Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2016 Sep 16;113(40):11172–11177. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1524982113

Absence of visual experience modifies the neural basis of numerical thinking

Shipra Kanjlia a,1, Connor Lane a, Lisa Feigenson a, Marina Bedny a
PMCID: PMC5056030  PMID: 27638209

Significance

Human numerical reasoning relies on a cortical network that includes frontal and parietal regions. We asked how the neural basis of numerical reasoning is shaped by experience by comparing congenitally blind and sighted individuals. Participants performed auditory math and language tasks while undergoing fMRI. Both groups activated frontoparietal number regions during the math task, suggesting that some aspects of the neural basis of numerical cognition develop independently of visual experience. However, blind participants additionally recruited early visual cortices that, in sighted populations, perform visual processing. In blindness, these “visual” areas showed sensitivity to mathematical difficulty. These results suggest that experience can radically change the neural basis of numerical thinking. Hence, human cortex has a broad computational capacity early in development.

Keywords: plasticity, blindness, number, development, vision

Abstract

In humans, the ability to reason about mathematical quantities depends on a frontoparietal network that includes the intraparietal sulcus (IPS). How do nature and nurture give rise to the neurobiology of numerical cognition? We asked how visual experience shapes the neural basis of numerical thinking by studying numerical cognition in congenitally blind individuals. Blind (n = 17) and blindfolded sighted (n = 19) participants solved math equations that varied in difficulty (e.g., 27 − 12 = x vs. 7 − 2 = x), and performed a control sentence comprehension task while undergoing fMRI. Whole-cortex analyses revealed that in both blind and sighted participants, the IPS and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices were more active during the math task than the language task, and activity in the IPS increased parametrically with equation difficulty. Thus, the classic frontoparietal number network is preserved in the total absence of visual experience. However, surprisingly, blind but not sighted individuals additionally recruited a subset of early visual areas during symbolic math calculation. The functional profile of these “visual” regions was identical to that of the IPS in blind but not sighted individuals. Furthermore, in blindness, number-responsive visual cortices exhibited increased functional connectivity with prefrontal and IPS regions that process numbers. We conclude that the frontoparietal number network develops independently of visual experience. In blindness, this number network colonizes parts of deafferented visual cortex. These results suggest that human cortex is highly functionally flexible early in life, and point to frontoparietal input as a mechanism of cross-modal plasticity in blindness.


Numerical reasoning pervades modern human culture. We readily represent quantity, whether thinking about apples, hours, people, or ideas. It has been suggested that this competence is rooted in a primitive nonsymbolic system of numerical representation that is shared among adults of diverse cultures, as well as with preverbal infants and nonhuman animals (1, 2). This nonsymbolic system allows these populations to estimate numbers of visual or auditory items and to compute over these quantities. For example, infants and monkeys can detect which of two arrays contains more items, and can add and subtract approximate quantities (14). The nonverbal, nonsymbolic system underlying this performance represents number in an inherently approximate way (5). However, numerate humans also have the unique ability to reason about quantities precisely using an acquired system of number symbols (5).

Reasoning about approximate and exact number depends on a frontoparietal network, a key node of which is the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) (6). The IPS is active when participants estimate the number of items in a nonsymbolic display as well as when they solve symbolic math problems (e.g., 23 − 19 = x), with more IPS activity during hard math problems than easier ones (6, 7). Temporary deactivation of the IPS with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) impairs performance on numerical tasks (8). In monkeys, the IPS contains numerosity-selective neurons that are tuned to specific numerosities (9).

Although these findings highlight the critical role of the IPS in numerical reasoning, the developmental origins of the neural basis of number representations remain largely unknown. IPS activity during numerical processing is seen in children as young as 4 y old, but these children have had years of experience with numerical information (10). How does the nature of early experience affect the development of the IPS? Here we investigated this question by probing numerical representations following atypical perceptual experience. Specifically, we tested the role of visual experience in the development of numerical representations by studying individuals who are blind from birth.

One possibility is that number is represented differently in blindness, because representations of number in the IPS are fundamentally visuospatial and develop from accumulated experience with seeing sets of items. Like early visual features such as color, contrast, and orientation, numerosity is susceptible to aftereffects. For example, viewing a large quantity of dots causes a subsequent set to be perceived as less numerous than its true quantity (11). Numerosity judgments are also influenced by the visual spatial frequency of arrays (12), suggesting that numerical estimation may tap a form of visual texture perception (13). Furthermore, the neuroanatomical location of number responses in the posterior parietal lobe is consistent with the suggestion that numerical processing is partially visual in nature (14, 15). The parietal lobe plays a central role in visuospatial processing: it is involved in guiding hand and eye movements, orienting spatial attention, mentally rotating objects, and maintaining spatial information in working memory (14, 16, 17). Hierarchical generative models trained on visual arrays develop “numerosity detectors” akin to the number neurons found in monkey IPS (18). Together, these findings suggest that visual experience may play a foundational role in the development of IPS number representations.

An alternative hypothesis is that IPS representations of number are modality independent. In sighted adults, the neurobiological underpinnings of number are similar across sensory modalities and input formats; the IPS is active not only when adults estimate numbers of visual objects but also when they estimate numbers of tones or view number symbols (19, 20). Behavioral evidence shows that newborn infants can approximate numbers of visual objects and numbers of events in auditory sequences (21). However, these findings leave open the question of whether visual experience is necessary to establish the role of the IPS in numerical thinking.

The available data are thus consistent with two possibilities. On the one hand, IPS number representations might depend on vision for normal development. Once instantiated, these representations become available for processing quantity in any modality. Alternatively, IPS quantity representations may be modality invariant throughout the lifespan. Studying the neural basis of numerical representations in congenitally blind individuals enables us to distinguish between these hypotheses.

The neural basis of numerical cognition in blind individuals is of interest for another, independent reason. In blindness, “visual” areas of the brain are colonized by nonvisual functions. These occipital areas respond to auditory and tactile stimuli, a phenomenon termed cross-modal plasticity (2225). Some of these plastic responses appear to be related to higher-cognitive functions—most prominently, language processing and verbal memory (23, 2527). For example, the visual system of blind individuals is active during spoken sentence comprehension and is sensitive to grammatical complexity (25, 27). There is evidence that this activity is functionally relevant: TMS to visual areas of blind participants impairs verb generation and Braille reading (28, 29). A key outstanding question is whether this plasticity is an example of a broader pattern whereby visual cortices are recruited for diverse higher cognitive functions, or whether it is an isolated phenomenon resulting from shared computations between vision and language processing (30). Here we addressed this question by asking whether the visual cortices of blind individuals are involved in mathematical reasoning. Like language, math is symbolic, yet it depends on different neural networks (6, 31); this raises the question of whether symbolic math and language elicit dissociable neural responses in the visual cortices of blind individuals.

In the current study, congenitally blind and blindfolded sighted adults (Table S1) performed a math task and a language control task while undergoing fMRI. In the math task, participants heard pairs of spoken subtraction equations, each containing an unknown variable x, and decided whether the value of x was the same in the two equations (e.g., 7 − 2 = x; 6 − 1 = x). We used subtraction because it requires active quantity manipulation rather than long-term memory retrieval, and has been shown to recruit the IPS more than operations such as addition and multiplication (32). We included two orthogonal math difficulty manipulations. Equation pairs contained either single-digit (easy) or double-digit (difficult) numbers (e.g., 7 − 2 = x vs. 27 − 12 = x), and were either algebraically simple (solving for an unknown difference) or complex (solving for an unknown minuend; e.g., 7 − 2 = x vs. x − 2 = 7). No “carry-over” operations were required to solve any of the math equations (e.g., 27 − 12 = x does not require carry-over, 27 − 19 = x requires carry-over). Thus, the difficulty of math problems did not depend on whether or not they involved carry-over. In the language control task, participants heard pairs of spoken sentences and judged whether the meanings of the sentences were the same.

Table S1.

Participant demographic information

Participant Gender Age, y Cause of blindness Light perception Education
B1 M 23 LCA Minimal Some college
B2 F 33 ROP Minimal BA
B3 F 70 ROP Minimal High School
B4* M 44 Unknown None JD
B5 F 68 ROP None MA
B6* F 27 ROP Minimal MA
B7* F 65 ROP None MA
B8* F 35 LCA Minimal MA
B9* M 48 LCA None JD
B10* F 40 ROP None MA
B11* F 50 LCA Minimal MA
B12* F 25 LCA Minimal MA
B13* F 63 ROP None MA
B14* M 37 CG/Cat None MA
B15* M 63 ROP None BA
B16* F 61 ROP None JD
B17* F 47 ROP None BA
Average
 Blind 12 F 47 BA
 Sighted 9 F 45 BA
*

Indicates that participant contributed resting-state data. −, Indicates that column feature was either not applicable for the group or incalculable for the group.

BA, bachelor of arts; Cat, cataracts; CG, congenital glaucoma; JD, juris doctor; LCA, Leber's congenital amaurosis; MA, master of arts; ROP, retinopathy of prematurity.

Results

Behavioral Results.

Accuracy (percentage correct) was analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and digit–number (single vs. double-digit) and algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex) as within-subject factors. Both blind and sighted participants made more errors on trials with double than single-digit problems (main effect of digit–number: F(1, 34) = 10.25, P = 0.003; group × digit–number interaction: F(1, 34) = 1.83, P = 0.19; Fig. S1). Both groups were also less accurate with algebraically complex than algebraically simple problems [main effect of algebraic complexity: F(1, 34) = 23.28, P < 0.001; group × algebraic complexity interaction: F(1, 34) = 0.31, P = 0.58] (Fig. S1). There was no effect of group on accuracy [main effect of group: F(1, 34) = 0.54, P = 0.47] and no other group interactions (SI Results, Behavioral Results).

Fig. S1.

Fig. S1.

Congenitally blind (n = 17) and sighted (n = 19) groups’ error rates (Left) and response times (Right) for all conditions in math task and language control task.

Response times (percentage correct) were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and digit–number (single vs. double-digit) and algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex) as within-subject factors. Both blind and sighted participants were slower to respond on trials with double-digit problems than single-digit problems [main effect of number of digits: F(1, 34) = 21.82, P < 0.001; group × digit–number interaction: F(1, 34) = 0.04, P = 0.85]. Both groups were also slower to respond on trials with algebraically complex than algebraically simple math problems [main effect of algebraic complexity: F(1, 34) = 72.60, P < 0.001; group × algebraic complexity interaction: F(1, 32) = 1.03, P = 0.32]. There was no effect of group on response times [main effect of group: F(1, 34) = 0.18, P = 0.68] and no other group interactions (SI Results, Behavioral Results).

To test whether accuracy was matched across the math and language tasks, we ran a 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subject factor and task (math vs. language) as a within-subject factor. Accuracy was similar across the tasks [main effect of task: F(1, 34) = 0.42, P = 0.53]. There was a marginal group × task interaction such that blind participants were slightly better on the language task, whereas sighted participants were slightly better on the math task [group × task interaction: F(1, 34) = 3.37, P = 0.08]. However, direct comparison of the blind and sighted groups revealed no significant group differences on either task [math: t(34) = −0.74, P = 0.74; language: t(34) = 1.01, P = 0.32] (see Fig. S1 and SI Results, Behavioral Results). Response times across the math and language tasks were analyzed using a separate 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and task (math vs. language) as a within-subject factor. Both blind and sighted participants were slower to respond on math trials than language trials [main effect of task: F(1, 34) = 27.6, P < 0.001; group × task interaction: F(1, 34) = 1.0, P = 0.33].

Preserved Frontoparietal Responses to Number in Congenital Blindness.

In whole-cortex analysis, both congenitally blind and sighted participants activated bilateral IPS more while solving math equations than during sentence comprehension (whole-brain analysis, P < 0.05, cluster corrected; Fig. 1, Upper, Fig. S2, and Table S2). A whole-cortex group × task interaction analysis failed to find any regions that responded more to math than language in sighted than blind individuals (group × task interaction, sighted > blind, math > language, P < 0.05, cluster corrected).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

(Upper) Brain regions active for math > language (warm colors) and language > math (cool colors) in blind (n = 17) and sighted (n = 19) individuals (P < 0.05, cluster corrected). (Lower) Brain regions more correlated with left and right IPS in blind (n = 13) relative to sighted (n = 9) individuals in resting-state data (within-hemisphere correlations; left hemisphere: P < 0.05, FDR corrected; right hemisphere: P < 0.001, uncorrected; see Fig. S6 for between-hemisphere correlations). Left and right IPS seeds shown in white. Functional activation for blind > sighted, math > sentences contrast (P < 0.05, cluster corrected) shown in white outline.

Fig. S2.

Fig. S2.

Brain regions active during math task (red) and language task (blue) in blind (B; n = 19) and sighted (S; n = 17) individuals, and brain regions more active in blind individuals relative to sighted individuals (B > S) (P < 0.05, cluster corrected). Response times included as regressor in general linear model (GLM) at time of response.

Table S2.

