial regions. From year to year, the greatest changes in intraindividual brain signal variability were recorded at electrodes covering the anterior cortical zones. These results provide converging evidence concerning the age-dependent expansion of functional cortical network states during a critical developmental period ranging from early to late childhood.
At the population level detectable by non-invasive surface recordings, groups of neurons sustain multiple oscillations that are implicated in functional communication between different neuronal assemblies (Voytek & Knight, 2015). Brain electrical activity exhibits a coherent frequency architecture, consisting of a number of bandwidth classes that are marked by oscillatory peaks with characteristic spectral centroids (Buzsáki & Draguhn, 2004; Klimesch, 2012). These frequency peaks are approximately arranged in a geometric series (i.e., the ratio between two successive bandwidth centroids is constant) possessing an irrational number, with the physiological consequence that perfect synchronization between bands is impossible and any observed periodic regularity is necessarily unstable and short-lived (Buzsáki, 2006; Pletzer et al., 2010). The presence of this mathematical ratio corresponds to the readily observable intermittent spatio-temporal fluctuations of electroencephalogram (EEG) and magnetoencephalogram (MEG) time series signals.
A defining feature of endogenous brain signals during the so-called default or resting mode is the absence of a fixed equilibrium state (Cabral et al., 2014; Friston, 1997; Tognoli & Kelso, 2014). Instead, the electrical and hemodynamic output of the cerebrum exhibits itinerant dynamics during which a repertoire of functional networks are explored in successive transient sequences (Britz et al., 2010; Deco et al., 2011, 2013; Hansen et al., 2015; Khana et al., 2015). In common with many other adaptive dynamical systems, the cerebral cortex is poised precariously between transient epochs of stability and noisy perturbations and this “meta-stable” regime is evident across multiple spatio-temporal scales (Faisal et al., 2008; Kelso, 2012; Rabinovich et al., 2008; Tognoli & Kelso, 2014; Werner, 2007). The complex nature of neuronal activity, in which transient periods of synchronizability are spontaneously punctuated by rapid state transitions is an emergent product of the myriad numbers and kinds of structural interactions supported by the underlying connectome (Deco et al., 2013; Honey et al., 2007; Nakagawa et al., 2013; Shen et al., 2015). In particular, the meta-stability of functional brain activity seems to arise as a consequence of two competing constraints that have shaped cortical architecture – namely, local segregation and global integration (Sporns, 2011). The cerebral cortex combines high modularity with short characteristic path lengths (Muldoon, Bridgeford, & Bassett, 2016), enabling canonical computations in regional circuits as well as the distributed dissemination of local computations in spatio-temporally specific ways.
The potentially beneficial effects of variability (often called “noise”) in physiology (Buchman, 2002) and in nervous systems more specifically have long been recognized (McDonnell & Ward, 2011; Pinneo, 1966). Similarly, theoretical models that view development as an emergent property of numerous decentralized and local interactions of the growing brain treat variability as functionally useful rather than as a nuisance factor (Smith & Thelen, 2003). A specific example of this in the developmental context is the acquisition of skilled motor actions in children, where the increased task proficiency that comes with age is sub-served by exploiting variability at a microstructural level (Manoel & Connolly, 1995). Within cortex, the benefits of meta-stability include optimizing circuits for information processing (Beggs, 2008; Friston et al., 2012; Garrett et al., 2010; Shew & Plenz, 2012), using the strategy of “liquid state” computing (Rabinovich et al., 2008), and enhancing the likelihood of adaptive responses in external environments that are noisy, uncertain and constantly changing (Garrett et al., 2013; Ward, 2009). The number of functional cortical network configurations (or the repertoire size) is a fluid feature that is capable of expansion with learning and maturation (Deco et al., 2013). In particular, increased synchronous coordination of neuronal assemblies across the cortex widens the space of potential functional architectures (Deco & Kringelbach, 2016; Fries, 2005). In the context of brain maturation and development, there is a wealth of evidence to suggest that ontogenetic sculpting of synaptic connectivity (including changes in pruning and axonal myelination of long-distance fascicular pathways) produces refinements in the functional integration of distant cortical regions (Luna & Sweeney, 2004; Stevens, 2009). The resulting synaptic retuning and network integration from infancy to adulthood manifests in functional cortical activity that becomes smaller in overall amplitude but more precisely coordinated (Liu, Woltering, & Lewis, 2014; Mathes et al., 2016; Papenberg et al., 2013; Uhlhaas et al., 2009) and globally integrated (Fair et al., 2009; Supekar et al., 2009). Although largely overlooked in the past, the amount of information integrated across cortical networks (i.e., greater information transmission) increases the unpredictability of signals recorded at the population level (McDonough & Nishira, 2014; Misic et al., 2011; Vakorin et al., 2011). The irregularity of large-scale neuronal signals, in this case, simultaneously reflects and promotes the exploration of a diversified network repertoire, thereby extending the brain’s representational capacity (Deco & Kringelbach, 2016; Ghosh et al., 2008). Within dynamic system perspectives, variability/noise (when properly tuned) provides the “kinetic energy” required for the brain to visit a richer state space (Golos, Jirsa, & Daucé, 2015). In principle then, developmental increases in the informational complexity of brain signals ought to be reflected in mathematical indices that are sufficiently sensitive to pattern regularity at multiple temporal scales (McIntosh et al., 2010).
