Abstract
Oceanic fronts are key habitats for a diverse range of marine predators, yet how they influence fine-scale foraging behaviour is poorly understood. Here, we investigated the dive behaviour of northern gannets Morus bassanus in relation to shelf-sea fronts. We GPS (global positioning system) tracked 53 breeding birds and examined the relationship between 1901 foraging dives (from time-depth recorders) and thermal fronts (identified via Earth Observation composite front mapping) in the Celtic Sea, Northeast Atlantic. We (i) used a habitat-use availability analysis to determine whether gannets preferentially dived at fronts, and (ii) compared dive characteristics in relation to fronts to investigate the functional significance of these oceanographic features. We found that relationships between gannet dive probabilities and fronts varied by frontal metric and sex. While both sexes were more likely to dive in the presence of seasonally persistent fronts, links to more ephemeral features were less clear. Here, males were positively correlated with distance to front and cross-front gradient strength, with the reverse for females. Both sexes performed two dive strategies: shallow V-shaped plunge dives with little or no active swim phase (92% of dives) and deeper U-shaped dives with an active pursuit phase of at least 3 s (8% of dives). When foraging around fronts, gannets were half as likely to engage in U-shaped dives compared with V-shaped dives, independent of sex. Moreover, V-shaped dive durations were significantly shortened around fronts. These behavioural responses support the assertion that fronts are important foraging habitats for marine predators, and suggest a possible mechanistic link between the two in terms of dive behaviour. This research also emphasizes the importance of cross-disciplinary research when attempting to understand marine ecosystems.
Keywords: seabird, marine predator, oceanography, ocean front, diving behaviour, foraging ecology
1. Introduction
Large marine predators, such as marine mammals, seabirds, turtles and sharks, forage over long distances in dynamic environments where prey are patchily distributed [1,2]. Many of these predators display targeted and individually consistent movement patterns [3–6], that are frequently linked to physical oceanographic features, including fronts [7–9], eddies [7,10], tidal flow fields [11] and regions of stratification [12], where low- to mid-trophic-level prey accessibility and availability is enhanced [13,14].
Ocean fronts are important habitats for an array of taxonomically diverse marine predators [7–9]. Fronts are physical structures, occurring between adjacent water masses of differing properties that produce strong gradients in density, temperature and/or salinity [15]. Physical attributes of fronts promote enhanced primary productivity [16,17] and biomass accumulation/redistribution [18], often in a predictable manner. This may have bottom-up effects that propagate across multiple trophic levels, resulting in the formation of dense, and sometimes shallow, aggregations of prey [19–21] that may be important for upper trophic-level consumers [22]. However, while such mechanisms are often posited to explain why fronts make attractive foraging habitats, to date, such links have yet to be fully explored. This shortfall is mostly owing to the logistical challenges of simultaneously measuring oceanography, lower- to mid-trophic-level prey and large marine predators at appropriate spatio-temporal scales.
Interactions between predators and oceanography have been predominantly revealed through the analysis of two-dimensional horizontal animal movement data obtained either directly from shipboard observations [23] or remotely through animal-borne telemetry [24]. However, for diving predators, much can be learnt through understanding vertical movements, particularly in species that employ different dive strategies. Specifically, changes in three-dimensional predator behaviours may be coupled to the fine-scale horizontal and vertical distributions of their prey [25–27]. As such, understanding how large marine predators respond to fronts in the vertical dimension may help us resolve the functional mechanisms that link the two.
Advances in biologging technologies mean we are now able to observe fine-scale animal movements in three dimensions across entire foraging trips [28]. These data can be supplemented with near real-time remotely sensed information on biological and physical oceanography. However, the relevance of traditionally used measurements such as sea surface temperature (SST) and surface chlorophyll a concentration has, at times, proved questionable, and these oceanographic descriptors are not always good at predicting the distributions of marine predators [29,30]. Composite front mapping [31–33] is a relatively new technique that attempts to address this by objectively identifying discrete oceanographic frontal features that are, a priori, thought to represent prosperous foraging habitats. In addition, features are quantified through the output of several front metric products, that can be derived over a number of spatio-temporal scales [32,34] allowing the dynamic nature of a front to be characterized [8,35] while also overcoming problems of cloud obfuscating signal. This may be particularly useful in highly dynamic environments where passing ephemeral activity can weaken links to marine predators [35].
In this study, we used the northern gannet, Morus bassanus, to investigate the influence of shelf-sea fronts on the distributions and characteristics of dives. We used bird-borne global positioning system (GPS) loggers and time-depth recorders (TDRs), deployed on centrally placed breeders from a large colony in the Celtic Sea (Grassholm, Wales, UK), to link dive events with multiple products derived from weekly and seasonal composite front maps. Specifically we asked: (i) do gannets preferentially dive around fronts, (ii) do gannets change dive strategy (i.e. dive shape) around fronts and (iii) do the depths and durations of dives decrease around fronts?
Gannets are large, medium-ranging piscivorous marine predators [5,36] that exhibit a nested search strategy, and dive infrequently, presumably only when prey have been located [5,37]. Foraging strategies range from short shallow plunge dives, to longer and deeper wing-propelled active pursuit dives that can reach up to 25 m in depth [37,38]. Gannets in the Celtic Sea feed on a variety of forage and pelagic fish such as mackerel Scomber scombrus, garfish Belone belone, herring Clupea harengus and sprat Sprattus sprattus, and a range of demersal fishes, scavenged from fishing boats [39–41]. Previous work has shown that both in the Celtic Sea and the Benguela upwelling region off western South Africa, gannets increase foraging effort (estimated via two-dimensional movement data) within regions where frontal activity is increased [35,42]. By examining their diving behaviour, we attempt to establish why.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Device deployment
Fieldwork was conducted on Grassholm, Wales, UK (51°43′ N, 5°28′ W; figure 1), during the breeding season in July 2012 and 2013. Chick-rearing gannets were caught on the nest during changeover (to ensure chicks were not left unattended, and so deployments began immediately with a foraging trip), using a brass crook attached to the end of an approximately 5 m carbon fibre pole. Birds were selected opportunistically, away from the edge of the colony (under licence from Natural Resources Wales). Upon capture, birds were weighed (to the nearest 50 g) and 1–2 ml of blood taken via the tarsal vein (under licence from the UK Home Office), a small aliquot of which was later used for molecular sexing (commercially outsourced to AvianBiotech.com). Birds were then equipped, using Tesa® tape, with (i) a 30 g GPS logger (i-gotU GT-120, Mobile Active Technology Inc.) attached to either the dorsal surface of the central pair of tail feathers (2012) or the central back feathers (2013) and (ii) a 5.7 g or 10.5 g TDR (CEFAS G5 or LOTEK LAT 1810, respectively) attached to the ventral surface of the central pair of tail feathers (under licence from the British Trust for Ornithology). Total handling time was around 12 min. The maximum combined weight of deployed loggers (40.5 g) was 1.37% the average bird body weight (2948.8 g ± 33.0 g). Deployment durations ranged from 1 to 7 days.
2.2. Particulars of device data acquisition
The GPS recorder logged location at 1 min intervals with an accuracy of ±4.4 m [43]. The G5 TDR logged pressure and temperature every 0.1 s (10 Hz) during dives, defined as wet periods (detected via a wet/dry sensor) below a depth of 1.5 m. Pressure resolution was 4 cm of water column with an accuracy of ±1 m. The LOTEK LAT 1810 TDR logged temperature and pressure continuously at 1 s intervals (1 Hz). Pressure resolution was 2.5 cm of water column with an accuracy of ±1 m.
2.3. Animal behaviour metrics
2.3.1. Global positioning system tracks
GPS fixes at night (between the end of civil dusk to the beginning of civil dawn) were stripped from the dataset to eliminate periods when birds rest on the water [44,45]. In addition, all activity within 2 km of the breeding colony was removed to account for bathing and rafting [46]. Tracks were then split into individual foraging trips. In some instances, the device deployment period exceeded the battery life of the GPS logger. Resultant incomplete foraging trips were included in analyses, unless otherwise stated.
2.3.2. Dive events
A bespoke algorithm, written in Matlab, was used to identify dive events by birds equipped with a LOTEK TDR. To be consistent with the technicalities of the CEFAS logger, dives were defined as periods where the registered depth was at least 1.5 m. This also accounted for shallow subsurface activity associated with non-foraging behaviours such as sitting on the water and bathing [38,47].
To allocate a location for each dive, GPS tracks were first interpolated to a resolution of 1 s (in time), using a cubic spline interpolation. The timestamp at the beginning of a dive event was then used to assign a corresponding location from the high-resolution interpolation. Allocations mismatched by more than a second (owing to the GPS logger battery life being surpassed or because a dive occurred within the colony exclusion zone) were excluded from analyses. A small number of shallow dives (five in total) occurred approximately 40 min after the end of civil dusk. These were considered atypical and excluded from analyses [44,45].
2.3.3. Dive characteristics
Gannets predominantly employ two dive strategies (figure 1a,b). Short and typically shallow plunge dives (termed V-shaped; figure 1a) involve little or no active swim phase [38,48], and may be used to catch fast, responsive prey in the upper section of the water column [49–51]. Indeed, this strategy is often associated with the predation of near-surface pelagic fish such as herring and mackerel [49,50]. In contrast, longer deeper pursuit dives (U-shaped; figure 1b) involve a substantial active swim phase that likely incurs higher energetic costs [37,52]. As such, this foraging strategy may only be used following a failed plunge attempt [37,48], or when prey are distributed at deeper depths (e.g. shoals of capelin Mallotus villosus or sand eels Ammodytes sp. at depth and/or near the seabed; [49,51]). In some instances, these dives may also be used to catch multiple prey items [51], although this is likely to be dependent upon the responsiveness and swimming speeds of prey [49,50]. To be able to examine if/how fronts influence dive strategy (which may reflect changes in prey accessibility and catchability), each dive was classified as either U- or V-shaped (figure 1a,b), as determined by the length of the active swim phase (U-shaped dives were those with an active swim phase of at least 3 or at least 4 s dependent upon the logger sampling rate; 1 s and 10 Hz, respectively; [38]). The start and end of the active swim phase was defined, using gradients in the vertical change in depth (electronic supplementary material, S1 and figure S1; [37,53]). In addition, for each dive, the maximum dive depth (metres) and total dive duration (seconds) was calculated.
2.4. Front metrics
Fronts vary in their strength, persistence and predictability [8]. To be able to determine the relative importance of these characteristics on foraging behaviour, thermal front activity across the Celtic Sea was summarized into three metrics that reflected this variability: (i) cross-front gradient strength Gdens, (ii) distance to closest front Fdist, and (iii) seasonal front frequency Ffreq. These were produced over two temporal scales: (i) 7 day composites [31] and (ii) seasonal composites [32], details of which are provided below (see §2.4.1 Seven day composites and §2.4.2 Seasonal composites). As the occurrences of thermal and chlorophyll fronts are typically linked across shelf-seas (e.g. at tidal-mixing fronts; [34,54]), we did not include extra analyses, using chlorophyll a derived front metrics in this study.
2.4.1. Seven day composites
Gdens and Fdist were based on 7 day composite front maps centred to the date of a GPS/TDR fix (figure 2). First, raw (level 0) advanced very high-resolution radiometer (AVHRR) infrared data were converted to an index of SST (level 2). SST data were then mapped across the Celtic Sea with a spatial resolution of approximately 1.2 km2/pixel. Thermal fronts were detected over frames of 32 × 32 pixels, using single image edge detection (SIED; [55]) with a temperature difference threshold of 0.4°C across the front [31] comparable to [35]. The SIED front map generated from a single satellite image is unsuitable for the description of fronts owing to cloud cover in the study region. Therefore, all frontal segments obtained during the 7 day window were combined to obtain a more synoptic frontal picture [31]. If cloud persisted for the entire 7 days in certain regions, then these were marked as missing in the front metrics, and any corresponding bird tracks were excluded from analysis.
Gdens was then taken as the spatially smoothed average temperature gradient across all frontal pixels detected over a 7 day composite to give a continuous distribution of frontal intensity. A Gaussian filter with a width of five pixels was used for the spatial smoothing [34,35]. This metric indicates the intensity of contemporaneous frontal structures that may be either transient or persistent in occurrence.
Fdist was taken as the distance from any point to the closest simplified front. Simplified fronts were defined, using a clustering algorithm to identify continuous contours through the strongest frontal pixels on the spatially smoothed Gdens front map [35]. This metric quantifies the distance from each dive/pseudo-absence location (see §2.5 Statistical analysis) to the continuous and discrete surface signature of more defined frontal systems, and generally excludes the smaller ephemeral features that are often picked up by the Gdens metric.
2.4.2. Seasonal composites
Ffreq was based on all front maps generated between June and August inclusive for each year (2012 and 2013) separately (figure 1). Maps generated before spatial smoothing were used, and Ffreq was taken as the percentage of total detections in which a frontal temperature gradient greater than or equal to 0.04°C was observed (see Average front gradient in [31]). This threshold reduced noise in front detections associated with minor discrepancies in temperature observations [32]. This metric indicates areas where fronts frequently manifest across a season and so are generally persistent and highly predictable in occurrence.
2.5. Statistical analysis
Three modelling approaches were used to investigate the influence of our three front metrics (Gdens, Fdist and Ffreq), on gannet dive behaviour (table 1). First, habitat use versus availability was modelled against frontal activity. This was achieved using generalized linear mixed effects models (GLMMs) from the MASS package in R [56] with a binomial error structure and complementary log–log (cloglog) link function [57,58]. For each dive event, the locations of five pseudo-absences were randomly selected from within the bounds of the 95% utilization distribution of the population sample (see electronic supplementary material, S2 and figure S2; [59]). Second, dive shape was modelled against frontal activity. This approach also used GLMMs from the MASS package in R [56] with a binomial error structure and complementary log–log (cloglog) link function. Finally, the influence of frontal activity on dive depth and duration was modelled separately for U- and V-shaped dives using linear mixed effects models (LMMs) from the R package nlme [60].
Table 1.
(a) Generalized linear mixed effects model (GLMM) with binomial error structure and complementary log–log (cloglog) link function |
(b) GLMM with binomial error structure and complementary log–log (cloglog) link function |
(c) linear mixed effects model (LMM) |
Across the three analyses, the Gdens, Fdist and Ffreq front metrics were tested for in separate models to avoid issues pertaining from multicollinearity. Sex was included as a fixed factor and in a two-way interaction with each front metric to account for sexual segregation in gannet foraging behaviour [47,53,61]. Time of day (DecTime) was included in the dive shape, depth and duration models as a continuous quadratic function to allow for potential changes in the profile of a dive in the middle of the day, possibly owing to the diel migration of prey above and below the thermocline [38,62]. In all models, a random intercept of bird was included to avoid pseudo-replication and account for any individual differences in dive behaviour [6]. Where appropriate, a nested autocorrelation structure was also incorporated to allow for similarities between dives that occurred in temporal clusters. In the habitat-use availability analysis, this was fitted as a spatial correlation structure, using the coordinates of each dive or pseudo-absence location on a universal transverse Mercator projection, whilst for the dive shape, depth and duration analyses, a continuous time correlation structure was used (table 1).
As GLMMs from the MASS package are estimated using penalized quasi-likelihood, maximum-likelihood (ML) selection techniques (e.g. Akaike's information criteria, AIC) were not available, and so the best temporal correlation structure (e.g. exponential, rational quadratic, autoregressive) for the random component (table 1) of the model was selected through inspection of residual plots. Model reduction of the fixed component (table 1) was performed by removing variables with small parameter estimates, relatively large standard errors, confidence intervals that passed through zero and large p-values [63–65].
For each LMM, the most appropriate random structure (table 1) was determined via restricted maximum-likelihood (REML) estimation [57] and selected by a comparison of AIC values and residual plots. Model selection of the fixed effects (table 1) was conducted by backwards and forwards selection via ML estimation and the use of AIC and likelihood ratio tests. The most parsimonious model was then refitted using REML to obtain parameter estimates and associated p-values [57].
Models were evaluated by plotting Pearson (GLMM) or normalized (LMM) residuals against all potential explanatory variables, bird ID, distance to colony, latitude, longitude, tag type, time and year to check for any patterns indicative of a violation of model assumptions. Fitted versus predicted values were inspected to check for satisfactory model fit, and for the binomial GLMMs the area under the receiving operator characteristic curve (AUC; [66,67]) was calculated. Pseudo-R2-values were generated as an indication of variance explained [68], using the MuMIn package in R [69]. All analyses were performed in R v. 3.0.2 (R Development Core Team 2013) and Matlab R2011b.
3. Results
3.1. Gannet tracking data
All 53 birds equipped with GPS and TDR loggers yielded useable data: 11 males and 11 females in 2012 and 17 females and 14 males in 2013. This produced a total of 74 complete and 12 partial foraging trips (figure 1). The number of complete foraging trips per bird averaged 1.4 ± 0.08 (range 1–4).
Maximum displacement (the furthest distance from the colony) averaged 138.5 ± 8.0 km (range 34.6–276.7 km), trip lengths averaged 424.0 ± 25.8 km (range 72.0–822.9 km) and trip duration averaged 23.2 ± 1.5 h (range 2.7–50.6 h).
3.2. Overview of gannet diving behaviour
The total number of dives made across all foraging trips was 1901 (figure 1); 712 and 1189 of these were made by males and females, respectively. The number of dives made per complete foraging trip averaged 22.2 ± 3.8, although 17.6% of these trips had no dives. All incomplete foraging trips included dive events. Six birds did not dive at all during deployments.
Dives tended to occur in short bursts rather than being spread out equally over the foraging trip. 25.1%, 39.9% and 58.2% of all dives were followed by another dive within 5, 10 and 20 min respectively, and only 22.9% of dives were spaced more than 1 h from the previous dive. Across complete foraging trips, the number of dives made per hour ranged from 0 to 5.5 with an average of 0.9 ± 0.13. The percentage of time spent underwater during a complete foraging trip was low and ranged from 0% to 0.7% with an average of 0.1 ± 0.02%.
3.3. Dive behaviour at fronts
3.3.1. Dive events
Gannets dived in a wide range of oceanographic conditions including areas of high frontal activity (figure 1). When compared with the available habitat types, dive distributions were significantly related to all three of the front metrics (Fdist, Ffreq and Gdens), although relationships varied by sex (figure 3 and table 2). Male dive probabilities were positively correlated with distance to front (Fdist), cross-front gradient strength (Gdens) and (weakly) with seasonal front frequency (Ffreq). Female dive probabilities were negatively correlated with distance to front (Fdist) and cross-front gradient strength (Gdens), and positively correlated with seasonal front frequency (Ffreq; figure 3 and table 2).
Table 2.
fixed effect | estimate | s.e. | lower CI | upper CI | p-value | pseudo-R2 (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(a) Gdens: AUC = 0.53 | ||||||
Intercept | −2.675 | 0.137 | −2.944 | −2.407 | <0.001 | — |
Sex (female) | −0.133 | 0.178 | −0.492 | 0.226 | 0.460 | — |
Gdens | 2.402 | 1.273 | −0.093 | 4.897 | 0.059 | — |
Sex (female) * Gdens | −5.593 | 1.819 | −9.159 | −2.028 | 0.002 | 5.0 |
(b) Fdist: AUC = 0.56 | ||||||
intercept | −2.745 | 0.171 | −3.080 | −2.411 | <0.001 | — |
Sex (female) | 0.014 | 0.223 | −0.436 | 0.464 | 0.949 | — |
Fdist | 0.012 | 0.007 | −0.001 | 0.026 | 0.077 | — |
Sex (female) * Fdist | −0.027 | 0.011 | −0.048 | −0.006 | 0.013 | 3.37 |
(c) Ffreq: AUC = 0.55 | ||||||
intercept | −2.745 | 0.167 | −3.070 | −2.417 | <0.001 | — |
Sex (female) | −0.620 | 0.203 | −1.029 | −0.212 | 0.004 | — |
Ffreq | 0.006 | 0.006 | −0.007 | 0.018 | 0.366 | — |
Sex (female) * Ffreq | 0.027 | 0.008 | 0.012 | 0.042 | <0.001 | 11.5 |
3.3.2. Dive shape
V-shaped (92.1% of dives) dives were more common than U-shaped dives (7.9% of dives; figure 4). All birds performed V-shaped dives and 69.6% performed U-shaped dives. Dive shape varied significantly with front frequency (Ffreq; figure 5 and table 3). When diving in areas of high front frequency, the probability that a bird performed a U-shaped dive compared with a V-shaped dive halved (from approx. 0.12 to 0.06). Dive shape did not change in response to DecTime, any of the other front metrics (Fdist and Gdens), sex or an interaction between sex and frontal activity.
Table 3.
fixed effect | estimate | s.e. | lower CI | upper CI | p-value | pseudo-R2 (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | −1.981 | 0.180 | −2.334 | −1.627 | <0.001 | — |
Ffreq | −0.016 | 0.006 | −0.028 | −0.004 | 0.011 | 3.3 |
3.3.3. Dive depth
Overall dive depths ranged from 1.6 to 14.9 m and were greater when birds performed U-shaped compared with V-shaped dives (figure 4). V- and U-shaped dive depths were not significantly related to any of the front metrics. Females consistently dived deeper than males when performing V-shaped dives (4.6 ± 0.4 versus 3.4 ± 0.3 m respectively; p = 0.003). U-shaped dive depths averaged 6.3 ± 0.3 m and did not differ between sexes. DecTime had no influence on dive depth for either strategy, and there was no significant effect of an interaction between sex and frontal activity.
3.3.4. Dive duration
Overall dive durations ranged from 0.7 to 39.1 s and were greater when birds engaged in U-shaped compared with V-shaped dives (figure 4). V-shaped dives were significantly shorter in proximity to fronts (Fdist; table 4 and figure 6). This response was more prominent in males (table 4 and figure 6), who had significantly shorter dives than females (table 4). V-shaped dive duration was not significantly related to any of the other front metrics (Ffreq and Gdens). V-shaped dives were longer in the middle of the day (DecTime; table 4 and electronic supplementary material, figure S3). U-shaped dive duration averaged 12.9 ± 0.8 s and did not vary in response to DecTime, any of the front metrics, sex or an interaction between sex and frontal activity.
Table 4.
fixed effect | estimate | s.e. | lower CI | upper CI | p-value | pseudo-R2 (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 1.468 | 0.348 | 0.785 | 2.152 | <0.001 | — |
Sex (female) | 0.686 | 0.236 | 0.210 | 1.161 | 0.006 | — |
Fdist | 0.027 | 0.007 | 0.014 | 0.040 | <0.001 | — |
DecTime | 3.754 | 1.317 | 1.171 | 6.337 | 0.004 | — |
DecTime2 | −2.965 | 1.232 | −5.381 | −0.549 | 0.016 | 1.2 |
Sex (female) * Fdist | −0.021 | 0.009 | −0.038 | −0.003 | 0.020 | 2.8 |
4. Discussion
This study provides novel insights into the influence of physical oceanography on habitat use and dive behaviour by a medium-ranging piscivorous predator. We build upon prior observations that gannets intensify restricted search behaviours in areas with seasonally persistent fronts [35] to show that these habitats are also favoured for diving, although there was variation between the sexes. In addition, we show that when diving around fronts, gannets are half as likely to engage in U-shaped compared with V-shaped dives and the average duration of V-shaped dives is significantly shortened, which was independent of sex. Combined, these findings are of particular significance because, as well as confirming the importance of shelf-sea fronts as foraging habitat, differences in diving behaviour provide a possible functional mechanism underpinning the links between large marine predators and these physical features.
4.1. Persistent fronts as predictable foraging habitats
Our analysis of gannet dive distributions showed females and, to a lesser extent, males, preferentially dived in regions of persistent frontal activity. This adds to a growing body of evidence documenting the importance of such features as foraging habitats for marine predators [21,34,35,70–72]. However, patterns were far less clear around shorter-term, more ephemeral fronts. Here, male dive probabilities were positively correlated with distance to front (Fdist) and cross-front gradient strength (Gdens), with the reverse for females. Across shelf-seas, persistence is a key feature of the bioaggregating fronts [8] that are associated with high levels of primary productivity and biomass accumulation which sustains low- to mid-trophic-level enhancement and increases prey abundance (e.g. tidal-mixing fronts; [16,20,21,73]). Moreover, dependent upon spatio-temporal scale [74,75], these features may occur in a highly predictable manner [34], which likely aids individuals in efficiently locating their prey through learning, knowledge transfer and/or memory [4,76–80]. The significance of small ephemeral features (Gdens) therefore requires further study.
4.2. Subsurface dive behaviour around fronts
The principal purpose of this study was to investigate the subsurface movements of gannets in relation to shelf-sea fronts, to better understand their functional significance. In addition to providing persistent and predictable foraging habitats, fronts are also thought to increase the catchability and accessibility of prey [20,22]. In gannets, foraging strategies are especially energetically expensive [48,52,81,82], and to maximize efficiency individuals adjust their underwater movements in response to the behaviours and depth distributions of their prey [49,51,83,84]. V-shaped dives dominated gannet foraging strategies across the Celtic Sea, which possibly suggests this method of prey capture is better suited than a U-shaped dive strategy to the types of prey naturally encountered in the region (e.g. fast swimming pelagic species; [39,49,50]). We hypothesize that the shorter duration and more frequent use of passive V-shaped dives (i.e. with little or no active swim phase) around fronts is related to improvements in the availability of this prey.
Strong biophysical coupling at fronts is thought to attract large numbers of the mid-trophic-level fish (e.g. garfish, herring and mackerel) that gannets feed on [39]. Resultant high prey densities [22] may increase encounter probabilities [85] which aid in capture, allowing for faster and shorter dives [37,48]. Moreover, pursuit dives (U-shaped) are thought to be less suitable for catching highly responsive shoaling fish [86], which instead are better ambushed during fast V-shaped dives [50]. Fronts may also concentrate fish close to the surface, making them easier for gannets to catch. This is due to increased primary productivity and biomass accumulation around the near-surface thermocline [16,20,73], and because some fish may actively avoid cool bottom-boundary layer waters (e.g. mackerel; [50,87]). V-shaped dives were shallower than U-shaped dives (3.43/4.62 m for males and females respectively versus 6.32 m) and so an increase in their use around fronts may reflect this shallow distribution of fish [37,51]. In some instances, gannets may additionally cue in on other marine predators such as cetaceans [88–90] or other seabirds [90] that also forage around fronts [71,91,92]. Under such instances, mixed-species foraging aggregations may increase foraging success through the disorganization of school cohesiveness [93], or by preventing prey escaping to deeper waters when under attack [94].
4.3. Identifying important habitat features for foraging marine predators in dynamic ecological systems
There was a low signal-to-noise ratio across all of our analyses, and as such, the resultant variances explained were low (less than 12%) whilst model fits were sometimes poor (AUC of use-availability models less than 0.6; [66]). This was likely to be a by-product of the highly dynamic nature of shelf-sea environments coupled to the multiple trophic connections required to link physical features to gannets. However, despite this complexity, we were able to identify biologically plausible mechanisms at play, consistent with our a priori predictions about the importance of fronts as foraging habitat. Composite front mapping techniques probably played an important role in this by objectively defining and identifying frontal features across multiple spatio-temporal scales, which allowed transient ephemeral features (Gdens) to be separated from the more persistent and predictable fronts used by gannets (Ffreq).
In some instances, suitable habitat locations may be unused owing to mechanisms other than those being investigated. For example, frequent frontal zones around the coast of Ireland (figure 1) were probably avoided as a result of competition with neighbouring colonies and resultant space segregation [95], whilst those occurring around the mouth of the Bristol Channel are possibly associated with waters whose turbidity hinders the foraging ability of a visual forager such as the gannet [96]. The use of a habitat-use availability analysis, that included only areas deemed accessible for foraging as defined by kernel density analyses of GPS tracks, minimized the influence of these potentially interfering processes.
Individual and sex-specific foraging specialization may further obfuscate relationships between gannet dive behaviours and physical oceanography [6,40,41,53,61]. Correlations between the dive distributions of males and frequent frontal zones (Ffreq) were weaker than those observed with females (figure 3c,d). Moreover, there were contrasting negative and positive relationships with distance to front (Fdist; figure 3a,b). Sex-specific differences in habitat use by gannets have also been observed in the North Sea, where males preferentially forage in near-shore coastal regions, whilst females target offshore areas of intermediate SSTs where fronts would ordinarily manifest [53]. The underlying mechanisms driving sex-specific differences in front use described here are unclear, but could be related to contrasting parental roles [97], interference competition [98], habitat segregation [53] and/or differences in nutritional requirements/prey preference [61]. As such, we highlight the need for a comprehensive knowledge of inter- and intraspecies-specific behaviours when investigating the drivers of marine predator habitat selection [6,95,99].
As well as feeding naturally on pelagic fishes, gannets also feed on discards from commercial fisheries [40,41,61], and scavenging may erode the relationships between diving gannets and fronts. To test this, we re-modelled habitat-use availability excluding all dives associated with fishing vessels (length > 15 m) and their pseudo-absences, and re-analysed changes in dive duration and shape around fronts including presence/absence of fishing vessels as a two-level factor. Dives were assumed to be at a fishing boat when within 10 km and 1 h of a logged vessel location (17.6% and 20.9% of female and male dives, respectively, as indicated by the UK vessel monitoring system provided by the Centre for Environment, Fisheries and Aquaculture Science; [39,41,100]). We found that across these re-analyses, model outputs were consistent with previous investigations that did not account for/exclude dives associated with fishing boats (for dive duration and shape re-analyses, the factor variable of fishing boat presence was not retained following model reduction). As such, whilst scavenging waste from fisheries may increase noise in this system (the influence of boats under 15 m could not be ascertained), there is no evidence that this systematically altered our findings, which is perhaps an indication of the strength of association between gannets and fronts in the Celtic Sea.
5. Conclusion
It has been suggested that shelf-sea fronts constitute key components in the functioning of marine ecosystems by providing marine predators with persistent, predictable and productive foraging habitats [8,20,21]. Our work reiterates this assertion and highlights the key role these features play in shaping both the distributions and foraging behaviours of gannets. Moreover, we suggest an increase in the use of short V-shaped dive strategies around fronts reflects improved prey accessibility and catchability. Our study highlights the complexities of interactions between marine vertebrate predators and their environment, and the subsequent importance of collaboration between disciplines (spatial ecology, oceanography and remote-sensing). We show that studies combining fine-scale foraging behaviours and remotely sensed measurements of physical oceanography can provide valuable insights towards the mechanisms that drive the at-sea distributions of marine predators. As such, there is a pressing need for cross-disciplinary research when attempting to understand marine vertebrate ecology and how marine ecosystems function.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Venture jet for providing safe transit to and from Grassholm. Lisa Morgan, Rocio Moreno, Matt Gummery, Nicola Childs, Pearl Costello, Tom Bodey and Stuart Bearhop provided support in the field. Ian Cleasby provided analytical advice. Work on Grassholm was conducted under permission from the Royal Society for the Protection of Birds, who own and manage the island. We thank three anonymous reviewers that provided useful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.
Ethics
Gannets were caught on Grassholm Island, Wales under a Schedule 1 Licence from Natural Resources Wales. The British Trust for Ornithology Special Methods Panel approved device attachment. Blood was taken under licence from the UK Home Office. All works were also approved by the University of Exeter's ethical review panel.
Data accessibility
Supporting datasets can be found in the electronic supplementary material.
Authors' contribution
S.L.C. and S.C.V. prepared the manuscript; S.L.C., K.L.S., A.B., G.M. and S.C.V. were involved in fieldwork; S.L.C., K.L.S. and P.I.M. were involved in data processing; S.L.C., S.C.V. and C.B.E. were involved in data analysis; S.L.C., P.J.H., S.N.I. and S.C.V. were involved in the design of the study; P.I.M., C.B.E., K.L.S., A.B., P.J.H., G.M. and S.N.I. commented on the manuscript. All authors gave final approval for publication.
Competing interests
There are no competing interests.
Funding
This work was supported by a PhD studentship to S.L.C. by the Natural Environment Research Council (NERC; NE/J500380/1), Natural Resources Wales (Seabirds Cymru) and an NERC grant (no. NE/H007466/1).
References
- 1.Weimerskirch H, Doncaster CP, Cuenot-Chaillet F. 1994. Pelagic seabirds and the marine environment: foraging patterns of wandering albatrosses in relation to prey availability and distribution. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 255, 91–97. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1994.0013) [Google Scholar]
- 2.Sims DW, et al. 2008. Scaling laws of marine predator search behaviour. Nature 451, 1098–1102. (doi:10.1038/nature06518) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Pinaud D, Weimerskirch H. 2005. Scale-dependent habitat use in a long-ranging central place predator. J. Anim. Ecol. 74, 852–863. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2005.00984.x) [Google Scholar]
- 4.Weimerskirch H. 2007. Are seabirds foraging for unpredictable resources? Deep-Sea Res. Part II 54, 211–223. (doi:10.1016/j.dsr2.2006.11.013) [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hamer KC, Humphreys EM, Magalhães MC, Garthe S, Hennicke J, Peters G, Grémillet D, Skov H, Wanless S. 2009. Fine-scale foraging behaviour of a medium-ranging marine predator. J. Anim. Ecol. 78, 880–889. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01549.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Patrick SC, et al. 2014. Individual differences in searching behaviour and spatial foraging consistency in a central place marine predator. Oikos 123, 33–40. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-0706.2013.00406.x) [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bost CA, Cotté C, Bailleul F, Cherel Y, Charrassin JB, Guinet C, Ainley DG, Weimerskirch H. 2009. The importance of oceanographic fronts to marine birds and mammals of the southern oceans. J. Mar. Syst. 78, 363–376. (doi:10.1016/j.jmarsys.2008.11.022) [Google Scholar]
- 8.Scales KL, Miller PI, Hawkes LA, Ingram SN, Sims DW, Votier SC. 2014. On the front line: frontal zones as priority at-sea conservation areas for mobile marine vertebrates. J. Appl. Ecol. 51, 1575–1583. (doi:10.1111/1365-2664.12330) [Google Scholar]
- 9.De Monte S, Cotté C, d'Ovidio F, Lévy M, Le Corre M, Weimerskirch H. 2012. Frigatebird behaviour at the ocean–atmosphere interface: integrating animal behaviour with multi-satellite data. J. R. Soc. Interface. 9, 3351–3358. (doi:10.1098/rsif.2012.0509) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cotté C, d'Ovidio F, Chaigneau A, Lévy M, Taupier-Letage I, Mate B, Guinet C. 2011. Scale-dependent interactions of Mediterranean whales with marine dynamics. Limnol. Oceanogr. 56, 219–232. (doi:10.4319/lo.2011.56.1.0219) [Google Scholar]
- 11.Benjamins S, Dale A, Hastie G, Waggitt J, Lea MA, Scott B, Wilson B. 2015. Confusion reigns? A review of marine megafauna interactions with tidal-stream environments. Oceanogr. Mar. Biol. Annu. Rev. 53, 1–54. (doi:10.1201/b18733-2) [Google Scholar]
- 12.Scott BE, Sharples J, Ross ON, Wang J, Pierce GJ, Camphuysen CJ. 2010. Sub-surface hotspots in shallow seas: fine-scale limited locations of top predator foraging habitat indicated by tidal mixing and sub-surface chlorophyll. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 408, 207–226. (doi:10.3354/meps08552) [Google Scholar]
- 13.Embling CB, Illian J, Armstrong E, van der Kooij J, Sharples J, Camphuysen KCJ, Scott BE. 2012. Investigating fine-scale spatio-temporal predator-prey patterns in dynamic marine ecosystems: a functional data analysis approach. J. Appl. Ecol. 49, 481–492. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2664.2012.02114.x) [Google Scholar]
- 14.Embling CB, Sharples J, Armstrong E, Palmer MR, Scott BE. 2013. Fish behaviour in response to tidal variability and internal waves over a shelf sea bank. Prog. Oceanogr. 117, 106–117. (doi:10.1016/j.pocean.2013.06.013) [Google Scholar]
- 15.Simpson JH, Hunter JR. 1974. Fronts in the Irish Sea. Nature 250, 404–406. (doi:10.1038/250404a0) [Google Scholar]
- 16.Franks PJS. 1992. Phytoplankton blooms at fronts: patterns, scales, and physical forcing mechanisms. Rev. Aquat. Sci. 6, 121–137. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yoder JA, Ackleson SG, Barber RT, Flament P, Balch WM. 1994. A line in the sea. Nature 371, 689–692. (doi:10.1038/371689a0) [Google Scholar]
- 18.Genin A, Jaffe JS, Reef R, Richter C, Franks PJS. 2005. Swimming against the flow: a mechanism of zooplankton aggregation. Science 308, 860–862. (doi:10.1126/science.1107834) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Decker MB, Hunt GL. 1996. Foraging by murres (Uria spp.) at tidal fronts surrounding the Pribilof Islands, Alaska, USA. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 139, 1–10. (doi:10.3354/meps139001) [Google Scholar]
- 20.Russell RW, Harrison NM, Hunt GL. 1999. Foraging at a front: hydrography, zooplankton, and avian planktivory in the northern Bering Sea. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 182, 77–93. (doi:10.3354/meps182077) [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jahncke J, Coyle KO, Zeeman SI, Kachel NB, Hunt GL. 2005. Distribution of foraging shearwaters relative to inner front of SE Bering Sea. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 305, 219–233. (doi:10.3354/meps305219) [Google Scholar]
- 22.Vlietstra LS, Coyle KO, Kachel NB, Hunt GL. 2005. Tidal front affects the size of prey used by a top marine predator, the short-tailed shearwater (Puffinus tenuirostris). Fish Oceanogr. 14, 196–211. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2419.2005.00369.x) [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ainley DG, Ribic CA, Woehler EJ. 2012. Adding the ocean to the study of seabirds: a brief history of at-sea seabird research. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 451, 231–243. (doi:10.3354/meps09524) [Google Scholar]
- 24.Wakefield ED, Phillips RA, Matthiopoulos J. 2009. Quantifying habitat use and preferences of pelagic seabirds using individual movement data: a review. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 391, 165–182. (doi:10.3354/meps08203) [Google Scholar]
- 25.Elliott KH, Woo K, Gaston AJ, Benvenuti S, Dall'Antonia L, Davoren GK. 2008. Seabird foraging behaviour indicates prey type. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 354, 289–303. (doi:10.3354/meps07221) [Google Scholar]
- 26.Boyd C, Castillo R, Hunt GL, Punt AE, VanBlaricom GR, Weimerskirch H, Bertrand S. 2015. Predictive modelling of habitat selection by marine predators with respect to the abundance and depth distribution of pelagic prey. J. Anim. Ecol. 84, 1575–1588. (doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12409) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Goldbogen JA, Hazen EL, Friedlaender AS, Calambokidis J, DeRuiter SL, Stimpert AK, Southall BL. 2015. Prey density and distribution drive the three-dimensional foraging strategies of the largest filter feeder. Funct. Ecol. 29, 951–961. (doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12395) [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bograd SJ, Block BA, Costa DP, Godley BJ. 2010. Biologging technologies: new tools for conservation. Introduction. Endanger Species Res. 10, 1–7. (doi:10.3354/esr00269) [Google Scholar]
- 29.Burger AE. 2003. Effects of the Juan de Fuca Eddy and upwelling on densities and distributions of seabirds off southwest Vancouver Island, British Columbia. Mar. Ornithol. 31, 113–122. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gremillet D, et al. 2008. Spatial match–mismatch in the Benguela upwelling zone: should we expect chlorophyll and sea-surface temperature to predict marine predator distributions. J. Appl. Ecol. 45, 610–621. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2664.2007.01447.x) [Google Scholar]
- 31.Miller P. 2009. Composite front maps for improved visibility of dynamic sea-surface features on cloudy SeaWiFS and AVHRR data. J. Mar. Syst. 78, 327–336. (doi:10.1016/j.jmarsys.2008.11.019) [Google Scholar]
- 32.Miller PI, Christodoulou S. 2014. Frequent locations of oceanic fronts as an indicator of pelagic diversity: application to marine protected areas and renewables. Mar. Policy 45, 318–329. (doi:10.1016/j.marpol.2013.09.009) [Google Scholar]
- 33.Scales KL, Miller PI, Varo-Cruz N, Hodgson DJ, Hawkes LA, Godley BJ. 2015. Oceanic loggerhead turtles Caretta caretta associate with thermal fronts: evidence from the Canary Current Large Marine Ecosystem. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 519, 195–207. (doi:10.3354/meps11075) [Google Scholar]
- 34.Miller PI, Scales KL, Ingram SN, Southall EJ, Sims DW. 2015. Basking sharks and oceanographic fronts: quantifying associations in the north-east Atlantic. Funct. Ecol. 29, 1099–1109. (doi:10.1111/1365-2435.12423) [Google Scholar]
- 35.Scales KL, Miller PI, Embling CB, Ingram SN, Pirotta E, Votier SC. 2014. Mesoscale fronts as foraging habitats: composite front mapping reveals oceanographic drivers of habitat use for a pelagic seabird. J. R. Soc. Interface 11, 20140679 (doi:10.1098/rsif.2014.0679) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hamer KC, Humphreys EM, Garthe S, Hennicke J, Peters G, Grémillet D, Phillips RA, Harris MP, Wanless S. 2007. Annual variation in diets, feeding locations and foraging behaviour of gannets in the North Sea: flexibility, consistency and constraint. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 338, 295–305. (doi:10.3354/meps338295) [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ropert-Coudert Y, Daunt F, Kato A, Ryan PG, Lewis S, Kobayashi K, Mori Y, Grémillet D, Wanless S. 2009. Underwater wingbeats extend depth and duration of plunge dives in northern gannets Morus bassanus. J. Avian Biol. 40, 380–387. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-048X.2008.04592.x) [Google Scholar]
- 38.Garthe S, Benvenuti S, Montevecchi WA. 2000. Pursuit plunging by northern gannets (Sula bassana) feeding on capelin (Mallotus villosus). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 1717–1722. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1200) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Votier SC, Bearhop S, Witt MJ, Inger R, Thompson D, Newton J. 2010. Individual responses of seabirds to commercial fisheries revealed using GPS tracking, stable isotopes and vessel monitoring systems. J. Appl. Ecol. 47, 487–497. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2664.2010.01790.x) [Google Scholar]
- 40.Votier SC, Bicknell A, Cox SL, Scales KL, Patrick S. 2013. A bird's eye view of discard reforms: bird-borne cameras reveal seabird/fishery interactions. PLoS ONE 8, e57376 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0057376) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Patrick SC, Bearhop S, Bodey TW, Grecian WJ, Hamer KC, Lee J, Votier SC. 2015. Inidividual seabirds show consistent foraging strategies in response to predictable fisheries discards. J. Avian Biol. 46, 431–440. (doi:10.1111/jav.00660) [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sabarros PS, Grémillet D, Demarcq H, Moseley C, Pichegru L, Mullers RHE, Stenseth NC, Machu E. 2014. Fine-scale recognition and use of mesoscale fronts by foraging Cape gannets in the Benguela upwelling region. Deep-Sea Res. II 107, 77–84. (doi:10.1016/j.dsr2.2013.06.023) [Google Scholar]
- 43.Vazquez-Prokopec GM, Stoddard ST, Paz-Soldan V, Morrison AC, Elder JP, Kochel TJ, Scott TW, Kitron U. 2009. Usefulness of commercially available GPS data-loggers for tracking human movement and exposure to dengue virus. Int. J. Health Geogr. 8, 68 (doi:10.1186/1476-072X-8-68) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Garthe S, Grémillet D, Furness RW. 1999. At-sea-activity and foraging efficiency in chick-rearing northern gannets Sula bassana: a case study in Shetland. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 185, 93–99. (doi:10.3354/meps185093) [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ropert-Coudert Y, Gremillet D, Kato A, Ryan PG, Naito Y, Le Maho Y. 2004. A fine-scale time budget of Cape gannets provides insights into the foraging strategies of coastal seabirds. Anim. Behav. 67, 985–992. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.09.010) [Google Scholar]
- 46.Carter MID, et al. 2015. GPS tracking reveals rafting behaviour of northern gannets (Morus bassanus): implications for foraging ecology and conservation. Bird Study J. Br. Trust Ornithol. 63, 83–95. (doi:10.1080/00063657.2015.1134441) [Google Scholar]
- 47.Lewis S, Benvenuti S, Dall-Antonia L, Griffiths R, Money L, Sherratt TN, Wanless S, Hamer KC. 2002. Sex-specific foraging behaviour in a monomorphic seabird. Proc. R. Soc. B 269, 1687–1693. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2083) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ropert-Coudert Y, Gremillet D, Ryan P, Kato A, Naito Y, Le Maho Y. 2004. Between air and water: the plunge dive of the Cape gannet Morus capensis. Ibis 146, 281–290. (doi:10.1111/j.1474-919x.2003.00250.x) [Google Scholar]
- 49.Garthe S, Montevecchi WA, Chapdelaine G, Rail JF, Hedd A. 2007. Contrasting foraging tactics by northern gannets (Sula bassana) breeding in different oceanographic domains with different prey fields. Mar. Biol. 151, 687–694. (doi:10.1007/s00227-006-0523-x) [Google Scholar]
- 50.Garthe S, Guse N, Montevecchi WA, Rail J-F, Grégoire F. 2014. The daily catch: flight altitude and diving behaviour of northern gannets feeding on Atlantic mackerel. J. Sea Res. 85, 456–462. (doi:10.1016/j.seares.2013.07.020) [Google Scholar]
- 51.Machovsky-Capuska GE, Vaughn RL, Wursig B, Katzir G, Raubenheimer D. 2011. Dive strategies and foraging effort in the Australasian gannets Morus serrator revealed by underwater videography. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 442, 255–261. (doi:10.3354/meps09458) [Google Scholar]
- 52.Green JA, White CR, Bunce A, Frappell PB, Butler PJ. 2010. Energetic consequences of plunge diving in gannets. Endanger Species Res. 10, 269–279. (doi:10.3354/esr00223) [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cleasby IR, Wakefield ED, Bodey TW, Davies RD, Patrick SC, Newton J, Votier SC, Bearhop S, Hamer KC. 2015. Sexual segregation in a wide-ranging marine predator is a consequence of habitat selection. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 518, 1–12. (doi:10.3354/meps11112) [Google Scholar]
- 54.Belkin IM, Cornillon PC, Sherman K. 2009. Fronts in large marine ecosystems. Prog. Oceanogr. 81, 223–236. (doi:10.1016/j.pocean.2009.04.015) [Google Scholar]
- 55.Cayula J-F, Cornillon P. 1992. Edge detection algorithm for SST images. J. Atmos. Ocean Technol. 9, 67–80. (doi:10.1175/1520-0426(1992)009<0067:EDAFSI>2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ripley B, Venables B, Bates DM, Hornik K, Gebhardt A, Firth D. 2014. MASS (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/MASS/MASS.pdf)
- 57.Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. 2009. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York, NY: Springer.
- 58.Hamel S, Yoccoz NG, Gaillard J-M. 2012. Statistical evaluation of parameters estimating autocorrelation and individual heterogeneity in longitudinal studies. Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 731–742. (doi:10.1111/j.2041-210X.2012.00195.x) [Google Scholar]
- 59.Aarts G, MacKenzie ML, McConnell B, Fedak M, Matthiopoulos J. 2008. Estimating space-use and habitat preference from wildlife telemetry data. Ecography 31, 140–160. (doi:10.1111/j.2007.0906-7590.05236.x) [Google Scholar]
- 60.Pinheiro J, Bates DM. 2014. nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/nlme/nlme.pdf)
- 61.Stauss C, et al. 2012. Sex-specific foraging behaviour in northern gannets Morus bassanus: incidence and implications. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 457, 151–162. (doi:10.3354/meps09734) [Google Scholar]
- 62.Garthe S, Montevecchi WA, Davoren GK. 2007. Flight destinations and foraging behaviour of northern gannets (Sula bassana) preying on a small forage fish in a low-Arctic ecosystem. Deep-Sea Res. II 54, 311–320. (doi:10.1016/j.dsr2.2006.11.008) [Google Scholar]
- 63.Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Stevens MHH, White J-SS. 2009. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 127–135. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Murtaugh PA. 2014. In defence of P values. Ecology 95, 611–617. (doi:10.1890/13-0590.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Stanton-Geddes J, Gomes de Freitas C, De Sales Dambros C. 2014. In defence of P values: comment on the statistical methods actually used by ecologists. Ecology 95, 637–642. (doi:10.1890/13-1156.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Zweig MH, Campbell G. 1993. Receiver-operating characteristic (ROC) plots: a fundamental evaluation tool in clinical medicine. Clin Chem. 39, 561–577. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Liu C, Berry PM, Dawson TP, Pearson RG. 2005. Selecting thresholds of occurrence in the prediction of species distributions. Ecography 28, 385–393. (doi:10.1111/j.0906-7590.2005.03957.x) [Google Scholar]
- 68.Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. 2013. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol. Evol. 4, 133–142. (doi:10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x) [Google Scholar]
- 69.Barton K. 2014. MuMIn (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/MuMIn/MuMIn.pdf)
- 70.Durazo R, Harrison NM, Hill AE. 1998. Seabird observations at a tidal mixing front in the Irish Sea. Estuar. Coast Shelf Sci. 47, 153–164. (doi:10.1006/ecss.1998.0339) [Google Scholar]
- 71.Doniol-Valcroze T, Berteaux D, Larouche P, Sears R. 2007. Influence of thermal fronts on habitat selection by four rorqual whale species in the Gulf of St. Lawrence. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 335, 207–216. (doi:10.3354/meps335207) [Google Scholar]
- 72.Kokubun N, Iida K, Mukai T. 2008. Distribution of murres (Uria spp.) and their prey south of St. George Island in the southeastern Bering Sea during the summers of 2003–2005. Deep-Sea Res. II 55, 1827–1836. (doi:10.1016/j.dsr2.2008.04.018) [Google Scholar]
- 73.Franks PJS. 1992. Sink or swim: accumulation of biomass at fronts. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 82, 1–12. (doi:10.3354/meps082001) [Google Scholar]
- 74.Nahas EL, Pattiaratchi CB, Ivey GN. 2005. Processes controlling the positions of frontal systems in Shark Bay, Western Australia. Estuar. Coast Shelf Sci. 65, 463–474. (doi:10.1016/j.ecss.2005.06.017) [Google Scholar]
- 75.Pisoni JP, Rivas AK, Piola AR. 2015. On the variability of tidal fronts on a macrotidal continental shelf, Northern Patagonia, Argentina. Deep-Sea Res. II 119, 61–68. (doi:10.1016/j.dsr2.2014.01.019) [Google Scholar]
- 76.Pettex E, Bonadonna F, Enstipp MR, Siorat F, Grémillet D. 2010. Northern gannets anticipate the spatio-temporal occurrence of their prey. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 2365–2371. (doi:10.1242/jeb.042267) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Regular PM, Hedd A, Montevecchi WA. 2013. Must marine predators always follow scaling laws? Memory guides the foraging decisions of a pursuit-diving seabird. Anim. Behav. 86, 545–552. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.06.008) [Google Scholar]
- 78.Machovsky-Capuska GE, Hauber ME, Libby E, Amiot C, Raubenheimer D. 2014. The contribution of private and public information in foraging by Australasian gannets. Anim. Cogn. 17, 849–858. (doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0716-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Wakefield ED, Cleasby IR, Bearhop S, Bodey TW, Davies RD, Miller PI, Newton J, Votier SC, Hamer KC. 2015. Long-term individual foraging site fidelity --- why some gannets don't change their spots. Ecology 96, 3058–3074. (doi:10.1890/14-1300.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Davoren GK. 2013. Distribution of marine predator hotspots explained by persistent areas of prey. Mar. Biol. 160, 3043–3058. (doi:10.1007/s00227-013-2294-5) [Google Scholar]
- 81.Furness RW, Tasker ML. 2000. Seabird-fishery interactions: quantifying the sensitivity of seabirds to reductions in sandeel abundance, and identification of key areas for sensitive seabirds in the North Sea. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 202, 253–264. (doi:10.3354/meps202253) [Google Scholar]
- 82.Amelineau F, Peron C, Lescroel A, Authier M, Provost P, Gremillet D. 2014. Windscape and tortuosity shape the flight costs of northern gannets. J. Exp. Biol. 217, 876–885. (doi:10.1242/jeb.097915) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Garthe S, Montevecchi WA, Davoren GK. 2011. Inter-annual changes in prey fields trigger different foraging tactics in a large marine predator. Limnol. Oceanogr. 56, 802–812. (doi:10.4319/lo.2011.56.3.0802) [Google Scholar]
- 84.Machovsky-Capuska GE, Vaughn RL, Wursig B, Raubenheimer D. 2013. Can gannets (Morus serrator) select their diving profile prior to submergence? Notornis 60, 255–257. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Enstipp MR, Grémillet D, Jones DR. 2007. Investigating the functional link between prey abundance and seabird predatory performance. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 331, 267–279. (doi:10.3354/meps331267) [Google Scholar]
- 86.Crook KA, Davoren GK. 2014. Underwater behaviour of common murres foraging on capelin: influences of prey density and antipredator behaviour. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 501, 279–290. (doi:10.3354/meps10696) [Google Scholar]
- 87.Grégoire F. 2006. Vertical distribution of the midwater trawl catches of Atlantic mackerel (Scomber scombrus L.) in relation with water temperature, Research document 2006/097. Fisheries and Oceans Canada; (http://www.dfo-mpo.gc.ca/csas-sccs/publications/resdocs-docrech/2006/2006_097-eng.htm) [Google Scholar]
- 88.Camphuysen CJ, Webb A. 1999. Multi-species feeding associations in North Sea seabirds: jointly exploiting a patchy environment. Ardea 87, 177–198. [Google Scholar]
- 89.Davoren GK, Garthe S, Montevecchi WA, Benvenuti S. 2010. Influence of prey behaviour and other predators on the foraging activities of a marine avian predator in a Low Arctic ecosystem. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 404, 275–287. (doi:10.3354/meps08370) [Google Scholar]
- 90.Tremblay Y, Thiebault A, Mullers R, Pistorius P. 2014. Bird-borne video-cameras show that seabird movement patterns relate to previously unrevealed proximate environment, not prey. PLoS ONE 9, e88424 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0088424) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Goold JC. 1998. Acoustic assessment of populations of common dolphin off the west Wales coast, with perspectives from satellite infrared imagery. J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. UK 78, 1353–1364. (doi:10.1017/S0025315400044544) [Google Scholar]
- 92.Dalla Rosa L, Ford JKB, Trites AW. 2012. Distribution and relative abundance of humpback whales in relation to environmental variables in coastal British Columbia waters. Cont. Shelf Res. 36, 89–104. (doi:10.1016/j.csr.2012.01.017) [Google Scholar]
- 93.Thiebault A, Semeria M, Lett C, Tremblay Y. 2015. How to capture fish in a school? Effect of successive predator attacks on seabird feeding success. J. Anim. Ecol. 85, 157–167. (doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12455) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Vaughn RL, Wursig B, Shelton DS, Timm LL, Watson LA. 2008. Dusky dolphins influence prey accessibility for seabirds in admiralty bay, New Zealand. J. Mammal. 89, 1051–1058. (doi:10.1644/07-MAMM-A-145.1) [Google Scholar]
- 95.Wakefield ED, et al. 2013. Space partitioning without territoriality in gannets. Science 341, 68–70. (doi:10.1126/science.1236077) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Machovsky-Capuska GE, Howland HC, Raubenheimer D, Vaughn-Hirshorn R, Wursig B, Hauber ME, Katzir G. 2012. Visual accommodation and active pursuit of prey in a plunge-diving bird: the Australasian gannet. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 4118–4125. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1519) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Fraser GS, Jones IL, Hunter FM. 2002. Male-female differences in parental care in monogamous crested auklets. The Condor 104, 413–423. (doi:10.1650/0010-5422(2002)104[0413:MFDIPC]2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 98.Shealer DA, Burger J. 1993. Effects of interference competition on the foraging activity of tropical roseate terns. The Condor 95, 322–329. (doi:10.2307/1369355) [Google Scholar]
- 99.Zavalaga CB, Benvenuti S, Dall'Antonia L, Emslie SD. 2007. Diving behaviour of blue-footed boobies Sula nebouxii in northern Peru in relation to sex, body size and prey type. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 336, 291–303. (doi:10.3354/meps336291) [Google Scholar]
- 100.Bodey TW, Jessopp MJ, Votier SC, Gerritsen HD, Cleasby IR, Hamer KC, Patrick SC, Wakefield ED, Bearhop S. 2014. Seabird movement reveals the ecological footprint of fishing vessels. Curr. Biol. 24, R514–R515. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.04.041) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Supporting datasets can be found in the electronic supplementary material.