Brain regions more active for math than sentences

Brain region x y z Peak t mm2 Pcluster
Blind
 Left superior parietal lobule −17 −70 45 8.25 3,268.82 0.0002
 Left precuneus −6 −72 50 7.95
 Left supramarginal gyrus −54 −39 47 6.82
 Left postcentral sulcus −35 −44 42 6.36
 Left intraparietal sulcus −28 −64 43 5.87
 Left superior parietal lobule −15 −58 60 5.47
 Left middle frontal gyrus −39 50 9 7.71 1,376.01 0.008
 Left transverse frontopolar gyri and sulci −21 59 −3 6.78
 Left middle frontal gyrus −45 31 29 6.56
 Left superior frontal sulcus −22 3 50 7.96 932.94 0.0172
 Left superior precentral sulcus −34 −8 46 4.1
 Left pericallosal sulcus −2 −30 27 6.47 792.97 0.021
 Left marginal branch of cingulate sulcus −11 −41 45 5.93
 Left middle-anterior cingulate gyrus and sulcus −8 5 48 5.9 487.78 0.0426
 Left superior frontal gyrus −7 31 31 5.58
 Right intraparietal sulcus 24 −60 50 7.89 1,585.75 0.0084
 Right precuneus 5 −61 56 7.82
 Right superior occipital gyrus 23 −75 44 7.09
 Right superior parietal lobule 24 −59 61 5.87
 Right middle occipital sulcus 33 −82 9 6.4 1,206.92 0.012
 Right middle occipital gyrus 40 −83 22 5.76
 Right occipital pole 24 −98 8 5.52
 Right superior occipital sulcus 26 −83 16 5.29
 Right inferior frontal sulcus 43 33 20 7.27 1,124.94 0.0128
 Right middle frontal gyrus 38 27 39 7.12
 Right superior frontal sulcus 26 34 34 5.25
 Right supramarginal gyrus 59 −25 36 4.79
 Right middle frontal sulcus 27 49 3 4.57
 Right middle frontal gyrus 35 4 55 8.39 829.84 0.018
 Right superior frontal gyrus 23 3 66 5.45
 Right posterior cingulate gyrus and sulcus 3 3 34 8.35 517.48 0.0316
 Right pericallosal sulcus 5 −15 30 4.04
 Right marginal branch of cingulate sulcus 7 −41 44 6.89 439.95 0.0418
 Right medial occipitotemporal sulcus 32 −43 −15 5.84 393.22 0.0488
Sighted
 Left intraparietal sulcus −34 −46 42 7.9 2,574.59 0.0016
 Left angular gyrus −29 −69 41 7.62
 Left superior parietal lobule −13 −61 61 5.99
 Left marginal branch of cingulate sulcus −16 −37 41 10.86 1,014.38 0.0086
 Left posterior-dorsal cingulate gyrus −3 −25 33 4.75
 Left pericallosal sulcus −2 −28 27 4.11
 Right marginal branch of cingulate sulcus 13 −28 38 7.48 2,090.13 0.0014
 Right superior parietal lobule 16 −75 45 5.99
 Right middle occipital gyrus 40 −80 30 5.98
 Right superior parietal lobule 17 −63 63 5.93
 Right precuneus 8 −54 59 5.23
 Right intraparietal sulcus 36 −46 36 5.96 907.48 0.008
 Right supramarginal gyrus 58 −36 44 4.96
 Right superior precentral sulcus 31 −4 46 4.92 432.29 0.0402
 Right superior frontal gyrus 18 14 62 4.61
Blind > sighted
 Left middle occipital sulcus −25 −95 1 5.08 508.99 0.0282
 Left medial occipitotemporal sulcus −29 −55 −12 4.84 399.62 0.0474
 Right middle occipital sulcus 33 −82 9 6.38 962.39 0.0064
 Right superior occipital sulcus 27 −84 15 5.19
 Right occipital pole 24 −98 8 5.08
 Right medial occipitotemporal sulcus 32 −45 −14 5.56 574.51 0.0194

Peaks of brain regions active more for math than language (P < 0.05, cluster corrected; P < 0.01 cluster-forming threshold; 10 mm minimum distance between peaks). Coordinates reported in MNI space. Peak t: t values corresponding to local maxima; mm2: area occupied by cluster on cortical surface; Pcluster: P value for entire cluster.

We next used a region of interest (ROI) analysis to ask whether math difficulty modulated IPS activity in blind and sighted individuals, using a 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and digit–number (single vs. double-digit), algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex), and hemisphere (left vs. right) as within-subject factors. Across blind and sighted groups, the IPS was sensitive to digit–number [main effect of digit–number: F(1, 34) = 46.08, P < 0.001] and algebraic complexity [main effect of algebraic complexity: F(1, 34) = 18.84, P < 0.001; main effect of group: F(1, 34) = 0.21, P = 0.65] (Fig. 2 and Figs. S3 and S4). Crucially, the factor of group did not interact with digit–number [F(1, 34) = 1.784, P = 0.18] or algebraic complexity [F(1, 34) = 0.28, P = 0.60]. Thus, we found no difference in IPS sensitivity to math difficulty between the blind and sighted groups (see SI Results, Preserved Frontoparietal Responses to Number in Congenital Blindness for within-group ANOVAs).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Responses to math difficulty in IPS (Left) and visual cortex (rMOG) (Right) regions of interest in blind (n = 17) and sighted (n = 19) individuals (error bars represent SEM). Percentage of signal change relative to rest was extracted from individual-subject ROIs defined within IPS search-space and average rMOG search-space, shown at the top. IPS results are averaged across left and right hemispheres (see Fig. S3 for results in left and right IPS, separately).

Fig. S3.

Fig. S3.

Responses to math difficulty in left and right IPS ROIs in blind (n = 17) and sighted (n = 19) individuals (error bars represent SEM). Percentage of signal change relative to rest was extracted from individual subject ROIs (Lower) defined within left and right IPS search spaces (Upper).

Fig. S4.

Fig. S4.

Percentage of signal change relative to rest over time in left and right IPS and right MOG in blind (Left; n = 17) and sighted (Right; n = 19) individuals.

In the sighted group, the effect of digit–number was more pronounced in the left hemisphere [group × digit–number × hemisphere interaction: F(1, 34) = 8.33, P = 0.007]. No other group interactions were significant (SI Results, Preserved Frontoparietal Responses to Number in Congenital Blindness).

Responses to Number in Visual Cortex of Blind Adults.

In whole-cortex analyses, distinct subregions of visual cortex responded to number vs. language in congenitally blind individuals. We observed greater responses to math equations than sentences in the right and left middle occipital gyri (MOG) in congenitally blind compared with sighted participants [group (blind > sighted) × task (math > language) interaction, P < 0.05, cluster-corrected] (Fig. 1. Upper; and Table S2). This effect was more pronounced in the right hemisphere. We also found that neighboring regions in right lateral occipital and ventral occipitotemporal (rVOT) cortices responded more during the language task than the number task in the blind compared with the sighted [group (blind > sighted) × task (language > math) interaction] (Fig. 1. Upper; and Table S2) (25).

We used an ROI analysis to test for sensitivity to math difficulty in the rMOG of the blind and sighted groups (individual subject rMOG ROIs defined based on math > language contrast using leave-one-run-out analysis) (Fig. 2 and Fig. S4). We ran a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and digit–number (single vs. double-digit) and algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex) as within-subject factors. We found a main effect of digit–number and a group × digit–number interaction [main effect of digit–number: F(1, 34) = 28.08, P < 0.001; group × digit–number interaction: F(1, 34) = 28.08, P < 0.001]. In post hoc comparisons, we found that the effect of digit–number was significant in the blind group but not the sighted group [main effect of digit–number in blind rMOG: F(1, 16) = 24.43, P < 0.001; main effect of digit–number in sighted rMOG: F(1, 18) = 1.80, P = 0.20]. Thus, in blind but not sighted individuals, rMOG is sensitive to the number of digits in math equations.

In the same ANOVA we observed a main effect of algebraic complexity [F(1, 34) = 6.50, P = 0.02] but no algebraic complexity × group interaction [F(1, 34) = 1.24, P = 0.27]. Although the algebraic complexity × group interaction was not significant, the algebraic complexity effect was numerically larger in the blind group (Fig. 2; see SI Results, Responses to Number in Visual Cortex of Blind Adults for within-group ANOVAs). We also observed a main effect of group, with larger overall occipital responses in the blind group than the sighted group [main effect of group: F(1, 34) = 34.53, P < 0.001]. No other group interactions were significant (SI Results, Responses to Number in Visual Cortex of Blind Adults).

Relationship Between Occipital Activity and Behavior.

In the blind group, visual cortex activity correlated with performance on the math task. Participants with larger accuracy differences between the hardest and easiest math conditions also showed a larger activation difference between these conditions in number-responsive visual cortex (rMOG) (R2 = 0.28, P = 0.03; Fig. S5). This correlation was not significant in the sighted participants (R2 = 0.06, P = 0.33; Fig. S5). A similar correlation with performance was observed in bilateral IPS in the blind (R2 = 0.24, P = 0.05) but not the sighted group (R2 = 0.07, P = 0.26). However, there was no difference between these correlations across groups (rMOG: P = 0.35; IPS: P = 0.48).

Fig. S5.

Fig. S5.

Percentage of signal change difference between hardest and easiest math trials in rMOG (Left), bilateral IPS (Right) correlated with difference in accuracy between hardest and easiest math trials [black: correlations for blind group (n = 17); gray: correlations for sighted group (n = 19)].

Increased Functional Connectivity Between Visual Cortices and the Frontoparietal Number Network in Blindness.

We found increased correlations between the left IPS and number-responsive visual cortices (left and right MOG) in blind relative to sighted participants (main effect of group, seed to whole-cortex analysis P < 0.05, FDR corrected; Fig. 1, Lower, Fig. S6, and Table S3). A similar but weaker pattern was observed for the right IPS (P < 0.001, uncorrected; Fig. 1, Lower, Fig. S6, and Table S3).

Fig. S6.

Fig. S6.

Brain regions more correlated with left and right IPS seeds (shown in white) at rest in blind group (n = 13) relative to sighted group (n = 9). Functional activation for blind > sighted, math > sentences contrast (P < 0.05, cluster corrected) shown in white outline.

Table S3.

Brain regions correlated with left and right IPS at rest

Brain region x y z Peak t Peak PFDR
Correlation with left IPS in blind > sighted
 Left posterior transverse collateral sulcus −30 −78 −11 5.99 0.01476
 Left middle occipital sulcus −30 −87 6 5.91 0.01476
 Left superior occipital gyrus −11 −95 13 5.83 0.01476
 Left fusiform gyrus −30 −54 −18 5.17 0.01476
 Left superior occipital sulcus −24 −84 22 5.13 0.01476
 Left medial occipitotemporal sulcus −23 −59 −10 4.89 0.01476
 Left lingual gyrus −10 −83 −12 4.63 0.01478
 Left middle frontal sulcus −30 46 19 3.83 0.02811
 Right superior temporal sulcus 50 −11 −20 5.53 0.12521
 Right superior occipital gyrus 15 −90 20 5.03 0.12521
 Right occipital pole 20 −82 −14 4.76 0.12521
 Right middle occipital gyrus 31 −89 15 4.47 0.12521
 Right medial occipitotemporal sulcus 24 −65 −5 4.12 0.12521
 Right lingual gyrus 20 −45 −11 3.97 0.12521
 Right inferior occipital gyrus and sulcus 38 −76 −15 3.94 0.12521
 Right middle occipital sulcus 34 −82 2 3.94 0.12521
 Right postcentral sulcus 32 −35 44 3.8 0.12521
Correlation with right IPS in blind > sighted
 Right middle occipital gyrus 42 −79 15 4.85 0.17848
 Right superior occipital sulcus 27 −78 20 4.47 0.17848
 Right medial occipitotemporal sulcus 28 −53 −7 4.23 0.17848
 Right cuneus 7 −82 9 3.56 0.24553

Peaks of brain regions correlated with left and right IPS during rest (10 mm minimum distance between peaks). Coordinates reported in MNI space. Peak t: t values corresponding to local maxima; peak PFDR: FDR-corrected P value corresponding to significance of peak.

We asked whether the increased functional connectivity between the IPS and visual cortex in blindness was specific to number-responsive visual areas, as opposed to language-responsive visual areas (rMOG vs. rVOT). In blind but not sighted individuals, we found a double-dissociation in the functional connectivity patterns of math-responsive visual cortices (rMOG) and language-responsive visual cortices (rVOT): rMOG was more correlated with the IPS, whereas rVOT was more correlated with language-responsive inferior frontal cortex (rIFC). A 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and seed (rMOG vs. rVOT) and ROI (IPS vs. IFC) as within-subject factors revealed a significant group × seed × ROI interaction [F(1, 20) = 5.0, P = 0.04; Fig. 3 and Fig. S7]. In blind individuals, there was a more pronounced dissociation in the functional connectivity patterns of rMOG and rVOT than in sighted individuals. Within-group ANOVAs revealed a stronger connectivity dissociation in the blind than the sighted group [seed (rMOG vs. rVOT) × ROI (IPS vs. IFC) interaction in blind group: F(1, 12) = 15.27, P = 0.002; seed × ROI interaction in sighted group: F(1, 8) = 5.88, P = 0.04]. There was also a significant effect of group [F(1, 20) = 16.01, P = 0.001].

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Resting-state correlations of visual and frontoparietal areas in the blind group (n = 13) are related to function. On the y axis is the difference between correlations across number- and language-responsive regions of visual cortex. Positive scores (red) indicate stronger correlation with number-responsive visual cortex (MOG), and negative scores (blue) indicate stronger correlation with language-responsive visual cortex (VOT) (error bars represent SEM). Number-responsive frontoparietal areas (IPS, PFC) are more correlated with number-responsive visual area (MOG), whereas language-responsive inferior frontal cortex (IFC) is more correlated with language-responsive visual area (VOT). See Fig. S7 for correlations in sighted group.

Fig. S7.

Fig. S7.

Resting-state correlations between visual and frontoparietal areas in the sighted group (n = 9). On the y axis is the difference between correlations across number- and language-responsive regions of visual cortex (defined math > language, blind > sighted). Positive scores (red) indicate stronger correlation with number-responsive visual cortex (MOG), and negative scores (blue) indicate stronger correlation with language-responsive visual cortex (VOT) (error bars represent SEM).

This resting-state dissociation also held when we compared rMOG and rVOT connectivity with two neighboring prefrontal regions. In blind but not sighted individuals, rMOG was more correlated with number-responsive right prefrontal cortex (rPFC) than language-responsive rIFC, whereas rVOT was more correlated with language-responsive rIFC than number-responsive rPFC [2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA: group (blind vs. sighted) × seed (rMOG vs. rVOT) × ROI (rPFC vs. rIFC) interaction: F(1, 20) = 8.50, P = 0.009]. Within-group ANOVAs showed that the dissociation was significant in the blind but not the sighted group [seed × ROI interaction in blind group: F(1, 12) = 14.80, P = 0.002; seed × ROI interaction in sighted group: F(1, 8) = 0.05, P = 0.84] (Fig. 3 and Fig. S7). The main effect of group was not significant [F(1, 20) = 2.82, P = 0.11].

Similar dissociations between the functional connectivity patterns of math- and language-responsive visual cortices were observed in the left hemisphere (detailed in SI Experimental Procedures, Definition of ROIs for Resting-State Correlation Analyses). In sum, in the blind group, number-responsive visual cortex had functionally specific increases in correlations with number-responsive areas in prefrontal and parietal cortices.

Discussion

IPS Number Representations Develop Independent of Visual Experience.

Previous studies show that the IPS is active when adults solve math equations and estimate nonsymbolic quantities (6, 7). This IPS sensitivity to number is present by 4 y of age, before formal math training (10, 33, 34). However, the effect of experience on the neural basis of number processing has remained largely unknown. Here we shed light on the role of early visual experience in the emergence of IPS number representations. We report that the functional profile of the IPS in numerical processing is preserved in individuals who are blind from birth, demonstrating that visual experience with numerical sets is not necessary for the typical development of IPS number responses.

The resilience of number representations in blindness is noteworthy in light of the links between number and visuospatial processing. In adults, individual differences in both nonsymbolic and symbolic number performance correlate with individual differences in visual discriminations involving area, density, and orientation (35, 36). Children who are better at mentally rotating visual objects perform better on math tasks (37). Numerical estimation and visuospatial functions, like orienting visual attention, are supported by neighboring regions of parietal cortex (1417). Despite these links between numerical and visual processing, we find that IPS representations of number develop independently of visual experience with sets.

The abstract nature of number representations is also noteworthy given the differences in how sensory modalities convey numerical information. Whereas humans can rapidly perceive large numbers of objects simultaneously through vision (e.g., hundreds within 1.5 s), the number of objects that can be concurrently perceived through audition or touch is limited to ∼10 or fewer (observers can estimate large numerosities through audition, but require sequential presentation) (12, 20, 3842). The present results suggest that IPS representations of number are resilient to such differences in input, and suggest that IPS representations of number are not rooted in any one sensory modality, but rather are intrinsically modality independent.

Visual Cortex of Congenitally Blind Adults Is Recruited into Number-Processing Network.

Although we found that visual experience is not required for IPS representations of number, blindness does change the neural basis of numerical cognition in a surprising way. We found that in blind individuals, a subset of early visual cortices is active while solving math equations, and this activity scales with mathematical difficulty.

Much evidence has documented responses to auditory and tactile stimuli in visual cortices of congenitally blind individuals (2227). The mechanisms and the scope of functional reorganization in cross-modal plasticity remain debated (43). On the one hand, some examples of visual cortex plasticity preserve aspects of the original visual functions. Visual motion-responsive area MT+ responds to auditory motion in blindness (44), and parts of visual cortex typically involved in visuospatial localization are active when blind individuals localize sounds (45). On the other hand, visual cortices of blind individuals are also active during high-level language tasks such as remembering words and understanding sentences (2327). Here we find that these visual cortex responses to language coexist with responses to number. Our results thus suggest that previously observed plasticity for language is part of a broader pattern whereby the visual system of blind individuals takes on higher cognitive functions.

The responses to math that we observed in the occipital cortices of blind individuals overlap with early visual areas that, in sighted individuals, contain retinotopic maps, support visual functions such as motion detection, shape representation, and visuospatial attention (4648). Unlike these visual functions, mathematics is symbolic and depends on cultural experience. The present results thus show that plasticity need not preserve the “typical” functions of cortex, and that the same cortical circuit can participate in widely different cognitive functions depending on experience (26, 27). A full test of this idea will require evidence of the functional relevance of visual cortices to numerical behavior. For example, studies using TMS suggest that visual cortices are functionally relevant for Braille reading, verb generation, and tactile discrimination (28, 29, 49). In the present study we observed a relationship between numerical performance and neural activity in number-responsive visual cortex of blind individuals, suggesting that visual cortex plasticity may play a role in modulating behavior. The functional relevance of the visual cortex for numerical cognition should be directly tested using techniques such as TMS. It is also important to investigate the representational content of number-responsive visual regions in blindness. It is not yet known whether, like the IPS, number-responsive visual regions participate in nonsymbolic number processing (e.g., numerical approximation), and whether activity in visual cortices codes for different numerosities (6).

Additionally, we found that in resting-state data, number-responsive occipital areas of blind individuals were correlated with the frontoparietal number network, whereas language-responsive visual areas correlated with the language network. These data show that resting-state patterns relate to functional dissociations in visual cortices of blind individuals. This pattern points to input from frontoparietal cortices as a possible mechanism for the dramatic functional reorganization from low-level vision to symbolic number.

Finally, this work raises questions regarding the timing of radical cortical plasticity. We hypothesize that such extreme functional repurposing—here from vision to symbolic number—is restricted to a critical period during development. Previous work has shown that congenital and late blindness lead to different plasticity patterns (50). An intriguing possibility, then, is that cortex is cognitively pluripotent only in early development. If so, the functions of visual cortices in late blind individuals may resemble the original functions of visual cortices in the sighted. Testing these predictions will further inform our understanding of how biology and experience shape the neural basis of thought.

Experimental Procedures

Participants.

Nineteen sighted (9 females, mean age 46 y, SD = 16) and 17 congenitally blind adults (12 females, mean age 47 y, SD = 16) participated (Table S1). Thirteen of the blind and nine of the sighted participants contributed resting-state data. No sighted or blind participants had cognitive or neurological disabilities (screened through self-report). All blind participants lost their vision due to pathology at or anterior to the optic chiasm, not due to brain damage, and had at most minimal light perception from birth (never saw colors, shapes, or motion; Table S1). Informed consent from participants was obtained in accordance with the Johns Hopkins Medicine Institutional Review Boards. Four additional blind participants were scanned but not included in the final sample because their average accuracy across math and language trials was significantly lower than the group mean (performance outside the 95% confidence interval). Two sighted participants were excluded due to an error in MRI data acquisition.

