Abstract
Background and Objectives
Both chronic smoking and trait anxiety have been associated with dysregulations in psychobiological stress response systems. However, these factors have not been studied in conjunction. We expected trait anxiety and smoking status to attenuate stress reactivity. Furthermore, we expected an allostatic load effect resulting in particularly attenuated stress reactivity in high-anxious smokers. In addition, high-anxious smokers were expected to exhibit increased urges to smoke in response to stress.
Methods
115 smokers and 37 nonsmokers, aged 18 – 64 years, completed a laboratory session including mental stressors such as evaluated public speaking and mental arithmetic. Trait anxiety was assessed using Spielberger’s State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Cardiovascular autonomic indices, salivary cortisol, and the desire to smoke were measured at baseline, during stressors, and at recovery.
Results
Regression analyses showed that smokers exhibited attenuated cardiovascular stress responses in comparison to nonsmokers. Higher trait anxiety predicted attenuated systolic blood pressure responses to stress. No interaction effect of smoking status and trait anxiety was found in stress response measures. Higher trait anxiety predicted an increased desire to smoke in response to stress among smokers.
Conclusion
Results indicate that both smoking status and trait anxiety are associated with blunted sympatho-adrenal cardiovascular stress reactivity. Elevated urges to smoke in response to stress found among smokers with high trait anxiety suggest an important role of anxiety in smoking propensity and relapse.
Keywords: Chronic smoking, trait anxiety, mental stress, psychophysiology, cardiovascular system, sympathetic nervous system
BACKGROUND
Tobacco use causes severe health problems worldwide annually. Epidemiological studies estimated about 1.1 billion daily smokers worldwide with 81 percent male smokers (Jha, Ranson, Nguyen, & Yach, 2002; World Health Organization, 2011) and an annual death rate of approximately 5 to 6 million people globally (Mathers & Loncar, 2006; World Health Organization, 2014). Smoking plays a role in regulating negative mood and emotions (Gilbert, 1995; Leung, Gartner, Dobson, Lucke, & Hall, 2011) supporting a high prevalence of nicotine dependence among individuals with emotional dysfunctions (Capron et al., 2012; Langdon et al., 2013; Luk & Tsoh, 2010; Sonntag, Wittchen, Hofler, Kessler, & Stein, 2000). Specifically, higher rates of smoking have been found among individuals diagnosed with anxiety disorders (Goodwin et al., 2012; Grover, Goodwin, & Zvolensky, 2012; Ziedonis et al., 2008).
Smoking behavior and anxiety disorders
Individuals diagnosed with an anxiety disorder are twice as likely to smoke as the general population and smoking is a risk factor for developing an anxiety disorder (Breslau, Davis, & Schultz, 2003; Breslau & Klein, 1999). Sonntag and colleagues (2000) demonstrated in a prospective longitudinal study that social fear and social phobias increase the risk for nicotine dependence in adolescents and young adults. Similarly, individuals diagnosed with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) show a greater prevalence of cigarette smoking than the general population (reviewed by Rasmusson, Picciotto, & Krishnan-Sarin, 2006). Furthermore, a population-based study revealed that panic disorder is associated with occasional and regular smoking and an increased risk for new onsets of panic attacks among prior nicotine dependent individuals (Isensee, Wittchen, Stein, Hofler, & Lieb, 2003). Individuals who quit smoking exhibit greater depressed mood, increased anxiety, and impaired brain functions, which in turn, increases the likelihood of smoking relapse (Hughes, Gust, Skoog, Keenan, & Fenwick, 1991). One study investigating anxiety in smoking relapse revealed that female smokers with current or lifetime diagnoses of anxiety disorders were less likely to be abstinent than female smokers never diagnosed with an anxiety disorder (Piper et al., 2010). In addition, Zvolensky and colleagues (2008) demonstrated that patients diagnosed with an anxiety disorder were at increased risk of relapse during the first week post-smoking cessation compared to control participants. These different observations support strong linkages between clinical anxiety and smoking behavior. However, these results only apply to a minority of smokers. To draw conclusions of the relationship between anxiety and smoking for the general population, it is necessary to investigate individual differences in nonclinical anxiety among smoking populations.
Smoking behavior and trait anxiety in the general population
Only few studies have examined the relationship of anxiety as a fundamental personality trait in the general population and smoking behavior. Trait anxiety is defined as experiencing negative feelings such as worry, discomfort, tension, and stress across specific situations on a regular basis (Spielberger, Gorusch, & Lushene, 1983). Several studies have shown higher trait anxiety levels among chronic smokers compared to nonsmokers (Houston & Schneider, 1973; Pietras, Witusik, Panek, Szemraj, & Gorski, 2011; Sheahan et al., 2006) while other studies did not find any differences (Canals, Domenech, & Blade, 1996; Farley & Lester, 1995; Henry, Jamner, & Whalen, 2012). Inconsistencies of these findings may be due to different sample populations (e.g., college students vs. non-academic samples), variation in sample sizes, and different measurements of anxiety. Some studies suggest that smoking reduces state anxiety (Pomerleau & Pomerleau, 1987; Pomerleau, Turk, & Fertig, 1984) while other research indicates smoking cessation as being an attenuating factor for state anxiety (West & Hajek, 1997). However, specifically in stressful situations anxiety-based coping strategies may contribute to a greater likelihood of nicotine intake to manage stress (Morissette, Tull, Gulliver, Kamholz, & Zimering, 2007). These different observations produce an inconsistent picture of the relationship between trait anxiety and smoking behavior and suggest that stress may play another important role in the linkage.