Intraindividual Brain Signal Variability
On the basis of the evidence reviewed above, there has been a rapidly growing interest in treating intraindividual brain signal variability as a new frontier for studies examining neural development, learning, and nervous system pathology (Garrett et al., 2013; McIntosh et al., 2010; Takahashi, 2013; Vakorin et al., 2013). Although there exist numerous ways of quantifying brain signal variability, one prominent class of computational methods that exhibit superior performance (as compared to conventional estimates based on simple mean and variance) involves computing regularity statistics that capture the degree of unpredictability inherent in time series recordings (Garrett et al., 2013; Takahashi, 2013; Vakorin & McIntosh, 2012). Although there are several families of statistics for computing temporal irregularity, sample entropy is an information theoretic measure that was introduced specifically for the analysis of non-stationary, physiological signals (Richman & Moorman, 2000). In brief, sample entropy captures the amount of self-similarity that is contained within a time series – a signal with high self-similarity is assigned low sample entropy values while a signal that is highly unpredictable or irregular results in high sample entropy.
A specific measure derived from this family of methods that is particularly suitable for the analysis of neuronal time series data (Vakorin & McIntosh, 2012) is known as multiscale entropy (MSE). An important advantage of the MSE technique over conventional sample entropy estimates is that it evaluates irregularity at multiple time scales, which increases its sensitivity for differentiating the non-stationary dynamical complexity of physiological signals (which is fractal-like and expressed across many temporal scales) from uncorrelated time series data (e.g., white noise) that exhibit no temporal structure (Costa et al., 2005, Goldberger et al., 2002). By virtue of its sensitivity to signal content at multiple time scales, the MSE method assigns low values to both completely deterministic and stochastic signals, but tends to be high for signals that straddle the middle ground between these extremes. By contrast, when sample entropy is computed at a single time scale, it assigns high values to processes generated entirely by random sampling, ignoring the fact that at longer time scales these signals carry very little information (Costa et al., 2005). The temporal sensitivity of the MSE method is an important consideration insofar as it accurately captures one of the hallmark features of complexity that exists in biological systems – namely, that it is a proportional mixture of system order and disorder (Tononi et al., 1998)1.
Several studies have established that the amount of signal complexity or “brain noise”, as measured by MSE, increases with cortical maturation (McIntosh et al., 2010; Misic et al., 2010) and subsequently begins to decline during senescence (Garrett et al., 2011; Grady & Garrett, 2014; Manor & Lipsitz, 2013; Yang et al., 2014). The amount of MSE present in surface recorded event-related EEG signals has been reported to increase parametrically from 8–9 years of age to young adulthood (20–33 years of age) and the increased brain noise predicts reduced within-subject behavioral variability on a simple decision task (McIntosh et al., 2008). The developmental trajectory of task-specific EEG signal complexity from infancy to 5 years of age corresponds to the differential structural maturation of distinct cortical lobes, such that MSE peaks first in those regions that exhibit earlier development of dendritic arborization and axonal myelination (Lippé et al., 2009). Consistent with the idea that network-wide information integration leads to increased temporal irregularity of signals recorded from large neuronal populations, increases in the amount of brain signal variability during normal development are related to the maturation of long-range functional connections between distal cortical regions (Vakorin et al., 2011).
The Present Study
Here we undertook what is, to our knowledge, the largest cross-sectional study investigating ongoing neuroelectrical signal variability from early to late childhood in the course of normal human brain development. Moreover, in contrast to previous developmental studies of EEG sample entropy reviewed above, which involved neurophysiological time series signals recorded in response to sensory stimulation and/or task performance, the present study focuses on ongoing, resting-state cortical signals unstructured by experimental demands.
We have recently documented extensive reorganization of spontaneous functional cortical networks during the transition from early to late childhood, resulting in increasingly dense and spatially variable functional connectivity networks as age increased from 7 to 11 years (Miskovic et al., 2015). Given that the complexity of neural signals is positively related to the degree of functional connectivity (McDonough & Nashiro, 2014; Misic et al., 2011), specifically between distal brain areas (Vakorin et al., 2011), we expected to observe a parametric increase in MSE values of EEG signals from early to late childhood at multiple time scales, perhaps with a stronger magnitude of differences at the coarser scales. Moreover, we hypothesized that these developmental trends would honor well-known spatial gradients of electrocortical maturation, with posterior cortical areas developing prior to more anterior zones (Matousek & Petersen, 1973). This particular topographic pattern is also generally consistent with evidence from structural imaging (Colby et al., 2011; Gogtay et al., 2004) and other sources (Guillery, 2005), indicating that cortical regions lower in the processing hierarchy develop earlier in time than higher-order “association” zones.
Method
Participants
Participants were a subset drawn from an ongoing study of children recruited from a rural community in northeast USA. Although the sample contains some children included in our earlier report (Miskovic et al., 2015), it has been substantially expanded with subsequently collected data. Moreover, the analytic methods used here provide unique information about cortical development as described in more detail below. The inclusion criterion was being between 7 and 11 years of age. Exclusionary criteria were the presence of severe developmental or learning disabilities (e.g., autism) in children per parent report. Other factors that would be expected to impact EEG signals, such as medication status, were not exclusion criteria and are addressed by a control analysis discussed below. Resting EEG data were available from a total of 464 children. The final number of participants included across all five age groups was as follows: 109 seven year olds, 100 eight year olds, 93 nine year olds, 88 ten year olds, and 74 eleven year olds. Of the children included in our sample, 47% were female, 86% were Caucasian (13% African American) and the median family income was $35,000 to $40,000.
Procedure
Potential participants were recruited from the community through a variety of means (e.g., television, newspaper and bus ads, flyers). Parents responding to the recruitment advertisements were initially screened over the phone to determine potential eligibility. Upon arrival, parents were asked to provide informed consent and children were asked to provide assent to be in the study. Next, participants were seated quietly in the experimental room and instructed to remain still with their eyes closed for a full minute. The eyes closed condition is considered to provide the most valid method for capturing spontaneous brain function (Logothetis et al., 2009). The eyes closed resting condition was collected at the beginning of a larger EEG/ERP experimental protocol, examining emotional processing in this population. However, here we focus only on the resting component since we were specifically interested in endogenous cortical activity, unstructured by experimental demands and stimulus onsets.
EEG Recording
Continuous EEG was recorded using a custom cap and the BioSemi ActiveTwo system. EEG was digitized at 24-bit resolution with a sampling rate of 512 Hz. Recordings were taken from 36 sintered Ag/AgCl active electrodes based on the International 10/20 system and evenly distributed across the scalp, and left and right mastoids. Two additional scalp electrodes were used in the study: an active Common Mode Sense (CMS) and a passive Driven Right Leg (DRL) electrode, located at the mid-line between C3 and CZ, and CZ and C4, respectively. Raw EEG were recorded relative to CMS. The CMS/DRL electrodes formed a loop that principally served as the ground for recordings through a feedback that subtracted the average potential of the subject (i.e. the Common Mode voltage) to drive EEG recordings as close as possible to the “zero” ADC reference voltage in the AD-box (please see http://www.biosemi.com/faq/cms&drl.htm for further details). The electrooculogram was recorded from four facial electrodes to capture vertical and horizontal eye movements that were subsequently used to aid visual inspection of the EEG time series.
EEG Analysis and Data Reduction
Offline processing of EEG data was accomplished using a combination of EEGLAB (Makeig et al., 2000) functions and in-house MATLAB routines. After re-referencing the EEG data to average mastoids, a two-way least squares finite impulse response (FIR) filter was used to bandpass the time domain signal (high pass cut-off: 0.01 Hz, low pass cut-off: 80 Hz). Since the preprocessing of neuronal time series data is especially important for analyses of brain signal variability (Garrett et al., 2013) and given that we wished to avoid sharp transients that might be introduced as a result of manually rejected data segments and data splicing, we implemented denoising procedures by independent components analysis (ICA), using the runica function in the EEGLAB toolbox (Makeig et al., 1996). Default settings of the runica function were used, based on the logistic infomax ICA algorithm of Bell and Sejnowski (1995), with the natural gradient feature of Amari, Cichocki, and Yang (1996) to speed computations. Generally, the runica ICA algorithm separates input data recorded at N electrodes × time into an equivalent number of temporally independent component time courses spatially filtered from the data of each electrode. ICA was applied to each participants’ full data (including ~ 1,203,200 sample points concatenated across the entire experimental session of each participant). Artifact correction consisted of manual inspection of EEG data by a trained observer for stereotypical ocular EEG artifacts (e.g. blink activity: brief, large, deflections at frontal electrode sites and deflections of opposite polarity at the vertical EOG) that were then matched with that of simultaneous ICA time courses. Potential artifactual ICA components were then verified by plotting their scalp topography and removed if maps provided further evidence that the component was artifact activity (e.g. ICA blink activity projects most strongly to far frontal sites). The flagged ICA components were then removed prior to back projection to the electrode level.
Multiscale Entropy
The ICA-denoised time domain EEG data were used for estimating MSE as originally implemented in Costa et al. (2005). Briefly, this procedure involves quantifying the sample entropy (Richman & Moorman, 2000) of EEG signals at multiple time scales. The first step involves temporal coarse graining where for a given time scale (τ) the corresponding time series is calculated by averaging neighboring data points within non-overlapping windows of length τ from the original time series signal. Subsequent to this coarse graining procedure, sample entropy is quantified separately at each of the time scale factors (from 1 to 20 in our case, where 1 represents the original signal and 20 indicates a window size of 20 sample points). Sample entropy represents the conditional probability that any two consecutive data sequences of pattern length (m + 1) will match each other given a match for the first m points across the duration of a time series at a given scale factor. Subsequent patterns are considered to recur if the absolute amplitude difference falls within a particular criterion or tolerance range, r. Sample entropy is calculated using the following equation:
where is the number of matches and N is the length of the original time series.
SE thus captures the regularity of a signal: low values indicate high self-similarity (low complexity) and high values denote irregularity (high complexity). On the basis of recommendations provided elsewhere (Richman & Moorman, 2000), as well as precedents established in previous EEG and MEG studies (Heisz et al., 2012, 2015; McIntosh et al., 2008; Misic et al., 2010, 2011), we set m = 2 and r = 0.5 (data amplitudes matched if the absolute amplitude difference between them was ≤50% of the time series standard deviation). An attractive feature of sample entropy (in contrast to related measures, such as approximate entropy) is that the estimated values remain stable around many different choices of m and r in addition to being less dependent on signal length (Richman & Moorman, 2000). We confirmed the essential stability of MSE estimates during preliminary analyses examining an extended range of criterion threshold values. Since MSE values can be biased by the presence of artifacts, we decided to omit electrodes that are most susceptible to residual ocular (e.g., eye rolling) and muscular activity: Fp1, Fp2, AF3, AF4, M1 and M2. We calculated MSE for all of the remaining (30) cephalic electrodes.
Although MSE is partially related to spectral power analyses, which were the focus of a previous report from our group (Miskovic et al., 2015), these two approaches are differentially sensitive to the non-linear brain dynamics reflected in the interactions of frequency components (Vakorin & McIntosh, 2012). The unique sensitivity of MSE, relative to spectral power analysis, to temporal dependencies embedded within brain signals is highlighted in Figure 1.
For illustrative purposes and to further demonstrate that our measure of signal complexity captured the crucial aspects of complexity in physiological systems, Figure 2 depicts sample MSE curves for the empirical EEG time series as well as for two simulated types of colored noise fluctuations with distinct 1/fα spectral properties. Similar to previous findings obtained using EEG, MEG and fMRI data (Garrett et al., 2013), the sample entropy of brain signals was relatively low (i.e., highly regular) at the fine timescales, but steadily increased at the more coarse timescales. Importantly, and in contrast to conventional single scale estimates of sample entropy, a completely uncorrelated stochastic data sequence (i.e., white noise), exhibited vanishing SE at longer timescales. Pink (1/f−1) noise exhibited high and stable entropy values, due to the fact that this form of noise contains information at both short and long timescales. Neurophysiological activity approximated pink noise at the longer timescales, indicative of long range temporal correlations consistent with “history” effects in this particular signal type (Costa et al., 2005).
Statistical Analyses
To examine the spatial distribution of MSE values, we averaged data across distinct regional and hemispheric clusters of electrodes (see Figure 4 topographic map), and then performed a repeated-measures ANOVA using the within-subject factors of Region (frontal, central and parieto-occipital) and Laterality (left and right hemisphere). The ANOVA model was evaluated using Type III Sums of Squares and Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied in cases where Mauchly’s test revealed violations of the sphericity assumption. The generalized eta squared is reported as a measure of effect size for these analysis on the basis of recommendations for repeated-measures designs (Bakeman, 2005).
To test for differences in EEG signal complexity as a function of chronological age, we submitted the sample entropy data to an independent samples, two-tailed permutation test based on the tmax statistic (Groppe et al., 2011) using a family-wise alpha level of 0.05, effectively controlling for the inflation of Type I error rates. All 30 cephalic electrodes and all 20 time scale factors were entered into these analyses, without averaging. Adopting a Monte Carlo approach, we used 1000 random between-participant permutations of the data to empirically approximate the distribution of the null hypothesis (i.e., no difference between age groups) for the contrasts of interest. The number of random permutations was based on previous suggestions in the literature (Manly, 1997). Based on this estimate, critical t-scores were derived and any between-group differences in the original data that exceeded the tmax statistic were deemed reliable. All statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB and R (R Development Core Team, 2008).
Results
Topographic distribution of brain signal variability
We first examined the spatial distribution of average MSE values (integrated over all time scales) across the scalp surface, ignoring chronological age. As illustrated in Figure 3, there was a clear posterior-to-anterior gradient, with sample entropy gradually decreasing over the frontal regions. This visual impression was confirmed by a main effect of Region (frontal, central, parieto-occipital; F2,926 = 503.59, Greenhouse-Geisser corrected p < 0.001, η2G = 0.04). Parieto-occipital regions exhibited enhanced MSE compared to the central and frontal regions, while the central electrodes exhibited increased MSE compared to the frontal channels (all ps < 0.001). Additionally, MSE values were greater for electrodes positioned over the right, compared to the left, cerebral hemisphere as indicated by a main effect of Laterality (left and right; F1,463 = 48.18, p < 0.001, η2G = 0.003).2
As is evident from examining the full sweep of MSE values (see Figure 2), both the regional and hemispheric differences increased in magnitude from fine to coarse scales. A set of paired permutation controlled t-test contrasts confirmed significant regional and hemispheric differences at virtually all time scales (all pperm < 0.05, from scales 2 to 20), except for the parieto-occipital region where the right hemispheric bias was only evident at the coarse time scales (scale factors from 10 to 20).
Age-related changes in moment-to-moment brain signal variability
To perform a detailed examination of differences in EEG signal variability as a function of chronological age, we conducted a series of mass univariate tests for each electrode and time scale factor. The results of these analyses are depicted in Figure 3. As can been seen from inspecting the t-test maps, eleven-year olds exhibited greater EEG sample entropy at nearly all electrode sites and time scales than seven-year olds. However, as the age gap diminished, the differences in sample entropy were most evident for the more anterior regions (e.g., the nine versus seven and eight versus seven-year-old contrasts). The seven to eight year age transition seems to mark a major stage in the development of EEG complexity, as the differences between eleven year olds and the older age groups grew increasingly sparse, circumscribed to the fine time scales and weakly lateralized over the right hemisphere. There were no observable instances of statistically reliable age-related reductions in sample entropy for the age ranges and time scales investigated here. To exclude the possibility that the children’s medication status influenced these findings, we performed an additional set of control analyses where we adopted a conservative approach excluding all children reported to be on a medication of any sort (including, for example, allergy medications). Although this set of exclusionary criteria led to a considerably reduced sample size relative to the original (seven year-olds N=84; eight year-olds N=71; nine year-olds N=70; ten year-olds N=66; eleven year-olds N=47), the mass univariate contrasts were almost identical to those shown in Figure 4.