Behavioral Paradigm.

Participants completed a math task and a language control task. On each trial in the math task, participants heard two subtraction equations, each containing an unknown variable x. Participants judged whether the value of x was the same across the two equations (e.g., 7 − 2 = x, 6 − 1 = x; value of x is same). Trials were divided into four categories based on two orthogonal difficulty manipulations: (i) digit–number and (ii) algebraic complexity. Half the math trials consisted of easy, single-digit problems, and half consisted of difficult, double-digit problems (e.g., 7 − 2 = x vs. 27 − 12 = x). Orthogonally, half the trials contained simpler algebraic equations in which the variable was to the right of the equal sign, and half contained harder equations in which the variable was to the left of the equal sign (e.g., 7 − 2 = x vs. x − 2 = 7).

Each trial began with a tone (0.25 s), followed by one spoken math equation (3.5 s), a brief delay (2.75 s), a second spoken math equation (3.5 s), and a response period (4 s; 14 s total). Participants responded by pressing the left (“same”) or right (“different”) button on an MRI-compatible response pad.

The control task was structured similarly to the math task: participants listened to pairs of sentences and indicated whether the sentences’ meanings were the same. One sentence in each pair was presented in the active voice and the other in passive voice, in counterbalanced order. On “different” trials, who-did-what-to-whom was changed across the pairs. Participants heard each sentence and each combination of math equations once during the course of the experiment.

Each of the six runs consisted of 24 trials (14 s each) and six periods of rest (16 s each) for a total of 7.2 min. The order of trial types was counterbalanced across runs in a Latin square design.

Responses were collected using a Cedrus response pad. Trials on which participants did not respond were excluded from the behavioral and fMRI data analysis [blind: 2.05% of trials, SD = 2.09; sighted: 2.81% of trials, SD = 3.23; t(34) = −0.83, P = 0.41].

fMRI Data Acquisition.

Whole-brain MRI structural and functional data were collected with a 3T Phillips scanner. T1-weighted anatomical images were collected in 150 1-mm axial slices (1-mm isotropic voxels). Functional BOLD data were acquired in 36 3-mm axial slices (2.4 × 2.4 × 3 mm voxels; repetition time 2 s). The same image-acquisition parameters were used for the task-based and resting-state data. Task-based fMRI data were acquired in six runs. One 8-min run of resting-state data was acquired for some participants. For the resting-state data collection, participants were instructed to relax and remain awake. All participants were blindfolded throughout the entire experiment.

fMRI Task-Based Data Analysis.

Data were analyzed using Freesurfer, FSL, HCP workbench, and custom in-house software (5153). Functional data were motion corrected, high-pass filtered (128 s), mapped to the cortical surface using Freesurfer, spatially smoothed on the surface (6-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel), and prewhitened to remove temporal autocorrelation.

Each type of math and language trial was entered as a separate predictor in a general linear model (GLM) after convolving with a canonical hemodynamic response function and its first temporal derivative. Trials in which participants failed to respond (Behavioral Paradigm) and trials with excessive motion [>1.5 mm; blind: 1.45 drops per run, SD = 1.32; sighted: 1.48 drops per run, SD = 3.06, t(34) = 0.03, P = 0.98] were excluded by modeling with a separate regressor.

Each run was modeled separately, and runs were combined within subject using a fixed-effects model. Random-effects analyses were used to analyze data within and across groups. Whole-cortex analyses were thresholded at a voxel-wise threshold of P < 0.01 and corrected for multiple comparisons at P < 0.05 using cluster-based permutation tests.

IPS and visual cortex (rMOG) ROIs were defined in individual subjects using the math > language contrast (orthogonal to the differences between math conditions). ROIs were defined using a leave-one-run-out procedure. For each participant, using all but one run, ROIs were defined as the top 5% of voxels within an IPS and visual cortex search-space with the highest math > language z value (search-space definition detailed in SI Experimental Procedures, Definition of ROI for Task-Based Analyses). For each ROI, we extracted percentage signal change (PSC) from 2 mm smoothed data during the stimulus portion of the trial (0.25–10 s after trial onset) and averaged PSC across voxels. PSC was computed relative to rest not including the 2 s following the offset of the previous trial. This process was repeated iteratively until every run was excluded from ROI definition. Therefore, ROIs were defined using independent data as well as using a contrast that is orthogonal to the conditions of interest.

Resting-State Functional Connectivity Analysis.

Resting-state data were analyzed using the standard procedures of the CONN 15.c Functional Connectivity Toolbox (54, 55) (detailed in SI Experimental Procedures, Definition of ROIs for Resting-State Correlation Analyses). BOLD data were smoothed 23 diffusion steps on the surface (corresponding to ∼6 mm of smoothing in the volume) (56), despiked, and bandpass filtered (0.008–0.1). White matter and cerebrospinal fluid BOLD signals were regressed out, and low-frequency drift was removed.

Anatomically defined left and right IPS ROIs were used as seeds in a whole-cortex analysis (57). The primary ROI-to-ROI analyses were conducted in the right hemisphere, where cross-modal responses were larger (left hemisphere analyses detailed in SI Experimental Procedures, Definition of ROIs for Resting-State Correlation Analyses). We defined three number-responsive ROIs: occipital rMOG (math > language, blind > sighted), parietal rIPS (math > language, blind and sighted groups’ averages), and prefrontal rPFC (math > language, blind and sighted groups’ averages); and two language-responsive ROIs: occipital rVOT (sentences > math, blind > sighted) and rIFC (sentences > math, blind and sighted groups’ averages) (58). Resting-state ROI definition detailed in SI Experimental Procedures, Definition of ROIs for Resting-State Correlation Analyses.

SI Experimental Procedures

Definition of ROI for Task-Based Analyses.

IPS and visual cortex (rMOG) ROIs were defined in individual subjects using the math > sentence contrast (orthogonal to the differences between math conditions). ROIs were defined in each participant as the top 5% of voxels with the highest math > sentences z value. Individual-subject ROIs were defined within search spaces created based on group data. Search space definition was orthogonal to the contrast of interest (differences between math conditions) and orthogonal to subject (see below).

The left and right IPS search spaces were defined using the sighted and congenitally blind groups’ average responses for the math > sentences contrast within the anatomical location of the IPS (P < 0.01, uncorrected) (57).

rMOG search spaces were defined using a leave-one-subject-out analysis (on 10 mm smoothed data). We iteratively excluded one subject and defined the rMOG search space, based on the remaining subjects, as the cluster within visual cortex that showed an interaction between the math > sentences contrast and blind > sighted contrast (P < 0.001, uncorrected). This procedure ensures that search space definition is orthogonal to subject—that is, a given subject did not contribute to the definition of his or her own search space. Before data extraction, the resulting search spaces of all sighted and blind participants were manually trimmed to ensure that they did not extend into the IPS and to avoid irregularly shaped search spaces.

Definition of ROIs for Resting-State Correlation Analyses.

We correlated activity in left and right IPS with the rest of the cortex in blind and sighted participants. Anatomically defined left and right IPS ROIs were used as seeds in the resting-state analyses (57). Time series were averaged over voxels in left and right IPS seeds and then correlated with voxels across the whole cortex. Results are reported for the left and right IPS seeds separately (Fig. 1, Lower and Fig. S6).

Five group ROIs were defined for the ROI-to-ROI resting-state correlation analyses in the right hemisphere: three math-responsive ROIs [rMOG (visual), rIPS (parietal), rPFC (frontal)] and two language-responsive ROIs [rVOT (visual) and rIFC (frontal)]. The IPS ROI was identical the IPS search spaces used in the functional ROI analysis (described above). A right math-responsive PFC ROI was defined for the blind and sighted group, separately, by taking a cluster in the right prefrontal cortex that responded to the math > language contrast in each group (blind threshold: P < 0.01, uncorrected; sighted threshold: P < 0.1, uncorrected). In visual cortex, the rMOG seed was defined by taking a cluster that responded to the math > language in all blind > sighted (P < 0.001, uncorrected; manually trimmed; see above). The language-responsive IFC seed was defined as a cluster within literature-defined language-responsive inferior frontal cortex that responded to language > math in the blind and sighted groups, separately (blind: P < 0.01, uncorrected; sighted P < 0.1, uncorrected). The rVOT seed was defined by taking a cluster in ventral occipitotemporal cortex that responded to the language > math contrast in all blind > sighted (P < 0.001, uncorrected; manually trimmed; see above).

A similar seed-to-seed resting-state analysis was performed in the left hemisphere using analogously defined seeds. In blind individuals, we find a dissociation in the functional connectivity patterns of left math and language visual areas with left math-responsive IPS and left language-responsive IFC. However, we did not find this same dissociation between left math and language visual areas and left math and language prefrontal areas.

SI Results

Behavioral Results.

Accuracy (percentage correct) and response time data were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and digit–number (single vs. double-digit) and algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex) as within-subject factors. Two-way interactions and main effects are reported in the main text. There were no three-way interactions among group, digit–number, and algebraic complexity [accuracy: group × digit–number × algebraic complexity interaction: F(1, 34) = 1.82, P = 0.19; response time: F(1, 34) = 1.11, P = 0.30].

Preserved Frontoparietal Responses to Number in Congenital Blindness.

Within-group (blind and sighted separately) results from the IPS ROI analysis were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with digit–number (single vs. double-digit), algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex), and hemisphere (left vs. right) as within-subject factors. The left and right IPS of congenitally blind adults responded more to trials with double-digit math equations than single-digit math equations [F(1, 16) = 36.70, P < 0.001; digit–number × hemisphere interaction: F(1, 16) = 0.02, P = 0.9] and more to algebraically complex equations than algebraically simpler equations [F(1, 16) = 8.13, P = 0.01; algebraic complexity × hemisphere interaction: F(1, 16) = 1.74, P = 0.21] (Fig. 2 and Fig. S3). A similar pattern was observed in the sighted group [main effect of digit–number: F(1, 18) = 13.85, P = 0.002; main effect of algebraic complexity: F(1, 18) = 11.05, P = 0.004] (Fig. 2 and Fig. S3).

Between-group results from the IPS ROI analysis were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and digit–number (single vs. double-digit), algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex), and hemisphere (left vs. right) as within-subject factors. Two-way interactions and main effects are reported in the main text. There was no digit–number × algebraic complexity interaction [F(1, 34) = 1.92, P = 0.18]. There was no three-way interaction among group, digit–number, and algebraic complexity [F(1, 34) = 0.00, P = 1.0].

Responses to Number in Visual Cortex of Blind Adults.

Within-group (blind and sighted separately) results from the rMOG ROI analysis were analyzed using a 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with digit–number (single vs. double-digit) and algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex) as within-subject factors. The rMOG of congenitally blind individuals was sensitive to algebraic complexity [marginal effect of algebraic complexity: F(1, 16) = 4.20, P = 0.06; digit–number × algebraic complexity interaction: F(1, 16) = 1.19, P = 0.30] (Fig. 2). By contrast, the rMOG of sighted individuals was not sensitive to algebraic complexity [main effect of algebraic complexity: F(1, 18) = 2.01, P = 0.17; digit–number × algebraic complexity interaction: F(1, 18) = 0.30, P = 0.59]. Within group effects of digit–number are reported in the main text.

Between-group results from the rMOG ROI analysis were analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 repeated-measures ANOVA with group (blind vs. sighted) as a between-subjects factor and digit–number (single vs. double-digit) and algebraic complexity (algebraically simple vs. complex) as within-subject factors. Two-way interactions and main effects are reported in the main text. There was no digit–number × algebraic complexity interaction [F(1, 34) = 1.0, P = 0.33]. There was no three-way interaction among group, digit–number, and algebraic complexity [F(1, 34) = 0.08, P = 0.78].

Acknowledgments

We thank the F. M. Kirby Research Center for Functional Brain Imaging at the Kennedy Krieger Institute for their assistance with data collection; Aloma Bouma for assisting with participant recruitment; and the Baltimore and Washington, DC, blind communities. This work was supported by Science of Learning Institute at Johns Hopkins University Award 80034917 and National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship DGE-1232825 (to S.K.).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1524982113/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Libertus ME, Brannon EM. Stable individual differences in number discrimination in infancy. Dev Sci. 2010;13(6):900–906. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00948.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tomonaga M. Relative numerosity discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Evidence for approximate numerical representations. Anim Cogn. 2008;11(1):43–57. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0089-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Flombaum JI, Junge JA, Hauser MD. Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers. Cognition. 2005;97(3):315–325. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McCrink K, Wynn K. Large-number addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants. Psychol Sci. 2004;15(11):776–781. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Feigenson L, Dehaene S, Spelke E. Core systems of number. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(7):307–314. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Piazza M, Pinel P, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. A magnitude code common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron. 2007;53(2):293–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Menon V, Rivera SM, White CD, Glover GH, Reiss AL. Dissociating prefrontal and parietal cortex activation during arithmetic processing. Neuroimage. 2000;12(4):357–365. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cohen Kadosh R, et al. Virtual dyscalculia induced by parietal-lobe TMS impairs automatic magnitude processing. Curr Biol. 2007;17(8):689–693. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Viswanathan P, Nieder A. Neuronal correlates of a visual “sense of number” in primate parietal and prefrontal cortices. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(27):11187–11192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1308141110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cantlon JF, Brannon EM, Carter EJ, Pelphrey KA. Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4-y-old children. PLoS Biol. 2006;4(5):e125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Burr D, Ross J. A visual sense of number. Curr Biol. 2008;18(6):425–428. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dakin SC, Tibber MS, Greenwood JA, Kingdom FAA, Morgan MJ. A common visual metric for approximate number and density. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(49):19552–19557. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113195108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Morgan MJ, Raphael S, Tibber MS, Dakin SC. A texture-processing model of the ‘visual sense of number’. Proc Biol Sci. 2014;281(1790):20141137. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6(6):435–448. doi: 10.1038/nrn1684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cohen Kadosh R, Walsh V. Numerical representation in the parietal lobes: Abstract or not abstract? Behav Brain Sci. 2009;32(3-4):313–328, discussion 328–373. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X09990938. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Culham JC, Kanwisher NG. Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2001;11(2):157–163. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00191-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Simon O, Mangin JF, Cohen L, Le Bihan D, Dehaene S. Topographical layout of hand, eye, calculation, and language-related areas in the human parietal lobe. Neuron. 2002;33(3):475–487. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00575-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Stoianov I, Zorzi M. Emergence of a ‘visual number sense’ in hierarchical generative models. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(2):194–196. doi: 10.1038/nn.2996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Eger E, Sterzer P, Russ MO, Giraud AL, Kleinschmidt A. A supramodal number representation in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron. 2003;37(4):719–725. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00036-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Piazza M, Mechelli A, Price CJ, Butterworth B. Exact and approximate judgements of visual and auditory numerosity: An fMRI study. Brain Res. 2006;1106(1):177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Izard V, Sann C, Spelke ES, Streri A. Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(25):10382–10385. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812142106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Watkins KE, et al. Early auditory processing in area V5/MT+ of the congenitally blind brain. J Neurosci. 2013;33(46):18242–18246. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2546-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Röder B, Stock O, Bien S, Neville H, Rösler F. Speech processing activates visual cortex in congenitally blind humans. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16(5):930–936. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02147.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sadato N, Okada T, Honda M, Yonekura Y. Critical period for cross-modal plasticity in blind humans: A functional MRI study. Neuroimage. 2002;16(2):389–400. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lane C, Kanjlia S, Omaki A, Bedny M. “Visual” cortex of congenitally blind adults responds to syntactic movement. J Neurosci. 2015;35(37):12859–12868. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1256-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Amedi A, Raz N, Pianka P, Malach R, Zohary E. Early ‘visual’ cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(7):758–766. doi: 10.1038/nn1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bedny M, Pascual-Leone A, Dodell-Feder D, Fedorenko E, Saxe R. Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(11):4429–4434. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014818108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cohen LG, et al. Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in blind humans. Nature. 1997;389(6647):180–183. doi: 10.1038/38278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Amedi A, Floel A, Knecht S, Zohary E, Cohen LG. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the occipital pole interferes with verbal processing in blind subjects. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(11):1266–1270. doi: 10.1038/nn1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chater N, Tenenbaum JB, Yuille A. Probabilistic models of cognition: Conceptual foundations. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(7):287–291. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Monti MM, Parsons LM, Osherson DN. Thought beyond language: Neural dissociation of algebra and natural language. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(8):914–922. doi: 10.1177/0956797612437427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Dehaene S, Piazza M, Pinel P, Cohen L. Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2003;20(3):487–506. doi: 10.1080/02643290244000239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cantlon JF, et al. The neural development of an abstract concept of number. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21(11):2217–2229. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lussier CA, Cantlon JF. Developmental bias for number words in the intraparietal sulcus. Dev Sci. 2016 doi: 10.1111/desc.12385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tibber MS, Greenwood JA, Dakin SC. Number and density discrimination rely on a common metric: Similar psychophysical effects of size, contrast, and divided attention. J Vis. 2012;12(6):8. doi: 10.1167/12.6.8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lourenco SF, Bonny JW, Fernandez EP, Rao S. Nonsymbolic number and cumulative area representations contribute shared and unique variance to symbolic math competence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(46):18737–18742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207212109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Reuhkala M. Mathematical skills in ninth-graders: Relationship with visuo-spatial abilities and working memory. Educ Psychol. 2001;21(4):387–399. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gallace A, Tan HZ, Spence C. Numerosity judgments for tactile stimuli distributed over the body surface. Perception. 2006;35(2):247–266. doi: 10.1068/p5380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.McAdams S. Segregation of concurrent sounds. I: Effects of frequency modulation coherence. J Acoust Soc Am. 1989;86(6):2148–2159. doi: 10.1121/1.398475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Marin CM, McAdams S. Segregation of concurrent sounds. II: Effects of spectral envelope tracing, frequency modulation coherence, and frequency modulation width. J Acoust Soc Am. 1991;89(1):341–351. doi: 10.1121/1.400469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Anobile G, Cicchini GM, Burr DC. Separate mechanisms for perception of numerosity and density. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(1):265–270. doi: 10.1177/0956797613501520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ferrand L, Riggs KJ, Castronovo J. Subitizing in congenitally blind adults. Psychon Bull Rev. 2010;17(6):840–845. doi: 10.3758/PBR.17.6.840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pascual-Leone A, Amedi A, Fregni F, Merabet LB. The plastic human brain cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:377–401. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Poirier C, et al. Specific activation of the V5 brain area by auditory motion processing: An fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25(3):650–658. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Collignon O, et al. Functional specialization for auditory-spatial processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(11):4435–4440. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1013928108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Martínez A, et al. Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(4):364–369. doi: 10.1038/7274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Grill-Spector K, et al. A sequence of object-processing stages revealed by fMRI in the human occipital lobe. Hum Brain Mapp. 1998;6(4):316–328. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:4&#x0003c;316::AID-HBM9&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Vinberg J, Grill-Spector K. Representation of shapes, edges, and surfaces across multiple cues in the human visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2008;99(3):1380–1393. doi: 10.1152/jn.01223.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Merabet L, et al. Feeling by sight or seeing by touch? Neuron. 2004;42(1):173–179. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00147-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Bedny M, Pascual-Leone A, Dravida S, Saxe R. A sensitive period for language in the visual cortex: Distinct patterns of plasticity in congenitally versus late blind adults. Brain Lang. 2012;122(3):162–170. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Glasser MF, et al. WU-Minn HCP Consortium The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Smith SM, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23(Suppl 1):S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: A functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2(3):125–141. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. Neuroimage. 2007;37(1):90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hagler DJ, Jr, Saygin AP, Sereno MI. Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data. Neuroimage. 2006;33(4):1093–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Destrieux C, Fischl B, Dale A, Halgren E. Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. Neuroimage. 2010;53(1):1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Fedorenko E, Hsieh P-J, Nieto-Castañón A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Kanwisher N. New method for fMRI investigations of language: Defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104(2):1177–1194. doi: 10.1152/jn.00032.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES