Abstract
Objective To clarify preferred labels for people receiving health care.
Background The proper label to describe people receiving care has evoked considerable debate among providers and bio‐ethicists, but there is little evidence as to the preferences of the people involved.
Design We analysed dictionary definitions as to the derivation and connotations of such potential labels as: patient, client, customer, consumer, partner and survivor. We then surveyed outpatients from four clinical populations in Ontario, Canada about their feelings about these labels.
Setting and participants People from breast cancer (n = 202), prostate disease (n = 202) and fracture (n = 202) clinics in an urban Canadian teaching hospital (Sharpe study), and people with HIV/AIDS at 10 specialty care clinics and three primary care practices affiliated with the HIV Ontario Observational Database (n = 431).
Variables and outcome measures The survey instruments included questions about opinion of label, role in treatment decision‐making (the Problem Solving Decision Making scale), trust, use of information and health status.
Results Our respondents moderately liked the label ‘patient’. The other alternatives evoked moderate to strong dislike.
Conclusions Many alternatives to ‘patient’ incorporate assumptions (e.g. a market relationship) which care recipients may also find objectionable. People who are receiving care find the label ‘patient’ much less objectionable than the alternatives that have been suggested.
Keywords: client, consumer, decision making, participation, patient, patient–physician relationships
Background
As the patient–physician relationship has shifted towards greater equality, there has been ongoing debate on whether the term ‘patient’ is still appropriate to use.
Current standards in bioethics and law call for active patient participation for purposes of empowerment and recognition of the patient's right to self‐determination and autonomy. Autonomy is now a dominant value in medical ethics. 1 , 2 , 3 , 4 , 5 Its primacy has been reinforced by the legal system, particularly by the array of consent to treatment/informed consent legislation found in many jurisdictions, which reflect a belief that individuals should be involved in making decisions about their own care. The shift from the historical patient–provider relationship where the physician was expected to direct care and decide treatment, to a relationship where the patient is expected to be much more autonomous, has placed both parties in new roles. Should the language used to refer to recipients of care change accordingly? Advocates have suggested a new terminology, variously referring to the recipients of care as ‘consumers’, ‘clients’, ‘purchasers’, ‘customers’ or ‘users’.
In one such debate in the British Medical Journal, Neuberger 6 made a strong case for doing away with the label ‘patient’ in favour of terms implying more active participation. In contrast, Tallis 7 defended the term ‘patient’, but called for research to be carried out on how people viewed the term ‘patient’ prior to making any changes to terminology. Among the several responses to the Neuberger and Tallis debate were suggestions to use such terms ‘actors’, ‘punter’, ‘citizens’, ‘plaintiff’, ‘employers’, and ‘neubergers’. 8 , 9 , 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 Similarly, Herxheimer and Goodare 14 noted that ‘the terminology used to describe individuals who come into contact with health services is problematic’.
Several empirical studies of preferred labels have been conducted. In selected populations, particularly mental health, the historical power imbalance has led some to reject the term ‘patient’, particularly among those care recipients living in the community, 15 although even in this population other studies have concluded that many still prefer the term ‘patient’. 16 , 17
Clearly, variation is likely to arise, related to how healthy people feel themselves to be (e.g. basically healthy people visiting for screening tests or health promotion vs. those with chronic conditions), and to the organization of health‐care systems (e.g. health‐care systems requiring people to pay for their care may encourage people to see themselves as consumers). Nonetheless, an Australian study asked 308 outpatients attending a university teaching hospital to select which of the labels ‘patient’, ‘clients’ or ‘consumers’ they liked best; the overwhelming majority (85% of women and 83% of men) preferred to be referred to as ‘patients’. 18 Similarly, Lloyd cites evidence that 87% in a New Zealand outpatient study wished to be called patients, 15 as did 73% of patients in a Canadian back pain clinic. 19 However, the study design in these surveys did not allow individuals to specify that they liked more than one term. We accordingly have complemented and extended, this work by asking for views of people receiving care about six potential labels –‘patient’, ‘consumer’, ‘customer’, ‘client’, ‘partner’ and ‘survivor’.
Labels and their meanings
Labels carry connotations and implications. We examined several dictionaries – from both sides of the Atlantic ocean – for their definitions of these terms. 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 In most cases, they tended to come from the Latin, occasionally via Middle English.
‘Patient’ is defined as ‘having or showing patience’ 21 and as ‘an individual awaiting or under medical care and treatment’. 20 To some, the term also carries connotations of passivity and deference to physicians, although that is not inherent in the definition. ‘Patience’, in turn, is derived from the Latin word ‘patiens’, the present participle of pati ‘to suffer’. It is accordingly defined as ‘the capacity to tolerate delay, trouble, or suffering without becoming angry or upset’, 21 ‘perseverance or forbearance, 20 as well as a card game. 23 One can see why the vocabulary of suffering is less than appealing; it may be too accurate a description of many encounters within the health‐care system. 14
Yet the terminology of ‘consumer’, ‘customer’ and ‘client’ can be seen as carrying potentially more objectionable overtones, to the extent that they imply that medical services are commodities to be managed in a market. Implicit in consumerism is that the consumer is the sole arbiter of his or her needs, and that the role of the tradesman is to satisfy them. If the consumer is seen as the ‘buyer’, then providers must assume the role of ‘seller’. It is no part of a seller's ethic to discourage a customer from buying on the basis that the item in question is not one that the customer ‘needs’; the ethics of trade are based on the premise of matching supply with demand. 24 ‘Client’ comes from a Latin root meaning ‘dependent’, and is variously defined as ‘one that is under the protection of another’, 20 ‘a person who engages the professional advice or services of another such as that from a lawyer’, or ‘customer’. 20 It thus carries connotations of an agency relationship, whereby one individual purchases professional services from another.
‘Customer’, in turn, comes from the Middle English word for tax collector, and is defined as ‘one that purchases some commodity or service’, 20 as well as such now obsolete meanings as ‘(obsolete) tax collector’, 23 and ‘(obsolete) prostitute’. 23 ‘Consumer’ comes from the Latin word consumere (to take up completely) and is defined in terms of one who consumes an item. In turn, ‘consume’ is variously described as ‘to destroy or do away with completely’, by fire, disease, famine, or decomposition, 20 ‘to spend wastefully, squander, use up’, 20 ‘to eat or drink especially in great quantity’, 20 and ‘to utilize economic goods’. 20 The connotations thus speak both of waste, and of economic/market commodities. These connotations can be disquieting, particularly to those who prefer to conceptualize receipt of health care as being based on need.
‘Partner’ comes from the Middle English word ‘partener’, an alteration of parcener, and is defined as ‘one that shares’, 20 one associated with another especially in an action’, 20 ‘or a member of a partnership’. 20 Although this term is not in wide use to refer to care recipients, it would seem compatible with the recent stress on shared decision making. 25 , 26 , 27 , 28 , 29 , 30 , 31
The term ‘survivor’ comes from the Latin supervivere, from super‐ +vivere ‘to live’, and is defined as ‘to continue to function or prosper despite’, 20 while ‘survive’ is describe as ‘to remain alive or in existence or live on’, 20 or ‘to continue to function or prosper’. 20
Methods
The view of labels question read: ‘There are a number of terms which people may use to refer to people who receive medical care’. Respondents were asked to indicate their view about being referred to as each of: patient, client, consumer, survivor, partner and customer. A five‐point Likert scale was used, with responses ranging from strongly dislike to strongly like. This item was included in two studies; the Sharpe study 29 was conducted in three outpatient clinics in an urban teaching hospital; the HIV study 32 was conducted with people living with HIV/AIDS (PHAs) enrolled in the HIV Ontario Observational Database (HOOD).
The Sharpe study surveyed individuals receiving care in each of three outpatient clinics (breast cancer, prostate cancer and fracture) of a Canadian teaching hospital. The fracture clinic population not only included patients with fractures, but also patients with spina bifida and low back pain. Potential participants were identified through a daily patient caseload sheet provided by the clinic staff at each site, and approached by the research assistant, using a standard script. They were asked to complete a questionnaire, which included the question on label preference, Problem Solving Decision Making scale and the Trust‐in‐Physician scale. Data collection began in January 1997 and was completed in April 1997.
The inclusion criteria that were used were: patients had to be over 18, speak English, agree to participate, and be attending a clinic that had agreed to participate. This last criterion implies that the participants were patients of clinicians who were comfortable with having their patients approached to participate in this study. Our respondents thus represented the views of the subset of people who used the particular clinics where the study was conducted, and omits individuals whose trust in physicians was so low that they would not have sought out care from a hospital‐based clinic. The study was conducted in Canada; respondents would be fully insured for all hospital and physician care. Using a single cross‐sectional survey also limited our ability to examine changes over time. We recognize that it is likely that individuals captured by our survey were at different stages of their illness trajectory and that individual viewpoints may change over time.
The HIV study drew respondents from the population of individuals enrolled in the HIV Ontario Observational Database (HOOD). HOOD enrolled PHAs from specialty care clinics and primary care practices with a large number of PHAs in the province; these clinics see over 50% of all reported cases of HIV in the province. Enrolment in HOOD was voluntary; however, over 80% of eligible persons agreed to enrol when approached. 33 , 34 Enrolees from 10 specialty care clinics and three primary care practices from across Ontario were surveyed. 32 In accordance with the research ethics requirements of HOOD, questionnaires were distributed only to those enrolled individuals who had previously consented to participate in additional research. To preserve confidentiality, HOOD staff generated a unique set of identifiers for eligible respondents; questionnaires were then pre‐labelled with the unique identifiers and distributed to participating clinics. Patients were not contacted by members of the study team; instead, HOOD staff at the clinics agreed to place the questionnaires in the corresponding patient chart for distribution at the next clinic visit. To further ensure anonymity, no follow‐up was allowed. Inclusion in the study required enrolment in HOOD, the ability to read and complete a questionnaire in English, and a scheduled clinic visit during the study period for questionnaire distribution. Questionnaires were distributed between 19 July 1999 and 3 February 2000.
Ethics approval for both of these studies (including scripts and questionnaires) was obtained from the University of Toronto Office of Research Services.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis employed SPSS‐PC and SAS‐PC. In addition to frequency distributions, a summary score for preference for label was computed by assigning scores of +2/−2 to strongly like/dislike, +1/−1 to moderately like/dislike; and 0 to neutral. The proportion giving each potential response was multiplied by that assigned score, and the total was divided by 200 to normalize the range from −1 to +1. Thus, strong views would approach +1/−1; moderate like/dislike would cluster around +0.5/−0.5, and indifference would approach 0.
Results
For the Sharpe study, of the 611 patients identified by clinic staff as eligible to participate in the study, 606 completed and returned a questionnaire while in the clinic, for a response rate of 99%. Three of the five non‐participating patients were excluded because of language difficulty; there were two refusals. The very high response rate appears to have resulted from a combination of personal distribution of the survey, relatively long waiting times in the clinics and few other distractions.
For the HIV study, a total of 1664 self‐administered questionnaires were sent to 13 sites with HOOD coordinators. The sites reported that 372 potential respondents were either deceased or lost to follow‐up; others may have been, but did not have a scheduled clinic visit over the study period. The sites distributed 809 surveys and obtained 431 responses (53.3% response rate), with no follow‐ups. There were 100 refusals. Respondent demographics were obtained from the HOOD database, and linked by them through anonymous data linkage, using the unique identifier placed on the survey instruments.
Table 1 reports key demographic characteristics of all the samples.
Table 1.
General characteristics of respondents, by clinic
Characteristic | Breast | Prostate | Fracture | HIV |
---|---|---|---|---|
n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | |
Gender | ||||
Male | 0 (0) | 202 (0) | 92 (46.5) | 380 (90.9) |
Female | 202 (100) | 0 (100) | 106 (53.5) | 38 (9.1) |
Total gender | 202 (100) | 202 (100) | 198 (100) | 418 (100) |
Age | ||||
34 and under | 7 (3.5) | 1 (0.5) | 55 (27.9) | 55 (12.8) |
35 to 49 | 55 (27.4) | 15 (7.4) | 63 (32.0) | 280 (65) |
50 to 64 | 89 (44.3) | 64 (31.7) | 54 (27.4) | 85 (19.7) |
65+ | 50 (24.9) | 122 (60.4) | 25 (12.7) | 11 (2.6) |
Total age | 201 (100.1) | 202 (100) | 197 (100) | 431 (100.1) |
Mean age | 55.7 | 66.1 | 46.0 | 42.6 |
Education | ||||
Elementary school | 17 (8.4) | 22 (10.9) | 20 (10.2) | 7 (1.7) |
Some high school | 36 (17.8) | 26 (12.9) | 29 (14.7) | 38 (9.1) |
Graduated high school | 38 (18.8) | 32 (15.8) | 33 (16.8) | 60 (14.3) |
Some post‐secondary | 35 (17.3) | 23 (11.4) | 37 (18.8) | 127 (30.2) |
Completed university/college | 54 (26.7) | 62 (30.7) | 60 (30.5) | 159 (37.9) |
Some/completed | 22 (10.9) | 37 (18.3) | 18 (9.1) | 29 (6.9) |
Total education | 202 (99.9) | 202 (100) | 197 (100.1) | 420 (100.1) |
Health (self‐reported) | ||||
Excellent | 5 (2.5) | 11 (5.5) | 13 (6.5) | 60 (14.2) |
Very good | 38 (19.1) | 42 (20.9) | 63 (31.7) | 131 (30.9) |
Good | 80 (40.2) | 82 (40.8) | 77 (38.7) | 142 (33.5) |
Fair | 59 (29.6) | 52 (25.9) | 37 (18.6) | 82 (19.3) |
Poor | 17 (8.5) | 14 (7.0) | 9 (4.5) | 9 (2.1) |
Total health | 199 (99.9) | 201 (100.1) | 199 (100) | 424 (100) |
Table 2 reports summary scores and distributions by sample. All populations moderately liked the label ‘patient’ (scores between +0.39 and +0.54). No other label went beyond the indifference range, and most terms evoked negative scores in all four populations studied, with particular rejection of the terms ‘customer’ and ‘consumer’. The highest approval for any label other than ‘patient’ came from the HIV population, where the neutral score (0.03) included about one‐third liking the term ‘client’; although the other clinical populations were less likely to endorse this term. Small groups among the HIV population also liked the terms ‘survivor’ and ‘partner’, although more did not.
Table 2.
Summary scores and distribution for preference for label, by clinic
Label | Group | Summary score | Strongly dislike (%) | Mod. dislike (%) | Neutral (%) | Mod. like (%) | Strongly like (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Customer | Breast | −0.69 | 41.1 | 56.4 | 2.5 | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Prostate | −0.72 | 51.7 | 41.8 | 5.5 | 1.0 | 0.0 | |
Fracture | −0.67 | 44.4 | 46.9 | 7.7 | 0.5 | 0.5 | |
HIV | −0.49 | 45.7 | 19.2 | 25.1 | 6.6 | 3.4 | |
Survivor | Breast | −0.38 | 26.7 | 47.5 | 4.0 | 18.3 | 3.5 |
Prostate | −0.69 | 47.3 | 44.8 | 7.5 | 0.5 | 0.0 | |
Fracture | −0.61 | 40.3 | 46.4 | 9.7 | 3.1 | 0.5 | |
HIV | −0.34 | 45.1 | 15.1 | 15.8 | 10.7 | 13.2 | |
Consumer | Breast | −0.57 | 27.2 | 62.9 | 5.9 | 4.0 | 0.0 |
Prostate | −0.67 | 45.3 | 44.8 | 9.0 | 1.0 | 0.0 | |
Fracture | −0.61 | 36.7 | 52.6 | 7.7 | 3.1 | 0.0 | |
HIV | −0.41 | 38.6 | 21.1 | 28.5 | 7.6 | 4.2 | |
Partner | Breast | −0.44 | 26.2 | 52.0 | 5.9 | 14.9 | 1.0 |
Prostate | −0.63 | 44.8 | 42.3 | 8.0 | 5.0 | 0.0 | |
Fracture | −0.53 | 32.7 | 50.0 | 11.2 | 3.6 | 2.6 | |
HIV | −0.07 | 25.3 | 11.8 | 31.8 | 13.5 | 17.5 | |
Client | Breast | −0.41 | 24.8 | 46.0 | 14.9 | 14.4 | 0.0 |
Prostate | −0.58 | 38.8 | 42.8 | 14.9 | 3.5 | 0.0 | |
Fracture | −0.45 | 27.6 | 45.4 | 16.3 | 10.2 | 0.5 | |
HIV | 0.03 | 14.4 | 13.9 | 35.0 | 25.1 | 11.7 | |
Patient | Breast | 0.39 | 2.0 | 9.9 | 8.9 | 66.8 | 12.4 |
Prostate | 0.54 | 0.5 | 1.0 | 8.0 | 71.1 | 19.4 | |
Fracture | 0.50 | 2.6 | 2.0 | 12.8 | 57.7 | 25.0 | |
HIV | 0.48 | 1.4 | 5.4 | 29.0 | 24.5 | 39.6 |
Discussion
The results show that the respondents from the four clinical populations tended to reject most of the labels suggested to replace ‘patient’. It should be noted that this did not constitute a rejection of involvement in treatment decision making; indeed, in our own work, we have found a consistently high desire among these and similar populations we have surveyed to be involved in making treatment choices. 29 , 32 , 35 , 36 , 37 , 38
At the micro level, we would argue that this label preference is appropriate; sick individuals are indeed patients, rather than ‘customers’ or ‘consumers’, and our results accordingly cannot say whether otherwise healthy individuals encountering the medical system for such services as screening might indeed endorse such labels. It must also be recognized that the term ‘patient’ tends to be moderately preferred, rather than achieve strong support. In that connection, the critics are also right. The modern patient is no longer patient in the sense of waiting without complaint, or blindly following doctors’ orders. As has been noted, people expect rapid delivery of pizzas, and will no longer tolerate waiting for services they believe they ‘need’. 39 Accommodating these higher expectations will not be simple. Yet our results suggest that the individuals we surveyed still place high value on a relationship with their providers that is based on a model other than that between buyer and seller. It seems to be captured by the label ‘patient’.
Acknowledgements
The Sharpe study was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, grant 410970299. The HIV study was supported by grants from the Ontario HIV Treatment Network, The Health Evidence Applications and Linkage Networks, and by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Training Award to Sara Urowitz.
References
- 1. Childress JF. The normative principles of medical ethics In: Veatch RM, (ed.) Medical Ethics. Boston, MA: Jones and Bartlett, 1989: 27–48. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Saunders P. Encouraging patients to take part in their own care. Nursing Times, 1995; 91: 42–43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Kassirer JP. Incorporating patients’ preferences into medical decisions. New England Journal of Medicine, 1994; 330: 1895–1896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Veatch RM. The Patient‐Physician Relation: The Patient as Partner, Part 2. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 5. Pellegrino ED. Patient and physician autonomy: conflicting rights and obligations in the physician–patient relationship. The Journal of Contemporary Health Law and Policy, 1994; 10: 47–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Neuberger J. Let's do away with patients. British Medical Journal, 1999; 318: 1756–1758. 10381717 [Google Scholar]
- 7. Tallis R. Leave well enough alone. British Medical Journal, 1999; 318: 1756–1758. 10381717 [Google Scholar]
- 8. Thode R. Do we need a new word for patients? ‘Users’ isn't an appropriate alternative. British Medical Journal, 1999; 319: 1436. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Kernick D. Do we need a new word for patients? Both patients and doctors should be known as actors. British Medical Journal, 1999; 319: 1437. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Stevens R. Do we need a new word for patients? Patients should obviously now be called ‘Neubergers’. British Medical Journal, 1999; 319: 1436–1437. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Bain W. Do we need a new word for patients? Continue to call them patients but treat them like customers. British Medical Journal, 1999; 319: 1436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Acar M. Do we need a new word for patients? What's in a name, after all? British Medical Journal, 1999; 319: 1437. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Bland JM. Patients or Employers. BMJ rapid response . Available at: http://bmj.bmjjournals.com/cgi/eletters/319/7222/1436/a#5644, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 14. Herxheimer A, Goodare H. Who are you, and who are we? Looking through some key words. Health Expectations, 1999; 2: 3–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Lloyd C, King R, Bassett H, Sandland S, Savige G. Patient, client or consumer? A survey of preferred terms. Australasian Psychiatry, 2001; 9: 321–324. [Google Scholar]
- 16. Mueser KT, Glynn SM, Corrigan PW, Baber W. A survey of preferred terms for users of mental health services. American Psychiatric Association, 1996; 47: 760–761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Sharma V, Whitney D, Kazarian SS, Manchanda R. Preferred terms for users of mental health services among service providers and recipients. Psychiatric Services, 2000; 51: 203–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Nair B. Patient, client or customer? Medical Journal of Australia, 1998; 169: 593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Wing PC. Patient or client? If in doubt, ask. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 1997; 157: 287–289. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Merriam W, ed. Merriam‐Webster's Online Dictionary. 2004. Available at: http://www.m‐w.com/dictionary. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Pearsall J, ed. The Concise Oxford Dictionary. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2001. Available at: http://oxfordreference.com. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Abate F, ed. The Oxford American Dictionary of Current English. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2002. Available at: http://oxfordreference.com. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Simpson J, ed. OED Online, 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2004. Available at: http://oxfordreference.com. [Google Scholar]
- 24. Deber RB. Getting what we pay for: myths and realities about financing Canada's health care system. Health Law in Canada, 2000; 21: 9–56. [Google Scholar]
- 25. Coutler A. Whatever happened to shared decision‐making? Health Expectations, 2002; 5: 185–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Kravitz RL, Melnikow J. Engaging patients in medical decision making: the end is worthwhile, but the means need to be more practical. British Medical Journal, 2001; 323: 584–585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Coulter A. After Bristol: putting patients at the centre. British Medical Journal, 2002; 324: 648–651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Say RE, Thomson R. The importance of patient preferences in treatment decisions‐challenges for doctors. British Medical Journal, 2003; 327: 542–545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Kraetschmer N, Sharpe N, Urowitz S, Deber RB. How does trust affect patient preferences for participation in decision making? Health Expectations, 2004; 7: 317–326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Towle A, Godolphin W. Framework for teaching and learning informed shared decision making. British Medical Journal, 1999; 319: 766–771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Entwistle VA. Participation: if we want to make progress, we must speak clearly. Health Expectations, 1999; 2: 1–2 (editorial). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Urowitz S, Deber RB, Myers T. Autonomy and decision‐making: patients’ preferences. Canadian Journal of Infectious Diseases, 2001; 12(Suppl. B): 36B. [Google Scholar]
- 33. Millson ME, Peter A, Rachlis A, Robinson G. The HIV Ontario Observational Database. Ontario Medical Review, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 34. Millson M, Robinson G, Rachlis A, Palmer R, Galli R, Major C. Representing diversity in an observational database: the current demographics of HOOD enrollees. Canadian Journal of Infectious Diseases, 1998; 9(Suppl. A): 39A (abstract). [Google Scholar]
- 35. Deber RB, Kraetschmer N, Irvine J. What role do patients wish to play in treatment decision making? Archives of Internal Medicine, 1996; 156: 1414–1420. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Deber RB. Physicians in health care management: 7. The patient‐physician partnership: changing roles, and the desire for information. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 1994; 151: 171–176. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Deber RB. Physicians in health care management: 8. The patient‐physician partnership: decision making, problem solving, and the desire to participate. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 1994; 151: 423–427. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Stewart DE, Rosen B, Irvine J et al. The disconnect: infertility patients’ information and the role they wish to play in decision making. Medscape Womens Health, 2001; 6: 1. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Decter M. Four Strong Winds: Understanding the Growing Challenges to Health Care. Toronto: Stoddart, 2000. [Google Scholar]