Abstract
Objective Attempts to synthesize the evidence on the effects of decision aids have been hampered by the lack of consensus regarding how such effectiveness should be measured. This paper seeks to describe and critically assess the range of measures of effectiveness used in randomized controlled trials of decision aids.
Search strategy The published systematic reviews of the field were used to identify primary studies evaluating the effects of decision aids.
Inclusion criteria Non‐randomized trials were excluded from this review. As were abstracts and theses of subsequently published studies, methodological papers and reports of subgroups of a study's main publication.
Main results A wide range of measures were used to evaluate the effectiveness of decision aids. The most commonly used measures sought to assess treatment decisions, patient's knowledge and the decision‐making process. This pattern was repeated when primary measures of effectiveness were examined. No study attempted to measure the extent to which decisions made were consistent with patient's values.
Conclusions Within the current literature there is little consensus on what the aims of decision aids should be. If we can agree that the aim of a decision aid is to help patients make specific personal treatment choices, then evaluations of decision aids should measure the primary effectiveness of their interventions in terms of the extent to which they enable patient's to undergo treatments that agree with their values.
Keywords: decision aids, effectiveness, outcome measures, review
Introduction
The first randomized controlled trial (RCT) of a decision aid was conducted in 1983. 1 Since then more than 30 subsequent studies investigating the effectiveness of such interventions have been published. These trials have employed a wide range of measures under which the effectiveness of decision aids has been judged, reflecting the breadth and complexity of evaluating interventions aimed at the doctor–patient decision‐making process. However, the lack of an accepted primary measure of effectiveness against which the effects of decision aids can be compared across these studies has hampered research in the field. 2
In many ways this is not surprising as decision aids have been promoted as tools for increasing the involvement of patients in treatment decision‐making, and this call for greater patient participation has come from a range of perspectives, including patient consumerism, 3 medical ethics, 4 evidence‐based medicine 5 and decision theory, both normative 6 and descriptive. 7 Some have put the case for specific models of decision‐making, 8 , 9 whilst others have argued from more pragmatic standpoints, seeing increased patient involvement as a vehicle through which research findings could be implemented, 10 specific health‐care decisions promoted 1 , 11 , 12 or health status improved. 13
Due to these wide ranging perspectives, there has been little consensus on what the aims of decision aids are, and hence under what criteria their effectiveness should be judged. The aims of this paper are to provide: a summary of the measures of effectiveness that have been used by previous evaluations of decision aids and the particular measures chosen as the primary measure of effectiveness; a critical discussion on the strengths and weaknesses of these measures; and an assessment of whether recommendations can be made on the choice of primary measure of effectiveness for future evaluations.
Methods
There have been four systematic reviews of decision aid RCTs, all published in 2000/1. Although the Cochrane review was first published in 1999, 2 the 2001 14 update has been used in this study. Two of the reviews (Molenaar et al. 15 and Estabrooks et al. 16 ) included reports from non‐randomized evaluations of decision aids. Only the randomized studies from these reviews have been included in this paper as many of the studies were either pilot evaluations carried out by teams who subsequently published randomized trials of the same decision aids, or were designed to assess the reliability, validity and acceptability of the aids rather than to measure effectiveness. The other review concentrated only on decision aids for patients with cancer. 17 The Cochrane review's list of RCTs of decision aids awaiting publication 14 was also used to identify studies published in 2001 and after.
To assess the effectiveness measures used within the published randomized trials of decision aids; data was abstracted on those measures for which results were presented. The effectiveness measures were classified as follows: knowledge; decision process; decision; health status; and other, the collection of economic data was also noted (see Box 1). Further data on the format of the measure, its source and the characteristics of the clinical situation faced by study subjects were also abstracted. Where the primary measure was not explicitly identified in the trial report, the order and prominence of the measures described in the introduction and methods sections were used in identification.
Table Box 1 .
Knowledge | Treatment or condition‐related knowledge |
Expectation of likely outcomes | |
Perception of the risks and benefits of the treatment options | |
Decision process | Perceptions of the decision‐making process |
Satisfaction with the decision‐making process | |
The level of control subjects had over the decision | |
The level of control subjects had over the decision‐making process | |
Subject's preferences for participation in the decision‐making process | |
Decisions | Treatment intention |
Treatment undergone | |
Whether a treatment decision had been made | |
Adherence to the decision made | |
Health status | Condition‐specific health status |
General health status | |
Economic variables | Intervention costs |
Resource use |
Results
Included studies
The Cochrane review included 27 articles published between 1983 and 2001, the Estabrooks review identified nine RCTs reported between 1986 and 1999, and the Molenaar review covered 14 trial reports published between 1983 and 1998. The Effective Health Care Bulletin identified five RCTs of decision aids relating to cancer, published between 1995 and 2001, and two further articles published in 2001 were identified from the Cochrane review's list of RCTs of decision aids awaiting publication. Each of these five sources contributed at least one report not included in any other.
In total, 38 articles were identified and 33 were included in this review of effectiveness measurement. 11 , 12 , 13 , 18 , 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 , 24 , 25 , 26 , 27 , 28 , 29 , 30 , 31 , 32 , 33 , 34 , 35 , 36 , 37 , 38 , 39 , 40 , 41 , 42 , 43 , 44 , 45 , 46 One study included in the Cochrane review was excluded as it was a report of the results of a subgroup of the main trial report, 47 another because it was a methodological paper rather than a trial report 48 and one as it was a thesis of a subsequently published paper. 49 One study was excluded from each of the Molenaar and Effective Health Care reviews as they were abstracts of studies, the full reports of which were identified from the Cochrane review. 50 , 51
Study characteristics
The majority of the trials examined the effects of decision aids on either patients with a diagnosed condition facing a choice between alternative treatment options (12/33), 13 , 21 , 23 , 27 , 28 , 32 , 34 , 37 , 39 , 40 , 44 , 46 or potentially eligible population samples interested in screening or some other preventative health‐care intervention (13/33). 11 , 18 , 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 , 24 , 25 , 26 , 29 , 31 , 32 , 33 , 34 , 35 , 36 , 38 , 41 , 42 , 45 The other studies looked at parents or pregnant women making health‐care decisions for their children or unborn child (5/33) 1 , 12 , 24 , 30 , 33 and volunteers set a hypothetical decision related to treatment or entry to a clinical trial (3/33). 19 , 20 , 22
Cancer was the subject of approaching half of the aids (14/33), 19 , 20 , 22 , 23 , 26 , 28 , 29 , 34 , 36 , 38 , 41 , 42 , 43 , 44 communicable diseases (4), 11 , 18 , 25 , 33 cardiovascular disease (3) 32 , 37 , 40 and the menopause (3) 31 , 35 , 45 were the subject of a number of studies each. The other studies focused on potential birth defects (Down's syndrome and cystic fibrosis, 2), 24 , 30 benign prostatic hyperplasia (2), 27 , 46 back pain, 39 facial deformity, 21 ulcer disease 13 and circumcision (2). 1 , 12
Effectiveness measures
The most frequently used effectiveness measures related to the decisions faced by subjects. All but four studies collected data on decisions, 1 , 11 , 12 , 18 , 19 , 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 , 24 , 25 , 26 , 27 , 29 , 30 , 31 , 32 , 34 , 35 , 36 , 37 , 38 , 39 , 40 , 41 , 42 , 43 , 44 , 45 as treatment intentions (14), treatments undergone (17), whether a treatment decision had been made (1), or as adherence to the decision made (2) (Table 1). Data were mostly collected in terms of the treatment options subject's intended to undergo or had undergone. However, three of the studies that assessed intentions, used likert type scales to assess strength of intention, rather than providing discrete choice options. One study 29 used provision of a blood sample to be used for future genetic testing, as a proxy measure for intention, in addition to a likert scale measure.
Table 1.
Article | Primary measure | Format | Secondary measures | Format |
---|---|---|---|---|
Herrera et al. 1 | Treatment undergone | Options | ||
Maisels et al. 12 | Treatment undergone | Options | When decision made | Options (pre/postnatal) |
Perceptions of treatment risks/benefits | Scale developed for study | |||
Perceptions of treatment risks/benefits | Scale developed for study | |||
Greenfield et al. 13 | Decisional control | Coded from recordings | Decisional control | Coded from recordings |
Decisional control | Coded from recordings | |||
General health status | Published scale (Functional status index 52 ) | |||
Condition‐specific health status | Scale developed for study | |||
Decisional control | Scale developed for study | |||
Knowledge | Scale developed for study | |||
Satisfaction with care | Scale developed for study | |||
Carter et al. 11 | Treatment undergone | Options | ||
Clancy et al. 18 | Treatment undergone | Options | ||
Fetting et al. 19 | Intention | Options | Perceived likelihood of outcomes | VAS |
Perceived likelihood of outcomes | VAS | |||
Llewellyn‐Thomas et al. 20 | Knowledge | Scale developed for study | Intention | Options |
Perceptions of treatment risks/benefits | Scale developed for study | |||
Perceptions of treatment risks/benefits | Scale developed for study | |||
Phillips et al. 21 | Perceived likelihood of outcomes | Published scale (long‐term expectations scale 53 ) | Intention | Options |
Sebban et al. 22 | Intention | Likert scale | Satisfaction with decision | Likert scale |
Reliability | Options | |||
Street et al. 23 | Knowledge | Scale developed for study | Optimism | Published scale (Optimism disposition scale 54 ) |
Decisional control | Coded from recordings | |||
Decisional control | Published scale (Perceived Involvement in Care scale 55 ) | |||
Decisional control | Published scale (Perceived decision control instrument 56 ) | |||
Intention | Options | |||
Thornton et al. 24 | Treatment undergone | Options | Treatment undergone | Options |
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
Anxiety | Published scale (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale 57 ) | |||
Anxiety | Published scale (State Trait Anxiety Inventory 58 ) | |||
O'Connor et al. 25 | Treatment undergone | Options | Perceived likelihood of outcomes | Probability scale |
Decisional conflict | Published scale (Decisional Conflict Scale 59 ) | |||
Condition‐specific health status | Published scale (Influenza vaccine symptom scale 60 ) | |||
General health status | Absenteeism | |||
Wolf et al. 26 | Intention | Likert scale | ||
Barry et al. 27 | Intention | Options | Treatment undergone | Options |
Knowledge | Scale developed for study | |||
Satisfaction with the decision‐making process | Scale developed for study | |||
Condition‐specific health status (1) | Published scale (AUA symptom index 61 ) | |||
Condition‐specific health status (2) | Published scale (Benign Prostatic Hyperplasia Impact index 62 ) | |||
General health status | Published scale (SF‐36 63 ) | |||
Decisional control | Published scale (Autonomy Preference Index 64 ) | |||
Satisfaction with decision | Scale developed for study | |||
Davison and Degner 28 | Decisional control | Published scale (Control preferences scale 65 ) | Anxiety | Published scale (State Trait Anxiety Inventory 58 ) |
Depression | Published scale (Centre for epidemiologic studies depression scale 66 ) | |||
Lerman et al. 29 | Knowledge | Published scale (Inherited breast cancer and BRCA1 testing knowledge scale 67 ) | Perceived likelihood of outcomes | Likert scale |
Perceptions of treatment risks/benefits | Published scale (BRCA1 testing attitudes scale 67 ) | |||
Intention | Likert scale | |||
Intention | Provision of blood sample | |||
Michie et al. 30 | Knowledge | Scale developed for study | Anxiety | Published scale (State Trait Anxiety Inventory 68 ) |
Systematic decision | Scale developed for study | |||
Satisfaction with the decision‐ making process | Scale developed for study | |||
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
Rothert et al. 31 | Knowledge | Scale developed for study | Decisional conflict | Subscale of published scale (Decisional Conflict Scale 58 ) |
Satisfaction with decision | Published scale (Satisfaction With Decision scale 69 ) | |||
Satisfaction with most recent encounter with health practitioner | Published scale (Satisfaction Scale 70 ) | |||
Decisional control | Published scale (Self efficacy Scale 71 ) | |||
Adherence | Options | |||
Bernstein et al. 32 | Satisfaction with the decision‐ making process | Published scale (Decision‐making process questionnaire 27 ) | Satisfaction with decision | Published scale (Satisfaction With Decision Scale 27 ) |
Knowledge | Published scale (Knowledge questionnaire 49 ) | |||
Condition‐specific health status | Published scale (Seattle Angina Questionnaire 72 ) | |||
General health status | Published scale (SF‐12 73 ) | |||
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
Dunn et al. 33 | Knowledge | Scale developed for study | ||
Maslin et al. 34 | Anxiety and depression | Published scale (Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale 59 ) | General health status | Published scale (SF‐36 74 ) |
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
Decisional control | Scale developed for study | |||
Satisfaction with decision | Not described | |||
O'Connor et al. 35 | Decisional conflict | Published scale (Decisional conflict scale 58 ) | Perceived likelihood of outcomes | Probability estimate |
Knowledge | Scale developed for study | |||
Intention | Options | |||
Perceptions of treatment risks/ benefits | VAS | |||
Davison et al. 36 | Decisional control | Published scale (Control preferences scale 65 ) | Anxiety | Published scale (State Trait Anxiety Inventory 75 ) |
Decisional conflict | Published scale (Decisional Conflict Scale 59 ) | |||
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
Man‐Son‐Hing et al. 37 | Whether decision made | Yes/no | Treatment undergone | Options |
Knowledge | Scale developed for study | |||
Perceived likelihood of outcomes | Probability estimate | |||
Decisional conflict | Published scale (Decisional Conflict Scale 59 ) | |||
Satisfaction with the decision‐ making process | Scale developed for study | |||
Adherence | Options | |||
Volk et al. 38 | Knowledge | Scale developed for study | Intention | Options |
Intention | Options | |||
Deyo et al. 39 | Condition‐specific health status | Published scale (Roland disability scale 76 ) | Treatment undergone | Options |
General health status | Absenteeism | |||
Condition‐specific health status | Published scale (Back pain symptom satisfaction 77 ) | |||
Satisfaction with the decision‐ making process | Scale developed for study | |||
Satisfaction with care | Scale developed for study | |||
Health‐care utilization | Resource use | |||
Morgan et al. 40 | Satisfaction with the decision‐making process | Published scale (Decision‐making process questionnaire 27 ) | Knowledge | Scale developed for study |
Intention | Options | |||
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
General health status | Published scale (SF‐36 63 ) | |||
Condition‐specific health status | Published scale (Canadian Cardiovascular Angina scale 78 ) | |||
Agreement of doctor recommendation and intention | Options | |||
Pignone et al. 41 | Treatment undergone | Options | Intention | Options |
Intention | Likert scale | |||
Whether screening discussed | Options | |||
Agreement of intention and treatment undergone | Likert scale | |||
Schapira and Vanruiswyk 42 | Treatment undergone | Options | Knowledge | Scale developed for study |
Perceptions of treatment risks/benefits | Scale developed for study | |||
Wolf and Schorling 43 | Intention | Likert scale | Intention | Options |
Knowledge | Scale developed for study | |||
Perceptions of treatment risks/benefits | Likert scale | |||
Goel et al. 44 | Decisional conflict | Published scale (Decisional conflict scale 59 ) | Knowledge | Published scale (Breast Cancer Information Test – Revised 79 ) |
Satisfaction with decision | Scale developed for study | |||
Anxiety | Published scale (State Trait Anxiety Inventory 58 ) | |||
Treatment undergone | Options | |||
Murray et al. 45 | Treatment undergone | Options | Decisional conflict | Published scale (Decisional Conflict Scale 59 ) |
Decisional control | Scale developed for study | |||
Total cost | Cost | |||
Anxiety | Published scale (State Trait Anxiety Inventory 68 ) | |||
General health status (1) | Published scale (SF 36 74 ) | |||
General health status (2) | Published scale (EQ‐5D 80 ) | |||
Condition‐specific health status | Published scale (Menqol 81 ) | |||
Murray et al. 46 | Condition‐specific health status | Published scale (AUA symptom index 61 ) | Anxiety | Published scale (State Trait Anxiety Inventory 68 ) |
Decisional conflict | Published scale (Decisional Conflict Scale 59 ) | |||
Decisional control | Scale developed for study | |||
Total cost | Cost | |||
General health status (1) | Published scale (SF 36 74 ) | |||
General health status (2) | Published scale (EQ‐5D 80 ) |
Measures assessing the decision‐making process were used in more than half the studies (18/33). 13 , 22 , 23 , 25 , 27 , 28 , 30 , 31 , 32 , 34 , 35 , 36 , 37 , 39 , 40 , 44 , 45 , 46 The most commonly used measure was the Decisional Conflict Scale used in nine studies (Table 1). Other outcome measures focused on the level of control subjects had over the decision or decision‐making process, or their desire to participate in decision‐making (9). Measures included coding schemes applied to tape recordings of the consultation, item response scales specially developed for the studies and a range of published scales (Table 1). Ten studies assessed the subjects’ satisfaction with the decision or the decision‐making process using likert scales, study specific item response scales or published scales. One trial developed and used a measure of the extent to which the decision was made in a systematic manner.
Subjects’ knowledge and beliefs were also used as effectiveness measures in more than half the studies (20/33). 12 , 13 , 19 , 20 , 21 , 23 , 25 , 27 , 29 , 30 , 31 , 32 , 33 , 35 , 37 , 38 , 40 , 42 , 43 , 44 These were generally specially developed item response scales. Other measurement methods included visual analogue scales, probability scales, likert scales and four studies used previously published scales (Table 1).
Health status was measured in just under half the studies (14/33). 13 , 24 , 25 , 27 , 28 , 30 , 32 , 34 , 36 , 39 , 40 , 44 , 45 , 46 Seven trials used published condition‐specific measures relevant to the conditions being studied (one study developed a condition‐specific scale for their project) and seven trials assessed the effects of the interventions on anxiety or depression (Table 1). Seven trials measured general health status using standard published scales and two studies used absence from work as a proxy measure for health status.
Other measures of effectiveness were: satisfaction with care (2); 13 , 39 optimism (1); 23 subjects’ satisfaction with their most recent encounter with their health practitioner (1); 31 whether the condition under study was raised as a subject during the consultation (1); 41 when the treatment decision was made (1); 12 agreement between the clinician's recommendation and the subject's treatment intention (1); 40 the agreement between treatment intentions and treatment undergone (1); 41 and one study assessed intention after a second application of the intervention to assess test–retest reliability. 22 Three studies collected data on economic variables. 39 , 45 , 46 Two calculated total costs, intervention costs and subsequent resource use, and one collected data on resource use only (Table 1).
Primary measure of effectiveness
Primary measures covered the range of main categories described above (Table 1). The most frequently used primary measures related to the decision (15/33). Five studies assessed differences in treatment intentions, nine in treatments undergone and one trial used the extent to which a decision had been made as the primary measure. The effectiveness of the decision aids to improve subjects’ knowledge was also used for a number of studies (8/33). The majority used general measures of treatment‐related knowledge (7/8) and one, the subjects’ perceptions of the likelihood of the potential outcomes.
Seven studies assessed the effect of their interventions on the decision‐making process. Three used the decisional conflict scale, two the level of decisional control held by the subject and two more the subjects’ satisfaction with the decision‐making process. Two studies used condition‐specific health status measures, the Roland disability scale in a study of treatment for back pain and the American Urological Association symptom index in a study of treatment for BPH; another assessed anxiety and depression (HAD scale) in a study of treatment for breast cancer.
Discussion
The wide range of measures of effectiveness used in these RCTs reflects the varying aims of the decision aids and the different effects potentially attributable to them. Whilst this may be useful for secondary measures of effectiveness, this lack of consensus was also evident in the selection of the primary measure of effectiveness which limits the generalizability of the findings of these evaluations and the scope for using meta‐analysis. This is likely to continue until decision aid researchers resolve the debate surrounding what decision aids are supposed to do.
A previous critique of effectiveness measures for evaluations of decision aids, by Entwistle et al. 82 argued that in a health‐care system with a primary aim to improve health and well‐being, decision aids should be considered in the context of whether they can improve health status. In addition, the use of generic health status measures to assess effectiveness permit the benefits of these interventions to be compared with other potential uses of health‐care resources. Few studies used health status as their primary effectiveness measure, perhaps because any improvement in health status associated with a decision aid is likely to be small and may not accrue for many years after the decision under evaluation has been made. Studies would therefore require large numbers of subjects and involve follow‐up for many years.
Decision aids are often applied to conditions for which there is no clinically best treatment, and therefore the patient's values will often be the determining factor in the choice between treatment options. In this situation observing a difference in health status as a result of receiving a decision aid is highly unlikely. In addition, in situations where there may be differences in the clinical effectiveness of available treatment options, certain characteristics of the options may make the choice between them less straightforward. For example, should the most effective treatment option involve a long recuperation period, patients may choose to undergo a less effective option that would interfere less with their life or lifestyle, or should the most effective treatment involve changes in diet and behaviour that the subjects value highly, they may choose a less effective treatment option without these limitations. The problem with using health status as the primary measure of effectiveness is that patients might quite rationally choose a treatment option that does not maximize their future health status.
A commonly employed definition of what constitutes a good decision, proposed by O'Connor et al. 83 is that a decision should be informed, should agree with subject's values and should be implemented. Two aspects of this definition map onto the effectiveness measures employed in the evaluations described above. A measure that tries to incorporate all three of these features has undergone initial validation, but has yet to be used in RCT. 84
Knowledge‐related outcomes attempt to assess the extent to which decisions made have been informed. However, improving knowledge should not be the primary aim of a decision aid. Knowledge may help patients to form treatment preferences but it does not ensure that patients are able to play a significant role in the clinical encounter or ensure that they receive the treatment that they want. 85 The use of knowledge measures also assumes that patients are able to use the information to come to a decision, whereas in practice subjects may need additional help to combine this evidence with their values to choose between the options. 86 The use of knowledge as a measure should be confined to the intervention development process, and the ability of a decision aid to increase knowledge should be established long before it undergoes evaluation in RCT.
Decision‐related measures, whether as treatment intention or treatment undergone, have been used to assess the extent to which the decision made is implemented. The use of intention rather than treatment undergone tends to overestimate the size of this effect. Whilst treatment intentions are highly correlated with treatments undergone, a significant proportion of subjects expressing an intention towards a treatment option, do not follow through their intention. However, treatments undergone cannot be used as the basis on which a decision aid should be evaluated. The purpose of a decision aid is not to promote one treatment option over another. A decision aid should provide evidence on the risks and benefits of all the options, the right choice for any individual patient will depend on how he or she values the risks and benefits of these options.
The one aspect of the O'Connor definition that has not been adopted as a primary outcome measure, is the extent to which decisions made agree with subject's values. Of the 33 studies included in the review none attempted to measure this outcome directly, however, the use of some decision process measures, may be interpreted as attempts to measure this outcome indirectly. The measures assessing decisional control and satisfaction with the decision‐making process may be tapping into a slightly different characteristic of the decision process, one of autonomy and participation in the consultation. Such measures are inappropriate as primary measures of effectiveness as again the aim of a decision aid is not to promote one model of decision‐making over any other. Individual patients will have preferences over the role they wish to play in the decision‐making process, and this role may even change during the course of this process, increasing as they become more informed or decreasing as they become more anxious. 8
Measures assessing satisfaction with the decision and the effective decision‐making subscale of the Decisional Conflict Scale could be interpreted as attempts to assess the underlying construct of the extent to which subject's values were incorporated into the decision. Neither of these measures quite achieves this.
Satisfaction with the decision does not assess the extent to which the decision corresponded with the patient's values. As with all assessments of satisfaction a whole range of other factors may influence the perception and it is subject to a range of potential biases. 87 The Decisional Conflict Scale measures subject's perceptions of the extent to which they are uncertain about which option to choose, the factors contributing to this uncertainty and the effectiveness of their decision. Of the 16 items that make up the scale just one, on the decision effectiveness subscale, addresses whether the choice reflected the patient's values, and this particular subscale exhibits low discriminant abilities. 59 Therefore, the most important contribution of this scale may be in quantifying uncertainty and factors contributing to uncertainty, rather than measuring the extent to which patient's values were incorporated into the decision.
If we adopt the O'Connor et al. argument that the purpose of a decision aid is to help patients to make ‘a specific, personal choice between options’, then the effectiveness of decision aids should be judged by the extent to which patients undergo treatments that are consistent with their values for the potential outcomes of the available options.
At its most basic a measure to assess agreement could take the form of a ranking of the most important characteristics influencing the decision, and effectiveness judged in terms of the extent to which the treatment undergone satisfied these characteristics. Such an approach was used by two of the observational studies included in the Molenaar and Estabrooks reviews. 62 , 83 At its most complex utility measurement would be the measure of value and decision aids evaluated in terms of maximizing expected utility. A methodological paper conducted as part of the Rothert study, 48 made the case for measuring agreement between values and the treatments undergone in terms of the correlation between subjective expected utility for potential treatment outcomes and the likelihood of undergoing therapy. However, no formal validations of such measures have so far been undertaken. The methodological challenge will be to ensure that the effectiveness measure used, is valid, in that it includes all the risks and benefits of importance to the patient, not just those that are health related; is unbiased, for example not influenced by framing effects or attitudes to risk; is sufficiently sensitive; and is practical for use in large RCTs.
In conclusion, if decision aid researchers can agree that the primary purpose of a decision aid is to help patients make specific personal choices between different treatment options, then decision aids should be evaluated in terms of the extent to which patients undergo treatments that agree with their values.
References
- 1. Herrera AJ, Cochran B, Herrera A, Wallace B. Parental information and circumcision in highly motivated couples with higher education. Pediatrics, 1983; 71: 233–234. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. O'Connor AM, Rostom A, Fiset V, Tetroe J, Entwistle V, Llewellyn‐Thomas H. Decision aids for patients facing health treatment or screening decisions: systematic review. BMJ, 1999; 319: 731–734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Shepperd S, Charnock D, Gann B. Helping patients access high quality health information. BMJ, 1999; 319: 764–766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Veatch RM. Models for ethical medicine in a revolutionary age. Hastings Centre Report, 1972; 2: 5–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Hope T. The Use of Evidence Based Information for Enhancing Patient Choice. Oxford: Anglia and Oxford Regional Health Authority, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Holmes‐Rovner M, Rovner D. Measuring improved patient choice. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 2000; 6: 263–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Frisch D, Clemen R. Beyond expected utility: rethinking behavioural decision research. Psychological Bulletin, 1994; 116: 46–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Charles C, Gafni A, Whelan T. Shared decision‐making in the medical encounter: what does it mean? (or it takes at least two to tango). Social Science and Medicine, 1997; 44: 681–692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Vick S, Scott A. Agency in health care. Examining patients’ preferences for attributes of the doctor–patient relationship. Journal of Health Economics, 1998; 17: 587–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Lilford RJ, Pauker S, Braunholtz D, Chard J. Decision analysis and the implementation of research findings. BMJ, 1998; 317: 405–409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Carter W, Beach L, Inui T. The flu shot study: using multiattribute utility theory to design a vaccination intervention. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 1986; 1986: 378–391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Maisels MJ, Hayes B, Conrad S, Chez RA. Circumcision: the effect of information on parental decision making. Pediatrics, 1983; 71: 453–455. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Greenfield S, Kaplan S, Ware JE. Expanding patient involvement in care. Effects on patient outcomes. Annals of Internal Medicine, 1985; 102: 520–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. O'Connor AM, Stacey D, Rovner D et al. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions (cochrane review). The Cochrane Library, Vol. 3. Oxford: Update Software, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Molenaar S, Sprangers MAG, Postma‐Schuit FCE, Rutgers EJT, Noorlander J, Hendriks J. Feasibility and effects of decision aids. Medical Decision Making, 2000; 20: 112–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Estabrooks C, Goel V, Thiel E, Pinfold P, Sawka C, Williams I. Decision aids: are they worth it? A systematic review. Journal of Health Services Research and Policy, 2001; 6: 170–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Effective Health Care . Informing, Communicating and Sharing Decisions with People who have Cancer. Leeds: University of Leeds, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Clancy CM, Cebul RD, Williams SV. Guiding individual decisions: a randomized, controlled trial of decision analysis. American Journal of Medicine, 1988; 84: 283–288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Fetting JH, Siminoff LA, Piantadosi S, Abeloff MD, Damron DJ, Sarsfield AM. Effect of patients’ expectations of benefit with standard breast cancer adjuvant chemotherapy on participation in a randomized clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 1990; 8: 1476–1482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Llewellyn‐Thomas H, Thiel E, Sem F, Harrison‐Woermke D. Presenting clinical trial information: a comparison of methods. Patient Education and Counselling, 1995; 25: 97–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Phillips C, Hill BJ, Cannac C. The influence of video imaging on patients’ perceptions and expectations. [See comments]. Angle Orthodontist, 1995; 65: 263–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Sebban C, Browman G, Gafni A. et al. Design and validation of a bedside decision instrument to elicit a patient's preference concerning allogenic bone marrow transplantation in. American Journal of Hematology, 1995; 48: 221–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Street RL, Voigt B, Geyer C, Manning T, Swanson GP. Increasing patient involvement in choosing treatment for early breast cancer. Cancer, 1995; 76: 2275–2285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Thornton JG, Hewison J, Lilford RJ, Vail A. A randomised trial of three methods of giving information about prenatal testing. [See comments]. BMJ, 1995; 311: 1127–1130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. O'Connor AM, Pennie RA, Dales RE. Framing effects on expectations, decisions, and side effects experienced: the case of influenza immunization. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 1996; 49: 1271–1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Wolf AM, Nasser JF, Wolf AM, Schorling JB. The impact of informed consent on patient interest in prostate‐specific antigen screening. [See comments]. Archives of Internal Medicine, 1996; 156: 1333–1336. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Barry MJ, Cherkin DC, Chang Y, Fowler FJ, Skates S. A randomized trial of a multimedia shared decision‐making program for men facing a treatment decision for benign prostatic hyperplasia. Disease Management and Clinical Outcomes, 1997; 1: 1–37. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Davison BJ, Degner LF. Empowerment of men newly diagnosed with prostate cancer. Cancer Nursing, 1997; 20: 187–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Lerman C, Biesecker B, Benkendorf JL et al. Controlled trial of pretest education approaches to enhance informed decision‐making for brca1 gene testing. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 1997; 89: 148–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Michie S, Smith D, McClellan A, Marteau TM. Patient decision making: an evaluation of two different methods of presenting information about a screening test. British Journal of Health Psychology, 1997; 2: 317–326. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Rothert ML, Holmes‐Rovner M, Rovner D et al. An educational intervention as decision support for menopausal women. Research in Nursing and Health, 1997; 20: 377–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bernstein SJ, Skarupski KA, Grayson CE, Starling MR, Bates ER, Eagle KA. A randomized controlled trial of information‐giving to patients referred for coronary angiography: effects on outcomes of care. Health Expectations, 1998; 1: 50–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Dunn RA, Shenouda PE, Martin DR, Schultz AJ. Videotape increases parent knowledge about poliovirus vaccines and choices of polio vaccination schedules. Pediatrics, 1998; 102: e26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Maslin AM, Baum M, Walker JS, A'Hern R, Prouse A. Using an interactive video disk in breast cancer patient support. Nursing Times, 1998; 94: 52–55. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. O'Connor AM, Tugwell P, Wells GA et al. Randomized trial of a portable, self‐administered decision aid for postmenopausal women considering long‐term preventive hormone therapy. Medical Decision Making, 1998; 18: 295–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Davison BJ, Kirk P, Degner LF, Hassard T. Information and patient participation in screening for prostate cancer. Patient Education and Counselling, 1999; 37: 255–263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Man‐Son‐Hing M, Laupacis A, O'Connor AM et al. A patient decision aid regarding antithrombotic therapy for stroke prevention in atrial fibrillation: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 1999; 282: 737–743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Volk RJ, Cass AR, Spann SJ. A randomized controlled trial of shared decision making for prostate cancer screening. Archives of Family Medicine, 1999; 8: 333–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Deyo RA, Cherkin DC, Weinstein J, Howe J, Ciol M, Mulley AG. Involving patients in clinical decisions: impact of an interactive video program on use of back surgery. Medical Care, 2000; 38: 959–969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Morgan MW, Deber RB, Llewellyn‐Thomas HA et al. Randomized, controlled trial of an interactive videodisc decision aid for patients with ischemic heart disease. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 2000; 15: 685–693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Pignone M, Harris R, Kinsinger L. Videotape‐based decision aid for colon cancer screening. A randomized, controlled trial. Annals of Internal Medicine, 2000; 133: 761–769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Schapira M, Vanruiswyk J. The effect of an illustrated pamphlet decision‐aid on the use of prostate cancer screening tests. Journal of Family Practice, 2000; 49: 418–424. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Wolf AM, Schorling JB. Does informed consent alter elderly patients’ preferences for colorectal screening? Journal of General Internal Medicine, 2000; 15: 24–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Goel V, Sawka CA, Thiel EC, Gort EH, O'Connor AM. Randomized trial of a patient decision aid for choice of surgical treatment for breast cancer. Medical Decision Making, 2001; 21: 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Murray E, Davis H, See Tai S, Coulter A, Gray A, Haines A. Randomised controlled trial of an interactive multimedia decision aid on hormone replacement therapy in primary care. BMJ, 2001; 323: 490–493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Murray E, Davis H, See Tai S, Coulter A, Gray A, Haines A. Randomised controlled trial of an interactive multimedia decision aid on benign prostatic hypertrophy in primary care. BMJ, 2001; 323: 493–496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Phelan E, Deyo RA, Cherkin DC et al. Helping patients decide about back surgery: a randomized trial of an interactive video program. Spine, 2001; 26: 206–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Holmes‐Rovner M, Kroll J, Rovner DR et al. Patient decision support intervention: increased consistency with decision analytic models. Medical Care, 1999; 37: 270–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Morgan MW. A randomized controlled trial of the ischemic heart disease shared decision making program: an evaluation of a decision aid. University of Toronto, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Van Ruiswijk J, Griffin R, Schapira M. Informing patients about prostate cancer screening risks (abstr). Medical Decision Making, 1995; 15: 421. [Google Scholar]
- 51. Goel V, Sawka C, Thiel E. A randomized trail of a decision aid for breast cancer surgery (abstr). Medical Decision Making, 1998; 18: 482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Stewart A, Ware JE, Brook R. Advances in the measurement of functional status: construction of aggregate indexes. Medical Care, 1981; 19: 473–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Kiyak H, McNeill R, West R. Personality characteristics as predictors and sequelae of surgical and conventional orthodontics. American Journal of Orthodontics, 1986; 89: 383–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Scheier M, Carver C. Optimism, coping and health: assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 1985; 4: 219–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Lerman C, Brody DS, Caputo G, Smith DG, Lazaro C, Wolfson H. Perceived involvement in care scale: relationship to attitudes about illness and medical care. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 1990; 5: 29–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. England S, Evans J. Patients’ choices and perceptions after an invitation to participate in treatment decisions. Social Science and Medicine, 1992; 34: 1217–1225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Zigmond A, Snaith R. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychologica Scandinavica, 1983; 67: 361–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene R. STAI Manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1970. [Google Scholar]
- 59. O'Connor AM. Validation of a decisional conflict scale. Medical Decision Making, 1995; 15: 25–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Scheifele D, Bjornson G, Johnston J. Evaluation of adverse events after influenza vaccination of hospital personnel. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 1990; 142: 127–130. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Barry MJ, Fowler FJ, O'Leary M. The American Urological Association Symptom Index for benign prostatic hyperplasia. Journal of Urology, 1992; 148(5): 1549–1557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Barry MJ, Fowler FJ, Mulley AG, Henderson JV, Wennberg JE. Patient reactions to a program designed to facilitate patient participation in treatment decisions for benign prostatic hyperplasia. Medical Care, 1995; 33: 771–782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Ware JE. SF‐36 Health Survey. Boston: Nimrod Press, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 64. Ende J, Kazis L, Ash A, Moskowitz M. Measuring patients’ desire for autonomy: decision making and information seeking preferences among medical patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 1989; 4: 23–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Degner LF, Sloan JA. Decision making during serious illness: what role do patients really want to play? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 1992; 45: 941–950. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Markush R, Favero R. Epidemiologic assessment of stressful life events, depressed mood and psychophysiological symptoms In: Dohrenwend B. (ed.) Stressful Life Events: Their Nature and Effects. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1973. [Google Scholar]
- 67. Lerman C, Narod S, Schulman K, Hughes C, Gomez‐Caminero A, Bonney G. BRCA1 testing in families with hereditary breast‐ovarian cancer. JAMA, 1996; 275: 1885–1892. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Marteau TM, Bekker H. The development of a six‐item short‐form of the state scale of the Spielberger State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1992; 31: 301–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Holmes‐Rovner M, Kroll J, Schmitt N et al. Patient satisfaction with health care decisions: the satisfaction with decision scale. Medical Decision Making, 1996; 16: 58–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Linder‐Pelz S, Struening E. The multidimensionality of patient satisfaction with a clinic visit. Journal of Community Health, 1985; 10: 42–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Woodward N, Wallston B. Age and health care beliefs; self‐efficacy as a mediator of low desire for control. Psychology and Aging, 1987; 2: 3–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Spertus J, Winder J, Dewhurst T. Development and evaluation of the Seattle Angina Questionnaire. Journal of the American College of Cardiologists, 1995; 25: 333–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Stewart A, Ware JE. Measuring Functioning and Wellbeing: the Medical Outcomes Study Approach. Durham: Duke University Press, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 74. Brazier JE, Harper R, Jones NM. Validating the SF‐36 health survey questionnaire: new outcome measure for primary care. BMJ, 1992; 305: 160–164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Spielberger C. STAI Manual. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 76. Roland M, Morris R. A study of the natural history of back pain, part I: development of reliable and sensitive measure of disability in low back pain. Spine, 1983; 8: 141–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Cherkin DC, Deyo RA, Street J, Barlow W. Predicting poor outcomes for back pain seen in primary care using patients’ own criteria. Spine, 1996; 21: 2900–2907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Campeau L. Grading of angina pectoris. Circulation, 1976; 54: 522–523. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Ward S, Griffin J. Developing a test of knowledge of surgical options for breast cancer. Cancer Nursing, 1990; 13: 191–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. EuroQol Group . EuroQol: a new facility for the measurement of health related quality of life. Health Policy, 1990; 16: 199–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Hilditch J, Lewis J, Peter A, van Maris B, Ross A, Franssen E. A menopause specific quality of life questionnaire: development and psychometric properties. Maturitas, 1996; 24: 161–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Entwistle V, Sowden A, Watt I. Evaluating interventions to promote patient involvement in decision‐making: by what criteria should effectiveness be judged. Journal of Health Services Research and Policy, 1998; 3: 100–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. O'Connor AM, Tugwell P, Wells GA. A decision aid for women considering hormone therapy after menopause: decision support framework and evaluation. Patient Education and Counselling, 1998; 33: 267–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Marteau TM, Dormandy E, Michie S. A measure of informed choice. Health Expectations, 2001; 4: 99–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Kennedy A, Sculpher M, Coulter A et al. A multicentre randomised controlled trial assessing the costs and benefits of using structured information and analysis of women's preferences in the management of menorrhagia. Health Technological Assessment, 2003; 7(8). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Kennedy A, Sculpher M, Coulter A et al. Effects of decision aids for menorrhagia on treatment choices, health outcomes and costs. JAMA, 2002; 288: 2701–2708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Cleary P. Satisfaction may not suffice! A commentary on ‘A patient's perspective’. International Journal of Technology Assessment in Health Care, 1998; 14: 35–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]