Psychobiological stress responses and anxiety
The stress response involves multiple levels of physiological activation. Upon perception of stress, the corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) is produced in the hypothalamus. CRH stimulates the release of the adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in the anterior pituitary, which in turn initiates the production of cortisol in the adrenal cortex (i.e., the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis; e.g., al’Absi & Arnett, 2000). The sympathetic nervous system (SNS), which is responsible for catecholamine release in the adrenal medulla, is also activated. This leads to an increased heart rate (HR), blood pressure (BP), and alterations in other physiological systems of the body (Hoehn-Saric & McLeod, 2000). Research examining anxiety and psychobiological stress responses has shown inconsistent results. For instance, one study found a hyperactive HPA axis with increased cortisol levels in response to stress in people suffering from social anxiety (Condren, O’Neill, Ryan, Barrett, & Thakore, 2002) compared to healthy controls while in PTSD basal cortisol levels are often found to be lower than in healthy controls (Meewisse, Reitsma, de Vries, Gersons, & Olff, 2007). Research on trait anxiety has found a hypoactive stress response with reduced cardiovascular and cortisol responses in individuals with high trait anxiety (de Rooij, Schene, Phillips, & Roseboom, 2010; Jezova, Makatsori, Duncko, Moncek, & Jakubek, 2004). These blunted physiological stress responses may result from malfunctioning of biological stress systems. In the short-term, anxiety may heighten psychophysiological stress reactivity (e.g., increased cortisol) as shown by Boudarene and colleagues (2002). However, chronic anxiety may induce a prolonged physiological activation that may lead to systemic allostatic load, resulting in maladaptive physiological stress response patterns (McEwen, 1998, 2007).
Psychobiological stress response and smoking behavior
Interestingly, a growing body of evidence also indicates attenuated cardiovascular (Roy, Steptoe, & Kirschbaum, 1994; Straneva, Hinderliter, Wells, Lenahan, & Girdler, 2000) and neuroendocrine (al’Absi, Amunrud, & Wittmers, 2002; al’Absi, Wittmers, Erickson, Hatsukami, & Crouse, 2003; Kirschbaum, Strasburger, & Langkrar, 1993; Roy et al., 1994) stress responses in habitual smokers. The pharmacological effects of nicotine and toxic effects of other smoke constituents may have additive negative impacts in biological adaptation mechanisms, which may lead to prominent allostatic changes resulting in attenuated physiological responses to stress. Allostatic load (McEwen, 2007) may be particularly elevated in smokers with high trait anxiety, resulting in an additive effect on blunting of the psychobiological stress response. However, research investigating tobacco use, anxiety, and stress adaptation in conjunction is lacking and to the best of our knowledge, no study has directly tested this hypothesis.
OBJECTIVES
The aim of the present study was to examine associations of trait anxiety and habitual smoking with psychobiological stress responses. Based on studies reviewed above, we hypothesized that trait anxiety is associated with deterioration in stress responding, and that the effect of trait anxiety is most prominent in smokers due to additional physiological effects of smoking on stress responsivity. In addition, we expected that trait anxiety among smokers predicts an increased urge to smoke in response to stress due to partial evidence of anxiolytic effects of smoking (reviewed by Morissette et al., 2007).
METHODS
Participants
A sample of 152 participants (71women) comprising 115 smokers and 37 nonsmokers with an average age of 33.47 years (SD = 12.00) was recruited as part of a smoking relapse study (al’Absi, Nakajima, Allen, Lemieux, & Hatsukami, 2015). We note that previous studies from our laboratory have focused on stress, pain, and stress-induced analgesia in habitual smokers (al’Absi, Nakajima, & Grabowski, 2013; Nakajima and al’Absi, 2014). The majority of participants were Caucasian (62.2 % of non-smokers; 83.5% of smokers) and single (67.6 % of non-smokers; 58.3 % of smokers). Participants underwent an on-site screening session including the assessment of smoking status, alcohol, drug use, and medical history. To be eligible for the study, participants had to meet the following criteria: (1) no history of a chronic illness; (2) no current use of prescriptive medication except contraceptives; (3) no current alcohol or drug dependence, as well as no current psychiatric disorder and (4) weighted within ± 30% of Metropolitan Life Insurance norms (i.e. body mass index between 18 and 30). Smokers must have smoked at least an average of ≥10 cigarettes per day for a minimum of two years with a strong desire to quit smoking: Smokers were asked “What is your desire to quit smoking at this time?” on a 7-point rating scale of 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely strong). A strong desire for quitting smoking required to report the score of 4 or higher to this question; M = 5.77, SD = 1.06). Smokers smoked their first cigarette at the age of M = 16.16 (SD = 3.45) years, with an average duration of M = 11.28 (SD = 9.52) years, a daily cigarette consumption of M = 18.31 (SD = 6.55), and a baseline carbon monoxide level of M = 24.01 (SD = 13.22) ppm. Eligible subjects read and signed a consent form that was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Minnesota. After obtaining the consent, participants were asked to complete a battery of questionnaires (described below) and provided a breath sample for assessment of expired carbon monoxide (CO) and the time of their last cigarette.
Self-report measures
Demographic information and smoking history
Prior to the laboratory session, participants completed a battery of questionnaires (e.g., anxiety questionnaires, stress level assessment) and were asked for demographic information. Smokers completed additional forms on smoking history (e.g., number of cigarettes per average day).
Trait anxiety
The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, Gorusch, & Lushene, 1983) including 20 statements on the trait assessment and on a 4-point rating scale from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always) was used to assess trait anxiety. Items such as “I worry too much over something that really doesn’t matter” were utilized (Cronbach’s alpha ranging from 0.87 – 0.93) with higher scores indicating higher anxiety levels.
Stress
Psychosocial stress levels were assessed as a trait using the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) (Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983) including ten items on a 4 point rating scale from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always) such as “How often have you felt nervous and “stressed”?” (Cronbach’s alpha between 0.84 – 0.86). Higher scores indicate higher stress levels.
Urge to smoke
The brief version of the Questionnaire for Smoking Urges (QSU-B) (Cox, Tiffany, & Christen, 2001; Tiffany & Drobes, 1991) was used to assess two components of craving (factor 1: positive desire to smoke for reward including 5 items such as “I have desire for a cigarette right now”; factor 2: need to smoke for relief including 5 items such as “Nothing would be better than smoking a cigarette”; Cronbach’s alpha between 0.92 – 0.97). Higher scores indicate greater smoking urges.
Stressors
In this study we combined different stress tasks: public speaking and mental arithmetic that have been shown to modulate the physiological stress response in habitual smokers (al’Absi, Hugdahl, & Lovallo, 2002; al’Absi, Nakajima, & Grabowski, 2013).
Public speaking
Anxiety provoking stressors such as the public-speaking task has been shown to induce cardiovascular, endocrine, and emotional state changes (e.g., al’Absi et al., 1997; Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). Participants were given one of two scenarios: In scenario one, participants were asked to imagine a situation where they were being accused of shoplifting whereas in scenario two, participants were asked to imagine a situation where they were being accused in a hit and run accident. For either scenario, participants were asked to develop a defense speech (preparation phase, 4 min) and then to give the speech in front of the experimenter audience (speech phase, 4 min). Participants were instructed that the speech would be videotaped and evaluated by staff members at a later time. They were instructed that evaluators were going to rate their performance on persuasion, organization, articulation, and devotion. Cardiovascular measurements were taken at one minute and three minutes during preparation and delivery phases. Afterwards, participants were asked to complete a cognitive task.
Mental arithmetic
This task was chosen to elicit mental stress and it has been shown to reliably activate the HPA axis (al’Absi et al., 1997; Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). The task required continuous addition in a ten-minute block starting with a three-digit number. Participants were instructed to add each digit of the three-digit number and adding the sum of these three digits to the three-digit number itself. This should have been continued until the experimenter told the subject that the given result was wrong. In this case, the experimenter repeated the last three-digit number and participants were instructed to start over again with the given number. They were asked to perform as quickly and accurately as possible. Cardiovascular measurements were taken at the beginning (initial measurement) and then after every two minutes during the task.
Adrenocortical and cardiovascular measures
Cortisol concentrations were assessed by saliva samples. Participants placed cotton dental rolls in their mouth until saturated and collected the rolls into a plastic tube (Salivette® tubes, Sarstedt, Rommelsdorf, Germany). Salivary cortisol samples were assayed by a timeresolved fluorescence immunoassay with a cortisol–biotin conjugate as a tracer, 43 with a sensitivity of 0.4 nmol/L and inter- and intra-assay coefficients of variation less than 10% and 12%. Systolic bloodpressure (SBP), diastolic bloodpressure (DBP), and heartrate (HR) were measured using the Dinamap oscillometric monitor system (Critikon, Tampa, FL).
Procedure
Smokers and nonsmokers were instructed to refrain from caffeine, alcoholic beverages, and physical exercise prior to the laboratory session, which started between 12 and 2 PM to control for diurnal pattern of hormonal levels. Each participant was seated in a comfortable chair and was instructed to sit upright with legs uncrossed and feet on the floor. Starting with a 45-minute baseline period, they watched documentary films (selected for emotionally neutral content) to be instrumented for cardiovascular measurements such as HR and blood pressure. Five cardiovascular measurements (including SBP, DBP, HR) were obtained during the last 20 min of this baseline. Afterward, individual stress procedures (public speaking, mental arithmetic, cold pressor) always followed a resting/recovery period of at least 20 minutes. During the public speaking and the mental arithmetic task, cardiovascular measures were collected every 2 min. After the stress procedures a 20 min recovery period and cardiovascular measurements were obtained every 5 min. Saliva samples and self-report data (QSU-B) were collected after baseline, stressors, and recovery periods. At the end of the laboratory session participants were debriefed and received monetary compensation for participation.
Data analysis
This paper presents secondary analyses obtained from a larger study conducted to examine psychobiological predictors of smoking relapse (al’Absi et al., 2015). A series of independent pooled variance estimate t-test statistics accounting for differences in sample sizes between smokers and nonsmokers was conducted to examine group differences of demographic variables, trait measures, and hormonal (salivary cortisol levels) baseline measures. We used regression analyses to determine the associations of trait anxiety and smoking group with primary outcome variables: Stress indices (difference between stress, baseline, and recovery) of hormonal and cardiovascular measures such as SBP, DBP, and HR. In a secondary analysis, the Perceived Stress Scale was included as a trait-level predictor, to explore to what degree effects were based on general stress levels of participants. Additionally, associations of the urge to smoke (stress index of the QSU-B), trait anxiety, and perceived stress were analyzed. Cardiovascular measurements were averaged for baseline, stress (speech and math), and recovery. Cortisol data were log-transformed to meet assumption for normality.
RESULTS
Participant characteristics
The smoking and nonsmoking group did not differ in age, body mass index, anxiety, and baseline salivary cortisol (ps > 0.076). However, smokers indicated higher daily caffeine consumption, higher levels of perceived stress, and less years of education (ts > 2.96, ps < 0.004; see Table 1).
Table 1.
Nonsmokers (n = 37) | Smokers (n = 115) | Parametric test statistics | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||
M (SD) | M (SD) | pooled SD | t (150) | p | Cohen’s d | 95% CI | |
Age (years) | 30.03 (11.10) | 34.15 (11.30) | 11.17 | −1.79 | 0.076 | −0.37 | [−8.68, 0.44] |
Education (years) | 16.18 (3.12) | 14.21 (2.61) | 2.72 | 3.50 | 0.001 | 0.72 | [ 0.85, 3.07] |
BMI (kg/m2) | 24.23 (3.18) | 25.12 (4.59) | 4.27 | −1.01 | 0.315 | −0.21 | [−2.64, 0.86] |
Caffeine (drinks/day) | 0.63 (0.98) | 1.63 (1.80) | 1.63 | −2.96 | 0.004 | −0.61 | [−1.67,−0.33] |
STAI - T | 34.26 (9.73) | 36.31 (9.00) | 9.12 | −1.09 | 0.277 | −0.22 | [−5.77, 1.67] |
Perceived stress (PSS) | 16.65 (4.59) | 19.46 (4.68) | 4.63 | −2.94 | 0.004 | −0.61 | [−4.70,−0.92] |
Salivary cortisol baseline (log nmmol/l) | 2.06 (0.61) | 2.06 (0.70) | 0.68 | 0.00 | 1.00 | 0.00 | [−0.25, 0.25] |
Note. BMI: body mass index; STAI-T: State-Trait-Anxiety-Inventory (trait version); PSS: perceived stress scale.
Trait anxiety, smoking group, and perceived stress in association with stress indices
Results from regression analyses showed that trait anxiety was significantly associated with the SBP stress index in Model 1, β = −0.38, p = 0.012 indicating that higher trait anxiety predicted blunted SBP responses to stress (see Table 2). Similarly, the smoking group also predicted the SBP stress index, β = −0.16, p = 0.043 (see Table 2) demonstrating that smokers exhibited a blunted SBP response to stress. Unexpectedly, the interaction between smoking group and trait anxiety did not reach significance, β = 0.24, p = 0.112 1. Model 2 included perceived stress as an additional predictor and showed no significant influence on the SBP stress response, β = 0.09, p = 0.744 (see Table 2). DBP and HR stress indices were only predicted by smoking group, βs < − 0.22, ps < 0.008 showing that smokers also exhibited attenuated DBP and HR responses to stress whereas trait anxiety and perceived stress did not have any influences on DBP and HR stress indices, βs > − 0.17, ps > 0.267. Furthermore, no associations were found for the cortisol stress index including trait anxiety, smoking group, and perceived stress as predictors in the model, βs > − 0.10, ps > 0.573.
Table 2.
Predictor | Stress reactivity in systolic blood pressure
|
||
---|---|---|---|
R2 | b | 95% CI (b) | |
Model 1 | 0.08 | ||
Smoking | −3.39* | [−6.67,−0.11] | |
STAI - T | −0.37* | [−0.66,−0.08] | |
Smoking * STAI-T | 2.56 | [−0.60, 5.71] | |
| |||
Model 2 | 0.09 | ||
Smoking | −3.73* | [−7.13,−0.33] | |
STAI-T | −0.43* | [−0.76,−0.10] | |
Smoking * STAI-T | 2.61 | [−0.56, 5.77] | |
PSS | 0.16 | [−0.26, 0.58] |
Note. STAI-T: State-Trait-Anxiety-Inventory (trait version); PSS: perceived stress scale.
CI: confidence intervals (95%).
p < 0.05.
Trait anxiety and perceived stress in association with the urge to smoke for relief
Trait anxiety was significantly associated with the urge to smoke for relief (factor II, QSU-B) in response to stress, β = 0.29, p = 0.002 (see Table 3). Model 2 including perceived stress did not reach significance, β = 0.08, p = 0.497 and trait anxiety only showed a marginal trend, β = 0.24, p = 0.055 (see Table 3). However, including caffeine consumption in Model 3 showed significant effects for trait anxiety, β = 0.25, p = 0.042 and caffeine consumption, β = 0.19, p = 0.040 as predictors for the urge to smoke for relief indicating higher levels of trait anxiety and higher caffeine intake predicting greater stress-related urges to smoke for relief (see Table 3). This was not the case for perceived stress in Model 3, β = 0.06, p = 0.607. No effects of trait anxiety and perceived stress were found for the urge to smoke for reward (factor I, QSU-B), βs < 0.15, ps > 0.111.
Table 3.
Predictor | Urge to smoke (for relief)
|
||
---|---|---|---|
R2 | b | 95% CI (b) | |
Model 1 | 0.09 | ||
STAI - T | 0.21** | [0.08, 0.34] | |
| |||
Model 2 | 0.09 | ||
STAI - T | 0.17 | [−0.00, 0.34] | |
PSS | 0.11 | [−0.22, 0.44] | |
| |||
Model 3 | 0.13 | ||
STAI - T | 0.18* | [0.01, 0.35] | |
PSS | 0.09 | [−0.24, 0.41] | |
Caffeine intake | 0.66* | [0.03, 1.29] |
Note. STAI-T: State-Trait-Anxiety-Inventory (trait version); PSS: perceived stress scale.
Caffeine intake per day. CI: confidence intervals (95%). N=114.
p < 0.05
p < 0.01.
CONCLUSIONS
This study investigated the effects of trait anxiety and chronic smoking on the physiological response to stress. Our expectation of blunted cardiovascular responses to stress among smokers was confirmed, i.e., smokers independent of their trait anxiety status exhibited blunted cardiovascular stress responses relative to nonsmokers. Furthermore, higher trait anxiety irrespective of smoking status was also associated with blunted systolic blood pressure responses to stress. However, both such effects were not found for the salivary cortisol response to stress. Contrary to our expectations and the allostatic load model, we did not find an interaction effect of trait anxiety and smoking status indicating that habitual smoking did not further accentuate the attenuating effect of trait anxiety on systolic blood pressure responses to stress. As expected, trait anxiety was associated with increased stress-related urges to smoke for relief among smokers. Overall, these findings indicate that high trait anxiety and habitual smoking were independently linked to deteriorated cardiovascular stress responses. The current study is among the first to demonstrate the role of trait anxiety and habitual smoking in response to stress and expands our prior work and work of others (al’Absi et al., 2013; al’Absi et al., 2003; Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Roy et al., 1994; Straneva et al., 2000).
Smokers, regardless of trait anxiety, showed attenuated cardiovascular stress responses, which was also shown in our recent work (e.g., al’Absi et al., 2013). Smoking has been associated with the HPA axis and autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity in response to stress. Studies examining acute effects of smoking reported an additive effect of nicotine and stress in psychobiological mechanisms (Parrott & Murphy, 2012) due to common modulating patterns in the central nervous system (CNS) where stress and nicotine play important roles in activating the cardiovascular system. In contrast, chronic smoking may lead to a prolonged ANS activation resulting in decreased sensitivity to stress as well as other potential additive effects that are not associated with nicotine (Kirschbaum, Scherer, & Strasburger, 1994) such as trait anxiety. This may explain why the expected sympatho-adrenal response was not observed among smokers compared to nonsmokers.
The finding of an association between high levels of trait anxiety with decreased cardiovascular stress reactivity is in line with Jezova and colleagues (2004) showing attenuated epinephrine and norepinephrine secretion to stress in high-anxious compared to low-anxious individuals. Results suggest that high trait anxiety is associated with an inadequate regulation of the sympatho-adrenal system during acute stress. Other studies found similar patterns of attenuated physiological responses to stress in patients with different anxiety disorders (e.g., Hoehn-Saric & McLeod, 2000; McTeague et al., 2010; Petrowski, Wintermann, Schaarschmidt, Bornstein, & Kirschbaum, 2013). It should be noted that, in the present study, DBP and HR responses to stress were not associated with trait anxiety levels. In contrast to systolic blood pressure, which is a rather pure sympathetic autonomic measure regulated almost exclusively by the sympatho-adrenal axis via beta-adrenergic receptors (e.g., Obrist, 1981; Silvestini and Gendolla, 2011), HR is dually innervated by cardiac sympathetic as well as vagal efferent pathways (Berntson et al., 1997). This may explain why it diverged from the systolic blood pressure pattern. Also, HPA axis activity did not show differential responses to stress between smoking groups and we did not find any associations between cortisol stress responses and trait anxiety. Previous research did not reveal evidence of relationships between personality traits and the cortisol stress response either (Bohnen, Nicolson, Sulon, & Jolles, 1991; Schommer, Kudielka, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 1999). However, one study found associations between trait anger and the cortisol stress response (al’Absi, Carr, & Bongard, 2007). Clearly, more research is needed to address the linkage of smoking, anxiety, and stress among subclinical as well as clinical samples diagnosed with anxiety disorders. Due to the increasing literature on anxiety and smoking, adding stress to the prediction models may elucidate important mechanisms.
Furthermore, general perceived stress did not influence the physiological response to an acute stressor although higher PSS scores have been associated with psychological distress, physical symptomatology, and more life events (e.g., Cohen and Lichtenstein, 1990). In the current study the experienced level of stress did not explain more variance than trait anxiety did. Although we did not find any associations between general experienced stress and physiological stress markers, it is important to note that high anxious smokers are more likely to smoke when they experience negative affect and stress due to the expectation that smoking may alleviate their negative mood (Brandon & Baker, 1991; Langdon & Leventhal, 2014).
Results of the present study provide supportive evidence of particularly high smoking urges in response to stress among smokers with high trait anxiety. Thus, trait anxiety may play an important role in the relationship between stress and tobacco use. This is consistent with prior observations (al’Absi, Amunrud, et al., 2002; al’Absi et al., 2003): Nicotine’s tranquilizing effects during anticipation of stressors are thought to provide distraction from anxiety-provoking triggers and nicotine itself may reduce approach-avoidance conflict-generated distress (see Kassel, Stroud, & Paronis, 2003 for review). It has been suggested that nicotine contributes to dopamine (DA) release in the mesolimbic system, which is an important component of the human reward system implying that rewarding properties may be enhanced by smoking during exposure to stress and anxiogenic situations (see Balfour, 1991 for review). Individuals exposed to chronic stress with an impaired ability of adapting to stressful and anxiogenic situations may consider smoking as a reward and relief factor that may be associated with the vulnerability of developing nicotine dependence. The current finding of a positive relationship between trait anxiety and greater urges to smoke in response to stress provides experimental support to this account. Furthermore, it has been shown that negative mood and stress are associated with increased risk of smoking relapse (al’Absi, Hatsukami, & Davis, 2005; Leventhal, Ramsey, Brown, LaChance, & Kahler, 2008). Thus, smokers with high trait anxiety may be more likely to fail smoking cessation attempts due to an increased desire to smoke for relief during stressful situations compared to smokers with low trait anxiety. We found a positive association with urges to smoke for relief and caffeine consumption which is in line with a prior work (see Swanson, Lee, and Hopp, 1994 for review; Treloar, Piasecki, McCarthy, & Baker, 2014). It has been suggested that appetitive effects of cigarettes decrease after time of smoking increases which may possibly indicate reduced dopaminergic tone (nicotine receptor desensitization; e.g., Pierce and Kumaresan, 2006). It is possible that caffeine alleviates this effect through its stimulating dopaminergic characteristics maintaining the dopaminergic tone. Caffeine intake may impact the dopaminergic system significantly when smoking urges begin to occur (for discussion see Treloar et al., 2014). The role of trait anxiety in this relationship is not clear at this point.
The results of the current study are limited by a number of factors. First, the findings cannot make any causal direction due to the cross-sectional nature of the research design. Thus, it is not clear whether chronic smoking and/or trait anxiety causes dysregulations in central stress regulatory systems, or whether such stress dysfunctions predispose individuals to smoke or have higher anxiety. Second, task difficulty was not assessed in this study. It is possible that habitual smoking and high levels of anxiety are associated with dysregulations in central motivational systems, which may lead smokers and anxious individuals to be less engaged in stress tasks, partially explaining attenuated physiological responses. Third, cardiovascular reactivity and subjective reports to laboratory stressors may not be equivalent to responses to naturalistic stressors in daily life (Gerin, Pickering, & Schwartz, 2004). Electronic diary methods integrated in smartphones could sample stress experience and nicotine use in stress situations occurring spontaneously in daily life and analyze their relationship using advanced statistical models (Mehl & Connor, 2012; Wilhelm & Grossman, 2010). Fourth, due to a different focus of examining predictors of smoking relapse in a larger study protocol (results reported elsewhere: al’Absi et al., 2015) more smokers than non-smokers were recruited. Thus, regression analysis results may have been somewhat biased due to unequal sample sizes (e.g., Zahn, 2010). Accounting for this issue, randomly generated subsamples confirmed a stable effect of trait anxiety on systolic blood pressure whereas the effect of smoking status was partially unstable. Thus, this effect should be interpreted with caution. Nonetheless, the study has several strengths such as the use of multiple biopsychological measures and the combined investigation of the role of trait anxiety in stress responses in smokers and nonsmokers.
In conclusion, the current investigation demonstrated that habitual smoking and trait anxiety were associated with attenuated cardiovascular responses to an acute stressor. The increased desire to smoke for relief associated with high trait anxiety among smokers may be an important risk factor of smoking relapse. Further research examining the relationship between anxiety and nicotine dependence may productively focus on naturalistic observations of stress and smoking behavior in daily life in high-vs. low-anxious individuals using novel ambulatory assessment methods (Wilhelm & Grossman, 2010).
Acknowledgments
Funding
This research was supported in part by a grant to the senior author (Prof. al’Absi) from the National Institute of Health (R01DA016351 and R01DA027232).
Funding Body: Foundation for the National Institutes of Health
We thank Angie Harjue, Elizabeth Ford, and Barbara Gay for assistance with data collection and management.
Footnotes
Due to large differences in sample sizes between smokers and nonsmokers, which may bias statistical analyses (Zahn, 2009), three subsamples were randomly generated. Subsample 1(39 smokers, 37 nonsmokers) showed the same pattern: higher trait anxiety predicted blunted SBP responses to stress, β = −0.36, p = 0.017 and smokers exhibited blunted SBP response to stress, β = −0.25, p = 0.023; the interaction effect was not significant, β = 0.13, p = 0.376 (R2 = 0.16). Subsample 2 (38 smokers, 37 nonsmokers) showed a similar pattern: higher trait anxiety predicted blunted SBP responses to stress, β = −0.39, p = 0.014, whereas the effect for smokers exhibiting blunted SBP response to stress was only marginally significant, β = −0.21, p = 0.059. Again, the interaction effect was not significant, β = 0.26, p = 0.091 (R2 = 0.13). In subsample 3 (38 smokers, 37 nonsmokers) higher trait anxiety predicted attenuated SBP responses to stress, β = −0.42, p = 0.010; however, no effect was found for the smoking group factor, β = −0.11, p = 0.341; the interaction effect was also not significant, β = 0.19, p = 0.233 (R2 = 0.12).
Declaration of interest
The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.
References
- al’Absi M, Amunrud T, Wittmers LE. Psychophysiological effects of nicotine abstinence and behavioral challenges in habitual smokers. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2002;72(3):707–716. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00739-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Arnett DK. Adrenocortical responses to psychological stress and risk for hypertension. Biomedicine and Pharmacotherapy. 2000;54(5):234–244. doi: 10.1016/S0753-3322(00)80065-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Bongard S, Buchanan T, Pincomb GA, Licinio J, Lovallo WR. Cardiovascular and neuroendocrine adjustment to public speaking and mental arithmetic stressors. Psychophysiology. 1997;34(3):266–275. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Carr SB, Bongard S. Anger and psychobiological changes during smoking abstinence and in response to acute stress: prediction of smoking relapse. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2007;66(2):109–115. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Hatsukami D, Davis GL. Attenuated adrenocorticotropic responses to psychological stress are associated with early smoking relapse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;181(1):107–117. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2225-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Hugdahl K, Lovallo WR. Adrenocortical stress responses and altered working memory performance. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(1):95–99. doi: 10.1017/S0048577202001543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Nakajima M, Allen S, Lemieux A, Hatsukami D. Sex differences in hormonal responses to stress and smoking relapse: a prospective examination. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2015;17(4):382–389. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Nakajima M, Grabowski J. Stress response dysregulation and stress-induced analgesia in nicotine dependent men and women. Biological Psychology. 2013;93(1):1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- al’Absi M, Wittmers LE, Erickson J, Hatsukami D, Crouse B. Attenuated adrenocortical and blood pressure responses to psychological stress in ad libitum and abstinent smokers. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2003;74(2):401–410. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)01011-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balfour DJ. The influence of stress on psychopharmacological responses to nicotine. British Journal of Addiction. 1991;86(5):489–493. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01795.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berntson GG, Bigger JT, Jr, Eckberg DL, Grossman P, Kaufmann PG, Malik M, … van der Molen MW. Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 1997;34(6):623–648. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02140.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohnen N, Nicolson N, Sulon J, Jolles J. Coping style, trait anxiety and cortisol reactivity during mental stress. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1991;35(2–3):141–147. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(91)90068-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boudarene M, Legros JJ, Timsit-Berthier M. Study of the stress response: role of anxiety, cortisol and DHEAs. Encephale. 2002;28(2):139–146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandon TH, Baker TB. The smoking consequences questionnaire: The subjective expected utility of smoking in college students. Psychological Assessment. 1991;3:484–491. [Google Scholar]
- Breslau N, Davis GC, Schultz LR. Posttraumatic stress disorder and the incidence of nicotine, alcohol, and other drug disorders in persons who have experienced trauma. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(3):289–294. doi: 10.1001/Archpsyc.60.3.289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslau N, Klein DF. Smoking and panic attacks: an epidemiologic investigation. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56(12):1141–1147. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canals J, Domenech E, Blade J. Smoking and trait anxiety. Psychological Reports. 1996;79(3 Pt 1):809–810. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1996.79.3.809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capron DW, Blumenthal H, Medley AN, Lewis S, Feldner MT, Zvolensky MJ, Schmidt NB. Anxiety sensitivity cognitive concerns predict suicidality among smokers. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2012;138(3):239–246. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1983;24(4):385–396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Lichtenstein E. Perceived stress, quitting smoking, and smoking relapse. Health Psychology. 1990;9(4):466–478. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.9.4.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Condren RM, O’Neill A, Ryan MC, Barrett P, Thakore JH. HPA axis response to a psychological stressor in generalised social phobia. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27(6):693–703. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(01)00070-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox LS, Tiffany ST, Christen AG. Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2001;3(1):7–16. doi: 10.1080/14622200020032051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Rooij SR, Schene AH, Phillips DI, Roseboom TJ. Depression and anxiety: Associations with biological and perceived stress reactivity to a psychological stress protocol in a middle-aged population. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(6):866–877. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130(3):355–391. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farley J, Lester D. Smoking and trait anxiety. Psychological Reports. 1995;76(3 Pt 1):858. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1995.76.3.858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerin W, Pickering T, Schwartz A. Cardiovascular reactivity. In: Anderson N, editor. Encyclopedia of health and behavior. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications; 2004. pp. 152–159. [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert DG. Smoking: Individual Differences, Psychopathology, and Emotion. Washington, D.C: Taylor & Francis; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Goodwin RD, Lavoie KL, Lemeshow AR, Jenkins E, Brown ES, Fedoronko DA. Depression, anxiety, and COPD: the unexamined role of nicotine dependence. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2012;14(2):176–183. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grover KW, Goodwin RD, Zvolensky MJ. Does current versus former smoking play a role in the relationship between anxiety and mood disorders and nicotine dependence? Addictive Behaviors. 2012;37(5):682–685. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry SL, Jamner LD, Whalen CK. I (should) need a cigarette: adolescent social anxiety and cigarette smoking. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2012;43(3):383–393. doi: 10.1007/s12160-011-9340-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoehn-Saric R, McLeod DR. Anxiety and arousal: physiological changes and their perception. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2000;61(3):217–224. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00339-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houston JP, Schneider NG. Further evidence on smoking and anxiety. Psychological Reports. 1973;32(1):322. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1973.32.1.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes JR, Gust SW, Skoog K, Keenan RM, Fenwick JW. Symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. A replication and extension. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1991;48(1):52–59. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810250054007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isensee B, Wittchen HU, Stein MB, Hofler M, Lieb R. Smoking increases the risk of panic: findings from a prospective community study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(7):692–700. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jezova D, Makatsori A, Duncko R, Moncek F, Jakubek M. High trait anxiety in healthy subjects is associated with low neuroendocrine activity during psychosocial stress. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2004;28(8):1331–1336. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jha P, Ranson MK, Nguyen SN, Yach D. Estimates of global and regional smoking prevalence in 1995, by age and sex. American Journal of Public Health. 2002;92(6):1002–1006. doi: 10.2105/ajph.92.6.1002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kassel JD, Stroud LR, Paronis CA. Smoking, stress, and negative affect: correlation, causation, and context across stages of smoking. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(2):270–304. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Scherer G, Strasburger CJ. Pituitary and adrenal hormone responses to pharmacological, physical, and psychological stimulation in habitual smokers and nonsmokers. Clinical Investigation. 1994;72(10):804–810. doi: 10.1007/BF00180552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Strasburger CJ, Langkrar J. Attenuated cortisol response to psychological stress but not to CRH or ergometry in young habitual smokers. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 1993;44(3):527–531. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(93)90162-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langdon KJ, Leventhal AM. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and tobacco abstinence effects in a non-clinical sample: Evaluating the mediating role of negative affect reduction smoking expectancies. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2014 doi: 10.1177/0269881114546708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langdon KJ, Leventhal AM, Stewart S, Rosenfield D, Steeves D, Zvolensky MJ. Anhedonia and anxiety sensitivity: prospective relationships to nicotine withdrawal symptoms during smoking cessation. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2013;74(3):469–478. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung J, Gartner C, Dobson A, Lucke J, Hall W. Psychological distress is associated with tobacco smoking and quitting behaviour in the Australian population: evidence from national cross-sectional surveys. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;45(2):170–178. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.534070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leventhal AM, Ramsey SE, Brown RA, LaChance HR, Kahler CW. Dimensions of depressive symptoms and smoking cessation. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2008;10(3):507–517. doi: 10.1080/14622200801901971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luk JW, Tsoh JY. Moderation of gender on smoking and depression in Chinese Americans. Addictive Behaviors. 2010;35(11):1040–1043. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathers CD, Loncar D. Projections of global mortality and burden of disease from 2002 to 2030. PLoS Medicine. 2006;3(11):e442. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1998;840:33–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiological Reviews. 2007;87(3):873–904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McTeague LM, Lang PJ, Laplante MC, Cuthbert BN, Shumen JR, Bradley MM. Aversive imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder: trauma recurrence, comorbidity, and physiological reactivity. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67(4):346–356. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meewisse ML, Reitsma JB, de Vries GJ, Gersons BP, Olff M. Cortisol and post-traumatic stress disorder in adults: systematic review and meta-analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;191:387–392. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.024877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehl MR, Connor TS. Handbook of Research Methods for Studying Daily Life. New York: Guilford Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Morissette SB, Tull MT, Gulliver SB, Kamholz BW, Zimering RT. Anxiety, anxiety disorders, tobacco use, and nicotine: a critical review of interrelationships. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133(2):245–272. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakajima M, al’Absi M. Nicotine withdrawal and stress-induced changes in pain sensitivity: a cross-sectional investigation between abstinent smokers and nonsmokers. Psychophysiology. 2014;51(10):1015–1022. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obrist PA. Cardiovascular Psychophysiology: A perspective. New York: Plenum Press; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Parrott AC, Murphy RS. Explaining the stress-inducing effects of nicotine to cigarette smokers. Human Psychopharmacology. 2012;27(2):150–155. doi: 10.1002/hup.1247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrowski K, Wintermann GB, Schaarschmidt M, Bornstein SR, Kirschbaum C. Blunted salivary and plasma cortisol response in patients with panic disorder under psychosocial stress. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2013;88(1):35–39. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce RC, Kumaresan V. The mesolimbic dopamine system: the final common pathway for the reinforcing effect of drugs of abuse? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30(2):215–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pietras T, Witusik A, Panek M, Szemraj J, Gorski P. Anxiety, depression and methods of stress coping in patients with nicotine dependence syndrome. Medical Science Monitor. 2011;17(5):Cr272–276. doi: 10.12659/MSM.881767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piper ME, Smith SS, Schlam TR, Fleming MF, Bittrich AA, Brown JL, … Baker TB. Psychiatric disorders in smokers seeking treatment for tobacco dependence: relations with tobacco dependence and cessation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78(1):13–23. doi: 10.1037/a0018065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau &, Pomerleau OF. The effects of a psychological stressor on cigarette smoking and subsequent behavioral and physiological responses. Psychophysiology. 1987;24:278–285. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00295.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau, Turk DC, Fertig JB. The effects of cigarette smoking on pain and anxiety. Addictive Behaviors. 1984;9:265–271. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(84)90018-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasmusson AM, Picciotto MR, Krishnan-Sarin S. Smoking as a complex but critical covariate in neurobiological studies of posttraumatic stress disorders: a review. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2006;20(5):693–707. doi: 10.1177/0269881106060193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roy MP, Steptoe A, Kirschbaum C. Association between smoking status and cardiovascular and cortisol stress responsivity in healthy young men. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1994;1(3):264–283. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0103_6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schommer NC, Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. No evidence for a close relationship between personality traits and circadian cortisol rhythm or a single cortisol stress response. Psychological Reports. 1999;84(3 Pt 1):840–842. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1999.84.3.840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheahan SL, Rayens MK, An K, Riegel B, McKinley S, Doering L, … Moser DK. Comparison of anxiety between smokers and nonsmokers with acute myocardial infarction. American Journal of Critical Care. 2006;15(6):617–625. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvestrini N, Gendolla GH. Beta-adrenergic impact underlies the effect of mood and hedonic instrumentality on effort-related cardiovascular response. Biological Psychology. 2011;87(2):209–217. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sonntag H, Wittchen HU, Hofler M, Kessler RC, Stein MB. Are social fears and DSM-IV social anxiety disorder associated with smoking and nicotine dependence in adolescents and young adults? European Psychiatry. 2000;15(1):67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0924-9338(00)00209-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Gorusch R, Lushene R. State-trait anxiety manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychological Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Straneva P, Hinderliter A, Wells E, Lenahan H, Girdler S. Smoking, oral contraceptives, and cardiovascular reactivity to stress. Obstetrics and Gynecology. 2000;95(1):78–83. doi: 10.1016/s0029-7844(99)00497-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson JA, Lee JW, Hopp JW. Caffeine and nicotine: a review of their joint use and possible interactive effects in tobacco withdrawal. Addictive Behaviors. 1994;19(3):229–256. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90027-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiffany ST, Drobes DJ. The development and initial validation of a questionnaire on smoking urges. British Journal of Addiction. 1991;86(11):1467–1476. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01732.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treloar HR, Piasecki TM, McCarthy DE, Baker TB. Relations Among Caffeine Consumption, Smoking, Smoking Urge, and Subjective Smoking Reinforcement in Daily Life. Journal of Caffeine Research. 2014;4(3):93–99. doi: 10.1089/jcr.2014.0007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West R, Hajek P. What happens to anxiety levels on giving up smoking? American Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;154(11):1589–1592. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.11.1589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilhelm FH, Grossman P. Emotions beyond the laboratory: theoretical fundaments, study design, and analytic strategies for advanced ambulatory assessment. Biological Psychology. 2010;84(3):552–569. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. WHO Report on Gender, Health, Tobacco and Equity. 2011 from http://www.who.int/tobacco/publications/gender/gender_tobacco_2010.pdf.
- World Health Organization. Fact Sheet No. 339: Tobacco. 2014 from http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs339/en/
- Zahn I. Working with unbalanced cell sizes in multiple regression with categorical predictors. 2010 Retrieved February 17, 2016, from http://psychology.okstate.edu/faculty/jgrice/psyc5314/SS_types.pdf.
- Ziedonis D, Hitsman B, Beckham JC, Zvolensky M, Adler LE, Audrain-McGovern J, … Riley WT. Tobacco use and cessation in psychiatric disorders: National Institute of Mental Health report. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2008;10(12):1691–1715. doi: 10.1080/14622200802443569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zvolensky MJ, Gibson LE, Vujanovic AA, Gregor K, Bernstein A, Kahler C, … Feldner MT. Impact of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder on early smoking lapse and relapse during a self-guided quit attempt among community-recruited daily smokers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2008;10(8):1415–1427. doi: 10.1080/14622200802238951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]