Abstract
Objective
The aim of this study was to characterise the association between the cognitive profile and weight restoration in children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa.
Methods
The study was a longitudinal, matched case–control, multicentre study. An assessment of cognitive functions was conducted by using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–III/the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–III, the Test of Memory and Learning–second edition, Trail Making Tests A and B, the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test and the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery.
Results
One hundred twenty individuals, 60 patients with anorexia nervosa with mean age of 14.65 (SD 1.820) years and 60 healthy controls with mean age of 14.76 (SD 1.704) years, participated. No association was found between weight recovery and cognitive functions. However, a significant increase in motor speed was found in Trail Making Test A (p = 0.004), Reaction Time (RTI) five‐choice movement time (p = 0.002) and RTI simple movement time (p = 0.011), resulting in a normalisation corresponding to that found in healthy controls. Furthermore, a significantly lower score in the perceptual organization index (p = 0.029) was found at follow‐up.
Conclusions
Weight recovery appears not to be associated with cognition. Copyright © 2016 The Authors European Eating Disorders Review published by Eating Disorders Association and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Keywords: anorexia nervosa, cognitive functions, children, adolescents, follow‐up, weight recovery, weight status
Introduction
Anorexia nervosa (AN) is characterised by restrictive eating behaviour, a refusal to maintain normal body weight, a disturbed body image and an intense fear of becoming fat or gaining weight (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The lifetime prevalence of AN is 0.16–0.30% (Bulik et al., 2006; Hudson, Hiripi, Pope, & Kessler, 2007; Raevuori et al., 2009a; Raevuori, Niemela, Keski‐Rahkonen, & Sourander, 2009b; Woodside et al., 2001), and the standardised mortality ratio in severe AN is 8.1 (95% CI = 1.6–23.6) in men and 10.6 (95% CI = 7.6–14.4) in women (Gueguen et al., 2012).
Cognitive inefficiencies have been identified in adult patients with AN, particularly with regard to set‐shifting abilities and central coherence, that is, the ability to switch between tasks, and a detailed information processing style and difficulty in seeing the big picture (Holliday, Tchanturia, Landau, Collier, & Treasure, 2005; Lang, Lopez, Stahl, Tchanturia & Treasure, 2014b; Tchanturia et al., 2004; Tchanturia et al., 2011; Tchanturia et al., 2012).
In contrast, the picture is less clear with respect to the cognitive profile of children and adolescents with AN. Several studies have found no evidence of cognitive inefficiencies (Buhren et al., 2012; Calderoni et al., 2013; Lang, Stahl, Espie, Treasure & Tchanturia, 2014a; Sarrar et al., 2011), while some have found inefficiencies in visuoconstruction set‐shifting abilities, sensory‐motor speed, reaction time and nonverbal intelligence of patients with AN (Andres‐Perpina et al., 2011; Hatch et al., 2010; Kjaersdam Telleus et al., 2015; Lange & Tchanturia, 2014; Lang et al., 2015; Rose, Frampton, & Lask, 2013; Stedal, Frampton, Landro, & Lask, 2012).
A mean full‐scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) of 101.1 (SD 16), which is close to the normative population mean of 100, was found in the largest study of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN to date (Kjaersdam Telleus et al., 2015). Thus, this study showed no evidence of an elevated IQ above the normal mean in the AN group, which is consistent with the results of several other studies (Bayless et al., 2002; Buhren et al., 2012; Gillberg, Gillberg, Rastam, & Johansson, 1996; Lang et al., 2015; McDowell et al., 2003).
There are inconclusive findings on differences in cognitive functions between the AN subtypes. In a systematic review, Van Autreve and Vervaet (2015) found that group differences in central coherence and set shifting between AN‐restricting type and AN‐bingeing/purging type were not strong enough to draw definitive conclusions.
Longitudinal studies of cognitive functions in adult patients with AN have been conducted. Cavedini et al. (2006) found that decision‐making performance in inpatients with AN did not improve significantly from admission to discharge. Likewise, Bodell et al. (2014) found no significant difference in decision‐making performance from baseline to weight restoration in a sample of AN patients. However, a significant improvement after weight restoration was found in the subgroup of patients with AN who were poor performers at baseline (Bodell et al., 2014). In a longitudinal study on young adults, Zwipp et al. (2014) found that motor speed increased with weight gain in patients with AN.
Only a few follow‐up studies of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN have been conducted (Buehren et al., 2011; Buhren et al., 2012; Hatch et al., 2010). In a follow‐up study conducted among juvenile patients with AN, it was found that when patients with AN had a low body weight, they performed significantly worse than control participants in tests of motor speed; these inefficiencies appeared to normalise with weight gain (Hatch et al., 2010). Another study showed an increased reaction time when engaging in set‐shifting tasks and a reduced error rate across time (Buhren et al., 2012).
Consequently, there is limited knowledge about how the cognitive profile of children and adolescents with AN progresses over time. As children and adolescents undergo cognitive maturation, it is important to examine how the cognitive status and development of children and adolescents with AN progress and to expand our knowledge of the impact that low body weight and other AN symptoms may have on cognition. Likewise, it is relevant to enhance our knowledge regarding the cognitive profile in children and adolescents with AN in comparison with that found in adult patients with AN.
Expanding our understanding of how cognitive development is affected as patients with AN recover or become chronic is best achieved by using longitudinal studies.
The aim of this study was to characterise the development of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN. Another aim was to examine the association between cognition and weight recovery in AN patients. This aim was accomplished through the re‐examination of children and adolescents with AN 1 year after baseline, when the patients were first assessed in the study. The hypotheses of the study were as follows.
The maturation of cognitive function, that is, intelligence and specific cognitive functions, in children and adolescents with AN is significantly reduced compared with that of matched healthy controls (HCs).
The change in intelligence and specific cognitive functions of patients who recover weight is significantly different from that of patients who do not recover normal body weight at 1 year follow‐up.
The baseline cognitive profile is a predictor of weight recovery operationalised by obtaining a body mass index (BMI) above the AN threshold, that is BMI above the WHO 15th percentile, corresponding to a BMI of 18.5 or above at age 18.
Methods
Participants
Study recruitment of patients with AN at baseline took place between January 2009 and December 2011, while recruitment of HCs at baseline was completed in December 2012. One‐year follow‐up assessment was completed in December 2013. At baseline, patients were recruited after being diagnosed with AN in accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM‐5) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
The inclusion criterion for patients was less than 18 years of age and diagnosed with AN in one of the participating centres. The exclusion criterion for patients was cognitive exam with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) within the last year. The inclusion criterion for HCs was less than 18 years of age. The exclusion criterion for HCs was cognitive exam with the WISC or WAIS within the last year and/or known psychiatric illness.
At baseline, the AN group and the HCs were matched 1:1 for gender and age (i.e., within 1 year). A total of 188 age‐ and gender‐matched participants were originally recruited for the study: patients with AN (N = 94) and HCs (N = 94). Of these participants, 60 matched pairs, a total of 120 individuals, participated 1 year later in the follow‐up study. Thus, the follow‐up participation rate was 64%. Of the 120 participants, 110 were female and 10 were male. The main reason that patients dropped out of the study was that they left treatment and wanted no additional contact with mental health services. This article includes the baseline and follow‐up data on these 60 matched pairs. Between baseline and follow‐up, the patients received treatment consisting of specialised psychotherapeutic eating disorder intervention. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. It was approved by the local Research Ethics Committee (file number: N‐20080060MCH) and authorised by the local Data Protection Agency. Participation was voluntary. Patients were informed that nonparticipation would not affect their treatment. Written and oral information about the study was provided before recruitment, and written informed consent was obtained in all cases.
A detailed description of the recruitment is included the paper of Kjaersdam Telleus et al. (2015).
Measures
Baseline
Assessment battery
AN symptomatology was assessed by using Eating Disorder Examination (EDE) version 16 (Cooper, Cooper, & Fairburn, 1989; Williamson, Barker, Bertman, & Gleaves, 1995). A structured examination of each patient's somatic condition was also conducted. This examination involved a collection of clinical observations and patient‐reported somatic symptoms according to generally recognised somatic complications in AN (Hebebrand & Bulik, 2011; Mitchell & Crow, 2006), including electrocardiogram recordings and clinical and paraclinical assessments, that is biochemical laboratory tests.
A parental interview was conducted to collect anamnestic data. Assessment of comorbidity was conducted by using the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Present and Lifetime Version (Cooper et al., 1989; Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Ryan, & Rao, 2000; Wilfley, Schwartz, Spurrell, & Fairburn, 2000), in addition to the Autism Screening Questionnaire (Ehlers, Gillberg, & Wing, 1999).
The healthy controls were screened for indications of psychiatric disorders during recruitment by using the Autism Screening Questionnaire (Ehlers et al., 1999) and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997), which is a brief behavioural screening questionnaire.
The assessment of cognitive functions was conducted by using a comprehensive neurocognitive assessment battery. The HCs and the patients with AN were assessed with an identical cognitive assessment battery in a fixed order. The neurocognitive assessment battery included the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–III (WISC‐III) (Wechsler, 1991) or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐III (WAIS‐III) (Wechsler, 1997), depending on age, both Danish versions. The Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (RCFT) (Meyers & Meyers, 1995), pen‐and‐paper version, and Trail Making Tests A (TMT‐A) and B (TMT‐B) (Lezak, 2012; Reitan & Wolfson, 1985), pen‐and‐paper version, were also administered.
Nine subtests from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) (Lowe & Rabbitt, 1998) and four subtests from the Test of Memory and Learning–second edition (TOMAL‐2) (Reynolds & Bigler, 1994) were also administered. A detailed description of the assessment battery used in the study is included in the paper of Kjaersdam Telleus et al. (2015).
Follow‐up
At 1 year follow‐up, the AN group was assessed by using EDE version 16 (Cooper, Grocutt, Deepak, & Bailey, 2007; Wilfley et al., 2000) to obtain the diagnostic status of the eating disorder. The AN group and the HCs were assessed by using the complete identical neuropsychological test battery from baseline.
Data analysis
Statistical analysis
Initially, a comparison was performed between the group of patients with AN who participated in the follow‐up study and the group of patients with AN who dropped out before the follow‐up. The two groups were compared at baseline with regard to age, gender and neurocognitive measures. Gender was compared by using Fisher's exact test, owing to the small proportion of males. The remaining variables were compared by using either the two‐sample t‐test or, if distributional assumptions were not met, its nonparametric equivalent, the Wilcoxon rank‐sum test.
Furthermore, the patients with AN and the HCs were compared on all neurocognitive measures at baseline. Likewise, the two groups were compared regarding change from baseline to follow‐up on the same neurocognitive measures. The tests were conducted by using the paired t‐test or, if distributional assumptions were not met, the nonparametric equivalent, the Wilcoxon signed‐rank test. Effect sizes were calculated by taking the dependence structure of the data into account by adjusting for correlation within the matched pairs.
In order to investigate whether there were any differences in change between AN patients with comorbidity and patients without comorbidity, a two‐sided unpaired t‐test was performed on all neurocognitive measures comparing these two groups.
Predictors of weight recovery were investigated by using logistic regression analyses, which were performed on only the patients with AN. As mentioned, a good outcome was defined as obtaining a body mass index (BMI) above the WHO)15th percentile, that is, corresponding to a BMI of 18.5 or above at age 18 of years, at follow‐up (Cole & Lobstein, 2012; World Health Organization, WHO), whereas the second group consisted of patients with AN who had not achieved normal body weight, that is weight below the AN cut‐off BMI of 18.5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). A separate regression model was generated for each of the neurocognitive variables, by using these as explanatory variables while controlling for gender and age. The explanatory variables included only baseline data. To determine whether the control parameters had any impact on weight recovery, an additional logistic regression model was used that included only gender and age as explanatory variables.
Finally, BMI was considered. The same method as that described earlier was used, that is the patients with AN were distributed into two groups. One group had a BMI higher than the WHO's 15th percentile at follow‐up, and the other group had a BMI lower than the WHO's 15th percentile at follow‐up (World Health Organization, WHO). The high BMI group was compared in terms of changes in neurocognitive measures with those patients with a low BMI at follow‐up. Likewise, those in the higher BMI group were compared with their matched controls with regard to changes in neurocognitive measures. The comparisons were performed as explained above, again differentiating between paired and unpaired comparisons.
A p value lower than 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. All analyses were performed in stata 13 (StataCorp, 2013; College Station, TX).
Inter‐rater reliability
Fully trained clinical psychologists performed the assessments of cognitive functions and the EDE‐16. Psychiatric doctors and medical doctors conducted the somatic assessment. Ongoing corating, supervision and retrospective inter‐rater reliability tests were conducted. The retrospective inter‐rater reliability tests were conducted on the WISC‐III/WAIS‐III in 14 cases. The observed inter‐rater reliability of the FSIQ in this study was satisfactory at 0.985, indicating a high uniformity rating. A detailed review of actions taken to obtain a high degree of reliability was described previously (Kjaersdam Telleus et al., 2015).
Results
Sixty of the 94 matched pairs from baseline participated at follow‐up. In general, the dropout group and the follow‐up group did not significantly differ from one another in regard to background information. Likewise, average BMI, weight, height and illness duration did not differ significantly between the two groups. However, there was a significant difference in age (p = 0.005), with patients in the dropout group having a mean age of 15.6 years and patients in the follow‐up group having a mean age of 14.5 years. With regard to cognitive functions, there was a significant difference between the two groups in verbal intelligence quotient (VIQ) (p = 0.048), with patients in the dropout group performing significantly poorer (VIQ mean = 99.9) than those in the follow‐up group (VIQ mean = 107.7). In the remaining measures of cognitive functions and IQ, no significant differences were found.
The average age at baseline in the 60 patients of the AN group was 14.7 years (SD 1.8), with a range of 10.6–17.9 years. The mean illness duration at baseline, measured by self‐reported duration of changes in eating behaviour, was 1.2 years (SD 1.2), with a range of 0.1–5.3 years.
At baseline, 98.3% of the patients met the diagnostic criteria for AN. One patient did not fully meet the diagnostic weight criteria for AN but did meet the diagnostic criteria for Other Specified Feeding or Eating Disorder, Atypical AN. Among the patients, 88.3% had the AN‐restricting subtype. whereas 11.7% had the AN‐bingeing/purging subtype. At baseline, the patients with AN had an average BMI of 15.7 (SD 1.8) and a mean resting pulse of 60.1 (SD 13.8); lanugo hair was found in 45.6% of the patients and peripheral cyanosis in 33.9%. Among the patients, 36.7% had symptoms of comorbidity, 23.3% had symptoms of affective disorder, 21.6% had anxiety, 6.6% had psychotic symptoms and 6.6% had symptoms of autism spectrum disorder at baseline; 25% of the patients had one comorbid disorder, 11.6% of the patients had two comorbid disorders and 3.3% of the patients had three comorbid disorders.
Of the patients with AN, 33.9% had a family history of eating disorders. At follow‐up, 36.6% of the patients had fully recovered from AN symptomatology, that is BMI, and both psychological criteria; 15% of the patients with AN did not obtain weight recovery.
As shown in Table 1, no significant difference was found between groups for the increase in test scores from baseline to 1‐year follow‐up, with the exception of motor speed. A significant improvement in motor speed, as measured by TMT‐A [difference (diff) = −6.5, p = 0.004, Effect Size (ES) = −0.262), RTI five‐choice movement time (CANTAB) (diff = −62.55, p = 0.002, ES = −0.633) and RTI simple movement time (CANTAB) (diff = −77.05, p = 0.011, ES = −0.511), indicated a greater development in the AN group than in the HCs. However, at baseline, thee patients performed significantly worse than the HCs in all three subtests: TMT‐A (diff = 8.2, p = 0.004, ES = 0.181), RTI five‐choice movement time (CANTAB) (diff = 67.71, p = 0.001, ES = 0.638) and RTI simple movement time (CANTAB) (diff = 58.52, p = 0.018, ES = 0.431). Thus, the significant greater improvement of motor speed represents a normalisation of performance in patients with AN.
Table 1.
Change | Paired diff | Paired diff | Paired ES | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
60 Matched pairs | Patients | Controls | |||||||
Mean | SD | p value | Mean | SD | p value | Mean | p value | ||
WISC‐III/WAIS‐III | |||||||||
Full‐scale IQ (FSIQ) | 4.9 | 12.8 | 0.001 | 6.6 | 6.6 | <0.001 | −1.5 | 0.951 | −0.012 |
Verbal IQ (VIQ) | 2.3 | 12.1 | 0.098 | 4.6 | 7.7 | <0.001 | −2.3 | 0.340 | −0.202 |
Performance IQ (PIQ) | 6.4 | 13.2 | <0.001 | 7.7 | 8.9 | <0.001 | −1.0 | 0.929 | 0.016 |
Difference (VIQ − PIQ) | −4.1 | 12.9 | 0.021 | −3.1 | 12.1 | 0.114 | −1.3 | 0.378 | −0.162 |
Verbal comprehension index (VCI) | 4.3 | 12.9 | 0.009 | 4.5 | 8.0 | <0.001 | 0.0 | 0.686 | 0.090 |
Working memory index (WMI) | −0.9 | 16.0 | 0.870 | 2.1 | 8.7 | 0.068 | −3.3 | 0.103 | −0.274 |
Perceptual organization index (POI) | 6.7 | 14.5 | 0.001 | 7.6 | 10.1 | <0.001 | −0.6 | 0.772 | 0.051 |
Processing speed index (PSI) | 4.4 | 15.3 | 0.021 | 4.5 | 16.2 | 0.081 | 1.0 | 0.702 | 0.070 |
Difference (VCI − POI) | −2.4 | 13.8 | 0.194 | −3.1 | 13.5 | 0.348 | 0.6 | 0.820 | 0.044 |
Rey Osterreith Complex Figure Test (RCFT) | |||||||||
Copy | −0.9 | 3.2 | 0.063 | −0.6 | 4.0 | 0.432 | −0.3 | 0.675 | −0.082 |
Time to copy | −47.0 | 81.5 | <0.001 | −49.3 | 61.8 | <0.001 | −0.7 | 0.778 | −0.062 |
Immediate recall | 1.7 | 5.5 | 0.028 | 1.3 | 5.2 | 0.115 | 0.2 | 0.590 | 0.108 |
Delayed recall | 1.2 | 5.8 | 0.079 | 1.1 | 5.3 | 0.179 | 0.0 | 0.747 | 0.065 |
Trail Making Tests (TMT) | |||||||||
A | −9.3 | 12.2 | <0.001 | −2.8 | 7.9 | 0.016 | −6.5 | 0.004 | −0.262 |
B | −6.2 | 41.8 | 0.003 | −3.0 | 20.3 | 0.329 | −0.7 | 0.356 | −0.087 |
Difference (B − A) | 2.9 | 39.0 | 0.811 | −0.1 | 18.5 | 0.962 | 5.4 | 0.418 | 0.101 |
TOMAL‐2 | |||||||||
Memory for stories | 1.4 | 8.0 | 0.209 | 0.1 | 6.3 | 0.516 | 1.7 | 0.174 | 0.233 |
Memory for stories—delayed Recall | 1.7 | 8.9 | 0.196 | 2.0 | 6.1 | 0.013 | 0.0 | 0.999 | 0.000 |
Word‐selective reminding | 3.2 | 10.4 | 0.019 | 1.1 | 6.3 | 0.064 | 2.3 | 0.379 | 0.184 |
Word‐selective reminding—delayed recall | 0.5 | 2.5 | 0.246 | 0.4 | 1.5 | 0.028 | 0.3 | 0.808 | 0.054 |
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) | |||||||||
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) stages completed | 0.3725 | 1.7772 | 0.174 | 0.1579 | 0.8407 | 0.247 | 0.2083 | 0.894 | 0.028 |
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) total errors adjusted | −10.0196 | 40.0592 | 0.025 | −7.0351 | 19.4853 | 0.011 | −3.6042 | 0.984 | 0.005 |
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) EDS errors | −0.5098 | 11.0587 | 0.423 | −2.5088 | 10.4542 | 0.022 | 1.8750 | 0.377 | 0.203 |
Recognition Memory (PRM) number correct | 0.4200 | 2.4082 | 0.405 | 0.2632 | 1.7781 | 0.115 | 0.0638 | 0.777 | −0.055 |
Spatial Recognition Memory (SRM) number correct | 0.2000 | 2.0898 | 0.410 | −0.1579 | 2.1111 | 0.580 | 0.3617 | 0.219 | 0.258 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) five‐choice movement time | −37.68 | 87.49 | 0.012 | 26.01 | 78.69 | 0.017 | −62.55 | 0.002 | −0.633 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) five‐choice reaction time | −33.49 | 197.12 | 0.214 | −3.48 | 46.51 | 0.782 | −31.71 | 0.621 | −0.109 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) simple movement time | −44.79 | 142.69 | 0.026 | 20.05 | 118.41 | 0.099 | −77.05 | 0.011 | −0.511 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) simple reaction time | −21.15 | 115.61 | 0.174 | −2.73 | 47.27 | 0.625 | −16.56 | 0.856 | −0.044 |
Information Processing (RVP) A | 0.2271 | 0.4280 | 0.001 | 0.2755 | 0.4601 | 0.000 | −0.0102 | 0.977 | 0.006 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) problems solved in minimum moves | 1.5102 | 2.2091 | 0.000 | 0.8070 | 2.0392 | 0.006 | 0.7826 | 0.116 | 0.301 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean initial thinking time 5 | 3226.75 | 8541.43 | 0.009 | 435.31 | 4578.06 | 0.412 | 2008.03 | 0.150 | 0.289 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean moves 5 | −1.1047 | 1.8454 | 0.001 | −0.4330 | 1.4836 | 0.032 | −0.5641 | 0.158 | −0.288 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean subsequent thinking time 5 | −566.27 | 1541.41 | 0.314 | 17.40 | 928.35 | 0.902 | −202.65 | 0.650 | −0.161 |
Spatial Span (SSP) span length | 0.3023 | 1.3190 | 0.181 | −0.0877 | 1.2995 | 0.763 | 0.3000 | 0.183 | 0.264 |
Spatial Working Memory (SWM) strategy | −1.4583 | 5.7721 | 0.143 | −2.4912 | 4.1796 | 0.000 | 0.7333 | 0.512 | 0.135 |
Spatial Working Memory (SWM) total errors | −5.1875 | 15.5134 | 0.083 | −4.6491 | 12.0052 | 0.017 | −1.0222 | 0.915 | −0.025 |
The patients with AN and the HCs had a nonsignificantly different change in development in FSIQ (diff = −1.5, p = 0.951, ES = −0.012) as the AN patients improved an average of 4.9 points (SD = 12.8) and the HCs improved an average of 6.6 points (SD = 6.6).
As shown in Figure 1, a significant difference in the perceptual organization index (POI) was found, with the patients with AN performing significantly worse than the HCs at follow‐up (diff = −4.7, p = 0.029, ES = −0.382); however, at baseline, the difference was nonsignificant (diff = −4.3, p = 0.120, ES = −0.286). Likewise, a significant difference in VIQ was found at baseline (diff = 5.4, p = 0.025, ES = 0.388) (Figure 1) but not at follow‐up (diff = 2.4, p = 0.441, ES = 0.152).
As seen in Figure 2, a significant difference in IQ scores was found, with the patients with AN having a significantly larger difference in IQ scores between VIQ and performance intelligence quotient (PIQ) as well as between verbal comprehension index (VCI) and POI than the HCs. Thus, a significant difference in IQ scores between VIQ and PIQ was found both at baseline (diff = 8.8, p = 0.002, ES = 0.594) and at follow‐up (diff = 6.6, p = 0.016, ES = 0.432). Likewise, a significant difference in IQ scores between VCI and POI was found both at baseline (diff = 10.1, p = 0.001, ES = 0.624) and at follow‐up (diff = 9.4, p = 0.002, ES = 0.588).
As shown in Table 2, there was a significant association between age at baseline and weight recovery (OR 0.57, CI 0.35–0.94, p = 0.027), indicating that the odds of weight recovery were diminished with age in the patients with AN. Other than age, no significant associations were found between weight recovery of BMI and the remaining baseline variables, that is gender, intelligence or specific cognitive functions (see Table 2).
Table 2.
OR | CI (95%) | p value | N | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 0.57 | 0.35 | 0.94 | 0.0276 | 55 |
Gender | 0.18 | 0.01 | 2.70 | 0.2119 | 55 |
WISC‐III/WAIS‐III | |||||
Full‐scale IQ (FSIQ) | 0.99 | 0.94 | 1.05 | 0.7982 | 54 |
Verbal IQ (VIQ) | 0.98 | 0.93 | 1.03 | 0.4275 | 54 |
Performance (PIQ) | 1.01 | 0.96 | 1.07 | 0.6317 | 54 |
Difference (VIQ − PIQ) | 0.96 | 0.90 | 1.02 | 0.1840 | 54 |
Verbal comprehension index (VCI) | 0.98 | 0.93 | 1.03 | 0.3985 | 54 |
Working memory index (WMI) | 1.00 | 0.95 | 1.06 | 0.9158 | 54 |
Perceptual organization index (POI) | 0.99 | 0.94 | 1.05 | 0.8078 | 54 |
Processing speed index (PSI) | 1.02 | 0.98 | 1.07 | 0.3653 | 54 |
Difference (VCI − POI) | 0.98 | 0.94 | 1.03 | 0.5288 | 54 |
Rey Osterreith Complex Figure Test (RCFT) | |||||
Copy | 0.96 | 0.73 | 1.28 | 0.7982 | 54 |
Time to copy | 1.00 | 0.99 | 1.01 | 0.9898 | 50 |
Immediate recall | 0.96 | 0.85 | 1.08 | 0.4722 | 55 |
Delayed recall | 0.96 | 0.85 | 1.07 | 0.4491 | 55 |
Trail Making Tests (TMT) | |||||
A | 0.98 | 0.92 | 1.03 | 0.3939 | 55 |
B | 1.01 | 0.98 | 1.05 | 0.4482 | 55 |
Difference (B − A) | 1.04 | 0.98 | 1.11 | 0.1495 | 55 |
TOMAL‐2 | |||||
Memory for stories | 0.92 | 0.84 | 1.02 | 0.1150 | 46 |
Memory for stories—delayed recall | 0.93 | 0.85 | 1.02 | 0.1202 | 46 |
Word‐selective Reminding | 1.01 | 0.94 | 1.10 | 0.7356 | 47 |
Word‐selective reminding—delayed recall | 0.77 | 0.40 | 1.51 | 0.4509 | 47 |
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) | |||||
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) | 1.12 | 0.77 | 1.62 | 0.5664 | 53 |
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) total errors adjusted | 1.00 | 0.98 | 1.01 | 0.5429 | 53 |
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) EDS errors | 0.97 | 0.89 | 1.06 | 0.4903 | 53 |
Recognition Memory (PRM) number correct | 0.98 | 0.66 | 1.44 | 0.9008 | 52 |
Spatial Recognition Memory (SRM) number correct | 0.99 | 0.64 | 1.52 | 0.9631 | 52 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) five‐choice movement time | 1.04 | 0.92 | 1.18 | 0.5137 | 46 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) five‐choice reaction time | 1.00 | 0.99 | 1.02 | 0.7762 | 46 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) simple movement time | 1.06 | 0.94 | 1.19 | 0.3424 | 46 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) simple reaction time | 1.00 | 0.99 | 1.01 | 0.7810 | 46 |
Information Processing (RVP) A (×10) | 0.76 | 0.13 | 4.62 | 0.7665 | 52 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) problems solved in minimum moves | 0.89 | 0.59 | 1.35 | 0.5736 | 51 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean initial thinking time 5 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.7207 | 51 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean moves 5 | 0.88 | 0.59 | 1.32 | 0.5329 | 51 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean subsequent thinking time 5 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 0.2148 | 51 |
Spatial Span (SSP) span length | 0.68 | 0.38 | 1.23 | 0.2054 | 51 |
Spatial Working Memory (SWM) strategy | 1.09 | 0.91 | 1.31 | 0.3448 | 51 |
Spatial Working Memory (SWM) total errors | 0.97 | 0.93 | 1.01 | 0.1244 | 51 |
Furthermore, no association between BMI and change in cognition was found. Thus, the study results do not indicate any association between weight recovery of BMI and changes in intelligence or specific cognitive functions (Table 3).
Table 3.
BMI > WHO 15th percentile at follow‐up (N = 46) | BMI ≤ WHO 15th percentile at follow‐up (N = 9) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Change | SD | Change | SD | Diff | p value | |
WISC‐III/WAIS‐III | ||||||
Full‐scale IQ (FSIQ) | 4.7 | 13.3 | 5.9 | 12.6 | −1.2 | 0.644 |
Verbal IQ (VIQ) | 1.5 | 12.7 | 5.9 | 9.4 | −4.4 | 0.222 |
Performance (PIQ) | 6.8 | 13.4 | 4.1 | 13.7 | 2.7 | 0.687 |
Difference (VIQ − PIQ) | −5.3 | 13.1 | 1.8 | 8.2 | −7.0 | 0.129 |
Verbal comprehension index (VCI) | 4.2 | 13.7 | 6.2 | 10.4 | −2.1 | 0.610 |
Working memory index (WMI) | −0.2 | 11.2 | −5.3 | 32.5 | 5.1 | 0.545 |
Perceptual organization index (POI) | 7.3 | 14.8 | 2.7 | 14.9 | 4.7 | 0.368 |
Processing speed index (PSI) | 4.5 | 16.4 | 4.1 | 8.7 | 0.4 | 0.775 |
Difference (VCI − POI) | −3.2 | 14.0 | 3.6 | 10.5 | −6.7 | 0.213 |
Rey Osterreith Complex Figure Test (RCFT) | ||||||
Copy | −0.8 | 3.2 | −0.3 | 2.5 | −0.5 | 0.855 |
Time to copy | −56.8 | 82.5 | −32.3 | 44.0 | −24.5 | 0.597 |
Immediate recall | 1.7 | 5.6 | 2.2 | 4.9 | −0.5 | 0.850 |
Delayed recall | 1.3 | 5.7 | 0.6 | 6.6 | 0.7 | 0.803 |
Trail Making Tests (TMT) | ||||||
A | −5.6 | 9.8 | −14.1 | 15.9 | 8.5 | 0.182 |
B | −3.3 | 41.7 | −4.9 | 18.6 | 1.6 | 0.887 |
Difference (B − A) | 2.1 | 38.5 | 9.2 | 16.1 | −7.2 | 0.155 |
TOMAL‐2 | ||||||
Memory for stories | 2.4 | 7.6 | −1.3 | 7.1 | 3.7 | 0.274 |
Memory for stories—delayed recall | 2.2 | 8.9 | −0.3 | 7.1 | 2.5 | 0.462 |
Word‐selective reminding | 2.0 | 10.8 | 5.5 | 4.0 | −3.5 | 0.129 |
Word‐selective reminding—delayed recall | 0.4 | 2.6 | 0.7 | 0.8 | −0.3 | 0.419 |
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) | ||||||
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) stages completed | 0.1579 | 1.6850 | 1.1250 | 2.4749 | −0.9671 | 0.160 |
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) total errors adjusted | −6.4474 | 39.8319 | −25.2500 | 49.6783 | 18.8026 | 0.252 |
Intra‐Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) EDS errors | −0.3684 | 9.4766 | 0.2500 | 14.0484 | −0.6184 | 0.977 |
Recognition Memory (PRM) number correct | 0.3514 | 2.6270 | −0.1250 | 1.7269 | 0.4764 | 0.940 |
Spatial Recognition Memory (SRM) number correct | 0.3243 | 2.1992 | −0.8750 | 1.8851 | 1.1993 | 0.126 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) five‐choice movement time | −39.27 | 95.67 | −1.95 | 34.22 | −37.32 | 0.392 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) five‐choice reaction time | −44.12 | 213.19 | −23.72 | 35.76 | −20.40 | 0.499 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) simple movement time | −52.40 | 152.18 | 56.05 | 90.64 | −108.45 | 0.079 |
Simple and Choice Reaction Time (RTI) simple reaction time | −30.45 | 118.98 | −17.18 | 22.13 | −13.27 | 0.589 |
Information Processing (RVP) A | 0.0246 | 0.0379 | 0.0263 | 0.0602 | −0.0017 | 0.411 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) problems solved in minimum moves | 1.6389 | 2.4860 | 1.0000 | 1.1952 | 0.6389 | 0.612 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean initial thinking time 5 | 2480.68 | 5379.49 | 5705.64 | 16 868.13 | −3224.96 | 0.390 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean moves 5 | −1.0833 | 2.0930 | −0.8750 | 0.9258 | −0.2083 | 0.801 |
Stockings of Cambridge (SOC) mean subsequent thinking time 5 | 29.84 | 1177.58 | −1005.06 | 2581.12 | 1034.90 | 0.417 |
Spatial Span (SSP) span length | 0.5000 | 1.3326 | −0.1250 | 1.5526 | 0.6250 | 0.415 |
Spatial Working Memory (SWM) strategy | −1.0833 | 5.9203 | −3.0000 | 5.9040 | 1.9167 | 0.512 |
Spatial Working Memory (SWM) total errors | −5.8056 | 16.9163 | −6.6250 | 10.7695 | 0.8194 | 0.493 |
Four patients had missing BMI at follow‐up, one patient is excluded owing to having a BMI > 18.5 at baseline.
With respect to an association between change in cognition and comorbidity in patients with AN, no significant associations were found besides in POI. Here a statistical significant difference was found in the change between the AN group without and with comorbidities (diff = 9.7, p = 0.0200) in favour of the latter.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the largest follow‐up study of cognitive functions conducted in children and adolescents with AN. A high degree of credibility was obtained through the use of a large number of participants, a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery and a high inter‐rater reliability.
One hypothesis of the study was that the development of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN would be significantly different from that of matched HCs. However, a similar nonsignificant statistical difference in cognitive development was found in all cognitive domains, with the exception of motor speed. Thus, both intelligence and specific cognitive functions developed uniformly. The study findings thus suggest that AN does not influence cognitive development in children and adolescents over a period of 1 year, contrary to the original hypothesis.
The significant differences in nonverbal intelligence, that is the significant difference in POI between the patients with AN and the HCs found at follow‐up, were similar to those differences found in the baseline study (Kjaersdam Telleus et al., 2015). This result is consistent with the results of a study by McCormick et al. (2008), where patients with AN performed significantly worse than controls in PIQ (p = 0.000).
The uniform development in cognitive functions resulted in an unaltered significant difference, indicating that inefficiencies in nonverbal intelligence in patients with AN do not normalise over time. POI is the component of intelligence that manifests in the performance of tasks requiring little or no acquired knowledge; it includes the ability to analyse information and solve problems by using visual reasoning (Flanagan & Kaufman, 2009). Fluid intelligence (Cattell, 1971) is a central component of POI (Kaplan & Saccuzzo, 2005). Fluid intelligence allows one to work in complex situations and underwrites the capacity to think logically and to analyse and solve problems in new situations without the use of previously acquired knowledge (Cattell, 1971).
The uneven cognitive profile highlighted by the significant difference between verbal and nonverbal intelligence functions, that is the significant difference between VIQ and PIQ as well as VCI and POI in the patients with AN and the HCs, is also consistent with the finding by McCormick et al. (2008), who found that patients with AN had a higher verbal intelligence than nonverbal intelligence function. Thus, in the study by McCormick et al. (2008), a Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale‐Revised (WAIS‐R) VIQ of 100.9 and a PIQ of 91.6 were found in patients with AN. An uneven cognitive profile highlighted by the significant difference in verbal and nonverbal intelligence functions in the patients with AN compared with that of the HCs was found both in the baseline study (Kjaersdam Telleus et al., 2015) and at follow‐up. Thus, the uneven cognitive profile in patients with AN is a stable trait and does not appear to be illness dependent. Therefore, it may be suggested that this inefficiency could be pre morbid and thereby may be a predisposing factor, among other predisposing factors such as psychological, genetic, biological and sociocultural factors, which contribute to illness progression (Bruch, 1973; Woodside et al., 2001). Another possibility could be that the uneven cognitive profile might be a sequelae of having/having had AN.
However, the occurrences of an uneven intelligence profile in children and adolescents with AN should be interpreted cautiously as the differences between VIQ and PIQ are relatively common in the general population. For example, 37.3% of Danish children in the standardisation group for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition (WISC‐III) achieved a difference of 15 IQ points or more between VIQ and PIQ, and 10.0% demonstrated a difference of 26 IQ points or more (Wechsler, 2009).
In general, no significant differences in the change in cognitive functions were found between the patients with AN and the HCs. However, motor speed was one exception, with a greater improvement in change found in the patients with AN than in the HCs. TMT‐A is a measure of visual attention and motor speed while RTI (five‐choice movement time and simple movement time form CANTAB) is a measure of response latency and motor speed. Thus, both TMT‐A and RTI have motor speed as a common component. This study's findings suggest that motor speed may normalise over time, and these results are consistent with the findings of other longitudinal studies. Similar findings by Hatch et al. (2010) also suggest that patients with AN who were underweight performed worse than HCs on motor speed, but these inefficiencies appeared to normalise when the patients’ weight was restored (Hatch et al., 2010). Likewise the study results comply with the findings by Zwipp et al. (2014), which included that motor speed was found to increase with weight gain in patients with AN.
A significant difference in VIQ between the patients with AN and the HCs was found at baseline but not at follow‐up. However, as this difference was not found in the large baseline cohort (Kjaersdam Telleus et al., 2015), this finding may be the result of reduced strength due to a smaller sample size in the follow‐up study.
The other hypotheses of the study were that changes in the cognitive functions of patients who recovered weight would be significantly different from those of patients who did not recover normal body weight at 1 year follow‐up and that baseline cognition would be a predictor of weight recovery.
No association between weight recovery, that is normalisation of BMI, and baseline cognition was found, indicating that cognitive functions and the odds of having a healthy BMI at 1 year follow‐up and at baseline are not associated. This finding is supported by the findings of Tchanturia et al. (2011), who also found no association between set shifting and BMI in adult patients with AN (Tchanturia et al., 2011).
These study findings suggest that cognition does not predict whether patients with AN will recover from their eating disorder. As most of the patients with AN showed a normalisation of BMI and only nine did not, there was not enough statistical power in the data of this small group to rule out that a relationship exists. However, the lack of association between BMI and change in cognitive functions, coupled with the observed uniform development of cognitive functions, indicates that weight recovery cannot be predicted from cognitive measures, at least not in a short time frame of 1 year as was the case in this study.
However, the correlation between age and normalisation of BMI suggests that older patients with AN have lower odds of normalising their weight and BMI and that younger patients have higher odds of recovering weight. This correlation further implies that the age of patients with AN is associated with the odds of overcoming the illness. This finding is consistent with the findings of other studies that found that early age at first hospitalisation was associated with clinical weight recovery and better outcomes (Errichiello, Iodice, Bruzzese, Gherghi, & Senatore, 2015; Papadopoulos, Ekbom, Brandt, & Ekselius, 2009). As the illness duration in the group of patients with AN in this study was short, with an average duration of 1.2 years and with only one patient with a illness duration of more than 3 years, the results indicate that the age of the patient rather than illness duration is the decisive factor. This finding might be vital, as it suggests the importance of early detection and treatment.
The significant difference in age between the dropout group and the follow‐up group, with patients in the follow‐up group being significantly younger than the dropout group, should probably be explained by the parents’ higher level of involvement and parental authority, keeping the patients in treatment and participation in the research project. Likewise, a group difference was found between the dropout group and the follow‐up group with respect to VIQ. This difference represents a trend towards lower VIQ in the group of patients who dropped out of the study. A possible explanation is that patients in the follow‐up group who had significantly higher VIQ compared with patients who did not participate at follow‐up might have a better understanding of the importance of participating in research.
To summarise, the similar cognitive development in the patients with AN and HCs may suggest that cognitive development in patients with AN was not affected by the illness, as the development of cognition was similar to that of healthy children and adolescents. Although some cognitive inefficiencies were stable, including nonverbal intelligence functions, another cognitive function, motor speed, appears to be illness dependent and normalises over time. Although this cognitive function appears to be reversible and illness dependent, most cognitive functions appear to be illness independent and may be seen as predisposing factors for the illness rather than being triggered by the illness itself. Cognitive functions do not seem to be potential predictors of weight recovery. Likewise, there does not seem to be an association between BMI and cognitive inefficiencies in patients with AN.
Strengths and limitations
This follow‐up study of cognitive functions conducted in children and adolescents with AN consisted of a large number of participants. This is one of only a few longitudinal studies of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN, and the study includes extensive data. A development component is included in the study, and the study results are controlled through a robust comparison group.
However, there are some limitations that should be mentioned. The main limitation is the reduced participant sample at follow‐up. Although 120 participants, 60 matched pairs, still constitute a considerable participant sample, the smaller sample size, compared with baseline, nonetheless weakens the strength of the study. Similarly, the lack of data, such as weight, in some patients with AN, further reduces the strength of the regression analyses.
This study includes a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery. RCFT is used to assess visual memory according to Meyer and Meyer's manual. In recent years RCFT has been used as a measure of central coherences in some AN studies. In this study the RCFT results cannot be used for this purpose. Likewise, the Wisconsin Card Sorting test (WCST) is often used in eating disorder research. A decision was made to include Intra–Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) from CANTAB, which is a computerised analogue of WCST. This introduces a limitation in regard to comparing this study's longitudinal data with data in some existing literature on AN.
The findings of this study suggest that, with the exception of motor speed, AN does not influence cognitive development in children and adolescents over a period of 1 year. However, it is possible that the relatively brief follow‐up period is too short a period to detect such differences and that a long‐term follow‐up might produce other results. Investigating this issue could be a highly relevant objective for future research.
Conclusions
As the study design included a 1‐year follow‐up, this study was able to investigate potential changes in cognitive functions over time. One of the hypotheses of the study was that the development of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN would be significantly reduced compared with that of matched HCs. However, the difference in the development of cognitive functions observed in the patients with AN, as well as in the HC participants, was nonsignificant, with the exception of motor speed. A significantly greater improvement was detected in motor speed, relative to a smaller improvement in the HC participants, resulting in a normalisation of performance in the patients with AN.
Based on the significant difference in POI in the patients with AN when compared with the HC participants, and a stable significantly uneven intelligence profile with a significantly larger difference between verbal and nonverbal intelligence abilities in the patients with AN than in the HC participants, suggests that the nonverbal intelligence inefficiencies could be pre morbid and thereby indicate a predisposition to the development of the illness.
Another hypothesis of the study was that the change in intelligence and specific cognitive functions of patients who recovered weight would be significantly different from that of patients who did not recover normal body weight at follow‐up. There does not seem to be an association between BMI and changes in cognitive functions in patients with AN. Thus, the study results display a similar cognitive performance change between groups over time, indicating that cognitive functions were not significantly affected by the illness.
A further hypothesis of the study was that the baseline cognitive profile would be a predictor of weight recovery. The cognitive profile does not appear to be associated with patients’ weight recovery from the eating disorder. Age, however, may be associated with the odds of obtaining a healthy BMI. Thus, the younger the patients, the higher the odds that they will recover weight.
The study contributes useful new information on the development of cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN over time. First of all, motor speed seems associated with recovery and may be related to AN and low body weight. Increased knowledge regarding the uneven intelligence profile and the inefficiencies in the nonverbal intelligence functions in children and adolescents with AN may have an impact on clinical practice. Any inefficiency in cognitive functions in these young patients may have an impact in relation to therapeutic work. Similarly, increased knowledge regarding the cognitive profile of children and adolescents with AN may be important for the organisation of cognitive training for patients (Tchanturia et al, 2014). Training of specific cognitive functions in children and adolescents with AN should be based on knowledge of this patient group rather than knowledge based on the adult patient group.
Kjærsdam Telléus, G. , Fagerlund, B. , Jepsen, J. R. , Bentz, M. , Christiansen, E. , Valentin, J. B. , and Thomsen, P. H. (2016) Are Weight Status and Cognition Associated? An Examination of Cognitive Development in Children and Adolescents with Anorexia Nervosa 1 Year after First Hospitalisation. Eur. Eat. Disorders Rev., 24: 366–376. doi: 10.1002/erv.2445.
References
- American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.n). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
- Andres‐Perpina, S. , Lozano‐Serra, E. , Puig, O. , Lera‐Miguel, S. , Lazaro, L. , & Castro‐Fornieles, J. (2011). Clinical and biological correlates of adolescent anorexia nervosa with impaired cognitive profile. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(11–12), 541–549. doi: 10.1007/s00787-011-0216-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayless, J. D. , Kanz, J. E. , Moser, D. J. , McDowell, B. D. , Bowers, W. A. , Andersen, A. E. , et al. (2002). Neuropsychological characteristics of patients in a hospital‐based eating disorder program. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry: Official Journal of the American Academy of Clinical Psychiatrists, 14(4), 203–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodell, L. P. , Keel, P. K. , Brumm, M. C. , Akubuiro, A. , Caballero, J. , Tranel, D. , et al. (2014). Longitudinal examination of decision‐making performance in anorexia nervosa: Before and after weight restoration. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 56, 150–157. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.05.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruch, H. (1973). Eating disorders: Obesity, anorexia nervosa, and the person within. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Buehren, K. , Konrad, K. , Schaefer, K. , Kratzsch, J. , KahramanLanzerath, B. , Lente, C. , et al. (2011). Association between neuroendocrinological parameters and learning and memory functions in adolescent anorexia nervosa before and after weight recovery. Journal of Neural Transmission, 118(6), 963–968. 10.1007/s00702-010-0567-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buhren, K. , Mainz, V. , HerpertzDahlmann, B. , Schafer, K. , KahramanLanzerath, B. , Lente, C. , et al. (2012). Cognitive flexibility in juvenile anorexia nervosa patients before and after weight recovery. Journal of Neural Transmission, 119(9), 1047–1057. 10.1007/s00702-012-0821-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bulik, C. M. , Sullivan, P. F. , Tozzi, F. , Furberg, H. , Lichtenstein, P. , & Pedersen, N. L. (2006). Prevalence, heritability, and prospective risk factors for anorexia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(3), 305–312. 10.1001/archpsyc.63.3.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calderoni, S. , Muratori, F. , Leggero, C. , Narzisi, A. , Apicella, F. , Balottin, U. , et al. (2013). Neuropsychological functioning in children and adolescents with restrictive‐type anorexia nervosa: An in‐depth investigation with NEPSY‐II. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35(2), 167–179. 10.1080/13803395.2012.760536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. [Google Scholar]
- Cavedini, P. , Bassi, T. , Gorini, A. , Baraldi, C. , Ubbiali, A. , & Bellodi, L. (2006). Decision‐making functioning as a predictor of treatment outcome in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Research, 145(2–3), 179–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole, T. J. , & Lobstein, T. (2012). Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut‐offs for thinness, overweight and obesity. Pediatric Obesity, 7(4), 284–294. 10.1111/j.2047-6310.2012.00064.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, Z. , Cooper, P. J. , & Fairburn, C. G. (1989). The validity of the eating disorder examination and its subscales. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science, 154, 807–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, M. J. , Grocutt, E. , Deepak, K. , & Bailey, E. (2007). Metacognition in anorexia nervosa, dieting and non‐dieting controls: A preliminary investigation. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology / the British Psychological Society, 46(Pt 1), 113–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehlers, S. , Gillberg, C. , & Wing, L. (1999). A screening questionnaire for Asperger syndrome and other high‐functioning autism spectrum disorders in school age children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 129–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Errichiello, L. , Iodice, D. , Bruzzese, D. , Gherghi, M. , & Senatore, I. (2015). Prognostic factors and outcome in anorexia nervosa: A follow‐up study. Eating and Weight Disorders: EWD. 10.1007/s40519-015-0211-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flanagan, D. P. , & Kaufman, A. S. (2009). Essentials of WISC‐IV assessment (2nd ed.n). New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc.. ISBN: 978-0-470-18915-3. [Google Scholar]
- Gillberg, I. C. , Gillberg, C. , Rastam, M. , & Johansson, M. (1996). The cognitive profile of anorexia nervosa: A comparative study including a community‐based sample. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37(1), 23–30. doi:S0010‐440X(96)90046‐2 [pii]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 38(5), 581–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gueguen, J. , Godart, N. , Chambry, J. , Brun‐Eberentz, A. , Foulon, C. , Divac, S. M. , et al. (2012). Severe anorexia nervosa in men: Comparison with severe AN in women and analysis of mortality. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45(4), 537–545. 10.1002/eat.20987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatch, A. , Madden, S. , Kohn, M. R. , Clarke, S. , Touyz, S. , Gordon, E. , et al. (2010). In first presentation adolescent anorexia nervosa, do cognitive markers of underweight status change with weight gain following a refeeding intervention? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 43(4), 295–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebebrand, J. , & Bulik, C. M. (2011). Critical appraisal of the provisional DSM‐5 criteria for anorexia nervosa and an alternative proposal. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 44(8), 665–678. 10.1002/eat.20875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holliday, J. , Tchanturia, K. , Landau, S. , Collier, D. , & Treasure, J. (2005). Is impaired set‐shifting an endophenotype of anorexia nervosa? The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(12), 2269–2275. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudson, J. I. , Hiripi, E. , Pope, H. G. Jr. , & Kessler, R. C. (2007). The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Biological Psychiatry, 61(3), 348–358. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan, R. M. , & Saccuzzo, D. P. (2005). Psychological testing (6th ed.n). Belmont: CA: Thomson Wadsworth. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman, J. , Birmaher, B. , Brent, D. A. , Ryan, N. D. , & Rao, U. (2000). K‐SADS‐PL. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(10): 1208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kjaersdam Telleus, G. , Jepsen, J. R. , Bentz, M. , Christiansen, E. , Jensen, S. O. , Fagerlund, B. , et al. (2015). Cognitive profile of children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review: The Journal of the Eating Disorders Association, 23(1), 34–42. 10.1002/erv.2337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang, K. , Lloyd, S. , Khondoker, M. , Simic, M. , Treasure, J. , & Tchanturia, K. (2015). Do children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa display an inefficient cognitive processing style? PloS One, 10(7), e0131724 10.1371/journal.pone.0131724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang, K. , Lopez, C. , Stahl, D. , Tchanturia, K. , & Treasure, J. (2014). Central coherence in eating disorders: An updated systematic review and meta‐analysis. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 15, 586–598. 10.3109/15622975.2014.909606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lang, K. , Stahl, D. , Espie, J. , Treasure, J. , & Tchanturia, K. (2014). Set shifting in children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa: An exploratory systematic review and meta‐analysis. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47(4), 394–399. 10.1002/eat.22235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lange, K. , & Tchanturia, K. (2014). A systematic review of central coherence in young people with anorexia nervosa. Journal of Child & Adolescent Behavior, 2(3). 10.4172/jcalb.1000140. [Google Scholar]
- Lezak M.D. (2012) Neuropsychological assessment. 5th edn Oxford University Press, Oxford, New York, ISBN: 10.1011/9780195395525. [Google Scholar]
- Lowe, C. , & Rabbitt, P. (1998). Test/re‐test reliability of the CANTAB and ISPOCD neuropsychological batteries: Theoretical and practical issues. Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery. International study of post‐operative cognitive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia, 36(9), 915–923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCormick, L.M. , Keel, P.K. , Brumm, M.C. , Bowers, W. , Wayze, V. , Andersen, A. , & Andreasen, N. (2008). mplications of starvation‐induced change in right dorsal anterior cingulate volume in anorexia nervosa. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41(7), 602–610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDowell, B. D. , Moser, D. J. , Ferneyhough, K. , Bowers, W. A. , Andersen, A. E. , & Paulsen, J. S. (2003). Cognitive impairment in anorexia nervosa is not due to depressed mood. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 33(3), 351–355. 10.1002/eat.10149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyers, J , Meyers, K . (1995). Rey complex figure and recognition trial: Professional manual. Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Published online 2010 December 23].
- Mitchell, J. E. , & Crow, S. (2006). Medical complications of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 19(4), 438–443. 10.1097/01.yco.0000228768.79097.3e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papadopoulos, F. C. , Ekbom, A. , Brandt, L. , & Ekselius, L. (2009). Excess mortality, causes of death and prognostic factors in anorexia nervosa. The British Journal of Psychiatry: the Journal of Mental Science, 194(1), 10–17. 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.054742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raevuori, A. , Hoek, H. W. , Susser, E. , Kaprio, J. , Rissanen, A. , & Keski‐Rahkonen, A. (2009a). Epidemiology of anorexia nervosa in men: A nationwide study of finnish twins. PloS One, 4(2), e4402 10.1371/journal.pone.0004402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raevuori, A. , Niemela, S. , Keski‐Rahkonen, A. , & Sourander, A. (2009b). Early risk factors of eating disorders—Do events of prenatal and perinatal periods bear significance? [Syomishairioiden varhaiset riskitekijat] Duodecim; Laaketieteellinen Aikakauskirja, 125(1), 38–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reitan, R.M. & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead–Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, Ariz.: Neuropsychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds, C. R. , & Bigler, E. D. (1994). Test of memory and learning. Austin, Texas: Pro‐Ed. [Google Scholar]
- Rose, M. , Frampton, I. J. , & Lask, B. (2013). Central coherence, organizational strategy, and visuospatial memory in children and adolescents with anorexia nervosa. Applied Neuropsychology. Child. 10.1080/21622965.2013.775064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarrar, L. , Ehrlich, S. , Merle, J. V. , Pfeiffer, E. , Lehmkuhl, U. , & Schneider, N. (2011). Cognitive flexibility and agouti‐related protein in adolescent patients with anorexia nervosa. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(9), 1396–1406. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp . (2013). Stata: Release 13. statistical software. college station, TX: StataCorp LP.
- Stedal, K. , Frampton, I. , Landro, N. I. , & Lask, B. (2012). An examination of the ravello profile—A neuropsychological test battery for anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review: The Journal of the Eating Disorders Association, 20(3), 175–181. 10.1002/erv.1160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia, K. , Anderluh, M. B. , Morris, R. G. , RabeHesketh, S. , Collier, D. A. , Sanchez, P. , et al. (2004). Cognitive flexibility in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(4), 513–520. 10.1017/S1355617704104086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia, K. , Davies, H. , Roberts, M. , Harrison, A. , Nakazato, M. , Schmidt, U. , et al. (2012). Poor cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: Examining the evidence using the wisconsin card sorting task. PloS One, 7(1), e28331 10.1371/journal.pone.0028331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia, K. , Harrison, A. , Davies, H. , Roberts, M. , Oldershaw, A. , Nakazato, M. , Treasure, J. (2011). Cognitive flexibility and clinical severity in eating disorders. PloS One, 6(6), e20462. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020462 [doi] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tchanturia, K. , Lounes, N. , & Holttum, S. (2014). European eating disorders review : the journal of the Eating Disorders Association. European Eating Disorders Review, 22(6), 454–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Autreve, S & Vervaet, M (2015). Are There differences in central coherence and set shifting across the subtypes of anorexia nervosa?: A systematic review. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease.2015; 203(10): 774–80. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. (1991). Manual for the Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.,, ). San Antonio: TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS‐III, Wechsler adult intelligence scale: Administration and scoring manual Psychological Corporation.
- Wechsler, D. (2009). WISC‐III—Vejledning, Dansk version. Denmark: NCS Pearson, Inc.. [Google Scholar]
- Wilfley, D. E. , Schwartz, M. B. , Spurrell, E. B. , & Fairburn, C. G. (2000). Using the eating disorder examination to identify the specific psychopathology of binge eating disorder. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27(3), 259–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson, D. A. , Barker, S. E. , Bertman, L. J. , & Gleaves, D. H. (1995). Body image, body dysphoria, and dietary restraint: Factor structure in nonclinical subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 85–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woodside, D. B. , Garfinkel, P. E. , Lin, E. , Goering, P. , Kaplan, A. S. , Goldbloom, D. S. , et al. (2001). Comparisons of men with full or partial eating disorders, men without eating disorders, and women with eating disorders in the community. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(4), 570–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization, WHO: (nd). http://www.who.int/growthref/who2007_bmi_for_age/en/index.html.
- Zwipp, J. , Hass, J. , Schober, I. , Geisler, D. , Ritschel, F. , Seidel, M. , et al. (2014). Serum brain‐derived neurotrophic factor and cognitive functioning in underweight, weight‐recovered and partially weight‐recovered females with anorexia nervosa. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 54(2014), 163–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]