Given that the most drastic age gains appeared to occur roughly during the seven to eight year transitional window, we next examined finer stratification solely within this age range. Figure 5 illustrates the grand mean MSE values amongst the two youngest cohorts, with sub-groups split into 6-month brackets. Although there were no sub-group differences that survived permutation-controlled correction for multiple testing between the youngest and oldest seven year olds, or the youngest and oldest eight year olds, there was nevertheless a linear progression in EEG MSE levels with increasing chronological age. Although not pictured, inter-cohort gaps grew progressively smaller beyond this particular range. The topographic maps highlight that the most salient sequential changes were roughly located within the anterior (fronto-central) zones.
Discussion
We investigated changes in endogenous (resting state) cortical fluctuations from early to late childhood in a large, cross-sectional community sample. Our main findings revealed that brain signal variability (an indicator of neuronal complexity) was greater in older children, with the largest changes seemingly occurring somewhere between the seven to eight year transition. Relative to seven year olds, the older age groups exhibited increased brain signal variability across a wide range of time scales. However, the spatial topography of these chronological effects varied as a function of cohort separation in years, with a widespread spatial distribution for the largest age difference (e.g., eleven year olds compared to seven year olds) and a primarily fronto-central distribution with a diminishing age gap (e.g., eight year olds compared to seven year olds). The eleven year olds continued to exhibit increased brain signal variability compared to eight, nine and ten year old cohorts; however, these effects were considerably weaker in magnitude and encompassed only the local time scales with a slight bias toward the more anterior zones. The increasingly anterior distribution of between-group differences with shrinking age gaps was, in some ways, a mirror image of the posterior-to-anterior gradient observed for the distribution of MSE values in the sample as a whole.
Our results extend previous work on the ontogenetic development of cortical fluctuations (see McIntosh et al., 2010, for a review) to the brain in a resting state. Unconstrained by sensory input or the execution of motor routines, ongoing large-scale neuronal activity is characterized by itinerant wandering along a set of linked, heteroclinic channels (Cabral et al., 2014; Deco et al., 2011, 2013). This meta-stability of neurophysiological time series signals is an emergent property of the underlying cortical architecture and neurochemistry and it reflects the fact that the brain is a loosely coupled system whose functional networks reflect both deterministic and stochastic processes (Buzsáki, 2006; Sporns, 2011). The age-dependent increase of moment-to-moment brain signal variability is a quantitative marker that reflects exploration of a growing dynamic repertoire of the cerebral cortex from early to late childhood. From a neural systems perceptive, the increased variability in the patterns of brain electrical activity likely emerges as a consequence of greater functional integration that gradually emerges in the developing cerebral cortex (Luna & Sweeney, 2004). An increased emphasis on network-level functional coordination can enrich neural constructivist theories of development, by accounting for age-dependent changes in cognitive function as arising from the increased information integration between hierarchically ‘lower’ and ‘higher’ cortical networks (Stevens, 2009). A corollary of this increase in the information content of neurophysiological signals, across multiple time scales, is the growing cognitive sophistication of the child brain – for example, the greater multi-scale entropy evident in EEG signals has been directly linked to age-dependent reductions in behavioral variability on simple decision tasks (McIntosh et al., 2008). Deviations from the normative pattern of increased EEG signal variability may be indicators of developmental risk. Reduced multi-scale entropy of brain signals has been successfully used to classify infants who are at high risk for neurodevelopmental disorders, such autism spectrum, compared to their typically developing counterparts (Bosl et al., 2011). An intriguing suggestion in the literature is that meta-stability of large-scale neuronal systems can serve as a proxy measure of cognitive potential (Deco et al., 2013). To further test these suggestions it will be important for future research to combine non-invasively obtained measures of brain signal variability in development with batteries of cognitive tests to probe putative brain-behavior links that are hypothesized to exist. Although we have emphasized the functional, adaptive aspects of brain signal variability, there is good reason to expect that this is scale-dependent and that the amount of noise has to be maintained within a particular range, outside of which impairments may arise (McDonnell & Ward, 2011). Excessive neuronal noise, or noise at specific spatio-temporal scales, is likely to impair functional communication and may be expected to underlie particular kinds of neuropsychiatric pathology (Takahashi et al., 2013, 2016).
Our observation that the topographic variation in MSE values is distributed along a general posterior-to-anterior gradient is reassuring insofar as it agrees with long established findings pertaining to a similar spatial structure in the development of EEG spectral power (Matousek & Petersen, 1973). To the extent that the dynamic range of neural activity is shaped by the underlying cortical architecture, it might reasonably be expected that MSE levels also follow a rostro-caudal arc, with the highest values in those regions that are most fully developed earlier in time (Colby et al., 2011; Gogtay et al., 2004; Guillery, 2005). The fact that between-group differences were increasingly confined to electrodes covering the more anterior zones as the age disparity between cohorts diminished is an interesting observation then, since it suggests that the most salient differences were roughly situated in those cortical regions that might have been the site of most active neuronal development. Converging evidence for this proposal comes from a previous study employing a distinct measure of neural complexity, which likewise reported the greatest age-related gains during childhood for electrodes covering the frontal associative cortices (Anokhin et al., 1996).
Although we had no a priori hypotheses concerning hemispheric differences, electrodes covering the right cerebral hemisphere tended to exhibit greater signal variability relative to those over the left hemisphere – especially at longer time scales that primarily reflect the contribution of slow cortical oscillations. Although this finding was unexpected, its regularity across all of the major anatomical clusters suggests that it is very likely not a fluke. Importantly, enhanced multi-scale entropy for electrodes covering the right cerebral hemisphere did not seem to differ as a function of handedness in our exploratory analysis. A potentially related finding is the observation of generally increased EEG coherence within the right compared to the left hemisphere of adults (Tucker et al., 1986), which was originally attributed to the greater white-to-gray matter ratio of the right hemisphere (Gur et al., 1980). Given evidence that increased functional connectivity is positively correlated with the variability of signals recorded using multiple imaging modalities (McDonough & Nashiro, 2014; Misic et al., 2011), this may provide a partial explanation for our findings. We remain skeptical of this interpretation, however, given that Barry and colleagues (2012) discovered enhanced left hemisphere EEG coherence in a sample of children that was similar in age to our community sample, and we are unaware of any systematic factors that would explain the apparent discrepancy.
Conclusion
Relative to the youngest cohort of children, the older groups exhibited uniformly increased brain signal variability across nearly all of the time scales investigated here. The results of our study stand to inform existing theories of development, especially those motivated by a dynamic systems perspective, which have emphasized the functional aspects of variability (Smith & Thelen, 2003). Notwithstanding the limitations of cross-sectional study designs when it comes to making inferences about intraindividual maturational trajectories, we believe that our large sample size represents a substantial contribution to the literature on the evolution of neuronal complexity during this sensitive developmental window. Our findings are also in close correspondence with previous studies that have investigated task-related EEG recordings (see McIntosh et al., 2010) as well as those that have used different mathematical estimates of signal complexity (Anokhin et al., 1996; Lindenberger, 1996; Müller & Lindenberger, 2012; Pierce et al., 2000). The spatial distribution of our effects suggests that MSE levels generally exhibited good correspondence with known spatial gradients of neural development. One promising avenue for future research will involve linking the neural dynamics documented here to a wider range of spatio-temporal scales as well as comprehensive cognitive batteries in order to explore the putative functional consequences of increased neuronal signal variability.
Acknowledgments
This project was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grant MH098060 awarded to B. E. Gibb and a Binghamton University (SUNY) Interdisciplinary Collaborative Grant awarded to V. Miskovic. We would like to thank Devra Alper, Cope Feurer, Eric Funk, Effua Sosoo, Katie Burkhouse, Mary Woody, Anastacia Kudinova, and Aliona Tsypes for their help in conducting assessments for this project.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
In contrast to algorithmic complexity which focuses solely on signal compressibility (Chaitin, 1977).
The Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) was administered to a sub-sample (56.3%) of the participants included in the present analyses. Within this sub-sample of participants, only approximately 11% were categorized as being left-handed. The MSE for electrodes covering the right hemisphere was consistently larger compared to the left hemisphere and this did not differ between the left- and righthanded children.
References
- Amari SI, Cichocki A, Yang HH. A new learning algorithm for blind signal separation. Advances in neural information processing systems. 1996:757–763. [Google Scholar]
- Anokhin AP, Birbaumer N, Lutzenberger W, Nikolaev A, Vogel F. Age increases brain complexity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1996;99:63–68. doi: 10.1016/0921-884x(96)95573-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bakeman R. Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behavior Research Methods. 2005;37:379–384. doi: 10.3758/bf03192707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry RJ, Clarke AR, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Johnstone SJ, Rushby JA. Age and gender effects in EEG coherence: I. Developmental trends in normal children. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2012;115:2252–2258. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural computation. 1995;7:1129–1159. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.6.1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beggs JM. The criticality hypothesis: how local cortical networks might optimize information processing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences. 2008;366:329–243. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2007.2092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosl W, Tierney A, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson C. EEG complexity as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorder risk. BMC Medicine. 2011;9:18. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britz J, Van De Ville D, Michel CM. BOLD correlates of EEG topography reveal rapid resting-state network dynamics. Neuroimage. 2010;52:1162–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G. Rhythms of the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004;304:1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabral J, Kringelbach ML, Deco G. Exploring the network dynamics underlying brain activity during rest. Progress in Neurobiology. 2014;114:102–131. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaitin GJ. Algorithmic information theory. IBM Journal of Research and Development. 1977;21:350–359. [Google Scholar]
- Colby JB, Van Horn JD, Sowell ER. Quantitative in vivo evidence for broad regional gradients in the timing of white matter maturation during adolescence. Neuroimage. 2011;54:25–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa M, Goldberger AL, Peng CK. Multiscale entropy analysis of biological signals. Physical Review E. 2005;71:021906. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.021906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. Resting brains never rest: computational insights into potential cognitive architectures. Trends in Neurosciences. 2013;36:268–274. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:43–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Kringelbach ML. Metastability and coherence: Extending the communication through coherence hypothesis using a whole-brain computational perspective. Trends in Neurosciences. 2016;39:125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel AK, Gerloff C, Hilgetag CC, Nolte G. Intrinsic coupling modes: multiscale interactions in ongoing brain activity. Neuron. 2013;80:867–886. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NU, Church JA, Miezin FM, et al. Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Computational Biology. 2009;5:e1000381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM. Noise in the nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. Transients, metastability, and neuronal dynamics. Neuroimage. 1997;5:164–171. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. The importance of being variable. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:4496–4503. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5641-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett DD, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. Blood oxygen level-dependent signal variability is more than just noise. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:4914–4921. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5166-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett DD, Samanez-Larkin GR, MacDonald SW, Lindenberger U, McIntosh AR, Grady CL. Moment-to-moment brain signal variability: a next frontier in human brain mapping? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;37:610–624. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh A, Rho Y, McIntosh AR, Kotter R, Jirsa VK. Noise during rest enables the exploration of the brain’s dynamic repertoire. PLoS Computational Biology. 2008;4(10):e1000196. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Thompson PM. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101(21):8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldberger AL, Amaral LA, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov P, Peng CK, Stanley HE. Fractal dynamics in physiology: alterations with disease and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2002;99(Suppl. 1):2466–2472. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012579499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golos M, Jirsa V, Daucé E. Multistability in large scale models of brain activity. PLoS Computational Biology. 2015;11:e1004644. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grady CL, Garrett DD. Understanding variability in the BOLD signal and why it matters for aging. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2014;8:274–283. doi: 10.1007/s11682-013-9253-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groppe DM, Urbach TP, Kutas M. Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: a critical tutorial review. Psychophysiology. 2011;48:1711–1725. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01273.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillery RW. Is postnatal neocortical maturation hierarchical? Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28:512–517. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gur RC, Packer IK, Hungerbuhler JP, Reivich M, Obrist WD, Amarnek WS, Sackheim HA. Differences in the distribution of gray and white matter in human cerebral hemispheres. Science. 1980;207:1226–1228. doi: 10.1126/science.7355287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen EC, Battaglia D, Spiegler A, Deco G, Jirsa VK. Functional connectivity dynamics: modeling the switching behavior of the resting state. Neuroimage. 2015;105:525–535. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heisz JJ, Shedden JM, McIntosh AR. Relating brain signal variability to knowledge representation. Neuroimage. 2012;63:1384–1392. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey CJ, Kötter R, Breakspear M, Sporns O. Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2007;104:10240–10245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701519104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JA. Multistability and metastability: understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;367:906–918. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khanna A, Pascual-Leone A, Michel CM, Farzan F. Microstates in resting-state EEG: current status and future directions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2015;49:105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention and controlled access to stored information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16:606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lippé S, Kovacevic N, McIntosh AR. Differential maturation of brain signal complexity in the human auditory and visual system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2009;3:48. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.048.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu ZX, Woltering S, Lewis MD. Developmental change in EEG theta activity in the medial prefrontal cortex during response control. Neuroimage. 2014;85(Part 2):873–887. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis NK, Murayama Y, Augath M, Steffen T, Werner J, Oeltermann A. How not to study spontaneous activity. Neuroimage. 2009;45:1080–1089. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luna B, Sweeney JA. The emergence of collaborative brain function: FMRI studies of the development of response inhibition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021:296–309. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makeig S, Bell AJ, Jung T-P, Sejnowski TJ. Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic data. In: Touretzky D, Mozer M, Hasselmo M, editors. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Vol. 8. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1996. pp. 145–151. [Google Scholar]
- Makeig S, Bell T, Lee TW, Jung TP, Enghoff S. Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience, Institute of Neural Computation. University of San Diego California; 2000. EEGLAB: ICA toolbox for psychophysiological research. WWW Site. Retrieved from www.sccn.ucsd.edu/eeglab. [Google Scholar]
- Manly BFJ. Randomization, bootstrap and Monte Carlo methods in biology. 2nd. London, UK: Chapman & Hall; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Manoel EDJ, Connolly KJ. Variability and the development of skilled motor actions. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 1995;19:129–147. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(94)00078-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manor B, Lipsitz LA. Physiologic complexity and aging: implications for physical function and rehabilitation. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2013;45:287–293. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathes B, Khalaidovski K, Wienke AS, Schmiedt-Fehr C, Basar-Eroglu C. Maturation of the P3 and concurrent oscillatory processes during adolescence. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.04.019. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matousek M, Petersen I. Automatic evaluation of EEG background activity by means of age-dependent EEG quotients. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1973;55:603–612. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(73)90213-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonnell MD, Ward LM. The benefits of noise in neural systems: Bridging theory and experiment. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2011;12:415–425. doi: 10.1038/nrn3061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonough IM, Nishira K. Network complexity as a measure of information processing across resting-state networks: evidence from the Human Connectome Project. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:409. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntosh AR, Kovacevic N, Itier RJ. Increased brain signal variability accompanies lower behavioral variability in development. PLoS Computational Biology. 2008;4(7):e1000106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntosh AR, Kovacevic N, Lippe S, Garrett D, Grady C, Jirsa V. The development of a noisy brain. Archives Italiennes de Biologie. 2010;148:323–327. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer-Lindenberg A. The evolution of complexity in human brain development: An EEG study. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1996;99:405–411. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(96)95699-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Misic B, Mills T, Taylor MJ, McIntosh AR. Brain noise is task dependent and region specific. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;104:2667–2676. doi: 10.1152/jn.00648.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Misic B, Vakorin VA, Paus T, McIntosh AR. Functional embedding predicts the variability of neural activity. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2011;5:90. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miskovic V, Ma X, Chou CA, Fan M, Owens M, Sayama H, Gibb BE. Developmental changes in spontaneous electrocortical activity and network organization from early to late childhood. Neuroimage. 2015;118:237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muldoon SF, Bridgeford EW, Bassett DS. Small-world propensity and weighted brain networks. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:22057. doi: 10.1038/srep22057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller V, Lindenberger U. Lifespan differences in nonlinear dynamics during rest and auditory oddball performance. Developmental Science. 2012;15:540–556. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01153.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakagawa TT, Jirsa VK, Spiegler A, McIntosh AR, Deco G. Bottom-up modeling of the connectome: linking structure and function in the resting brain and their changes in aging. Neuroimage. 2013;80:318–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papenberg G, Hämmerer D, Müller V, Lindenberger U, Li SC. Lower theta inter-trial phase coherence during performance monitoring is related to higher reaction time variability: A lifespan study. Neuroimage. 2013;83:912–920. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce TW, Kelly SP, Watson TD, Replogle D, King JS, Pribram KH. Age differences in dynamic measures of EEG. Brain Topography. 2000;13:127–134. doi: 10.1023/a:1026659102713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinneo LR. On noise in the nervous system. Psychological Review. 1966;73:242–247. doi: 10.1037/h0023240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pletzer B, Kerschbaum H, Klimesch W. When frequencies never synchronize: the golden mean and the resting EEG. Brain Research. 2010;1335:91–102. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabinovich M, Huerta R, Laurent G. Transient dynamics for neural processing. Science. 2008;321:48–50. doi: 10.1126/science.1155564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richman JS, Moorman JR. Physiological time-series analysis using approximate entropy and sample entropy. American Journal of Physiology. 2000;278:H2039–H2049. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.2000.278.6.H2039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roland PE, Hilgetag CC, Deco G. Cortico-cortical communication dynamics. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2014;8:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen K, Hutchison RM, Bezgin G, Everling S, McIntosh AR. Network structure shapes spontaneous functional connectivity dynamics. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:5579–5588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4903-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shew WL, Plenz D. The functional benefits of criticality in the cortex. The Neuroscientist. 2012;19:88–100. doi: 10.1177/1073858412445487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith LB, Thelen E. Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:343–348. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00156-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O. Networks of the brain. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Stevens MC. The developmental cognitive neuroscience of functional connectivity. Brain and Cognition. 2009;70:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Supekar K, Musen M, Menon V. Development of large-scale functional brain networks in children. PLoS Biology. 2009;7:e1000157. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi T. Complexity of spontaneous brain activity in mental disorders. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2013;45:258–266. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi T, Yoshimura Y, Hiraishi H, Hasegawa C, Munesue T, Higashida H, et al. Enhanced brain signal variability in children with autism spectrum disorder during early childhood. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37:1038–1050. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tognoli E, Kelso JA. The metastable brain. Neuron. 2014;81:35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G, Edelman GM, Sporns O. Complexity and coherency: integrating information in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998;2:474–484. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01259-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker DM, Roth DL, Bair TB. Functional connections among cortical regions: topography of EEG coherence. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1986;63:242–250. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(86)90092-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhlhaas PJ, Roux F, Singer W, Haenschel C, Sireteanu R, Rodriguez E. The development of neural synchrony reflects late maturation and restructuring of functional networks in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2009;106:9866–9871. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900390106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vakorin VA, Lippé S, McIntosh AR. Variability of brain signals processed locally transforms into higher connectivity with brain development. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:6405–6413. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3153-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vakorin VA, McIntosh AR. Mapping the multi-scale information content of complex brain signals. In: Rabinovich M, Friston K, Varona P, editors. Principles of brain dynamics: global state interactions. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2012. pp. 183–208. [Google Scholar]
- Vakorin VA, McIntosh AR, Misic B, Krakovska O, Poulsen C, Martinu K, Paus T. Exploring age-related changes in dynamical non-stationarity in electroencephalographic signals during early adolescence. PLoS One. 2013;8:e57217. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voytek B, Knight RT. Dynamic network communication as a unifying neural basis for cognition, development, aging, and disease. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;77:1089–1097. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward LM. Physics of neural synchronisation mediated by stochastic resonance. Contemporary Physics. 2009;50:563–574. [Google Scholar]
- Werner G. Brain dynamics across levels of organization. Journal of Physiology - Paris. 2007;101:273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2007.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang AC, Huang CC, Liu ME, Liou YJ, Hong CJ, Lo MT, et al. The APOE ε4 allele affects complexity and functional connectivity of resting brain activity in healthy adults. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35:3238–3248. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]