Abstract
Background
Traffic-related air pollution exposure may influence brain development and function and thus be related to neurobehavioral problems in children, but little is known about windows of susceptibility.
Aims
Examine associations of gestational and childhood exposure to traffic-related pollution with executive function and behavior problems in children.
Methods
We studied associations of pre- and postnatal pollution exposures with neurobehavioral outcomes in 1,212 children in the Project Viva pre-birth cohort followed to mid-childhood (median age 7.7 years). Parents and classroom teachers completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Using validated spatiotemporal models, we estimated exposure to black carbon (BC) and fine particulate matter (PM2.5) in the third trimester of pregnancy, from birth to 3 years, from birth to 6 years, and in the year before behavioral ratings. We also measured residential distance to major roadways and near-residence traffic density at birth and in mid-childhood. We estimated associations of BC, PM2.5, and other traffic exposure measures with BRIEF and SDQ scores, adjusted for potential confounders.
Results
Higher childhood BC exposure was associated with higher teacher-rated BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) scores, indicating greater problems: 1.0 points (95% confidence interval (CI): 0.0, 2.1) per interquartile range (IQR) increase in birth-age 6 BC, and 1.7 points (95% CI: 0.6, 2.8) for BC in the year prior to behavioral ratings. Mid-childhood residential traffic density was also associated with BRI score (0.6, 95% CI: 0.1, 1.1). Birth-age 3 BC was not associated with BRIEF or SDQ scores. Third trimester BC exposure was not associated with teacher-rated BRI scores (−0.2, 95% CI: −1.1, 0.8), and predicted lower scores (fewer problems) on the BRIEF Metacognition Index (−1.2, 95% CI: −2.2, −0.2) and SDQ total difficulties (−0.9, 95% CI: −1.4, −0.4). PM2.5 exposure was associated with teacher-rated BRIEF and SDQ scores in minimally adjusted models but associations attenuated with covariate adjustment. None of the parent-rated outcomes suggested adverse effects of greater pollution exposure at any time point.
Conclusions
Children with higher mid-childhood exposure to BC and greater near-residence traffic density in mid-childhood had greater problems with behavioral regulation as assessed by classroom teachers, but not as assessed by parents. Prenatal and early childhood exposure to traffic-related pollution did not predict greater executive function or behavior problems; third trimester BC was associated with lower scores (representing fewer problems) on measures of metacognition and behavioral problems.
Keywords: Traffic, air pollution, black carbon, executive function, neurodevelopment
1. Background
An accumulating body of evidence suggests that early life exposure to traffic-related air pollution can affect the developing brain (Block et al. 2012; Calderon-Garciduenas et al. 2014; Costa et al. 2014). The authors of a recent review identified polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAH), fine particulate matter with aerodynamic diameter ≤ 2.5 μm (PM2.5), and nitrogen dioxides as air pollutants potentially harmful to neuropsychological development in children (Suades-Gonzalez et al. 2015). Recent epidemiologic studies have examined whether air pollution exposure increases risk for adverse behavioral development including behaviors related to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Higher childhood exposure to particulate matter with aerodynamic diameter ≤ 10 μm (PM10, of which one component is PM2.5) was associated with increased prevalence of ADHD among school age children in India, where vehicular emissions are one important source of particulate pollution (Siddique et al. 2011). In a Boston cohort of 7-14 year olds, lifetime (since birth) childhood exposure to black carbon (BC), traffic-related particles commonly used as a marker of traffic pollution, was associated with worse performance on computerized assessments of attention (Chiu et al. 2013). Among Cincinnati children, higher exposure to traffic-related elemental carbon in the first year of life predicted greater hyperactive behaviors at age 7 (Newman et al. 2013). In China, 8-10 year old children from a more polluted city performed worse on assessments of attention, motor skills, and sensory function than children from a less polluted city (Wang et al. 2009). Seven-11 year old children in Barcelona with higher exposure to elemental carbon, black carbon and nitrogen dioxide at school displayed greater behavioral problems (Forns et al. 2015); children with higher traffic-related air pollution exposure also displayed slower improvements in attentiveness and working memory over a 12 month period (Sunyer et al. 2015). Prenatal exposure to PAH, air pollutants related to traffic and other forms of combustion, predicted increased attention problems and anxiety/depression behaviors at age 6-7 and greater ADHD-related behaviors at age 9 among children in New York City (Perera et al. 2012; Perera et al. 2014). While findings to date suggest a link between traffic-related pollution and neurobehavioral outcomes, additional research is needed to identify particular components of traffic-related pollution that are most influential, developmental windows that are most sensitive, and aspects of childhood neurobehavior that are most affected. To investigate these questions in a large prospective cohort followed from pregnancy onwards, we examined several measures of prenatal and childhood exposure to traffic-related pollution [BC and PM2.5 estimated at the residence level, residential proximity to major roadways, and near-residence traffic density] in relation to ratings of children’s executive function and behavior problems as reported by parents and classroom teachers.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1 Study population
Participants were drawn from Project Viva, a longitudinal pre-birth cohort of 2,128 mother and child pairs (Oken et al. 2015). Mothers were enrolled from April 1999 to November 2002 during initial prenatal visits at eight obstetrical offices of Atrius Harvard Vanguard Medical Associates, a multi-specialty group practice with locations in urban and suburban Eastern Massachusetts. To be eligible, mothers needed to be ≤22 weeks gestation at enrollment (median=9.9 weeks), able to communicate in English, report no plans to move out of the study area, and give birth to live singleton infants. Viva staff administered a range of health and developmental assessments at prenatal visits and periodic follow-up visits during infancy and childhood, and participants completed annual questionnaires regarding health-related behaviors. Mothers reported their residential address at enrollment and each follow-up encounter (at mid-pregnancy, delivery, six months, and annually from age 1 forward). At a Project Viva study visit in mid-childhood (median age 7.7 years, range 6.6– 10.9), mothers and classroom teachers reported behavior of child participants using validated questionnaires. The present study included participants with at least one behavioral rating scale and one measure of traffic-related pollution exposure (n=1,212). Depending on the analysis, final sample sizes ranged from 758 to 1,185. The Institutional Review Board of Harvard Pilgrim Health Care approved this analysis. All mothers provided written informed consent and children provided verbal assent at the mid-childhood visit.
2.2. Traffic-related pollutant exposures
We geocoded participants’ residential addresses using ArcGIS 10.1 with Street MapTM North America (ESRI, Redlands, CA), aerial photographs, and internet resources. Based on these addresses, and using daily estimates from validated spatiotemporal land use regression models, we assessed mean exposure of participants to BC and PM2.5 during four periods: the third trimester of gestation, the first three years of life, the first six years of life, and the year preceding behavioral ratings. The third trimester mean was selected a priori to represent prenatal exposure because third trimester exposure estimates were available for a larger number of participants than first or second trimester exposures; the third trimester is also the period of gestation when brain development is most rapid (Tau and Peterson 2010). 90 percent of mothers remained at the same residential address from enrollment through date of delivery.
Methodologies for the pollutant models have been described (Gryparis et al. 2007; Kloog et al. 2012; Zanobetti et al. 2014). Briefly, the BC model incorporated data on area land use, traffic density, meteorology, and daily BC measurements from a central monitoring site on the roof of the Harvard Countway Library of Medicine in Boston, and BC measurements from 148 permanent and temporary BC monitors operating in Eastern Massachusetts between January 1999 and August 2011 (Gryparis et al. 2007; Zanobetti et al. 2014). The PM2.5 model used satellite aerosol optical depth measurements at the 10 km × 10 km grid scale for the years 2000-2010 (from the Moderate Resolution Imaging Spectroradiometer aboard the Earth Observing System satellites), and also included daily ground-level measurements of PM2.5 from United States Environmental Protection Agency and Interagency Monitoring of Protected Visual Environments networks, along with data on area land use, meteorology, and locations of major roads and other sources of PM2.5 (Kloog et al. 2012). We estimated BC and PM2.5 exposures for all participants who lived in the areas covered by the models (Eastern Massachusetts for the BC model, New England for the PM2.5 model) for at least 90% of the days in a given exposure period.
2.3 Residential distance to major roadway and near-residence traffic density
We calculated residential distance to a major roadway and near-residence traffic density at two timepoints: child’s birth date (to represent prenatal and perinatal exposure) and date of mid-childhood behavioral ratings (to represent proximal exposure). Using ArcGIS, we measured the distance in meters from each geocoded residence to nearest major roadway (US Census feature class A1 or A2, denoting major federal and state roads likely to have significant volumes of traffic). Near-residence traffic density was defined as the length in kilometers of all roads within 100 meters of a residence, multiplied by the traffic counts for those roads (vehicles/day) [as in (Zeka et al. 2008)]. We accessed traffic count data from the Massachusetts Department of Transportation through the Office of Geographic Information (MassGIS).
2.4 Neurobehavioral outcomes
Parents and teachers were asked to complete two validated behavioral rating scales of child participants: the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). The BRIEF evaluates executive function, assessing behaviors including planning and organization, working memory, inhibition of inappropriate impulses, emotional control, and ability to re-evaluate and shift problem solving approaches and is validated and standardized for use in children aged 5-18 (Gioia et al. 2000). Trained Project Viva staff scored completed BRIEF questionnaires according to published guidelines to generate two index scores (Metacognition (MI) and Behavioral Regulation (BRI)), and one overall Global Executive Composite score (GEC), which combines the MI and BRI. The MI, BRI, and GEC scores were each standardized to mean=50, standard deviation (SD)=10 using published reference data; higher scores represent greater problems (Gioia et al. 2000). The SDQ assesses problem behaviors in four categories (hyperactivity, emotional problems, conduct problems, and peer problems) (Goodman 1997), and has good agreement with the Child Behavior Checklist (Goodman and Scott 1999; Stone et al. 2010). It is frequently used in research and clinical settings and is valid and reliable among children aged 4-16 (Vostanis 2006). SDQ questionnaires were scored by trained Project Viva staff, yielding sub-scores in each behavioral category and a measure of total behavioral difficulties (possible scores range from 0–40 with higher scores representing greater problems).
2.5 Covariates
We accessed information on participant demographics and health-related behaviors from Project Viva questionnaires and interviews. We collected data on children’s birth weight and date of birth from hospital medical records. At the mid-childhood study visits, we administered the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (KBIT-2) to mothers to determine maternal full scale IQ (Kaufman and Kaufman 2004). Mothers also completed the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment-Short Form (HOME-SF), a validated measure of emotional support and cognitive stimulation in the child's home (Frankenburg and Coons 1986). We analyzed maternal blood lead level using blood samples provided by mothers (n=630) during pregnancy. We also accessed available clinical blood lead measures from early childhood from medical records for a subset of child participants (n=434). Using data from the 2000 US Census and geocoded addresses, we recorded median household income for the census tract of residence at the time of behavioral ratings (United States Census Bureau, 2000).
2.6 Statistical analyses
We evaluated correlation among exposures, outcomes, and covariates by calculating Spearman rank correlation coefficients. We ran separate multivariate linear regression models to assess relationships of each exposure measure (BC, PM2.5, distance to major roadway, near-residence traffic density) with each outcome score (parent- and teacher-rated BRIEF GEC, BRIEF MI, BRIEF BRI and SDQ total difficulties) for each exposure window (third trimester, first three years of life, first six years of life, and year prior to behavioral ratings for BC and PM2.5, birth address and address at the time of behavioral ratings for distance to distance to major roadway and near-residence traffic density). We also conducted sensitivity analyses examining effect estimates for year-long exposure intervals between birth and age 6 for BC and PM2.5.
Although outcome scores had skewed distributions, model residuals were approximately normal. Sensitivity analyses employing standard errors robust to violations of normality and homoscedasticity assumptions confirmed the results of linear models.
Initial models were minimally adjusted for child sex and age at behavioral ratings. Primary models were adjusted for child sex and age at behavioral ratings, and a set of covariates hypothesized to be potential confounders based on prior knowledge. These covariates included: duration of any breastfeeding (months up to 12), maternal IQ, parity (0, 1, ≥2), age at enrollment (<25, 25-34, ≥35 years), marital/cohabitation status (yes/no), education (≥college graduate/<college graduate), race/ethnicity (black, white, Hispanic, Asian, other), smoking status (never, former, smoked during pregnancy), exposure to secondhand smoke during pregnancy (<1 hour/≥1 hour per week), and alcohol consumption during pregnancy (g/day), paternal education (≥college graduate/<college graduate), presence of a gas stove in household at infancy (yes/no), annual household income at time of behavioral ratings (<$40K, $40-70K, $70-150K, ≥$150K), median income for census tract of residence at the time of behavioral ratings, HOME-SF score, maternal blood lead level from pregnancy (μg/dL), and child blood lead level in early childhood (μg/dL). We additionally adjusted third trimester BC and PM2.5 models for seasonal trends (using sine and cosine functions of the date of the mid-childhood visit when behavioral ratings were completed) (Schwartz et al. 1991). Birth weight and gestational age were hypothesized to be potential causal intermediates between prenatal traffic exposure and neurobehavioral outcomes, so we did not adjust for these variables in primary models, but included gestational age (in weeks) and birth weight/gestational age z-score (Oken et al. 2003) as covariates in sensitivity analyses.
In initial analyses, BC, PM2.5 and near residence traffic density exposures were assessed linearly as continuous variables and scaled by the interquartile range (IQR) of each variable. Distance to major roadway was assessed as a three category variable (<50 m, 50– <200 m, and ≥200 m), reflecting previously observed patterns of spatial distributions for important traffic-related pollutants in relation to major roadways, with rapid decay as distance increases (Hart et al. 2009; Karner et al. 2010; Zhu et al. 2002). To assess the assumption of linearity for BC, PM, and near-residence traffic density models, we ran generalized additive models with cubic regression splines for continuous exposures (with three degrees of freedom), adjusted for the full set of primary covariates. Splines suggested consistent patterns of non-linearity in associations between childhood BC exposure and BRIEF and SDQ scores; based on visual inspection of observed patterns we re-ran models for BC exposures using two-section piecewise linear models for mean BC concentration with a knot at 0.5 μg/m3. Splines for PM and traffic density models did not suggest consistent non-linear dose-response patterns.
To reduce bias and improve model precision (through increased sample size), we imputed missing covariates using a chained equation multiple imputation model (PROC MI in SAS) including exposure and outcome variables, study covariates, and other potential predictors. We generated 50 imputed data sets including all Project Viva participants (n=2,128) (White et al. 2011). We combined the imputed data sets using PROC MIANALYZE (Rubin 2004). Final models included participants with imputed covariate data, but participants with missing measurements of exposure or outcome for a given exposure-outcome analysis were excluded from that analysis.
Prior investigators have suggested that sex (Chiu et al. 2013) and maternal education (Newman et al. 2013) may modify associations between air pollution exposure and behavioral outcomes. We assessed potential effect measure modification by child sex and maternal education level by re-running primary models for BC, PM2.5 and near-residence traffic density exposure with cross-product interaction terms for exposure by sex or exposure by maternal education (≥college graduate/<college graduate). We did not assess effect measure modification in major roadway proximity models because statistical power was limited by the low numbers of participants in most highly exposed category.
We performed analyses in SAS Version 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC) and R Version 3.1.3 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).
3. Results
3.1 Participant characteristics and traffic-related pollution exposures
Table 1 outlines characteristics of the 1,212 study participants, overall and by category of BC exposure (≤0.5μg/m3 vs. >0.5μg/m3) in the year prior to behavioral ratings. On average, participants with higher mid-childhood BC exposure had higher teacher-rated and slightly higher parent-rated SDQ and BRIEF scores (representing greater behavioral and executive function problems), lower birth weight and birth weight/gestational age z-score, lower maternal IQ, lower levels of maternal and paternal education, lower household income, higher rates of gas stove ownership (a potential source of indoor air pollution), and lower neighborhood median income. Mothers of participants with higher mid-childhood BC exposure were more likely to be black or Hispanic and less likely to be white. Correlations between parent and teacher ratings on the same instrument were moderate (Spearman r=0.35 for the BRIEF GEC, 0.44 for the SDQ), while intra-rater correlations of BRIEF GEC with SDQ scores were higher (Spearman r=0.78 for teachers, 0.69 for parents). Participants excluded due to missing exposure or outcome data (n=916, or 43% of the originally enrolled 2,128) had somewhat lower birth weight (3,429 versus 3,485 g), shorter duration of breastfeeding (4.2 versus 6.3 months), higher rates of maternal smoking in pregnancy (17 versus 10%) and somewhat lower levels of parental education and household income than included participants, and were more likely to be black or Hispanic and less likely to be white (Supplemental Table 1). Distributions of covariates in the original and the imputed data sets were very similar (Supplemental Table 1).
Table 1.
Characteristics of study participants overall and by childhood BC exposure
All participantsa (n=1,212) |
Participants with mean BC exposure in year prior to behavioral ratings ≤ 0.5 μg/m3 (n=614) |
Participants with mean BC exposure in year prior to behavioral ratings > 0.5 μg/m3 (n=395) |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
N | Mean ± SD or % |
|||
Behavioral Rating Scales | ||||
Teacher-rated BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) scoreb |
878 | 51.0 ± 10.5 | 49.5 ± 8.7 | 54.0 ± 12.5 |
Teacher-rated BRIEF Behavior Regulation Index (BRI) scoreb |
878 | 50.8 ± 10.1 | 49.1 ± 7.7 | 53.9 ± 12.7 |
Teacher-rated BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) scoreb |
878 | 51.1 ± 10.7 | 49.9 ± 9.5 | 53.6 ± 12.2 |
Teacher-rated SDQ Total Difficulties scorec | 901 | 6.4 ± 5.9 | 5.9 ± 5.3 | 7.5 ± 6.6 |
Parent-rated BRIEF GEC scoreb | 1,172 | 48.7 ± 9.2 | 48.5 ± 9.2 | 49.0 ± 9.1 |
Parent-rated BRIEF BRI scoreb | 1,172 | 48.2 ± 8.8 | 48.0 ± 8.8 | 48.6 ± 8.9 |
Parent-rated BRIEF MI scoreb | 1,172 | 48.4 ± 8.7 | 48.3 ± 8.8 | 48.7 ± 8.9 |
Parent-rated SDQ Total Difficulties scorec | 1,189 | 6.6 ± 4.8 | 6.2 ± 4.6 | 7.2 ± 4.9 |
Child characteristics | ||||
Age when teacher completed behavioral rating scales (years) |
903 | 8.1 ± 0.8 | 8.0 ± 0.8 | 8.1 ± 0.9 |
Age when parent completed behavioral rating scales (years) |
1,195 | 7.9 ± 0.8 | 7.9 ± 0.7 | 8.0 ± 0.9 |
Sex | 1,212 | |||
Female (%) | 608 | 50 | 51 | 50 |
Male (%) | 604 | 50 | 49 | 50 |
Gestational age (weeks) | 1,212 | 39.5 ± 1.8 | 39.6 ± 1.7 | 39.5 ± 1.9 |
Birth weight (grams) | 1,212 | 3,485 ± 555 | 3,532 ± 536 | 3,417 ± 581 |
Birth weight/gestational age z-score | 1,212 | 0.19 ± 0.96 | 0.27 ± 0.95 | 0.06 ± 0.98 |
Duration of breastfeeding (months up to 12) | 1,212 | 6.3 ± 4.8 | 6.2 ± 4.7 | 6.0 ± 4.9 |
Early childhood blood lead (μg/dL) | 1,212 | 2.3 ± 2.2 | 2.2 ± 1.9 | 2.5 ± 2.0 |
Maternal characteristics | ||||
Age at enrollment (years) | 1,212 | 32.2 ± 5.2 | 32.6 ± 4.6 | 31.3 ± 6.3 |
IQ (KBIT-2 composite) | 1,212 | 106.6 ± 15.7 | 108.5 ± 14.5 | 102.6 ± 16.3 |
Parity | 1,212 | |||
Nulliparous (%) | 584 | 48 | 46 | 45 |
1 (%) | 434 | 36 | 39 | 35 |
≥2 (%) | 194 | 16 | 15 | 20 |
Education | 1,212 | |||
College degree or beyond (%) | 836 | 69 | 75 | 55 |
Less than college degree (%) | 376 | 31 | 25 | 45 |
Race/ethnicity | 1,212 | |||
White (%) | 834 | 69 | 81 | 50 |
Black (%) | 182 | 15 | 8 | 28 |
Asian (%) | 66 | 5 | 6 | 4 |
Hispanic (%) | 76 | 6 | 4 | 11 |
Other (%) | 53 | 4 | 2 | 7 |
Alcohol consumption during pregnancy (g/day) | 1,212 | 0.18 ± 0.26 | 0.19 ± 0.25 | 0.16 ± 0.26 |
Smoking status | 1,212 | |||
Smoked during pregnancy (%) | 117 | 10 | 10 | 11 |
Former smoker (%) | 232 | 19 | 19 | 17 |
Never smoker (%) | 863 | 71 | 71 | 72 |
Exposure to secondhand smoke during pregnancy | 1,212 | |||
≥1 hour per week (%) | 207 | 17 | 15 | 22 |
<1 hour per week (%) | 1,005 | 83 | 85 | 78 |
Marital/cohabitation status | 1,212 | |||
Married or cohabitating (%) | 1,113 | 92 | 96 | 84 |
Not married or cohabitating (%) | 99 | 8 | 4 | 16 |
Blood lead in pregnancy (μg/dL) | 1,212 | 1.3 ± 0.8 | 1.2 ± 0.7 | 1.3 ± 0.9 |
Paternal characteristics | ||||
Education | 1,212 | |||
College degree or beyond (%) | 780 | 64 | 70 | 48 |
Less than college degree (%) | 431 | 36 | 30 | 52 |
Household/neighborhood characteristics | ||||
Household income at mid-childhood | 1,212 | |||
<$40K (%) | 145 | 12 | 7 | 21 |
$40-70K (%) | 172 | 14 | 10 | 19 |
$70-150K (%) | 552 | 46 | 49 | 39 |
>$150 K (%) | 343 | 28 | 34 | 20 |
HOME-SF scored | 1,212 | 18.3 ± 2.2 | 18.6 ± 2.1 | 17.9 ± 2.4 |
Gas stove in home at age 1 | 1,212 | |||
Yes (%) | 703 | 58 | 50 | 72 |
No (%) | 509 | 42 | 50 | 28 |
Census tract median annual household income, mid- childhood address ($) |
1,212 | 65,311 ± 25,205 | 73,580 ± 22,817 | 51,664 ± 19,978 |
Includes participants with multiply imputed covariates.
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) Index and Composite scores standardized to mean=50, standard deviation=10 with higher scores representing greater executive function problems. BRIEF Global Executive Composite score combines Metacognition Index and Behavior Regulation Index scores.
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) Total Difficulties scores have possible values of 0-40 with higher scores representing greater behavioral problems.
The HOME-SF, or Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (Short Form) assessment, used to measure emotional support and cognitive stimulation in the child's home; Scale: 0-22, with higher scores representing greater support.
Table 2 summarizes levels of exposure to traffic-related pollution. BC exposure in the year prior to behavioral ratings [mean (SD)=0.47 (0.14) μg/m3] was positively correlated with third trimester mean BC (Spearman r =0.58), birth-age 6 BC (0.88), and all exposure periods of PM2.5 exposure and near-residence traffic density. As expected, BC exposure also tended to be higher among participants living <50 m or 50 to <200m from a major roadway compared to ≥200 m from a major roadway. Excluded participants had similar levels of exposure to traffic-related pollution in the prenatal period (or at birth address) as included participants (Supplemental Table 1).
Table 2.
Traffic-related pollution exposures and relation to BC exposure in year prior to behavioral ratings
N | Mean ± SD | Correlation with mean BC exposure in year prior to behavioral ratings (Spearman r)a |
|
---|---|---|---|
Black carbon (BC) exposure | |||
Third trimester (μg/m3) | 1197 | 0.69 ± 0.23 | 0.58 |
Birth– age 3 (μg/m3) | 1069 | 0.61 ± 0.17 | 0.81 |
Birth– age 6 (μg/m3) | 992 | 0.56 ± 0.16 | 0.88 |
Year prior to behavioral ratings (μg/m3) | 1009 | 0.47 ± 0.14 | - |
Fine particulate (PM2.5) exposure | |||
Third trimester (μg/m3) | 1046 | 12.3 ± 2.5 | 0.32 |
Birth– age 3 (μg/m3) | 1082 | 12.0 ± 1.8 | 0.49 |
Birth– age 6 (μg/m3) | 1023 | 11.3 ± 1.7 | 0.54 |
Year prior to behavioral ratings (μg/m3) | 1084 | 9.4 ± 1.9 | 0.44 |
Near-residence traffic density | |||
Birth address (km*vehicles/day) | 1203 | 1,425 ± 1,813 | 0.40 |
Mid-childhood address (km*vehicles/day) | 1035 | 1,130 ± 1,598 | 0.65 |
N | N (%) |
BC exposure in year prior to
behavioral assessment exposure by category (μg/m3) (Mean ± SD ) |
|
Proximity to major roadway, birth address | 1207 | ||
<50 m | 34 (3%) | 0.53 ± 0.18 | |
50– <200 m | 108 (9%) | 0.52 ± 0.16 | |
≥200 m | 1065 (88%) | 0.46 ± 0.14 | |
Proximity to major roadway, mid-childhood address | 1198 | ||
<50 m | 27 (2%) | 0.56 ± 0.15 | |
50– <200 m | 93 (8%) | 0.56 ± 0.16 | |
≥200 m | 1078 (90%) | 0.46 ± 0.14 |
SD, standard deviation.
All Spearman correlation coefficient p-values <.0001
3.2 BC exposure and neurobehavioral scores
In minimally adjusted (child age and sex-adjusted) linear models, an IQR increase in BC exposure (0.20 μg/m3) in the year prior to behavioral ratings was associated with higher teacher-rated scores (indicating greater problems) on the BRIEF BRI (3.2 points, 95% confidence interval (CI): 2.3, 4.1), MI (2.1, 95% CI: 1.2, 3.1) and SDQ (1.1, 95% CI: 0.6, 1.7). In models adjusted for the full set of covariates, associations of BC in the year prior to behavioral ratings and teacher-rated scores were attenuated (BRI: 1.7 points per IQR increase, 95% CI: 0.6, 2.8; MI: 0.5, 95% CI: −0.6, 1.6; SDQ: 0.1, 95% CI: −0.5, 0.8). Patterns of association were similar for birth-age 6 BC exposure (Table 3). Birth-age 3 BC exposure was not associated with BRIEF or SDQ scores. In sensitivity analyses examining exposure during each year of life from birth to age 6, associations of BC exposure with teacher-rated BRIEF BRI scores were stronger in years 4, 5, and 6 than in years 1, 2, and 3 (see Supplemental Figure 2).
Table 3.
Linear model results for mean difference in teacher-rated executive function and behavior problems associated with residential major roadway proximity and interquartile range increases in exposure to traffic density, black carbon, and fine particulate matter (+ 95% confidence intervals) among 1,212 Project Viva children
Model 0 (adjusted for child age and sex)a | Model 1 (covariate-adjusted)b | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
|
Black carbon
(BC) exposure |
||||||||
Third trimester | 0.6 (−0.4, 1.5) | 1.2 (0.3, 2.2) | 0.2 (−0.8, 1.1) | −0.1 (−0.6, 0.4) | −1.0 (−1.9, 0.0) | −0.2 (−1.1, 0.8) | −1.2 (−2.2, −0.2) | −0.9 (−1.4, −0.4) |
Birth– age 3 | 1.9 (1.0, 2.8) | 2.3 (1.4, 3.2) | 1.5 (0.6, 2.5) | 0.7 (0.2, 1.2) | 0.0 (−1.0, 1.0) | 0.6 (−0.4, 1.5) | −0.3 (−1.4, 0.7) | −0.3 (−0.9, 0.2) |
Birth– age 6 | 2.3 (1.3, 3.2) | 2.7 (1.8, 3.7) | 1.8 (0.8, 2.8) | 0.9 (0.3, 1.4) | 0.4 (−0.7, 1.5) | 1.0 (0.0, 2.1) | 0.0 (−1.1, 1.2) | −0.1 (−0.7, 0.5) |
Year before behavioral ratings |
2.7 (1.8, 3.7) | 3.2 (2.3, 4.1) | 2.1 (1.2, 3.1) | 1.1 (0.6, 1.7) | 1.0 (−0.1, 2.1) | 1.7 (0.6, 2.8) | 0.5 (−0.6, 1.6) | 0.1 (−0.5, 0.8) |
Fine particulate
(PM2.5) exposure |
||||||||
Third trimester | 0.9 (−0.1, 2.0) | 1.3 (0.3, 2.4) | 0.7 (−0.4, 1.8) | 0.3 (−0.3, 0.9) | −0.1 (−1.2, 0.9) | 0.2 (−0.8, 1.3) | −0.3 (−1.4, 0.8) | −0.3 (−0.9, 0.3) |
Birth– age 3 | 1.7 (0.9, 2.6) | 1.8 (1.0, 2.6) | 1.5 (0.6, 2.4) | 0.8 (0.3, 1.3) | 0.3 (−0.6, 1.2) | 0.4 (−0.5, 1.3) | 0.2 (−0.7, 1.1) | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.6) |
Birth– age 6 | 1.9 (1.1, 2.8) | 2.1 (1.2, 2.9) | 1.6 (0.7, 2.5) | 0.9 (0.4, 1.4) | 0.5 (−0.5, 1.4) | 0.7 (−0.2, 1.6) | 0.3 (−0.7, 1.3) | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.6) |
Year before behavioral ratings |
1.3 (0.3, 2.2) | 1.3 (0.3, 2.2) | 1.1 (0.1, 2.1) | 0.6 (0.1, 1.2) | 0.2 (−0.8, 1.1) | 0.1 (−0.8, 1.1) | 0.1 (−0.9, 1.1) | 0.1 (−0.5, 0.6) |
Near-residence
traffic density |
||||||||
Birth address | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.6) | 0.4 (−0.1, 0.9) | 0.0 (−0.5, 0.5) | 0.1 (−0.2, 0.4) | −0.1 (−0.6, 0.4) | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.6) | −0.2 (−0.7, 0.3) | 0.0 (−0.3, 0.3) |
Mid-childhood address |
0.8 (0.3, 1.4) | 1.2 (0.7, 1.7) | 0.5 (0.0, 1.1) | 0.3 (0.0, 0.6) | 0.2 (−0.3, 0.8) | 0.6 (0.1, 1.1) | 0.0 (−0.5, 0.5) | 0.1 (−0.2, 0.3) |
Proximity to
major roadway, birth address |
||||||||
<50 m | 2.9 (−1.5, 7.4) | 3.8 (−0.7, 8.2) | 2.0 (−2.5, 6.6) | 1.1 (−1.4, 3.7) | 2.7 (−1.6, 7.1) | 3.5 (−0.7, 7.8) | 1.8 (−2.6, 6.3) | 1.5 (−1.0, 3.9) |
50– <200 m | −1.0 (−3.4, 1.4) | −0.1 (−2.5, 2.3) | −1.5 (−3.9, 1.0) | 0.2 (−1.2, 1.5) | −1.3 (−3.6, 0.9) | −0.6 (−2.8, 1.7) | −1.7 (−4.0, 0.7) | 0.0 (−1.3, 1.3) |
≥200 m | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) |
Proximity to
major roadway, mid-childhood address |
||||||||
<50 m | 3.5 (−1.0, 8.0) | 4.6 (0.1, 9.0) | 2.5 (−2.1, 7.1) | 1.3 (−1.2, 3.8) | 1.2 (−3.1, 5.5) | 2.0 (−2.2, 6.3) | 0.5 (−3.9, 5.0) | 0.1 (−2.4, 2.5) |
50– <200 m | 1.6 (−0.9, 4.0) | 2.0 (−0.4, 4.5) | 1.1 (−1.4, 3.6) | 0.9 (−0.5, 2.3) | 0.3 (−2.1, 2.6) | 0.5 (−1.8, 2.9) | 0.0 (−2.4, 2.5) | 0.1 (−1.3, 1.4) |
≥200 m | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) |
BRIEF is Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function; BRIEF scores standardized to mean=50, standard deviation=10 with higher scores representing greater executive function problems. SDQ is Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SDQ total difficulties scores have possible values of 0-40 with higher scores representing greater behavioral problems Interquartile range= 1,425 km*vehicles/day for traffic density at birth, 1,241 km*vehicles/day for mid-childhood traffic density, 0.33 μg/m3 for third trimester BC, 0.23 μg/m3 for birth-age 3 BC, 0.22 μg/m3 for birth-age 6 BC, 0.20 μg/m3 for BC in year before behavioral ratings, 3.8 μg/m3 for third trimester PM2.5, 2.2 μg/m3 for birth-age 3 PM2.5, 2.1 μg/m3 for birth-age 6 PM2.5, 2.5 μg/m3 for PM2.5 in year before behavioral ratings.
Model 0 adjusted for of child age at behavioral ratings and sex. Third trimester BC and PM2.5 models also adjusted for seasonal trends.
Model 1 adjusted for characteristics of child (age, sex, breastfeeding duration, early childhood blood lead), mother (age, parity, race/ethnicity, education, IQ, marital/cohabitation status, and blood lead, smoking, secondhand smoke exposure, and alcohol in pregnancy), father (education), household (income, home caretaking environment, gas stove) and neighborhood (census tract median income). Third trimester BC and PM2.5 models also adjusted for seasonal trends.
Higher third trimester BC was associated with higher teacher-rated BRI in minimally adjusted models (1.2 points per IQR increase, 95% CI: 0.3, 2.2) but showed no association with teacher-rated BRI scores and an association with lower (better) MI scores (−1.2, 95% CI: −2.2, −0.2) in covariate-adjusted models (Table 3).
Covariate-adjusted spline models suggested non-linear relationships between mean BC in the year prior to behavioral ratings and BRIEF and SDQ scores; visual inspection of plotted splines suggested inflection points at 0.4– 0.6 μg/m3 (see Supplemental Figures 1a-d) (patterns were generally similar for birth-age 6 BC). Covariate-adjusted piecewise linear regression models showed that among participants with year prior to behavioral ratings mean BC exposure above 0.5 μg/m3, a 0.2 μg/m3 increase in BC was associated with larger increases in BRIEF scores than seen in linear models: 4.8 points (95% CI: 1.7, 7.8) for the BRI and 2.9 points (95% CI: −0.3, 6.2) for the MI. Teacher-rated SDQ scores were also higher among these participants: 2.0 points per 0.2 μg/m3 BC (95% CI: 0.2, 3.7) (Table 4). Among participants with mean exposure in the year prior to behavioral ratings below 0.5 μg/m3, greater BC was not associated with teacher-rated BRIEF or SDQ scores. Higher third trimester, birth-age 3 and birth-age 6 BC exposure were associated with higher (worse) teacher-rated BRIEF scores among participants with BC exposures >0.5 μg/m3, but appeared associated with lower (better) scores among participants with BC exposures ≤0.5 μg/m3 (Table 4).
Table 4.
Piecewise linear model results for mean difference in teacher-rated Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) scores associated with 0.2 μg/m3 increase in exposure to black carbon (+ 95% confidence intervals) above and below mean exposure of 0.5 μg/m3 black carbon (BC)a
≤ 0.5 μg/m3 BC | > 0.5 μg/m3 BC | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
|
Third trimester BC |
−2.3 (−4.6, 0.1) | −1.5 (−3.8, 0.8) | −2.3 (−4.7, 0.2) | −1.1 (−2.4, 0.3) | 2.0 (−0.7, 4.7) | 1.7 (−1.0, 4.3) | 1.8 (−1.0, 4.6) | 0.6 (−0.9, 2.2) |
Birth– age 3 BC |
−1.4 (−3.8, 1.1) | −1.5 (−3.8, 0.9) | −0.9 (−3.5, 1.6) | −0.9 (−2.3, 0.5) | 1.8 (−1.3, 4.9) | 2.6 (−0.4, 5.6) | 0.9 (−2.3, 4.1) | 0.8 (−0.9, 2.6) |
Birth– age 6 BC |
−1.8 (−4.1, 0.5) | −1.7 (−4.0, 0.5) | −1.4 (−3.8, 0.9) | −0.9 (−2.2, 0.4) | 3.3 (0.2, 6.4) | 4.1 (1.1, 7.1) | 2.3 (−1.0, 5.5) | 1.2 (−0.6, 3.0) |
Year before behavioral ratings BC |
−0.9 (−2.8, 1.0) | −0.6 (−2.5, 1.2) | −0.9 (−2.9, 1.0) | −0.8 (−1.9, 0.3) | 3.9 (0.8, 7.1) | 4.8 (1.7, 7.8) | 2.9 (−0.3, 6.2) | 2.0 (0.2, 3.7) |
All models adjusted for characteristics of child (age, sex, breastfeeding duration, early childhood blood lead), mother (age, parity, race/ethnicity, education, IQ, marital/cohabitation status, and blood lead, smoking, secondhand smoke exposure, and alcohol in pregnancy), father (education), household (income, home caretaking environment, gas stove) and neighborhood (census tract median income). Third trimester BC and PM2.5 models also adjusted for seasonal trends.
In minimally adjusted linear models, BC exposures in the year prior to behavioral ratings or the birth-age 6 period appeared associated with slightly higher parent-rated BRIEF and SDQ scores, but were not associated with parent-rated behavioral scores in covariate-adjusted models (Table 5). In covariate-adjusted linear models, third trimester BC exposure appeared associated with slightly lower parent-rated BRIEF scores (representing better executive function) (Table 5). Covariate-adjusted piecewise linear models showed no association for third trimester, birth-age 3, or birth-age 6 BC exposure with parent-rated BRIEF scores in either BC exposure category, but suggested that an increase of 0.2 μg/m3 BC in the year prior to behavioral testing was associated with higher parent-rated BRIEF BRI (2.5 points; 95% CI: 0.0, 5.1) and GEC scores (2.7 points; 95% CI: 0.1, 5.3) among participants with mean exposure >0.5 μg/m3 and somewhat lower parent-rated BRIEF scores among participants with mean exposure ≤0.5 μg/m3 (Table 6).
Table 5.
Linear model results for mean difference in parent-rated executive function and behavior problems associated with residential major roadway proximity and interquartile range increases in exposure to traffic density, black carbon, and fine particulate matter (+ 95% confidence intervals)
Model 0 (adjusted for child age and sex)a | Model 1 (covariate-adjusted)b | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
|
Black carbon
(BC) exposure |
||||||||
Third trimester | −0.1 (−0.8, 0.7) | 0.2 (−0.5, 0.9) | −0.1 (−0.8, 0.7) | 0.4 (0.0, 0.7) | −0.9 (−1.6, −0.1) | −0.6 (−1.4, 0.1) | −0.8 (−1.5, 0.0) | −0.2 (−0.6, 0.2) |
Birth– age 3 | 0.4 (−0.3, 1.2) | 0.4 (−0.3, 1.2) | 0.4 (−0.3, 1.1) | 0.6 (0.2, 1.0) | −0.5 (−1.3, 0.4) | −0.5 (−1.3, 0.3) | −0.3 (−1.1, 0.5) | −0.1 (−0.5, 0.3) |
Birth– age 6 | 0.6 (−0.1, 1.4) | 0.7 (−0.1, 1.4) | 0.6 (−0.2, 1.3) | 0.7 (0.3, 1.1) | −0.3 (−1.2, 0.6) | −0.3 (−1.2, 0.6) | −0.1 (−1.0, 0.7) | 0.0 (−0.5, 0.4) |
Year before behavioral ratings |
0.7 (−0.1, 1.4) | 0.7 (−0.1, 1.4) | 0.6 (−0.2, 1.3) | 0.7 (0.3, 1.1) | −0.1 (−1.0, 0.8) | −0.2 (−1.1, 0.7) | 0.1 (−0.8, 0.9) | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.5) |
Fine
particulate (PM2.5) exposure |
||||||||
Third trimester | 0.0 (−0.8, 0.9) | 0.6 (−0.2, 1.4) | −0.2 (−1.0, 0.6) | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.5) | −0.5 (−1.3, 0.4) | 0.1 (−0.7, 1.0) | −0.7 (−1.5, 0.2) | −0.3 (−0.7, 0.1) |
Birth– age 3 | 0.6 (0.0, 1.3) | 0.8 (0.2, 1.5) | 0.4 (−0.2, 1.1) | 0.4 (0.1, 0.8) | 0.0 (−0.7, 0.7) | 0.2 (−0.5, 0.9) | −0.1 (−0.8, 0.6) | 0.0 (−0.4, 0.3) |
Birth– age 6 | 0.5 (−0.1, 1.2) | 0.7 (0.1, 1.4) | 0.4 (−0.3, 1.0) | 0.5 (0.1, 0.8) | −0.2 (−0.9, 0.5) | 0.0 (−0.7, 0.7) | −0.3 (−1.0, 0.5) | −0.1 (−0.5, 0.3) |
Year before behavioral ratings |
0.2 (−0.5, 1.0) | 0.4 (−0.3, 1.1) | 0.1 (−0.6, 0.8) | 0.3 (−0.1, 0.7) | −0.2 (−0.9, 0.6) | 0.0 (−0.8, 0.7) | −0.2 (−1.0, 0.5) | −0.1 (−0.5, 0.3) |
Near-residence
traffic density |
||||||||
Birth address | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.6) | 0.0 (−0.4, 0.4) | 0.1 (−0.2, 0.5) | 0.1 (−0.1, 0.4) | −0.1 (−0.5, 0.3) | −0.1 (−0.5, 0.3) | 0.0 (−0.4, 0.4) | 0.1 (−0.1, 0.3) |
Mid-childhood address |
0.2 (−0.2, 0.7) | 0.4 (0.0, 0.9) | 0.1 (−0.4, 0.5) | 0.3 (0.1, 0.6) | 0.0 (−0.5, 0.4) | 0.2 (−0.2, 0.6) | −0.2 (−0.6, 0.3) | 0.1 (−0.1, 0.3) |
Proximity to
major roadway, birth address |
||||||||
<50 m | −0.2 (−3.4, 3.0) | 0.3 (−2.8, 3.3) | 0.0 (−3.1, 3.0) | 0.9 (−0.7, 2.6) | 0.2 (−2.9, 3.4) | 0.4 (−2.6, 3.5) | 0.5 (−2.5, 3.5) | 0.9 (−0.6, 2.5) |
50– <200 m | 0.1 (−1.7, 1.9) | 1.2 (−0.5, 3.0) | −0.4 (−2.1, 1.3) | −0.1 (−1.1, 0.8) |
0.0 (−1.8, 1.8) | 1.1 (−0.7, 2.8) | −0.5 (−2.2, 1.2) | −0.3 (−1.2, 0.6) |
≥200 m | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) |
Proximity to
major roadway, mid- childhood address |
||||||||
<50 m | −2.5 (−6.1, 1.1) | −1.9 (−5.4, 1.6) | −2.4 (−5.9, 1.1) | 0.7 (−1.2, 2.6) | −3.6 (−7.1, −0.1) | −3.2 (−6.7, 0.2) | −3.1 (−6.5, 0.3) | −0.3 (−2.1, 1.5) |
50– <200 m | 0.6 (−1.4, 2.5) | 1.3 (−0.5, 3.2) | −0.2 (−2.0, 1.6) | 0.0 (−1.0, 1.0) | 0.2 (−1.7, 2.1) | 0.9 (−0.9, 2.8) | −0.5 (−2.3, 1.3) | −0.4 (−1.3, 0.6) |
≥200 m | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) | 0.0 (Ref) |
BRIEF is Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function; BRIEF scores standardized to mean=50, standard deviation=10 with higher scores representing greater executive function problems. SDQ is Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SDQ total difficulties scores have possible values of 0-40 with higher scores representing greater behavioral problems Interquartile range= 1,425 km*vehicles/day for traffic density at birth, 1,241 km*vehicles/day for mid-childhood traffic density, 0.33 μg/m3 for third trimester BC, 0.23 μg/m3 for birth-age 3 BC, 0.22 μg/m3 for birth-age 6 BC, 0.20 μg/m3 for BC in year before behavioral ratings, 3.8 μg/m3 for third trimester PM2.5, 2.2 μg/m3 for birth-age 3 PM2.5, 2.1 μg/m3 for birth-age 6 PM2.5, 2.5 μg/m3 for PM2.5 inyear before behavioralratings.
Model 0 adjusted for of child age at behavioral ratings and sex. Third trimester BC and PM2.5 models also adjusted for seasonal trends.
Model 1 adjusted for characteristics of child (age, sex, breastfeeding duration, early childhood blood lead), mother (age, parity, race/ethnicity, education, IQ, marital/cohabitation status, and blood lead, smoking, secondhand smoke exposure, and alcohol in pregnancy), father (education), household (income, home caretaking environment, gas stove) and neighborhood (census tract median income). Third trimester BC and PM2.5 models also adjusted for seasonal trends.
Table 6.
Piecewise linear model results for mean difference in parent-rated Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) scores associated with 0.2 μg/m3 increase in exposure to black carbon (+ 95% confidence intervals) above and below mean exposure of 0.5 μg/m3 black carbon (BC)a
≤ 0.5 μg/m3 BC | > 0.5 μg/m3 BC | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
BRIEF Global Executive Composite (GEC) |
BRIEF Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) |
BRIEF Metacognition Index (MI) |
SDQ Total Difficulties |
|
Third trimester BC |
−0.9 (−2.8, 1.0) | −0.5 (−2.4, 1.4) | −0.7 (−2.6, 1.1) | 0.4 (−0.6, 1.3) | 0.4 (−1.8, 2.7) | 0.1 (−2.0, 2.3) | 0.3 (−1.8, 2.4) | −0.6 (−1.7, 0.5) |
Birth– age 3 BC |
−0.5 (−2.5, 1.6) | −0.4 (−2.4, 1.6) | −0.1 (−2.1, 1.8) | 0.7 (−0.4, 1.7) | 0.1 (−2.5, 2.7) | −0.1 (−2.6, 2.5) | −0.1 (−2.6, 2.3) | −1.0 (−2.3, 0.3) |
Birth– age 6 BC |
−0.5 (−2.4, 1.4) | −0.5 (−2.4, 1.3) | −0.2 (−2.0, 1.7) | 0.5 (−0.4, 1.5) | 0.3 (−2.3, 2.9) | 0.4 (−2.2, 2.9) | 0.1 (−2.4, 2.6) | −0.8 (−2.1, 0.5) |
Year before behavioral ratings BC |
−1.4 (−2.9, 0.2) | −1.4 (−2.9, 0.1) | −1.0 (−2.5, 0.5) | −0.3 (−1.1, 0.5) | 2.7 (0.1, 5.3) | 2.5 (0.0, 5.1) | 2.2 (−0.3, 4.6) | 0.7 (−0.6, 2.0) |
All models adjusted for characteristics of child (age, sex, breastfeeding duration, early childhood blood lead), mother (age, parity, race/ethnicity, education, IQ, marital/cohabitation status, and blood lead, smoking, secondhand smoke exposure, and alcohol in pregnancy), father (education), household (income, home caretaking environment, gas stove) and neighborhood (census tract median income). Third trimester BC and PM2.5 models also adjusted for seasonal trends.
3.3 PM2.5 exposure and neurobehavioral scores
Mean PM2.5 exposures in the third trimester, birth-age 3 period, birth-age 6 period, and year prior to behavioral ratings were associated with higher teacher-rated BRIEF and SDQ scores in minimally adjusted models, but not in covariate-adjusted models (Table 3). PM2.5 exposures did not appear associated with parent-rated BRIEF scores in either model, with the exception of associations of higher mean birth-age 3 and birth-age 6 PM2.5 with higher parent-rated BRIEF BRI and SDQ scores in minimally adjusted models (Table 5).
3.4 Traffic exposure measures and neurobehavioral scores
Near-residence traffic density at birth address was not associated with teacher- or parent-rated BRIEF scores in minimally adjusted or covariate-adjusted models. Higher near-residence traffic density at mid-childhood address was associated with higher teacher-rated BRI and MI scores in minimally adjusted models, but associations attenuated with adjustment for covariates, and only BRI score remained associated with mid-childhood near-residence traffic density (Table 3). Higher mid-childhood near-residence traffic density was associated with slightly higher parent-rated BRI scores in minimally adjusted models, but not in covariate-adjusted models (Table 5).
Teacher-rated BRIEF scores were higher among participants living <50 m from a major roadway at birth compared to those living ≥200 m in both minimally adjusted and covariate-adjusted models, but confidence intervals for estimates were wide (owing to the small number of participants in the <50 m category; n=34) and contained the null value (Table 3). In minimally adjusted models, there was also a suggestion of higher teacher-rated BRIEF scores among children living close to major roadways in mid-childhood, but associations attenuated following adjustment for covariates (Table 3). Parent-rated BRIEF scores were not associated with roadway proximity at birth, but scores were lower (suggesting better executive function) among children living <50 m from a major roadway in mid-childhood compared to those living ≥200 m in covariate-adjusted models (Table 5).
3.6 Sensitivity analyses
Models additionally adjusted for birth weight and birth weight for gestational age z-score yielded results very similar to primary covariate-adjusted models (not shown). There were no consistent patterns of effect measure modification by child sex or maternal education level in observed associations (not shown).
4. Discussion
In our study cohort of Eastern Massachusetts children, higher average BC exposure from birth through age 6 and in the year proximal to behavioral ratings predicted greater problems with behavioral regulation (a component of executive function involving inhibitory control of emotion and impulses) as assessed by classroom teachers in mid-childhood (median age 7.7) using the BRIEF questionnaire. BC exposures in the birth-age 3 period and the third trimester of pregnancy were not associated with worse teacher-rated BRIEF scores, and BC exposure in the fourth, fifth, and sixth years of life appeared more strongly associated with teacher-rated BRIEF BRI scores than exposure in the first, second, and third years, suggesting that mid-childhood exposure may have a greater adverse effect than prenatal or early childhood exposure on executive function at ages 6-10. These results may also indicate that the observed associations are related to cumulative BC exposure throughout early life; high correlations of BC exposure in the year prior to behavioral ratings with birth-age 3 BC exposure (Spearman r=0.81) and birth-age 6 BC exposure (Spearman r=0.88) limit our ability to fully isolate the effects of exposure in later versus earlier childhood.
Childhood BC exposure did not appear associated with the Metacognition Index of the BRIEF (which measures “higher level” executive functions related to planning and problem solving) (Gioia et al. 2002), indicating that the BRIEF Behavior Regulation Index may be a more sensitive measure of the neurodevelopmental effects of childhood exposure to traffic-related pollution than the Metacognition Index among children in the studied age range (6-10 years).
The observed results are consistent with findings in another Boston cohort (aged 7-14 years) that childhood BC exposure (average since birth) was associated with errors of commission on the Connors’ Continuous Performance Test, suggesting poorer inhibitory control; BC exposure also predicted slower reaction time in that cohort (Chiu et al. 2013). Our findings are also broadly similar to those of previous studies that have observed greater neurobehavioral problems among children with higher childhood exposure to traffic-related pollution, although direct comparability is limited because these studies employed different outcome assessments and exposure measures (Forns et al. 2015; Sunyer et al. 2015; Newman et al. 2013; Siddique et al. 2011; Wang et al. 2009). While earlier studies in New York City reported associations between prenatal PAH exposure and neurobehavioral problems (anxiety/depression behaviors at ages 6-7 and greater ADHD-related behaviors at age 9) (Perera et al. 2012; Perera et al. 2014), prenatal exposure to BC in our cohort did not predict worse outcomes (in fact, teacher-rated metacognition was somewhat better among children with higher prenatal BC exposure, although this is an association that we do not believe has a plausible casual explanation). In general, differences between our findings and those from New York City may relate to differences in sources and exposure levels of pollution in New York versus Boston, differences in mechanism of effect of PAH versus BC, differences in the precision of exposure estimates, or differences in susceptibility of the study populations.
Multiple mechanisms may underlie the associations we observed between childhood exposure to traffic-related air pollution and behavior regulation problems. Systemic inflammatory responses or ultrafine particles that reach the brain may trigger neuroinflammation leading to brain damage through oxidative stress; pollution components including metals, solvents, and PAHs may also be directly neurotoxic (Block et al. 2012). In contrast to some brain regions that are more highly developed at birth, the prefrontal cortex, a region thought to be important in facilitating behavioral regulation through inhibitory control, undergoes substantial and critical development throughout childhood and adolescence (Casey et al. 2000; Tsujimoto 2008).
Evidence suggests that the prefrontal cortex may be a particularly sensitive target for neurological effects of air pollution exposure. Children exposed to high levels of urban air pollution in Mexico City had white matter lesions in the prefrontal cortex that were not seen in Mexican children with lower air pollution exposure (Calderon-Garciduenas et al. 2008). Dogs exposed to Mexico City air pollution displayed similar white matter lesions, along with increased neuroinflammation in frontal white matter compared to control dogs, and deposits of ultrafine particles in frontal blood vessels (Calderon-Garciduenas et al. 2008). Higher PAH exposure at age 5 in New York City children was associated with reduced surface white matter in the dorsal prefrontal regions at mean age 8 (while prenatal PAH exposure was independently associated with reductions in white matter surfaces throughout the left hemisphere of the brain) (Peterson et al. 2015). Among 8-12 year old children in Barcelona, childhood exposure to traffic-related air pollution was associated with differences in functional connectivity of neural networks involving the medial frontal cortex; these patterns of functional connectivity were opposite to those associated with increased age and faster reaction time, suggesting that childhood exposure to air pollution was associated with slower brain maturation (Pujol et al. 2016). Adult volunteers given experimental short-term exposures to diesel exhaust displayed changes in frontal cortex brain activity, suggesting a stress response in that region of the brain (Cruts et al. 2008). Mice experimentally exposed to ultrafine particles (aerodynamic diameter <100 nm) at environmentally-relevant concentrations in the postnatal period showed greater preference for immediate reward, which may be an indicator of impulsivity (Allen et al. 2013); exposed mice were also observed to have alterations in neurotransmitter levels, including increased glutamate in the frontal cortex, which investigators postulated could indicate an excitotoxic mechanism of particulate air pollution in the brain (Allen et al. 2014).
In contrast to our findings for teacher-rated BRIEF scores, in linear models parent-rated BRIEF scores were not associated with childhood BC exposure and third trimester BC exposure predicted slightly better parent-rated BRIEF GEC scores. Parent and teacher ratings assess behaviors in different environments. The discrepancy between results for parent- and teacher-rated questionnaires may indicate that subtle, sub-clinical executive functioning problems may be more apparent in a school setting rather than at home. We observed only moderate inter-rater correlation between parent and teacher rating scale scores in our study population, which is consistent with patterns observed by other researchers and in normative population samples (Gioia et al. 2000; Mares et al. 2007).
None of the traffic-related exposures we examined predicted worse parent- or teacher-rated SDQ total difficulties scores in covariate-adjusted linear models. The SDQ is a relatively short questionnaire, and while it has been shown to have good validity as a primary screening instrument for childhood psychosocial disorders (Stone et al. 2010), its usefulness in assessing subtle effects in community populations has been less well-studied.
Our data suggested that associations between childhood BC exposure and teacher- and parent-rated BRIEF and SDQ scores were not linear. Below a mean exposure of 0.5 μg/m3, higher BC exposure in the year prior to behavioral ratings appeared to have no effect (or a slightly beneficial effect) on parent- and teacher-rated BRIEF and SDQ scores, whereas above 0.5 μg/m3, higher exposure generally predicted worse scores. This observed non-linearity may suggest a concentration threshold around 0.5 μg/m3 below which childhood exposure to BC has less harmful influence on child executive function and behavior. It is also possible that non-linearity could stem from residual confounding or effect modification by factors that differ between participants with higher and lower BC exposure.
In our study population, BC exposure was higher, on average, among black and Hispanic participants compared to white and Asian participants. Participants with higher exposure to BC tended to have lower household income and live in neighborhoods with lower median annual household income. These findings are consistent with prior evidence that exposures to ambient traffic-related air pollution in urban areas are higher among racial and ethnic minority populations and in lower income communities (Hajat et al. 2013; Jerrett 2009; Jones et al. 2014). We adjusted for potential confounding by race, household income, neighborhood income, and other factors in our primary models, but influences of traffic-related pollutants and socioeconomic factors may be challenging to fully differentiate in this population, and it is possible that residual confounding may fully or partially explain the associations we observed.
Through correlated with BC exposure, childhood PM2.5 exposure was a weaker predictor of teacher-rated BRIEF scores than childhood BC exposure. Although PM2.5 is emitted by vehicles, it also has many non-traffic sources, meaning PM2.5 is considered a less specific marker of traffic-related pollution than BC (Karner et al. 2010). Our results have similarities to those of a study of Barcelona school-children, in which total PM2.5 measured in classrooms was not associated with measures of cognitive development but higher exposure to the portion of PM2.5 mass attributable to traffic (determined through source apportionment) was associated with slower improvements in attentiveness and working memory over the study period (Basagaña et al. 2016). In our study, near-residence traffic density at mid-childhood address, which we employed as a proxy measure of childhood exposure to traffic-related pollution, was also associated with a smaller increase in teacher-rated BRIEF scores than childhood BC exposure, suggesting that BC may be a relatively more harmful component of the mix of pollutants related to traffic.
We observed suggestive evidence that closer proximity to major roadways at mid-childhood might be associated with worse teacher-rated BRIEF BRI scores. Teachers also rated children living within 50 meters of a major roadway at birth as having more executive function and behavior problems on average (higher BRIEF and SDQ scores), but the small number of children in this category (n=34) limited the precision of effect estimates. This pattern reflects earlier findings in the Project Viva cohort suggesting that children with birth addresses <50 meters from a major roadway had lower non-verbal intelligence, verbal intelligence and visual motor abilities in mid-childhood (although precision was also limited in that study) (Harris et al. 2015). In contrast, parent-rated BRIEF scores were lower, indicating fewer problems, for children living <50 meters from a major roadway at birth. It seems unlikely that this association is causal; it may be attributable to confounding we were unable to account for in our analyses, or may be a chance association.
Additional limitations of our analyses should be noted. We assessed executive function and behavior based on validated rating scales completed by teachers and parents, but did not have access to performance-based tests of specific neurobehavioral functions such as attention or inhibitory control. Rating scales and neurobehavioral tests are complementary measures ideally used in combination to assess neurobehavioral development; while rating scales have the advantage of evaluating behavior in everyday life, they may assess different underlying mental constructs than performance-based measures (Toplak et al. 2013). Additional measures might have allowed us to identify effects of air pollution exposure on childhood neurobehavior that could not be identified through observer responses to behavioral rating scales. In addition, because executive function develops throughout childhood and adolescence (Anderson 2002), the behavioral rating scales conducted in the study population at ages 6-10 may not have captured potential effects of air pollution exposure on aspects of executive function that are better solidified later in development (or those relevant primarily at younger ages). The association of air pollution exposure with neurobehavioral development should be further explored in studies employing combined batteries of multi-informant rating scales and performance-based neuropsychological tests conducted longitudinally at multiple points in childhood and adolescence.
Although our primary models were adjusted for maternal IQ, we lacked other measures of parental neurobehavior (e.g., executive function). Exposure estimates were based on residential addresses and time-activity data were not available, so exposure measures did not incorporate exposure outside the home and were therefore certainly misclassified to some degree (Lane et al. 2013). Median census tract household income, which we used as a covariate representing neighborhood socioeconomic characteristics, was drawn from year 2000 census data, so this variable may have been misclassified if neighborhood characteristics changed substantially between the 2000 census and the mid-childhood study visits (conducted in 2007-2011). Primarily as a result of loss to follow-up soon after birth in Project Viva, our study included only a subset (57%) of participants originally enrolled in early pregnancy. While prenatal traffic-related pollution exposures appeared similar for excluded and included participants, some demographic characteristics and health-related behaviors differed between excluded and included participants, so selection bias is a possibility. Lastly, although the study cohort included participants with a range of racial/ethnic backgrounds and income levels, enrolled mothers all had health insurance coverage and access to early prenatal care and were on average highly educated and higher income, which may limit the applicability of findings to less advantaged populations.
Our study also had a number of strengths. We assessed multiple measures of traffic-related pollution exposure and multiple developmental windows in an effort to investigate influential pollution components and sensitive exposure periods in relation to childhood executive function and behavior. We used multiple behavioral rating scales completed by informants in multiple settings (classroom teachers and parents). We were able to examine and adjust for a broad range of potential confounding variables, including co-exposures previously observed to influence neurodevelopment and multiple measures of household- and area-level socioeconomic status.
Our results add to a growing literature suggesting that exposure to traffic-related pollution may contribute to neurobehavioral problems in children. Given the ubiquity of traffic exposure in urban and suburban areas, potential adverse impacts to neurodevelopment represent a serious public health concern, and interventions to limit exposure of children to traffic-related pollution are warranted.
5. Conclusions
Children with higher childhood exposure to BC had greater problems with behavioral regulation as assessed by classroom teachers (but not as assessed by parents), and evidence suggested that mid-childhood might be a more sensitive exposure period than early childhood or gestation for negative effects of BC exposure on executive function measured at 6-10 years. We observed evidence of non-linearity in relationships between childhood BC exposure and rating scales of executive function, which might indicate a potential threshold for adverse neurodevelopmental effects around 0.5 μg/m3 ambient BC concentration. Childhood BC exposure appeared to be more strongly predictive of teacher-rated executive function problems than PM2.5 exposure, near-residence traffic density, or residential proximity to major roadway. Our results suggest that childhood exposure to traffic-related pollution may adversely affect executive function development. Together with prior literature reporting adverse neurodevelopmental effects of traffic exposure, these findings provide support for public health interventions to reduce exposure to traffic-related pollution among children.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
-
-
We studied early life exposure to traffic-related air pollution and child behavior.
-
-
Exposure to ambient black carbon predicted worse behavioral regulation ratings.
-
-
Childhood black carbon exposure appeared more influential than prenatal exposure.
-
-
Other traffic exposure measures were less strongly associated with behavioral scores.
Acknowledgments
We thank the participants and staff of Project Viva. The authors have received support from the National Institutes of Health (R01AI102960, R01HD034568, P01ES009825, P30ES000002, R37HD 034568, K24HD069408, T32ES014562) and the US Environmental Protection Agency (US EPA) (RD83479801, RD83241601). Sponsors played no role in study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation, manuscript preparation, or the decision to submit the article for publication.
This publication’s contents are solely the responsibility of the grantee and do not necessarily represent the official views of the US EPA. Further, US EPA does not endorse the purchase of any commercial products or services mentioned in the publication.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Allen JL, Conrad K, Oberdorster G, Johnston CJ, Sleezer B, Cory-Slechta DA. Developmental exposure to concentrated ambient particles and preference for immediate reward in mice. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2013;121(1):32–38. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1205505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen JL, Liu X, Weston D, Prince L, Oberdorster G, Finkelstein JN, et al. Developmental exposure to concentrated ambient ultrafine particulate matter air pollution in mice results in persistent and sex-dependent behavioral neurotoxicity and glial activation. Toxicological Sciences: an official journal of the Society of Toxicology. 2014;140(1):160–178. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfu059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology. 2002;8:71–82. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basagana X, Esnaola M, Rivas I, Amato F, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Forns J, et al. Neurodevelopmental deceleration by urban fine particles from different emission sources: A longitudinal observational study. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2016;124(5) doi: 10.1289/EHP209. [Online 29 April 2016] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Block ML, Elder A, Auten RL, Bilbo SD, Chen H, Chen JC, et al. The outdoor air pollution and brain health workshop. Neurotoxicology. 2012;33(5):972–984. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2012.08.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calderon-Garciduenas L, Mora-Tiscareno A, Ontiveros E, Gomez-Garza G, Barragan-Mejia G, Broadway J, et al. Air pollution, cognitive deficits and brain abnormalities: A pilot study with children and dogs. Brain and Cognition. 2008;68(2):117–127. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calderon-Garciduenas L, Torres-Jardon R, Kulesza RJ, Park SB, D'Angiulli A. Air pollution and detrimental effects on children's brain. The need for a multidisciplinary approach to the issue complexity and challenges. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:613. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Thomas KM. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology. 2000;54(1-3):241–257. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00058-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiu YH, Bellinger DC, Coull BA, Anderson S, Barber R, Wright RO, et al. Associations between traffic-related black carbon exposure and attention in a prospective birth cohort of urban children. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2013;121(7):859–864. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1205940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa LG, Cole TB, Coburn J, Chang YC, Dao K, Roque P. Neurotoxicants are in the air: Convergence of human, animal, and in vitro studies on the effects of air pollution on the brain. BioMed Research International. 2014;2014:736385. doi: 10.1155/2014/736385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cruts B, van Etten L, Tornqvist H, Blomberg A, Sandstrom T, Mills NL, et al. Exposure to diesel exhaust induces changes in EEG in human volunteers. Particle and Fibre Toxicology. 2008;5:4. doi: 10.1186/1743-8977-5-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forns J, Dadvand P, Foraster M, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Rivas I, Lopez-Vicente M, et al. Traffic-related air pollution, noise at school, and behavioral problems in Barcelona schoolchildren: A cross-sectional study. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2015 doi: 10.1289/ehp.1409449. [Online 4 August 2015] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frankenburg WK, Coons CE. Home screening questionnaire: Its validity in assessing home environment. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1986;108(4):624–626. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(86)80853-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia G, Isquith P, Guy S, Kenworthy L. Behavioral rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; Lutz, FL: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Retzlaff PD, Espy KA. Confirmatory factor analysis of the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychology : a Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence. 2002;8(4):249–257. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 1997;38(5):581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman R, Scott S. Comparing the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire and the Child Behavior Checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1999;27(1):17–24. doi: 10.1023/a:1022658222914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gryparis AC, Coull BA, Schwartz J, Suh HH. Semiparametric latent variable regression models for spatio-teomporal modeling of mobile source particles in the greater Boston area. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series C (Applied Statistics) 2007;56(2):183–209. [Google Scholar]
- Hajat A, Diez-Roux AV, Adar SD, Auchincloss AH, Lovasi GS, O'Neill MS, et al. Air pollution and individual and neighborhood socioeconomic status: Evidence from the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) Environmental Health Perspectives. 2013;121(11-12):1325–1333. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1206337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris MH, Gold DR, Rifas-Shiman SL, Melly SJ, Zanobetti A, Coull BA, et al. Prenatal and childhood traffic-related pollution exposure and childhood cognition in the Project Viva cohort (Massachusetts, USA) Environmental Health Perspectives. 2015;123(10):1072–1078. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1408803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart JE, Laden F, Puett RC, Costenbader KH, Karlson EW. Exposure to traffic pollution and increased risk of rheumatoid arthritis. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2009;117(7):1065–1069. doi: 10.1289/ehp.0800503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jerrett M. Global geographies of injustice in traffic-related air pollution exposure. Epidemiology. 2009;20(2):231–233. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31819776a1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones MR, Diez-Roux AV, Hajat A, Kershaw KN, O'Neill MS, Guallar E, et al. Race/ethnicity, residential segregation, and exposure to ambient air pollution: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104(11):2130–2137. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karner AA, Eisinger DS, Niemeier DA. Near-roadway air quality: Synthesizing the findings from real-world data. Environmental Science & Technology. 2010;44(14):5334–5344. doi: 10.1021/es100008x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman A, Kaufman N. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition (KBIT-2) 2004.
- Kloog I, Nordio F, Coull BA, Schwartz J. Incorporating local land use regression and satellite aerosol optical depth in a hybrid model of spatiotemporal PM(2.5) exposures in the mid-atlantic states. Environmental Science & Technology. 2012;46(21):11913–11921. doi: 10.1021/es302673e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane KJ, Kangsen Scammell M, Levy JI, Fuller CH, Parambi R, Zamore W, et al. Positional error and time-activity patterns in near-highway proximity studies: an exposure misclassification analysis. Environmental Health. 2013;12(1):75. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-12-75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mares D, McLuckie A, Schwartz M, Saini M. Executive function impairments in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Do they differ between school and home environments? Canadian Journal of Psychiatry/Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie. 2007;52(8):527–534. doi: 10.1177/070674370705200811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman NC, Ryan P, Lemasters G, Levin L, Bernstein D, Hershey GK, et al. Traffic-related air pollution exposure in the first year of life and behavioral scores at 7 years of age. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2013;121(6):731–736. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1205555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oken E, Kleinman KP, Rich-Edwards J, Gillman MW. A nearly continuous measure of birth weight for gestational age using a United Atates national reference. BMC Pediatrics. 2003;3:6. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-3-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oken E, Baccarelli AA, Gold DR, Kleinman KP, Litonjua AA, De Meo D, et al. Cohort profile: Project Viva. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2015;44(1):37–48. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perera FP, Tang D, Wang S, Vishnevetsky J, Zhang B, Diaz D, et al. Prenatal polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon (PAH) exposure and child behavior at age 6-7 years. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2012;120(6):921–926. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1104315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perera FP, Chang HW, Tang D, Roen EL, Herbstman J, Margolis A, et al. Early-life exposure to polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and ADHD behavior problems. PloS one. 2014;9(11):e111670. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson BS, Rauh VA, Bansal R, Hao X, Toth Z, Nati G, et al. Effects of prenatal exposure to air pollutants (polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons) on the development of brain white matter, cognition, and behavior in later childhood. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(6):531–40. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pujol J, Martinez-Vilavella G, Macia D, Fenoll R, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Rivas I, et al. Traffic pollution exposure is associated with altered brain connectivity in school children. Neuroimage. 2016;129:175–184. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys. Wiley-Interscience; Hoboken, N.J.: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz J, Spix C, Wichmann HE, Malin E. Air pollution and acute respiratory illness in five German communities. Environmental Research. 1991;56(1):1–14. doi: 10.1016/s0013-9351(05)80104-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siddique S, Banerjee M, Ray MR, Lahiri T. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children chronically exposed to high level of vehicular pollution. European Journal of Pediatrics. 2011;170(7):923–929. doi: 10.1007/s00431-010-1379-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone LL, Otten R, Engels RC, Vermulst AA, Janssens JM. Psychometric properties of the parent and teacher versions of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire for 4- to 12-year-olds: A review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2010;13(3):254–274. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0071-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suades-Gonzalez E, Gascon M, Guxens M, Sunyer J. Air pollution and neuropsychological development: A review of the latest evidence. Endocrinology. 2015;156:3473–3482. doi: 10.1210/en.2015-1403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sunyer J, Esnaola M, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Forns J, Rivas I, López-Vicente M, et al. Association between traffic-related air pollution in schools and cognitive development in primary school children: a prospective cohort study. PLOS Med. 2015;12(3):e1001792. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tau GZ, Peterson BS. Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(1):147–168. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toplak ME, West RF, Stanovich KE. Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2013;54(2):131–143. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsujimoto S. The prefrontal cortex: Functional neural development during early childhood. The Neuroscientist. 2008;14(4):345–358. doi: 10.1177/1073858408316002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- United States Census Bureau [Accessed 18 September 2013];US Census 2000: Summary File 3. 2000 Available: http://www.census.gov/census2000/sumfile3.html.
- Vostanis P. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: Research and clinical applications. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2006;19:367–372. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000228755.72366.05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang S, Zhang J, Zeng X, Zeng Y, Wang S, Chen S. Association of traffic-related air pollution with children's neurobehavioral functions in Quanzhou, China. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2009;117(10):1612–1618. doi: 10.1289/ehp.0800023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Statistics in Medicine. 2011;30(4):377–399. doi: 10.1002/sim.4067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zanobetti A, Coull BA, Gryparis A, Kloog I, Sparrow D, Vokonas PS, et al. Associations between arrhythmia episodes and temporally and spatially resolved black carbon and particulate matter in elderly patients. Occupational and Environmental medicine. 2014;71(3):201–207. doi: 10.1136/oemed-2013-101526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeka A, Melly SJ, Schwartz J. The effects of socioeconomic status and indices of physical environment on reduced birth weight and preterm births in eastern Massachusetts. Environmental Health. 2008;7:60. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-7-60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu Y, Hinds WC, Kim S, Sioutas C. Concentration and size distribution of ultrafine particles near a major highway. Journal of the Air & Waste Management Association. 2002;52(9):1032–1042. doi: 10.1080/10473289.2002.10470842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.