Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016 Aug;25(4):239–245. doi: 10.1177/0963721416655883

Beyond Cumulative Risk: A Dimensional Approach to Childhood Adversity

Katie A McLaughlin 1, Margaret A Sheridan 2
PMCID: PMC5070918  NIHMSID: NIHMS791976  PMID: 27773969

Abstract

Children who have experienced environmental adversity—such as abuse, neglect, or poverty—are more likely to develop physical and mental health problems, perform poorly at school, and have difficulties in social relationships than children who have not encountered adversity. What is less clear is how and why adverse early experiences exert such a profound influence on children’s development. Identifying developmental processes that are disrupted by adverse early environments is the key to developing better intervention strategies for children who have experienced adversity. Yet, much existing research relies on a cumulative risk approach that is unlikely to reveal these mechanisms. This approach tallies the number of distinct adversities experienced to create a risk score. This risk score fails to distinguish between distinct types of environmental experience, implicitly assuming that very different experiences influence development through the same underlying mechanisms. We advance an alternative model. This novel approach conceptualizes adversity along distinct dimensions, emphasizes the central role of learning mechanisms, and distinguishes between different forms of adversity that might influence learning in distinct ways. A key advantage of this approach is that learning mechanisms provide clear targets for interventions aimed at preventing negative developmental outcomes in children who have experienced adversity.

Keywords: childhood adversity, cumulative risk, stress, trauma, deprivation, learning, abuse, neglect, poverty


Few people dispute the notion that adverse environmental experiences in childhood—such as exposure to violence and chronic poverty—create a lasting imprint on emotion, cognition, behavior, and chances for success in adulthood. Children who have experienced adversity are more likely to develop psychopathology and chronic diseases, perform poorly at school, and have social and economic difficulties than children who have not encountered adversity (Felitti et al., 1998; Lansford et al., 2002; McLaughlin et al., 2012). What is less clear is how and why adverse early experiences exert such a profound influence on children’s development. Identifying the developmental processes that are disrupted by adverse early environments is the key to developing better intervention strategies to prevent the onset of problems in children who have experienced adversity.

Yet, most current research is not designed in a way that can reveal mechanisms linking childhood adversity with developmental outcomes. We argue that the prevailing approach for conceptualizing and measuring childhood adversity is, at best, not suited for studying mechanisms and, at worst, obscuring them. We first describe current approaches to studying childhood adversity and the dominant perspective on stress as the central mechanism linking these experiences with downstream outcomes. Next, we advance an alternative model for studying mechanisms linking childhood adversity with psychopathology and other developmental outcomes. This novel approach conceptualizes adversity along distinct dimensions, emphasizes the central role of learning mechanisms, and distinguishes between different forms of childhood adversity that might influence learning in distinct ways. A key advantage of our approach is that learning mechanisms provide clear targets for intervention.

THE PREVAILING APPROACH: CUMULATIVE RISK

Until recently, most research focused on single types of adversity, such as physical abuse, parental death, or poverty. A critical limitation of this approach is that it does not account for the fact that most children who have been exposed to one type of adversity have also experienced numerous others (Green et al., 2010; McLaughlin et al., 2012). When examining single types of adversity it is impossible to determine whether a particular outcome (e.g., depression) is a consequence of the focal adversity of interest (e.g., physical abuse) or of other adversities the child experienced (e.g., poverty, neglect).

Recognition of the high co-occurrence of adversities led to a shift to the prevailing cumulative risk approach. This approach tallies the number of adversities experienced to create a risk score (Evans, Li, & Whipple, 2013). For example, a child who experienced physical abuse, sexual abuse, and domestic violence would have a risk score of three; a child who experienced poverty, neglect, and maternal depression would also have a risk score of three. Cumulative risk thus focuses on the number of distinct adverse experiences rather than the severity or type of adversity (Evans et al., 2013).

The cumulative risk approach has been widely adopted and proved useful for highlighting the public health importance of childhood adversity. Risk scores also can be used as a screening tool to identify children in greatest need of intervention. However, the cumulative risk approach has significant limitations when used to identify mechanisms linking childhood adversity with developmental outcomes. Most notably, cumulative risk scores fail to distinguish between distinct types of environmental experience, implicitly assuming that all adverse experiences influence development through the same underlying mechanisms. In other words, risk scores assume that physical abuse, sexual abuse, and domestic violence influence children’s development in exactly the same way as poverty, neglect, and maternal depression. This assumption is highly tenuous.

Stress Response System Dysregulation as the Central Mechanism

Is it possible that the vastly different social and environmental experiences encompassed by the construct of childhood adversity each influence development through the same underlying mechanisms? Advocates of cumulative risk argue that disruptions in the regulation of stress response systems represent this common mechanism (Evans et al., 2013). Specifically, allostatic load has been proposed as the process that explains how numerous forms of seemingly disparate adverse experiences influence the wide range of developmental outcomes associated with childhood adversity. The concept of allostatic load has been reviewed extensively elsewhere (McEwen, 2012; McEwen & Gianaros, 2010). Briefly, physiological regulatory systems—including the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and the autonomic nervous system (ANS)—respond to changing environmental demands, producing physiological changes that are adaptive in the short-term but maladaptive in the long-term. For example, chronic release of glucocorticoids leads to rapid improvements in immunity but maladaptive long-term changes in brain regions with high concentrations of glucocorticoid receptors, including the hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex (PFC) (McEwen, 2012). This long-term wear-and-tear resulting from chronic adaptation to stress is referred to as allostatic load. Extensive evidence suggests that adverse early environments disrupt stress response system functioning (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). These disruptions are the central mechanism explaining downstream consequences of adversity in the cumulative risk model.

Dysregulation in stress response systems is clearly one pathway linking childhood adversity with developmental outcomes. But are stress pathways a universal mechanism? There are several problems with this assumption. First, associations of childhood adversity with stress response system functioning are inconsistent. Although numerous forms of adversity are associated with HPA axis and ANS function, the specific nature of these associations varies widely across studies. The most commonly observed pattern involves blunted reactivity to environmental demands and globally reduced output (Gunnar & Vazquez, 2001; McLaughlin et al., 2015). However, numerous studies document the opposite pattern—elevated reactivity or globally increased output (Fries, Shirtcliff, & Pollak, 2008; Gunnar, Morison, Chisolm, & Schuder, 2001).

Second, stress response system dysregulation is inconsistently associated with developmental outcomes. Disruptions in stress response systems are clearly involved in the onset of chronic physical health problems (Heim, Ehlert, & Helhammer, 2000). But they do not explain many disturbances in cognitive and social development commonly observed among children who experienced adversity. For example, children exposed to neglect and poverty often have deficits in language abilities (Farah et al., 2006; Hildyard & Wolfe, 2002). There is no obvious link between stress response system dysregulation and language ability. Other mechanisms must be involved.

Finally, this pathway provides little in the way of intervention targets. How might we intervene to prevent the downstream consequences of childhood adversity based on the allostatic load model other than attempting to prevent exposure to adversity in the first place? Although psychosocial interventions can influence cortisol regulation, the direction of these effects is remarkably inconsistent (Slopen, McLaughlin, & Shonkoff, 2014). Few effective intervention approaches for children exposed to adversity have been developed a result of cumulative risk models or stress dysregulation mechanisms. Thus, although these models are useful in identifying children in need of intervention, they provide little guidance about how to intervene.

A NOVEL APPROACH: DIMENSIONS OF ADVERSITY

We have proposed an alternative model to facilitate the identification of developmental processes disrupted by childhood adversity other than the frequently-invoked stress pathways (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). Identifying these mechanisms is critical for intervention development.

Our approach attempts to distill complex adverse experiences into core underlying dimensions that cut across multiple forms of adversity. Here, we focus on two specific dimensions of adversity whose influences on emotional, cognitive, and neurobiological development are at least partially distinct. Specifically, our model differentiates between experiences of threat—experiences involving harm or threat of harm, and deprivation—experiences involving an absence of expected inputs from the environment (see Figure 1). Each dimension encompasses numerous experiences that reflect the underlying dimension to varying degrees. Experiences involving threat include observing community violence, witnessing domestic violence, and being the victim of chronic physical abuse. These experiences vary in the severity of threat involved, but all involve harm or threat of harm. Examples of deprivation include poverty, neglect, and institutional rearing, each of which involve an absence of expected cognitive inputs (e.g., complex language), social stimulation, and consistent interactions with adults to varying degrees.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

A dimensional model of childhood adversity involving two central dimensions of threat and deprivation. Examples of commonly studied forms of adversity are placed along these dimensions based on the degree to which the experience typically involves threat and deprivation. Larger circles indicate greater variance in the degree to which the experience reflects the underlying dimension.

Rather than counting the total number of adversities, our approach assesses the frequency and severity of experiences reflecting each dimension and examines them simultaneously in predicting developmental outcomes. This approach retains many benefits of cumulative risk and provides additional advantages. Most notably, it can identify developmental mechanisms that are specific to certain dimensions of adversity and not others and determine whether such mechanisms vary in relation to severity of exposure.

Beyond Stress: The Importance of Learning

Our model focuses on the central role of learning in explaining myriad developmental consequences of adversity not fully accounted for by stress pathways. Learning is a central process through which the environment shapes emotion, cognition, and behavior. Emerging research shows that learning processes are influenced by childhood adversity, that at least some of these associations vary across types of adversity, and that disruptions in learning are a mechanism in the link between adversity and developmental outcomes.

Emotional learning processes are particularly important mechanisms in this regard. Associative learning processes detect environmental cues associated with threat and reward and shape emotion, behavior, and neurobiological responses to those cues. Fear learning processes rapidly detect potential threats and mobilize resources to respond (Delgado, Olsson, & Phelps, 2006). These processes govern acquisition of fear to stimuli associated with threat, extinction of fear following repeated stimulus encounters in the absence of threat, and competition between fear and extinction memories based on context. Reward learning processes track the probability and magnitude of reward associated with particular cues and influence feelings of pleasure during anticipation and receipt of rewards as well as actions toward future rewards (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008).

These forms of learning share basic neurobiological pathways with other associative learning processes such as pattern learning. Pattern learning refers to the detection of regularities in the environment when these regularities are not linked with rewards or punishments (e.g., that cue-A always follows cue-B). Pattern learning plays a central role in language acquisition and expertise (Romberg & Saffran, 2010; Spencer, Kaschak, Jones, & Lonigan, 2015).

Higher-order learning processes such as long-term memory and executive functions are also important candidate mechanisms explaining downstream consequences of adversity. In contrast to associative learning involving automatic processes present from early in development, long-term memory and executive functions are explicit learning skills where effort and strategy can be employed to improve performance and develop throughout childhood and adolescence.

Childhood Adversity Influences Learning

Accumulating evidence shows that childhood adversity influences learning and that disruptions in learning processes are associated with downstream developmental outcomes, including psychopathology.

Exposure to threat is a core dimension of our model. We argue that disruptions in fear learning are an important mechanism through which threatening environments influence later development. Recent work from our labs is consistent with this prediction, finding atypical fear conditioning among children who experienced environmental threats, including abuse and domestic violence. Children exposed to threat demonstrate poor discrimination of threat and safety cues during fear-conditioning (McLaughlin et al., in press). Whereas children without adversity exposure exhibit stronger fear responses to a stimulus paired with threat compared to one paired with safety, children exposed to threat exhibit fear responses of similar magnitude to threat and safety cues (McLaughlin et al., in press), reflecting either generalization of fear to the safety cue or a generalized problem with associative learning. This pattern is specific to threat exposure, is not observed following deprivation, and explains the association of threat exposure with externalizing psychopathology.

Atypical reward learning has also been observed in children exposed to adversity, particularly those who experienced deprivation. In reward learning tasks, typically-developing children are faster and more accurate in responding to cues associated with high reward; children raised in deprived institutional settings fail to show this pattern (Sheridan, McLaughlin, Fox, Zeanah, & Nelson, under review). Disruptions in the neural circuitry that supports reward learning has been found in institutionally-reared children (Mehta et al., 2010), and adolescents who experienced neglect (Hanson, Hariri, & Williamson, 2015). It is possible that disruptions in reward learning emerge after other forms of adversity. Atypical reward learning and neural response to reward has been found following child maltreatment (Dillon et al., 2009; Guyer et al., 2006). Because these studies did not distinguish between abuse and neglect, however, it is unknown whether reward learning is influenced only by deprivation or also by threat. Across studies, disruptions in reward processing and underlying neural circuitry explain the link between deprivation and depression (Hanson et al., 2015; Sheridan et al., under review).

Finally, disturbances in pattern learning, language, and executive functions have been found in children exposed to deprivation. Deficits in language and executive functions have been observed consistently among children raised in poverty (Noble, McCandliss, & Farah, 2007) and in deprived institutional settings (Tibu et al., 2016; Windsor et al., 2011). These deficits explain the link between institutional rearing and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Tibu et al., 2016). Emerging evidence suggests that deprivation might also influence pattern learning (Sheridan et al., under review) and long-term memory (Sheridan, How, Araujo, Schamberg, & Nelson, 2013), although greater work is needed in these areas.

Implications for Intervention

What does a learning perspective provide that stress models do not? Perhaps the most important advantage is that it provides clear targets for intervention. Behavioral interventions directly targeting emotional learning have been developed, evaluated, and refined for decades. These intervention techniques form the backbone of most empirically-supported psychosocial treatments for anxiety, depression, behavior problems, and substance abuse in youth (Chorpita & Daleiden, 2009). For example, exposure-based interventions for anxiety facilitate extinction learning when that process does not occur naturally (Waters & Pine, in press). Behavioral activation for depression aims to increase motivation for reward through repeated engagement in pleasurable activities. Behavioral interventions thus target the precise learning mechanisms that appear to be disrupted following childhood adversity. Intervention development to improve memory and executive functions is a burgeoning area of research (Shipstead, Redick, & Engle, 2012).

Although behavioral interventions are effective in treating psychopathology, little research has examined their utility in preventing psychopathology onset in children who experienced adversity. However, a recent study highlights the promise of behavioral interventions in this regard. A brief intervention providing behavioral skill training to children who experienced traumatic violence prevented the onset of post-traumatic stress disorder and anxiety 3-months later (Berkowitz, Stover, & Marans, 2011). Greater research on the efficacy of brief behavioral interventions in preventing psychopathology following childhood adversity represents a critical next step for the field.

CONCLUSION

There is little debate about the pervasive detrimental influence of childhood adversity on developmental outcomes. Similarly, broad consensus exists about the importance of developing effective interventions to prevent the downstream consequences of adversity. Although the prevailing cumulative risk approach is useful for identifying children in need of intervention, it has done little to shed light on how to intervene. A dimensional approach focused on examining how specific types of adversity influence learning and other mechanisms may hold greater promise in this regard. This approach is likely to reveal that some mechanisms are common across multiple dimensions of adversity and some are unique to particular experiences. Identifying these mechanisms and their specificity to particular forms of adversity is critical to inform the development of efficient, effective interventions to prevent the negative developmental consequences of childhood adversity.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (R01-MH103291 to McLaughlin).

References

  1. Berkowitz SJ, Stover CS, Marans SR. The child and family traumatic stress intervention: Secondary prevention for youth at risk of developing PTSD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:676–685. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02321.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199:457–480. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1099-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Chorpita BF, Daleiden EL. Mapping evidence-based treatments for children and adolescents: application of the distillation and matching model to 615 treatments from 322 randomized trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009;77:566–579. doi: 10.1037/a0014565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Delgado MR, Olsson A, Phelps EA. Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans. Biological Psychology. 2006;73:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Dillon DG, Holmes AJ, Birk JL, Brooks N, Lyons-Ruth K, Pizzagalli DA. Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66(3):206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Evans GW, Li D, Whipple SS. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139:1342–1396. doi: 10.1037/a0031808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Farah MJ, Shera DM, Savage JH, Betancourt L, Gianetta JM, Brodsky NL, Malmud EK, Hurt H. Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research. 2006;1110:166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Koss MP, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1998;14:245–258. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Fries ABW, Shirtcliff EA, Pollak SD. Neuroendocrine dysregulation following early social deprivation in children. Developmental Psychobiology. 2008;50:588–599. doi: 10.1002/dev.20319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund P, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;62:113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gunnar MR, Morison SJ, Chisolm K, Schuder M. Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:611–628. doi: 10.1017/s095457940100311x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gunnar MR, Quevedo K. The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:145–173. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gunnar MR, Vazquez DM. Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: potential indices of risk in human development. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13(3):515–538. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Guyer AE, Kaufman J, Hodgdon HB, Masten CL, Jazbec S, Pine DS, Ernst M. Behavioral alterations in reward system function: the role of childhood maltreatment and psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:1059–1067. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000227882.50404.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hanson JL, Hariri AR, Williamson DE. Blunted ventral striatum development in adolescence reflects emotional neglect and predicts depressive symptoms. Biological Psychiatry. 2015;78:598–605. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Heim C, Ehlert U, Helhammer DH. The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;25:1–35. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(99)00035-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hildyard KL, Wolfe DA. Child neglect: developmental issues and outcomes. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2002;26:679–695. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00341-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lansford JE, Dodge KA, Petit GS, Bates JE, Crozier JC, Kaplow JB. A 12-year prospective study of the long-term effects of early child physical maltreatment on psychological, behavioral, and academic problems in adolescence. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2002;156:824–830. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.156.8.824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. McEwen BS. Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:17180–17185. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121254109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ. Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: links to socioeconomic status, health, and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1186:190–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05331.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky A, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and first onset of psychiatric disorders in a national sample of adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69:1151–1160. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Gold AL, Lambert HK, Heleniak C, Duys A, Shechner T, Wojcieski Z, Pine DS. Maltreatment exposure, brain structure, and fear conditioning in children. Neuropsychopharmacology. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.365. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Lambert HK. Childhood Adversity and Neural Development: Deprivation and Threat as Distinct Dimensions of Early Experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;47:578–591. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Tibu F, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Causal effects of the early caregiving environment on stress response system development in children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112:5637–5642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423363112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mehta MA, Gore-Langton E, Golembo N, Colvert E, Williams SCR, Sonuga-Barke E. Hyporesponsive reward anticipation in the basal ganglia following severe institutional deprivation early in life. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22(10):2316–2325. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science. 2007;10:464–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Romberg AR, Saffran JR. Statistical learning and language acquisition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 2010;1:906–914. doi: 10.1002/wcs.78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Sheridan MA, How J, Araujo M, Schamberg MA, Nelson CA. What are the links between maternal social status, hippocampal function, and HPA axis function in children? Developmental Science. 2013;16:665–675. doi: 10.1111/desc.12087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Dimensions of Early Experience and Neural Development: Deprivation and Threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18:580–585. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Early caregiving environment causally impacts reward response and neural structure, mediating risk for depression in adolescence under review. [Google Scholar]
  31. Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Engle RW. Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin. 2012;138:628–654. doi: 10.1037/a0027473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Slopen N, McLaughlin KA, Shonkoff JP. Interventions to improve cortisol regulation in children: A systematic review. Pediatrics. 2014;133:312–326. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-1632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Spencer M, Kaschak MP, Jones JL, Lonigan CJ. Statistical learning is related to early literacy-related skills. Reading and writing. 2015;28:467–490. doi: 10.1007/s11145-014-9533-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Tibu F, Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Disruptions of working memory and inhibition mediate the association between exposure to institutionalization and symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2016;46:529–541. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Waters AM, Pine DS. Evaluating differences in Pavlovian fear acquisition and extinction as predictors of outcome from cognitive behavioural therapy for anxious children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12522. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Windsor J, Benigno JP, Wing CA, Carroll PJ, Koga SF, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. Effect of foster care on young children’s language learning. Child Development. 2011;82(4):1040–1046. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01604.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Recommended Readings

  • 1.McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Lambert HK. Childhood Adversity and Neural Development: Deprivation and Threat as Distinct Dimensions of Early Experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;47:578–591. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012. This paper provides a comprehensive review of the dimensional model of childhood adversity. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Gold AL, Lambert HK, Heleniak C, Duys A, Shechner T, Wojcieski Z, Pine DS. Maltreatment exposure, brain structure, and fear conditioning in children. Neuropsychopharmacology. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.365. in press. This paper examines fear learning mechanisms in children exposed to threatening early environments. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Evans GW, Li D, Whipple SS. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139:1342–1396. doi: 10.1037/a0031808. A comprehensive review of the prevailing cumulative risk approach; an approach distinct from the one we advance in this paper. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.McEwen BS. Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:17180–17185. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121254109. An excellent and accessible review of the allostatic load model. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES