Summary
Objective
Large portions of food are often blamed for rising rates of obesity. We tested the possibility that people who are heavier may tend to select or prefer larger portions than do people who are lighter.
Methods
Participants (total N = 798) were asked to choose between a small and larger portion of pasta for a hypothetical meal (Studies 1, 2 and 4), to indicate their ideal portion from a range of portion‐size options (Study 2), or to select their preferred portion size from each of 28 portion pairs (Study 3).
Results
Across all studies, there were no significant differences between heavier and lighter participants in their portion‐size selection (effect sizes ranged from d = −0.06 to 0.33). The pattern was the same regardless of whether we grouped participants as having a body mass index (BMI) <25 vs. ≥25, as having a BMI of <30 vs. ≥30, or treated BMI as a continuous predictor.
Conclusions
Given the lack of association between BMI and portion‐size preference, we suggest that factors other than portion size, such as differences in meal frequency, food type, plate clearing or compensation at subsequent meals, may need to be considered in order to explain the increasing prevalence of obesity.
Keywords: Food choice, obesity, overweight, portion size
Introduction
Increasing portion size has received considerable attention in recent years, with many authors suggesting that larger portion sizes are one of the main contributors to excess energy intake, weight gain and even obesity 1, 2, 3, 4. There is indeed substantial evidence that larger portions lead to greater energy intake 5. However, Herman and colleagues recently examined the role of large portions in the increasing prevalence of overweight and obesity in the last few decades, commonly referred to as the ‘obesity epidemic’, and concluded that the evidence indicting large portions is not particularly compelling 6. Although some studies indicate that people who are heavier tend to eat larger portions 7, other studies find no difference between heavier and leaner people in the size of the portions that they consume 8. Further complicating the picture is the fact that ‘large portions’ sometimes refer to the amount consumed (irrespective of how much was initially selected or served) 9, whereas at other times ‘large portions’ refer to the amount selected or served (irrespective of how much is consumed) 10. When large portions are blamed for increasing levels of obesity, what is usually meant is that increasingly large portions are being made available to people, with greater intake as a consequence. Can larger portions account for the increase in the prevalence of obesity over the past few decades?
Considerable evidence suggests that when larger portions are served, people tend to eat more, but this effect appears to be uniform across different body mass index (BMI) levels 10, 11, 12. In fact, a recent meta‐analysis indicated that, if anything, individuals with higher BMIs are less responsive to larger portions 5. Furthermore, epidemiological evidence suggests that it is specifically those who are already high in BMI who have displayed the greatest weight increases over time 13. These two facts together pose a conundrum for the view that larger portions are responsible for the rise in the prevalence of obesity. One would need to argue that people who are overweight or obese are not simply responding to the same secular increase in portion sizes that everyone else is responding to 14, 15, but rather that they are disproportionately likely to select larger portions in the first place. In other words, it might not be that people who are heavier eat more of a given (large) portion than do people who are lighter, but rather that they choose larger portions to begin with. If it is the case that there is a tendency for people who are heavier to select larger portions and for people who are lighter to select smaller portions, then it becomes increasingly difficult to blame large portions for the increase in obesity; more of the blame would appear to accrue to individuals who choose larger portions from an array of available portions.
There is scant evidence pertaining to the question of whether people with higher BMIs select larger portions. A few studies have found that people who are heavier select larger amounts of food when they are self‐selecting their portion size 16, 17, but other studies have found no difference as a function of BMI 18. Furthermore, self‐selecting portion sizes differs from a real‐world context in which individuals are choosing between two or more fixed portion sizes (such as when ordering in a restaurant or when ordering from a menu online). One 40‐year‐old field study of 68 fast‐food restaurant patrons indicated that participants who were obese ordered larger portions than did participants who were not obese 19, but we have been unable to find any other studies examining this particular question. The present set of studies was designed to explore whether people with higher BMIs (in the overweight category, BMI ≥ 25; or in the obese category, BMI ≥ 30) would select larger portions than would people with lower BMIs (BMI < 25). We examined this question in four online samples totalling 798 participants, who were asked to select larger or smaller portions for a hypothetical meal.
Study 1
Participants completed an online portion‐size choice task. They were asked to imagine that they were being served a portion of pasta for lunch and to indicate whether they preferred the small portion or the large portion.
Method
Participants
Participants were 128 women residing in the USA who were recruited online via the Amazon Mechanical Turk website and were reimbursed $1.00 for their participation. Their mean age was 25.83 years (SD = 3.22) and their mean BMI (kg/m2) was 25.35 (SD = 6.16). Fifty‐nine percent (n = 75) had a BMI < 25; 41% (n = 58) had a BMI ≥ 25; and 17% (n = 21) had a BMI ≥ 30. With regard to ethnicity, 77.2% were Caucasian, 11.0% were African‐American, 6.3% were Asian, 3.9% were Hispanic and 1.6% identified as ‘other’. All studies were approved by the university's human research ethics committee.
Materials and procedure
Participants signed up for a study on ‘hunger and food perception’. After providing informed consent, they were instructed to imagine that they were just about to have lunch and to think about how hungry they typically are at lunch time, and how much they typically feel like eating for lunch. Participants were then shown an image of a small portion of pasta and a large portion of pasta (with the accompanying labels ‘small’ and ‘large’) side‐by‐side, and were asked to choose which one they would prefer for lunch. Pasta was used as the target food in all studies because it is a food that is familiar to most people, and one that we assumed would be eaten regularly and at least moderately well‐liked (assumptions that were confirmed by participants' ratings; Table 1). The small portion of pasta consisted of 238 g of pasta and tomato sauce, which is equivalent to approximately two standard servings of pasta, or 1/3 of recommended daily carbohydrate intake 20; the large portion consisted of 477 g of pasta and tomato sauce, which is equivalent to approximately four standard servings of pasta, or 2/3 of recommended daily carbohydrate intake. The portions were presented on a placemat with cutlery and a glass of water in the picture to provide a reference point for the bowl size 21 (Figure 1). After choosing their preferred portion size and completing some unrelated measures, participants rated their hunger level on a visual analogue scale from 0 (not at all) to 100 (extremely) and indicated the number of hours since they last ate. They also rated how much they generally liked pasta with tomato sauce on a visual analogue scale from 0 (not at all) to 100 (very much) and indicated how frequently they eat pasta on a six‐point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (once a day). Hunger, hours since previous meal, liking of pasta and frequency of eating pasta were all examined as potential covariates. Finally, participants provided their age, ethnicity, and height and weight (which were used to calculate their BMI).
Table 1.
Study 1 | Study 2 | Study 3 | Study 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Liking of pasta | 73.64 (25.74) | 65.60 (28.94) | 64.56 (28.45) | 72.89 (23.81) |
Frequency of eating pasta | 3.69 (0.90) | 3.64 (0.97) | 3.63 (0.87) | 3.70 (0.86) |
For liking of pasta, participants made ratings on a visual analogue scale anchored by 0 (not at all) and 100 (very much). For frequency of eating pasta, response options were 1 = Never, 2 = Less than once a month, 3 = Once a month, 4 = Once a week, 5 = A few times a week, 6 = Once a day.
Results and discussion
A chi‐square analysis was used to determine whether the proportion of participants selecting the small vs. large portion varied as a function of their BMI category. A somewhat greater proportion of participants who were overweight (37.7%) chose the large portion than did participants who were not overweight (22.7%), but this difference was not statistically significant, χ 2 (1, N = 128) = 3.43, p = 0.06, Cohen's d = 0.33. Repeating the analysis by grouping participants as obese (BMI ≥ 30) vs. non‐obese (BMI < 30) showed the same pattern of results (p = 0.12, Cohen's d = 0.28). Similarly, including BMI as a continuous measure in a logistic regression analysis showed no significant association between BMI and portion choice (whether or not hunger, hours since last meal, liking of pasta and frequency of eating pasta were included as covariates; ps > 0.07). Thus, Study 1 did not find strong evidence that individuals who are overweight are more likely to choose larger portions than are those who are not overweight.
Study 2
The aim of Study 2 was to provide a more thorough examination of whether choice of portion size varied according to participants' weight status. The choice task used in Study 1 was expanded to include multiple different presentation formats, and participants also completed an additional measure of portion‐size preference in which they selected their preferred portion from a broader range of portion sizes.
Method
Participants
Participants were 293 women residing in the USA who were recruited online via the Amazon Mechanical Turk website and were reimbursed $1.00 for their participation. Their mean age was 25.81 years (SD = 2.99), and their mean BMI was 25.47 (SD = 6.51). Fifty‐seven percent (n = 168) had a BMI < 25; 43% (n = 125) had a BMI ≥ 25; and 19% (n = 55) had a BMI ≥ 30. With regards to ethnicity, 77.1% were Caucasian, 10.6% were African‐American, 4.8% were Hispanic, 6.1% were Asian and 1.3% identified as ‘other’.
Materials and procedure
The materials and procedure were identical to Study 1 with two exceptions:
Two new choice formats were added to the image + label format used in Study 1. In the image only condition, participants were shown unlabelled images of the small and large portions and were asked to choose which one they would prefer for lunch. In the label only condition, participants read that ‘small’ and ‘large’ portion options were available, and were asked to choose which one they would prefer for lunch, without seeing images of the portions.
As an additional measure of ideal portion size, participants were also shown an array of eight different portions of pasta ranging from 119 to 953 g (representing 1–8 standard servings) and were asked to select the portion that they would eat in order to feel comfortably full.
Results and discussion
A chi‐square analysis was conducted to determine whether the proportion of participants selecting the small vs. large portion varied as a function of their BMI category. As in Study 1, a somewhat greater proportion of participants who were overweight (36.0%) chose the large portion than did participants who were not overweight (29.8%), but this difference was not statistically significant, χ 2(1, N = 293) = 1.27, p = 0.26, Cohen's d = 0.13. The same pattern of results emerged if we examined each of the choice formats separately (Table 2). The same pattern also emerged if we grouped participants as obese vs. non‐obese (p = 0.96, Cohen's d = 0.02). There was also no significant association between BMI and portion choice if we conducted a logistic regression with BMI as a continuous measure (whether or not we included hunger, hours since last meal, liking of pasta and frequency of eating pasta as covariates; ps > 0.13).
Table 2.
Overweight (%) | Non‐overweight (%) | χ 2 | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Image only | 50.0 | 31.3 | 3.43 | 0.06 |
Label only | 22.4 | 27.4 | 0.36 | 0.55 |
Image + label | 31.0 | 36.7 | 0.28 | 0.59 |
We next conducted a t‐test to determine whether there were any BMI‐group differences in ideal portion size. Participants who were overweight reported an ideal portion size (M = 373.82 g, SD = 172.23) that was slightly, but not significantly, larger than the ideal portion size reported by participants who were not overweight (M = 340.51 g, SD = 174.26), t(291) = 1.63, p = 0.11, d = 0.19. Again, the pattern was the same if we grouped participants as obese vs. non‐obese (p = 0.12, Cohen's d = 0.24), and there was no significant correlation between BMI as a continuous measure and ideal portion size (r = 0.10, p = 0.09).
Overall, using a broader range of choice formats (image only, label only or image + label), as well as assessing ideal portion size in a continuous rather than dichotomous fashion, the results of this study are consistent with those of Study 1 in that they failed to provide any evidence that individuals who are overweight reliably choose or prefer larger portions.
Study 3
Study 3 sought to build on the previous studies by generating a more reliable and sensitive estimate of participants' portion‐size preference.
Method
Participants
Participants were 104 women residing in the USA who were recruited online via the Amazon Mechanical Turk website and were reimbursed $1.00 for their participation. Their mean age was 26.11 years (SD = 2.85), and their mean BMI was 24.74 (SD = 6.70). Sixty‐four percent (n = 66) had a BMI < 25; 36% (n = 38) had a BMI ≥ 25; and 15% (n = 16) had a BMI ≥ 30. With regards to ethnicity, 84.6% were Caucasian, 7.7% were African‐American, 4.8% were Hispanic, 1.0% were Asian and 2.0% identified as ‘other’.
Materials and procedure
This study used a forced‐choice task in which participants were shown pairs of portion size images and were asked to indicate which portion from the pair they would prefer to eat for lunch. The same eight portion sizes used for the continuous measure in Study 2 were used in this study. Pairs of images were created by combining each portion size with every other portion size (1–2, 1–3, 1–4,…,7–8), resulting in 28 pairs in total. Order of presentation of the 28 pairs was randomized, and the position of the larger portion (left or right) was randomized within each pair. Participants' preferred portion size was defined as the portion size that they selected most frequently across all 28 decisions. In cases where participants selected more than one portion with equal frequency, their preferred portion was calculated as the mean of the values. Participants then completed all the same control and demographic measures as in the previous two studies.
Results and discussion
A t‐test indicated that participants who were overweight preferred a portion size (M = 427.46 g, SD = 215.13) that was somewhat larger than the portion size preferred by participants who were not overweight (M = 369.80 g, SD = 219.20). However, this difference was not statistically significant, t(102) = 1.30, p = 0.20, d = 0.27. Controlling for hunger, hours since last meal, liking of pasta and frequency of eating pasta had no bearing on the results. Furthermore, grouping participants as obese vs. non‐obese showed the same pattern of results (p = 0.09, d = 0.51), and there was no significant correlation between BMI as a continuous variable and preferred portion size (r = 0.12, p = 0.21). Thus, the results of Study 3 were consistent with Studies 1 and 2, finding insufficient evidence to suggest that there is a difference in portion‐size preference according to BMI classification.
Study 4
Participants from Studies 1–3 were involved in a larger project on eating behaviours among young women. Because the first three studies focused exclusively on women, Study 4 examined the relationship between BMI and portion‐size choice in men.
Method
Participants
Participants were 273 men residing in the USA who were recruited online via the Amazon Mechanical Turk website and were reimbursed $1.00 for their participation. Their mean age was 32.29 years (SD = 9.88), and their mean BMI was 26.25 (SD = 5.55). Forty‐nine percent (n = 133) had a BMI < 25; 51% (n = 140) had a BMI ≥ 25; and 21% (n = 57) had a BMI ≥ 30. With regards to ethnicity, 76.6% were Caucasian, 3.7% were African‐American, 6.2% were Hispanic, 12.1% were Asian, and 1.4% identified as “other”.
Method
The method for Study 4 was identical to that of Study 1.
Results and discussion
A chi‐square analysis was used to determine whether the proportion of participants selecting the small vs. large portion varied as a function of their BMI category. Participants who were overweight (60.0%) and participants who were not overweight (63.2%) were equally likely to choose the large portion, χ 2 (1, N = 273) = 0.29, p = 0.59, Cohen's d = −0.06. Repeating the analysis by grouping participants as obese vs. non‐obese showed the same pattern of results (p = 0.56, Cohen's d = 0.07). Similarly, including BMI as a continuous measure in a logistic regression analysis showed no significant association between BMI and portion choice (whether or not hunger, hours since last meal, liking of pasta and frequency of eating pasta were included as covariates; ps > 0.81). Thus, consistent with the previous studies, Study 4 found no evidence that individuals who are overweight select larger portions than do individuals who are not overweight.
General discussion
In four separate samples, totaling nearly 800 respondents and using different methodologies, we found no significant differences in selection of larger portions as a function of BMI. That is, participants with higher BMIs did not select larger portions than did participants with lower BMIs. We should acknowledge that, in Studies 1–3, there was a slight tendency for the heavier female participants (more so than for lighter female participants) to select the large portion over the small portion in a forced‐choice situation, or to select larger portions from an array. Although none of these differences was statistically significant, even small differences in the tendency to select larger portions could accumulate over time or occasions to contribute to weight gain in a practically significant way 22. It may be that future research tracking food choices on multiple occasions over a longer period would reveal differences between heavier and lighter individuals. Nonetheless, our data do not provide any strong indication that portion‐size preferences vary as a function of participants' BMI.
If portion size affects food intake to more or less the same degree across BMI 5, 10, and if there are no differences in portion‐size preference among people who are heavier and people who are lighter (as observed in the present studies), then other factors need to be considered in attempting to explain the connection between portion size and obesity. For example, rather than ordering larger portions, people with higher BMIs might eat more of what they order than do people with lower BMIs 23. It is also possible that people with higher BMIs select foods that are more calorically dense than do people with lower BMIs. However, some recent search suggests that the frequency of self‐reported consumption of high‐calorie indulgent foods does not differ across BMI classification 24. It might also be that people who are heavier are less likely to compensate for the increased intake that accompanies larger portions by self‐regulating at subsequent meals, although the limited available evidence suggests that BMI does not predict subsequent compensation 25. Another possibility, discussed by Herman et al., 6 is that the increasing rates of obesity may be a matter of more frequent meals rather than of larger meals, so perhaps when we look at portion selection, we are looking in the wrong place. Of course, it may be that those more frequent meals are themselves also larger and higher in calories, compounding the problem of frequent snacking.
There are some limitations in the present study that should be noted. The fact that the choices made in these studies were hypothetical necessitates some caution in interpreting the results. In real‐life portion‐selection situations, the pattern might be different. For example, the hypothetical scenario meant that participants were choosing portions without paying for them. A validation study did find that portion‐size decisions and actual purchasing behaviour were highly correlated with virtual purchasing 26. However, it is conceivable that if people were required to pay for their portions, they might choose larger portions on the grounds that larger portions often offer more value for the money 27. This economic consideration might be particularly forceful for less affluent people, who also tend to be heavier 28. Future research could address these questions in contexts where consumers pay for their food and can also assess the role of socioeconomic status in people's portion‐size preferences in those contexts. Another limitation is that our studies included only one food (pasta). Although participants in all studies indicated that they regularly ate pasta (almost once per week), and generally like pasta (mean liking rating across all studies was 69/100), there could be differences in portion‐size selection across different types of foods. Thus, it will be important to determine whether the observed pattern generalizes to other foods (e.g. high‐calorie snack foods).
Conclusion
We found no compelling evidence that people who are heavier select larger portions than do people who are lighter. Given the lack of association between BMI and portion‐size preference observed in the present studies, as well as evidence that portion size influences food intake uniformly across BMI levels 5, 10, it is likely that other factors beyond portion‐size selection would also be necessary to account for rising rates of obesity. It is also worth noting that choice of portion sizes is not always available in the real word and that, even when choice is available, a ‘smaller’ portion is not necessarily a ‘small’ portion (e.g. relative to nutritional needs). If the only portions available are moderately large or larger, we might well find the entire population becoming heavier, even while the heavier segment of the population becomes even heavier even faster.
Conflict of interest
No conflict of interest was declared.
Funding
This research was supported under the Australian Research Council's Discovery Projects funding scheme (project number DP140101041). The Australian Research Council had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Acknowledgements
CPH conceived the research question. NMR and LRV designed the studies and collected the data. LRV analyzed the data. All authors were involved in writing the paper and had final approval of the submitted manuscript.
Reily, N. M. , Herman, C. P. , and Vartanian, L. R. (2016) Portion‐size preference as a function of individuals' body mass index. Obesity Science & Practice, 2: 241–247. doi: 10.1002/osp4.59.
References
- 1. Brownell KD, Horgen KB. Food fight: the inside story of the food industry, America's obesity crisis, and what we can do about it. McGraw‐Hill: Columbus, OH, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Chaput JP, Klingenberg L, Astrup A, Sjodin AM. Modern sedentary activities promote overconsumption of food in our current obesogenic environment. Obes Rev 2011; 12: e12–e20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Nestle M. Food Politics: How the Food Industry Influences Nutrition and Health. University of California Press: Berkeley, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, et al. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet 2011; 378: 804–814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Zlatevska N, Dubelaar C, Holden SS. Sizing up the effect of portion size on consumption: a meta‐analytic review. J Mark 2014; 78: 140–154. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Herman CP, Polivy J, Vartanian LR, Pliner P. Are large portions responsible for the obesity epidemic? Phys Behav 2016; 156: 177–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Westerterp‐Plantenga MS, Pasman WJ, Yedema MJW, Wijckmans‐Duijsens NE. Energy intake adaptation of food intake to extreme energy densities of food by obese and non‐obese women. Eur J Clin Nutr 1996; 50: 401–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Kelly MT, Rennie KL, Wallace JMW, et al. (2009). Associations between the portion sizes of food groups consumed and measures of adiposity in the British National Diet and Nutrition Survey. Brit J Nutr 2009; 101: 1413–1420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Piernas C, Popkin BM. Food portion patterns and trends among US children and the relationship to total eating occasion size, 1977–2006. J Nutr 2011; 141: 1159–1164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Rolls BJ, Morris EL, Roe LS. Portion size of food affects energy intake in normal‐weight and overweight men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 2002; 76: 1207–1213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Fisher JO, Arreola A, Birch LL, Rolls BJ. Portion size effects on daily energy intake in low‐income Hispanic and African American children and their mothers. Am J Clin Nutr 2007; 86: 1709–1716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Marchiori D, Waroquier L, Klein O. Smaller food item sizes of snack foods influence reduced portions and caloric intake in young adults. J Am Diet Assoc 2011; 111: 727–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Flegal KM, Troiano RP. Changes in the distribution of body mass index of adults and children in the US population. Int J Obes 2000; 24: 807–818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Young LR, Nestle M. Expanding portion sizes in the US marketplace: implications for nutrition counseling. J Am Diet Assoc 2003; 103: 231–234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Young LR, Nestle M. Reducing portion sizes to prevent obesity: a call to action. Am J Prev Med 2012; 43: 565–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Burger KS, Kern M, Coleman KJ. Characteristics of self‐selected portion size in young adults. J Am Diet Assoc 2007; 107: 611–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Lewis HB, Forwood SE, Verlaers K, Robinson E, Higgs S, Jebb SA. Personal and social norms for food portion sizes in lean and obese adults. Int J Obes 2015; 39: 1319–1324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Brunstrom JM, Rogers PJ, Pothos EM, Calitri R, Tapper K. Estimating everyday portion size using a ‘method of constant stimuli’: in a student sample, portion size is predicted by gender, dietary behaviour, and hunger, but not BMI. Appetite 2008; 51: 296–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Dodd DK, Birky HJ, Stalling RB. Eating behavior of obese and normal weight females in a natural setting. Addict Behav 1976; 1: 321–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. National Health and Medical Research Council . Eat for health: Australian Dietary Guidelines Summary. Author: Canberra, Australia, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Hieke S, Palascha A, Jola C, Wills J, Raats MM. The pack size effect: influence on consumer estimates of portion size. Appetite 2016; 96: 225–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Hill JO, Wyatt HR, Reed GW, Peters JC. Obesity and the environment: where do we go from here? Science 2003; 299: 853–855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Robinson E, Aveyard P, Jebb SA. Is plate clearing a risk factor for obesity? A cross‐sectional study of self‐reported data in US adults. Obesity 2015; 23: 301–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Just DR, Wansink B. Fast food, soft drink and candy intake is unrelated to body mass index for 95% of American adults. Obes Sci Prac 2015; 1: 126–130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Kral TVE, Meengs JS, Wall D. Increasing the portion size of a packaged snack increases energy intake in men and women. Appetite 2004; 42: 63–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Sharpe KM, Staelin R, Huber J. Using extremeness aversion to fight obesity: policy implications of context dependent demand. J Consum Res 2008; 35: 406–422. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Steenhuis IHM, Vermeer WM. Portion size: review and framework for interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2009; 6DOI: 10.1186/1479-5868-6-58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. McLaren L. Socioeconomic status and obesity. Epidemiol Rev 2007; 29: 29–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]