Abstract
Background
Self-esteem and life satisfaction are important aspects of positive mental health in young people, and both are socially distributed. However, the majority of evidence is based on socioeconomic characteristics of the family. As children enter adolescence and gain independence, perceptions of their own social position are likely to influence mental health.
Design and objectives
Using data on 11-year-olds from the UK Millennium Cohort Study, we investigated associations of both family income and young adolescents’ perception of their social position with self-esteem and life satisfaction. We hypothesised that there would be differences in the impact of perceived social position on positive mental health when investigating the full scale scoring distribution or the bottom of the distribution. Therefore, we estimated proportional odds for having greater positive mental health (across the distribution of scores) and ORs for poor outcomes (lowest 10% scores).
Results
The likelihood of greater self-esteem and life satisfaction increased with income; similarly, the risk of having poor self-esteem and life satisfaction increased as income decreased. Young adolescents who perceived their family as poorer than their friends (instead of about the same) were less likely to have greater self-esteem and life satisfaction and were more likely to have poor outcomes. Young adolescents who perceived their family as richer were more likely to have poor self-esteem, but were not less likely to have greater self-esteem. For life satisfaction, young adolescents who perceived their families as richer were less likely to have greater and more likely to have poor life satisfaction.
Conclusions
Policies to redistribute income in families with children are likely to benefit the mental health of young people. However, it is also important to consider the impact of social comparison on young people's mental health as they enter adolescence.
Keywords: Child Psychology, Adolescent Health, Comm Child Health
What is already known on this subject.
Socioeconomic inequalities in the mental health of adults and children are widely established.
Research suggests that adolescents from lower income families have worse outcomes, as may those who perceive their family to be poorer (compared with families of peers).
However, there is scant research evidence examining the association between perceived social position and positive mental health of young people.
What this study adds.
Low family income was associated with lower self-esteem and life satisfaction in a representative sample of UK 11-year-olds.
After adjustment for income, young people who perceived their family to be poorer (rather than ‘about the same’) than their peers had lower self-esteem and life satisfaction.
Those who perceived their family to be richer (as opposed to about the same) than their peers also had worse self-esteem and life satisfaction.
Future research should focus on the pathways from perceived social position to positive mental health outcomes in young adolescents.
Introduction
Traditionally, research examining child health inequalities has focused on the socioeconomic circumstances of the child's parents and family (such as household income or maternal education).1–6 However, as a child starts to gain independence from their family, awareness of their social position relative to peers is likely to increase.7 Research suggests that adolescents will have worse health outcomes when they perceive their family to be lower in the socioeconomic hierarchy compared with families of peers.8 However, there is scant research evidence examining the association between perceived social position and health (particularly mental health) among children who are within the transitional period between childhood and adolescence (referred to as ‘young adolescents’ hereafter). In addition, there is a paucity of research comparing the social gradients in health according to perceived social position and family socioeconomic circumstances, or differentiating the impact on a young person's mental health of being richer, as well as poorer, than their peers.
Mental health problems are common in adolescence9 10 and are a priority for researchers and policy makers alike.11 12 Two important aspects of positive mental health are self-esteem (evaluative attitude towards the self13) and life satisfaction (a global judgement of one's life14). Low self-esteem and low life satisfaction are associated with poorer academic achievement and anxiety, depression and eating pathology.15 16 Given the importance of self-esteem and life satisfaction in the development and general functioning of adolescents, insight into the effect of social inequality on self-esteem and life satisfaction is required. However, relatively few researchers have examined inequalities in young people's life satisfaction17–21 or self-esteem.17 20–22
A comparison of inequalities in positive mental health according to family socioeconomic circumstances and young adolescents’ perceptions of their social position will increase our knowledge of the mechanisms behind socioeconomic inequalities, inform policy and practice and ultimately contribute to global efforts to inequality reduction. This study investigates the associations of family income and young adolescents’ perception of their social position with self-esteem and life satisfaction, using a large, contemporary UK cohort of 11-year-olds born at the turn of the century.
Methods
Subjects and design
We used data from the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS), a longitudinal study of children born in the UK between September 2000 and January 2002, which has been described elsewhere.23 The first study contact with the cohort child was around age 9 months, when information was collected on 72% of those approached, providing data on 18 818 infants (our analyses were restricted to 18 296 singletons). Survey interviews were carried out by interviewers in the home with the main respondent (usually the mother).The fifth data collection sweep was conducted in 2012–2013, when the children were age 11, and 13 112 (71.7%) main respondents participated. Cohort children were also invited to complete a survey in this sweep, and 12 824 (97.8%) did so. Response weights are used in all analyses to account for sample design and attrition.24 25
Data were obtained from the UK Data Archive, University of Essex in March 2014. Ethical approval for the fifth sweep of the MCS was granted by the Northern and Yorkshire multicentre research ethics committee in July 2011 (Ethics Committee reference: 11/YH/0203).26 Further information about the MCS can be found elsewhere (http://www.cls.ioe.ac.uk/MCS).
Measurements
Exposures
We examined two socioeconomic measures; one pertaining to the objective socioeconomic circumstances of the household (family income) and one capturing the young adolescents’ subjective perceptions of their family's wealth relative to their peers (perceived social position).
Family income. Family income was reported by the main respondent at the 11-year sweep. Income quintiles were derived using a modified Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) equivalence scale, ranging from the lowest income group (first quintile) to the highest income group (fifth quintile). Missing income data were multiply imputed by the Centre for Longitudinal Studies before data from the sweep were deposited.24
Perceived social position. The cohort children responded to the item, ‘Compared to your friends, is your family richer, about the same, poorer or don't know?’. Of all children, 1.3% (n=172) did not answer this question. Since the focus of the paper was on a perceived rating of comparative social position, those who answered don't know were excluded from the main analyses, including this measure. However, these children were included in analyses examining only family income and results focusing on the don't know category are provided in online supplementary appendices 1–3.
archdischild-2015-309651supp.pdf (208.6KB, pdf)
Outcomes
Two measures were examined representing different aspects of positive mental health. The majority of children had high scores indicating good mental health. Because the impact of perceived social position across the distribution of mental health may be different from impacts at the extreme of the distribution (ie, very poor mental health), we examined both outcomes as ordinal and dichotomised variables.
Self-esteem. Self-esteem was measured with a shortened and adapted version of Rosenberg's Self-Esteem Scale,13 comprising five items reflecting a positive view of self (see online supplementary appendix 4). Item responses were reported on a four-point scale ranging from Strongly disagree to Strongly agree. The items showed good internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha=0.74) and results from principal component analysis (PCA) showed that items loaded on a single construct. Responses were summed to provide a score ranging from 5 to 20, with higher scores indicating greater self-esteem. Most children had a high self-esteem score (25th, 50th and 75th centiles: scores 15, 17 and 19, respectively). In addition, children were classified as having poor self-esteem if they were in the bottom 10% of scores (score range: 5–14).
Life satisfaction. Life satisfaction was measured with six items reflecting children's feelings about different parts of their life, including school, family and friends (see online supplementary appendix 4). Responses were reported on a seven-point scale ranging from Not at all happy to Completely happy. Internal consistency of the scale was good (Cronbach's alpha=0.83), and PCA results showed that items loaded on a single construct. Responses to items were summed to provide a score ranging from 6 to 42, with higher scores indicating greater life satisfaction. Most children had a high life satisfaction score (25th, 50th and 75th centiles: scores 33, 37 and 40, respectively). In addition, children were classified as having poor life satisfaction if they were in the bottom 10% of scores (score range: 6–28).
Seven hundred and fifty-two children (6.4%) were missing a self-esteem score and 281 (2.4%) a life satisfaction score, largely due to missing entries for just one or two items on the self-esteem or life satisfaction scale. Therefore, we rescaled the average using the other completed items when children had responded to at least three items of the self-esteem or life satisfaction scale. This reduced missingness to 1.5% for the self-esteem scale and 0.4% for the life satisfaction scale. We ran sensitivity analyses with the self-esteem and life satisfaction scores rescaled when only a single item was missing and for complete data on the scales. Patterns of results were largely similar to those reported (data not shown).
Potential confounders. We selected potential confounders that were hypothesised to relate to the outcomes, family income and perceived social position (maternal age at first live birth, child's ethnicity and gender). Furthermore, in the case of the child's perceived social position, analyses adjusted for family income in order to test if the influence of perceived social position persisted after taking into account family socioeconomic circumstances. Missing data on the confounding variables were as follows: ethnicity (n=70; 0.6%) and maternal age at first live birth (n=837; 6.6%).
We examined whether the association between socioeconomic exposures and outcomes varied by gender. No interaction with gender was found for any model, and so all results are presented for girls and boys combined.
We ran sensitivity analyses with maternal education as an additional potential confounder. Patterns of results were largely similar to those reported. Furthermore, results were very similar when maternal education was used as an alternative measure of socioeconomic circumstances in place of family income (not shown).
Statistical analyses
All analyses were conducted in STATA/SE V.13 (Stata, Texas, USA), using ‘svy’ commands to allow for the sampling design and attrition up to age 11 years. Kendall's tau-b test was used to examine the extent to which family income and perceived social position were associated. Two types of regression analyses were conducted. Proportional ORs (PORs) and 95% CIs were estimated using ordinal regression analysis to examine positive mental health (ordinal scores), according to family income and perceived social position. ORs and 95% CIs were estimated using logistic regression to examine poor self-esteem and life satisfaction (binary outcomes), according to family income and perceived social position. Models were estimated before and after adjustment for confounders.
Results
Family income and perceived social position
Young adolescents whose families were in higher income quintiles were more likely to rate their family as richer than their peers; however, the association was weak (Kendall's tau-b=0.04; p<0.001) (see online supplementary appendix 1), indicating that the two measures capture different aspects of the socioeconomic experience.
Inequalities in self-esteem
The proportional odds analysis showed that young people from families in lower income quintiles were less likely to have greater self-esteem compared with those from the highest income quintile, both before and after adjustment (table 1). Young adolescents who rated their family as poorer were also less likely to have greater self-esteem compared with young adolescents who rated their family as about the same (Model A2: POR=0.45, 95% CI 0.37 to 0.56). Young adolescents who considered their families to be richer were similar to young adolescents who perceived their family to be about the same (Model A2: POR=1.09, 95% CI 0.94 to 1.26). These differences remained after adjustment for confounding and family income.
Table 1.
Greater self-esteem (ordinal scores) | Poor self-esteem (bottom 10% scores)* | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Model A1 | Model A2† | Model B1 | Model B2† | |
Proportional OR (POR) (95% CI) | POR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | |
Family income | ||||
Lowest income quintile | 0.81 (0.70 to 0.94) | 0.76 (0.64 to 0.89) | 1.86 (1.48 to 2.33) | 1.70 (1.27 to 2.29) |
Second quintile | 0.79 (0.70 to 0.89) | 0.80 (0.71 to 0.90) | 1.64 (1.32 to 2.03) | 1.45 (1.12 to 1.87) |
Third quintile | 0.87 (0.77 to 0.98) | 0.89 (0.89 to 1.00) | 1.36 (1.07 to 1.73) | 1.27 (0.99 to 1.63) |
Fourth quintile | 0.86 (0.77 to 0.96) | 0.88 (0.79 to 0.99) | 1.15 (0.93 to 1.43) | 1.11 (0.88 to 1.40) |
Highest income quintile | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. |
Compared with your friends, is your family?‡ | ||||
Poorer | 0.45 (0.36 to 0.56) | 0.45 (0.37 to 0.56) | 2.76 (2.05 to 3.70) | 2.62 (1.95 to 3.53) |
About the same | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | |
Richer | 1.15 (0.99 to 1.33) | 1.09 (0.94 to 1.26) | 1.25 (0.98 to 1.59) | 1.37 (1.07 to 1.75) |
*Score range: 5–14 (out of a possible range of 5–20).
†Models A2 and B2 are adjusted for maternal age at first live birth, sex and ethnicity of the child. The analysis of perceived social position also adjusted for family income.
‡N=9785; 1895 young adolescents who answered don't know were excluded from these analyses.
The binary odds analysis (focusing on poor outcomes) indicated that young adolescents from families in lower income quintiles were more likely to have poor self-esteem compared with young adolescents whose family income was in the highest quintile (table 1). Similarly, young adolescents who rated their family as poorer were more likely to have poor self-esteem than young adolescents who rated their family as about the same (Model B2: OR=2.62, 95% CI 1.95 to 3.53). However, young adolescents who rated their family as richer were also slightly more likely to have poor self-esteem (Model B2: OR=1.37, 95% CI 1.07 to 1.75). The findings were similar before and after adjustment.
Inequalities in life satisfaction
Young adolescents from lower income quintile families were less likely to report greater life satisfaction compared with young adolescents whose family income was classified in the highest income quintile, both before and after adjustment (table 2). Young adolescents who rated their family as richer (Model C2: POR=0.76, 95% CI 0.65 to 0.89), as well as young adolescents who rated their families as poorer (Model C2: POR=0.33, 95% CI 0.27 to 0.39), were less likely to have greater life satisfaction (compared with young adolescents who rated their family as about the same). This remained the case after adjustment for confounding and family income.
Table 2.
Greater life satisfaction (ordinal scores) | Poor life satisfaction (bottom 10% scores)* | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Model C1 | Model C2† | Model D1 | Model D2† | |
Proportional OR (POR) (95% CI) | POR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | OR (95% CI) | |
Family income | ||||
Lowest income quintile | 0.74 (0.64 to 0.87) | 0.71 (0.60 to 0.85) | 1.81 (1.44 to 2.23) | 2.11 (1.58 to 2.83) |
Second quintile | 0.75 (0.66 to 0.84) | 0.75 (0.66 0 0.86) | 1.50 (1.20 to 1.87) | 1.65 (1.26 to 2.15) |
Third quintile | 0.88 (0.79 to 0.98) | 0.88 (0.78 to 0.98) | 1.33 (1.09 to 1.63) | 1.43 (1.14 to 1.79) |
Fourth quintile | 0.92 (0.83 to 1.02) | 0.92 (0.83 to 1.02) | 0.98 (0.78 to 1.22) | 1.01 (0.81 to 1.27) |
Highest income quintile | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. |
Compared with your friends, is your family?‡ | ||||
Poorer | 0.31 (0.26 to 0.37) | 0.33 (0.27 to 0.39) | 3.76 (2.87 to 4.92) | 3.58 (2.72 to 4.70) |
About the same | Ref. | Ref. | Ref. | |
Richer | 0.78 (0.67 to 0.92) | 0.76 (0.65 to 0.89) | 1.68 (1.34 to 2.12) | 1.77 (1.41 to 2.23) |
*Score range: 6–28 (out of a possible range of 6–42).
†Models C2 and D2 are adjusted for maternal age at first live birth, sex and ethnicity of the child. The analysis of perceived social position also adjusted for family income.
‡N=9869; 1895 young adolescents who answered don't know were excluded from these analyses.
Young adolescents from lower income quintile families were more likely to have a poor life satisfaction compared with young adolescents from the highest income quintile families (table 2). Young adolescents who rated their family as poorer (Model D2: OR=3.58, 95% CI 2.72 to 4.70) or richer (Model D2: OR=1.77, 95% CI 1.41 to 2.23) were significantly more likely to have a poor life satisfaction. Associations were similar before and after adjustment.
Discussion
Summary of findings
This is the first study, to our knowledge, that has compared the associations of family income and perceived social position with positive mental health in young adolescents. We found that while the majority of 11-year-olds had high self-esteem and life satisfaction, levels of both tended to decline as family income decreased. In addition, the risk of having poor self-esteem and life satisfaction increased as income fell. Young adolescents who perceived their family as poorer were less likely to have greater self-esteem and life satisfaction and more likely to fall in the bottom 10% of scores. Patterns of positive mental health for young adolescents who perceived their families to be richer (as opposed to about the same) were more complex. For self-esteem, differences were only observed for the lowest scores: young adolescents who perceived themselves to be richer were slightly more likely to have poor self-esteem, whereas there were no differences in self-esteem when looking at greater self-esteem across the distribution. For life satisfaction, the findings were consistent across the distribution: young adolescents who perceived their families to be richer were less likely to have greater scores and more likely to have poor life satisfaction.
Comparison with other findings
There is a large body of research examining socioeconomic inequalities in the health of children.1–6 However, the existence of health inequalities within the transitional period from childhood to adolescence has not yet been well researched. This has possibly been exacerbated by difficulties in obtaining an accurate picture of family socioeconomic circumstances directly from the young person (in studies where they are the sole respondent).27 Furthermore, it is unclear whether parental socioeconomic circumstances are an appropriate measures of a child's social position.27 For example, some research has suggested that differences in health by family socioeconomic circumstances, evident in childhood and adulthood, are lessened in adolescence because of the increased influence of peers, school environment and youth culture.7 However, in the current study, we found that, at age 11, the socioeconomic situation of the family (household income) was still influential. Importantly, we also found that in children as young as 11, perceptions of their position in the socioeconomic hierarchy can have a negative impact on their mental health, even after controlling for family income. This indicates that these measures represent different aspects of the socioeconomic experience (supported by the weak association found between family income and social position in this study and in earlier work17 28–30). In particular, we found that those who perceived themselves as being richer (as well as poorer) had worse outcomes. Studies in older adolescents17–22 have shown that those who perceived their family as poorer compared with their friends’ families had lower self-esteem and life satisfaction. However, perceived social position was analysed in a way that did not always allow for the possibility that young people who perceived themselves as richer than their peers might also have worse outcomes. Our findings indicate that in doing so they may have overlooked important associations.
Strengths and limitations
This study has examined inequalities in different positive mental health scales, using a large, contemporary and nationally representative cohort of early adolescents in the UK. The results varied according to the two socioeconomic measures, which although correlated were intended to capture different aspects of the child's socioeconomic circumstances. The child's perceived social position was subjective and with reference to the child's own social circles, whereas income captured the family financial circumstances (compared with families with children of a similar age across the UK). Although life satisfaction and self-esteem were not assessed using standard measures, the scales showed good internal consistency and items loaded on a single construct. Furthermore, life satisfaction was a multi-item scale, which is considered to be preferable to single-item scales that are often used to measure this outcome.31 Correlations of self-esteem and life satisfaction with a validated tool for assessing children's mental health status by parents and teachers (the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire) were examined and the directions of the associations were as expected (data available on request). Weights were used in all analyses to account for the sampling design and differential response to age 11 years. These weights cannot account for item missingness, although our analytic sample consisted of the majority (91.6%) of MCS children who took part in the 11-year sweep.
Implications for policy and research
The mental health of young people is critical for educational attainment, employment and adult health. We have shown substantial inequalities in self-esteem and life satisfaction in early adolescence, which may persist into adult life. In addition, we found that the influence of perceived social position on positive mental health was independent of household income, and perceptions of being different from peers (richer or poorer) appeared detrimental to mental health. This implies that there may be multiple mechanisms through which socioeconomic circumstances influence the mental health of a young person, related both to absolute disadvantage (such as not being able to afford basic necessities) and the psychosocial consequences of disadvantage (where feeling different from peers may in itself have negative consequences).
Policies to redistribute income are likely to benefit the mental health of young people from poorer families through the reduction of material disadvantage. However, the impact of inequalities on mental health may additionally involve more subtle psychosocial factors, which could be detrimental for all young people living in unequal societies, such as the UK. While income redistribution has the potential to benefit everyone, an exploration of the impact of pressures created by social comparison at the level of the individual is also necessary. This will help to inform health-based and school-based interventions to alleviate or prevent poor mental health. Health professionals have an important role to play in health inequality reduction, through considering the root causes of patients’ mental health, and (in the case of children) taking a family-centred approach.32 For young people, it is essential to also consider wider influences, including neighbourhoods, schools and peers.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all the Millennium Cohort families for their participation, the director of the MCS and colleagues in the management team at the Centre for Longitudinal Studies, UCL Institute of Education. We would also like to thank colleagues from the Institute of Child Health, Catherine Law, Kathryn Hesketh, Chloe Parkin, Emeline Rougeaux and Helen Bedford for their comments and suggestions during the course of this work.
Footnotes
Funding: AP is funded by a Medical Research Council Population Health Scientist fellowship (MR/J012351/1). Research at the UCL Institute of Child Health and Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children receives a proportion of the funding from the Department of Health's National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centres funding scheme. The MCS is funded by grants to former and current directors of the study from the Economic and Social Research Council (Professor Heather Joshi, Professor Lucinda Platt and Professor Emla Fitzsimons) and a consortium of government funders. The study sponsors played no part in the design, data analysis and interpretation of this study; the writing of the manuscript or the decision to submit the paper for publication, and the authors’ work was independent of their funders
Competing interests: None declared.
Ethics approval: The Northern and Yorkshire multicentre research ethics committee.
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
References
- 1.Braveman PA, Cubbin C, Egerter S, et al. . Socioeconomic disparities in health in the United States: what the patterns tell us. Am J Public Health 2010;100(Suppl 1):S186–96. 10.2105/AJPH.2009.166082 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pearce A, Lewis H, Law C. The role of poverty in explaining health variations in 7-year-old children from different family structures: findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2013;67:181–9. 10.1136/jech-2012-200970 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Howe LD, Lawlor DA, Propper C. Trajectories of socioeconomic inequalities in health, behaviours and academic achievement across childhood and adolescence. J Epidemiol Community Health 2013;67:358–64. 10.1136/jech-2012-201892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Marmot M. Fair society, healthy lives: The Marmot Review. 2010. http://www.instituteofhealthequity.org/Content/FileManager/pdf/fairsocietyhealthylives.pdf (accessed 3 Aug 2015).
- 5.Law C. Mother, fetus, infant, child and family: socio-economic inequalities. In: Gordon D, Dorling D, Davey Smith G, eds. Inequalities in health: the evidence presented to The Independent Inquiry into Inequalities in Health. Bristol: The Policy Press, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Blumenshine P, Egerter S, Barclay CJ, et al. . Socioeconomic disparities in adverse birth outcomes: a systematic review. Am J Prev Med 2010;39:263–72. 10.1016/j.amepre.2010.05.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.West P. Health inequalities in the early years: is there equalisation in youth? Soc Sci Med 1997;44:833–58. 10.1016/S0277-9536(96)00188-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Quon EC, McGrath JJ. Subjective socioeconomic status and adolescent health: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol 2014;33:433–47. 10.1037/a0033716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Costello EJ, Pine DS, Hammen C, et al. . Development and natural history of mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2002;52:529–42. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01372-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wille N, Bettge S, Ravens-Sieberer U, et al. . Risk and protective factors for children's and adolescents’ mental health: results of the BELLA study. Eur Child Adoles Psy 2008;17:133–47. 10.1007/s00787-008-1015-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Viner RM, Ross D, Hardy R, et al. . Life course epidemiology: recognising the importance of adolescence. J Epidemiol Community Health 2015;69:719–20. 10.1136/jech-2014-205300 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Viner RM. Chapter 8. Life stage: Adolescence. In: Lemer C, Todd K, Cheung R, eds. Annual report of the Chief Medical Officer 2012 our children deserve better: prevention pays. London: Department of Health, 2013:1–11. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rosenberg M. Society and the adolescent Self Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Levin KA, Torsheim T, Vollebergh W, et al. . National income and income inequality, family affluence and life satisfaction among 13 year old boys and girls: a multilevel study in 35 countries. Soc Indic Res 2011;104:179–94. 10.1007/s11205-010-9747-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bos AER, Muris P, Mulkens S, et al. . Changing self-esteem in children and adolescents: a roundmap for future interventions. Netherlands J Psychol 2006;62:26–33. 10.1007/BF03061048 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bartels M, Cacioppo JT, van Beijsterveldt TC, et al. . Exploring the association between well-being and psychopathology in adolescents. Behav Genet 2013;43:177–90. 10.1007/s10519-013-9589-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Iversen AC, Holsen I. Inequality in health, psychosocial resources and health behavior in early adolescence: the influence of different indicators of socioeconomic position. Child Indic Res 2008;1:291–302. 10.1007/s12187-008-9015-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Piko BF. Satisfaction with life, psychosocial health and materialism among Hungarian youth. J Health Psychol 2006;11:827–31. 10.1177/1359105306069072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Piko BF, Hamvai C. Parent, school and peer-related correlates of adolescents’ life satisfaction. Child Youth Serv Rev 2010;32:1479–82. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.07.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Shek DTL. Economic stress, psychological well-being and problem behavior in Chinese adolescents with economic disadvantage. J Youth Adolescence 2003;32:259–66. 10.1023/A:1023080826557 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shek DTL. Economic stress, emotional quality of life, and problem behavior in Chinese adolescents with and without economic disadvantage. Soc Indic Res 2005;71:363–83. 10.1007/s11205-004-8028-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Chen E, Paterson LQ. Neighborhood, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: How do they relate to adolescent health? Health Psychol 2006;25:704–14. 10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.704 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Connelly R, Platt L. Cohort profile: UK Millennium Cohort Study (MCS). Int J Epidemiol 2014;43:1719–25. 10.1093/ije/dyu001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hansen K. Millennium Cohort Study: a guide to the datasets (eighth edition). First, second, third, fourth and fifth surveys. London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gallop K, Rose N, Wallace E, et al. . Millennium Cohort Study fifth sweep (MCS5): Technical report. London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 26.University of London. Institute of Education. Centre for Longitudinal Studies, Millennium Cohort Study: Fifth Survey, 2012 [computer file]. Colchester, Essex: UK Data Archive [distributor], February 2014. SN: 7464 10.5255/UKDA-SN-7464-1 (accessed 3 Aug 2015). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Currie C, Molcho M, Boyce W, et al. . Researching health inequalities in adolescents: the development of the Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children (HBSC) Family Affluence Scale. Soc Sci Med 2008;66:1429–36. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sweeting H, Hunt K. Adolescent socio-economic and school-based social status, health and well-being. Soc Sci Med 2014;121:39–47. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.09.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Goodman E, Adler NE, Daniels SR, et al. . Impact of objective and subjective social status on obesity in a biracial cohort of adolescents. Obes Res 2003;11:1018–26. 10.1038/oby.2003.140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Finkelstein DM, Kubzansky LD, Goodman E. Social status, stress, and adolescent smoking. J Adolescent Health 2006;39:678–85. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Raboteg-Saric Z, Brajsa-Zganec A, Sakic M. Life satisfaction in adolescents: the effects of perceived family economic status, self-esteem and quality of family and peer relationships. Drus Istraz 2009;18:547–64. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cheng TL, Emmanuel MA, Levy DJ, et al. . Child health disparities: What can a clinician do? Pediatrics 2015;136:961–8. 10.1542/peds.2014-4126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
archdischild-2015-309651supp.pdf (208.6KB, pdf)