Abstract
Exposure to light at night (LAN) is associated with insomnia in humans. Light provides the main input to the master clock in the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) that coordinates the sleep-wake cycle. We aimed to develop a rodent model for the effects of LAN on sleep. Therefore, we exposed male Wistar rats to either a 12 h light (150–200lux):12 h dark (LD) schedule or a 12 h light (150–200 lux):12 h dim white light (5 lux) (LDim) schedule. LDim acutely decreased the amplitude of daily rhythms of REM and NREM sleep, with a further decrease over the following days. LDim diminished the rhythms of 1) the circadian 16–19 Hz frequency domain within the NREM sleep EEG, and 2) SCN clock gene expression. LDim also induced internal desynchronization in locomotor activity by introducing a free running rhythm with a period of ~25 h next to the entrained 24 h rhythm. LDim did not affect body weight or glucose tolerance. In conclusion, we introduce the first rodent model for disturbed circadian control of sleep due to LAN. We show that internal desynchronization is possible in a 24 h L:D cycle which suggests that a similar desynchronization may explain the association between LAN and human insomnia.
Millions of humans and animals around the world are exposed to artificial light during the night, when the natural environment should be dark1,2. In mammals, environmental light is the main input that synchronizes the master circadian clock in the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) to the outside world. The SCN communicates the external time of day to other brain areas, including areas that regulate sleep and food intake such as the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus and the lateral hypothalamus3. The SCN also communicates with peripheral clocks in metabolic tissues such as liver and adipose tissue4, and regulates glucose metabolism5. Sleep and metabolism are tightly interconnected, since sleep quality directly affects human glucose metabolism6 and people with metabolic syndrome showed disturbed sleep7. Thus, exposure to artificial light at night may contribute to the high prevalence of insomnia8 and type 2 diabetes9,10.
Indeed, observational studies in humans showed that exposure to bedroom light at night is associated with insomnia11, obesity12,13, and dyslipidemia13. In addition, in two small studies in healthy humans, exposure to dim light during sleep at night decreased sleep depth and increased arousal frequency14,15. In nocturnal rodents, bright light at night increased sleep16 and decreased food intake17 within minutes. Furthermore, long term exposure to constant light disrupted circadian rhythmic behavior18,19,20, decreased total time spent awake20 and caused obesity and type 2 diabetes18,19 in rodents. As constant light conditions are uncommon outside the laboratory, dim light at night is a better model of exposure to artificial light at night. One study in free-living birds showed that light pollution with 2 lux at night acutely decreased total sleep time21. In Sprague Dawley rats, dim light at night decreased the amplitude of hormonal rhythms22,23. Nelson’s laboratory extensively studied the effect of dim light at night in Swiss Webster mice, and their studies showed that 5 lux light at night reduced the amplitude of the daily rhythms of locomotor activity and food intake, and induced obesity and diabetes18,24, whereas dim light at night had no significant effects on sleep distribution25.
To our knowledge, at present there is no working rodent model to investigate the mechanism underlying the disturbing effects of dim light at night on sleep. The rat provides more opportunities for physiological measurements of sleep and metabolism, since the larger size of the animal facilitates higher volume plasma sampling, targeted hypothalamic manipulations, and electrophysiological studies as compared to the mouse5,26. Therefore, in the present study we investigated the effects of 5 lux dim light at night on sleep-wake behavior and energy metabolism in Wistar rats.
Results
Vigilance states and sleep architecture
To investigate the effects of light at night on sleep-wake behavior we exposed rats to either a 12 h light (150–200 lux):12 h dark (LD) schedule or a 12 h light (150–200 lux):12 h dim white light (5 lux) (LDim) schedule and performed EEG/EMG recordings. LDim strongly diminished the amplitude of the daily rhythm in all three vigilance states. Introduction of LDim decreased the time spent awake in the first dim phase compared to the baseline dark phase, with a further decrease over the following days. LDim gradually increased the time spent awake in the light phase (Phase P < 0.001, Day P = 0.136, Interaction P < 0.001; Fig. 1). Specifically, LDim reduced the duration of waking episodes during the dark (dim) phase and increased the duration of waking episodes during the light phase (Supplementary Figure 1). LDim also increased the time spent in NREM sleep in the first dim phase compared to the baseline dark phase, with a further increase over the days. LDim gradually decreased the time spent in NREM sleep in the light phase (Phase P < 0.001, Day P < 0.001, Interaction P < 0.001; Fig. 1). Specifically, LDim increased NREM sleep episode duration in the dark (dim) phase and reduced NREM sleep episode duration in the light phase (Supplementary Figure 1). Finally, LDim directly increased the time spent in REM sleep during the first dim phase compared to the baseline dark phase, and gradually decreased the time spent in REM sleep during the light phase (Phase P = 0.002, Day P = 0.003, Interaction P = 0.002; Fig. 1). The increase in REM sleep during the dark (dim) phase was mainly due to increased REM sleep episode numbers (Supplementary Figure 1). In LD, the daily rhythm in the vigilance states did not significantly change over the 14 days period (Fig. 1). Total 24 h sleep time was not significantly affected by LDim (LD: 43.8 ± 1.7%, 14 days LDim: 47.2 ± 1.0%, P = 0.293).
In order to investigate homeostatic sleep drive, we analyzed the rhythm of slow wave activity (SWA, EEG power density between 0.75–4.0 Hz). SWA showed a clear day/night rhythm with a peak at the beginning and a trough at the end of the light phase. LDim gradually decreased the amplitude of the SWA rhythm (Day P = 0.001; Fig. 2a).
When analyzing the changes in power density of all frequencies between 0.25 and 25.0 Hz within the NREM sleep EEG, we observed reduced power in the 16–19 Hz frequency domain in the course of 14 days LDim (P < 0.05, after significant Interaction P < 0.05). Therefore we analyzed the rhythm of this frequency domain in more detail. The 16–19 Hz frequency domain showed a pronounced daily rhythm at baseline with a peak at the beginning of the dark phase. LDim strongly reduced the peak and the amplitude of this 16–19 Hz rhythm (Day P < 0.001; Fig. 2b).
Circadian locomotor activity
To further investigate the effect of LDim exposure on the circadian timing system, we exposed male Wistar rats to 10 days LD followed by 30 days LDim and 20 days constant darkness (DD). Subsequently, the animals were re-entrained during 30 days to LD and finally the animals were released into DD for 10 days (circadian experiment A). LDim reduced the strength of the 24 h rhythm, and induced a second, interacting, free running rhythm with a period of 25.1 ± 0.0 h (see arrow in Fig. 3b). This interacting free running rhythm was observed in all animals exposed to LDim. After transfer from LDim to DD, rhythmic strength remained low in the first ten days with a period similar to the second rhythmic component in LDim, followed by an increase in rhythmic strength in the following 10 days (Fig. 3 and Table 1). Three out of 8 animals were virtually arrhythmic in the first days after transfer to DD (in these animals no significant period could be detected in the first 7 days after release in DD) and showed an improvement of the rhythm in the course of DD. After re-entrainment to LD and transfer from LD to DD all animals showed a free running period of 24.2 ± 0.1 h in DD.
Table 1. LDim induces an endogenous free run interacting with the entrained 24 hr rhythm in locomotor activity.
Circadian experiment A (n = 8) |
Circadian experiment B (n = 8) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Period | Strength | Period | Strength | ||
LD (10 days) | 24.0 ± 0.0A | 0.38 ± 0.02A | LD (10 days) | 24.0 ± 0.0A | 0.21 ± 0.00A |
LDim (30 days) | 1) 23.9 ± 0.0A 2) 25.1 ± 0.0B | 1) 0.17 ± 0.01B 2) 0.09 ± 0.01C | DimDim (16 days) | 24.9 ± 0.3B | 0.11 ± 0.00B |
DD1 (10 days) | 24.8 ± 0.3B | 0.11 ± 0.02C | LL (22 days) | Arrhythmic | n/a |
DD2 (10 days) | 24.5 ± 0.1AB | 0.18 ± 0.02B | DimDim (32 days) | Arrhythmic | n/a |
LD (30 days) | 24.0 ± 0.0A | 0.21 ± 0.02AB | LD (14 days) | 24.0 ± 0.0A | 0.20 ± 0.02A |
DD (10 days) | 24.2 ± 0.1A | 0.14 ± 0.01B | DimDim (16 days) | 24.8 ± 0.4B | 0.06 ± 0.01C |
Within each experiment values that are annotated by different letters are significantly different from each other (Duncan P < 0.05, after a significant main effect of Light in the ANOVA analysis). Data are represented as mean ± SEM. DD, continuous darkness. DimDim, continuous dim white light. LL continuous light. n/a, not applicable.
As a control, we verified whether the occurrence of two interacting free running rhythms was specific for LDim and not due to continuous exposure to minimally 5 lux, by exposing Male Wistar rats consecutively to at least 10 days LD, 16 days constant dim light (DimDim), 22 days constant light (LL), 32 days DimDim, 14 days LD, and 16 days DimDim (circadian experiment B). DimDim exposure induced period lengthening to 24.9 ± 0.3 hr, but in contrast to LDim exposure, no dual rhythms were observed (Table 1, Supplementary Figure 2). LL exposure induced arrhythmic behavior and subsequent DimDim exposure did not restore rhythmicity. After re-entrainment to LD and re-exposure to DimDim, again period lengthening was observed.
SCN clock gene expression
In order to investigate the representation within the SCN of the two rhythmic components observed in locomotor behavior, we analyzed SCN clock gene expression at the time when the two rhythmic components in behavior were aligned or misaligned. Per1 expression in the SCN showed a significant diurnal variation with higher expression at ZT6 compared to ZT18 (Time P = 0.004, Group P = 0.137, Interaction P = 0.495). Post hoc tests showed that the effect of Time was only significant in the LD group (P = 0.025), but not in the LDim-aligned (P = 0.272) and LDim-misaligned (P = 0.136) groups (Fig. 4). There was no difference in dorsal/ventral ratio between ZT6 and ZT18 in misaligned animals (P = 0.324).
Arntl expression in the SCN showed a significant diurnal variation in antiphase to Per1, with lower expression at ZT6 compared to ZT18 (Time P = 0.027, Group P = 0.574, Interaction P = 0.145). Post hoc testing showed that the diurnal variation nearly reached significance in the LD group (P = 0.058), but not in the LDim-aligned (P = 0.130) and LDim-misaligned (P = 0.899) groups (Fig. 4). There was no difference in dorsal/ventral ratio between ZT6 and ZT18 in misaligned animals (P = 0.176).
Energy metabolism and body weight
Previous experiments in mice showed that dim light at night can influence energy metabolism18. Therefore, we investigated the effects of LDim compared to LD on food intake, energy expenditure, and body weight, both on a chow diet and on a high fat diet (HFD), with metabolic cages. On a chow diet, LDim induced a decrease of the amplitude of the daily rhythm of food intake from 84 ± 2% dark phase food intake at baseline to 59 ± 2% dark phase food intake in week 7. This decrease was much stronger compared to the amplitude decrease in LD from 83 ± 2% dark phase food intake at baseline to 77 ± 3% dark phase food intake in week 7 (Light schedule P = 0.426, Week P < 0.001, Interaction P = 0.026). LDim also induced a decrease in the daily rhythm of energy expenditure compared to the LD animals (Light schedule P = 0.212, Week P = 0.001, Interaction P = 0.003) (Supplementary Figure 3). Animals exposed to LD showed a small increase over time of total 24-h food intake compared animals in LDim (Light schedule P = 0.927, Week P = 0.002, Interaction P = 0.025), but total 24-h energy expenditure did not differ between LDim and LD (Light schedule P = 0.908, Week P < 0.001, Interaction P = 0.084) (Supplementary Table 1).
On a HFD, LDim again induced a stronger reduction of the daily rhythm of food intake compared to LD (Light schedule P = 0.132, Week P < 0.001, Interaction P = 0.001) (Fig. 5), however, total 24-h food intake was not significantly affected by LDim (Light schedule P = 0.079, Week P = 0.321, Interaction P = 0.238) (Supplementary Table 1). LDim gradually decreased the daily rhythm in energy expenditure compared to LD (Light schedule P = 0.002, Week P < 0.001, Interaction P < 0.001 (Supplementary Figure 3). Total 24-h energy expenditure was not affected by LDim (Light schedule P = 0.990, Week P < 0.001, Interaction P = 0.249) (Supplementary Table 1).
The increase in body weight did not differ between LDim and LD, neither on a chow diet nor on a HFD (Fig. 5 and Supplementary Table 2). White adipose tissue (WAT), adrenal, and thymus weights were not different between LDim and LD on either a chow diet or a HFD (Supplementary Table 2).
Glucose tolerance as assessed with the plasma glucose incremental area under the curve (iAUC) after an intravenous glucose tolerance test was not different between LDim and LD, either when the two rhythms in the LDim animals were aligned (P = 0.715) or misaligned (P = 0.368). Within the animals exposed to LDim, there was no difference in iAUC between the misaligned and the aligned state (P = 0.902) (Supplementary Figure 4).
Discussion
In the present paper we show that in the male Wistar rat, LDim strongly reduced the daily rhythms in sleep-wake behavior. LDim acutely increased sleep in the active phase during the first dim light (active) phase with a further increase over the following days, whereas LDim gradually decreased sleep during the light (inactive) phase. The reduced sleep-wake rhythm amplitude was paralleled by reduced rhythms in the 16–19 Hz EEG frequency domain and SCN clock gene expression. In a circadian analysis, LDim surprisingly induced an endogenous free running rhythm in locomotor activity with a period of ~25 h that interacted with the entrained 24 h rhythm. The reduced daily rhythms of sleep, food intake, and energy expenditure in LDim did not cause obesity or impaired glucose tolerance.
Already during the first 12 hours of dim light exposure, LDim reduced waking and increased NREM and REM sleep. This acute increase in NREM and REM sleep may be a direct effect of the dim light on sleep regulatory areas of the brain such as the ventrolateral preoptic nucleus or the ventral subparaventricular zone, since these areas receive direct information from intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs)27 and ipRGCs are sensitive to dim light levels28. Alternatively, the acute increase of NREM and REM sleep due to LDim may be mediated by decreased melatonin levels, as the dim light levels used are sufficient to decrease melatonin levels29 and increased melatonin has been reported to reduce REM and NREM sleep in rats30.
The effects of LDim on sleep increased over time. While LDim did not affect vigilance states in the light phase directly following the first 12 hours of dim light exposure, after 7 days LDim decreased NREM sleep during the light phase, and after 14 days LDim also increased waking during the light phase. The rhythm in NREM sleep slow wave activity, representing homeostatic sleep drive in the rat31,32,33 was nearly abolished after 2 weeks of LDim exposure, in accordance with a near-equal distribution of sleep over the 24-h cycle. LDim also strongly decreased the rhythm in the 16–19 Hz frequency domain, which is within the NREM sleep EEG frequency range identified to reflect the rhythm of the endogenous circadian timing system in the Wistar rat33,34.
Together, the gradual increase of the effect of LDim on waking, NREM sleep, and REM sleep over the days, and the loss of rhythm in the 16–19 Hz frequency domain indicated an effect of LDim on the endogenous circadian timing system. To further investigate the effect of LDim on the endogenous period and rhythm strength, we exposed animals to LDim for 30 days followed by continuous darkness. Strikingly, in all animals LDim induced a free running rhythm in locomotor activity with a period of 25.1 h that interfered with the 24-h rhythm. LDim persistently affected the endogenous rhythm, since the transfer from LDim to DD resulted in a free running rhythm with a period of 24.8 h and low rhythmic strength, whereas the transfer from LD to DD resulted in a free running rhythm with a period that was close to 24 h and a higher rhythmic strength. Theoretically, the occurrence of two interacting rhythms in locomotor behavior in LDim can be due to either two separate endogenous rhythms or one free running endogenous rhythm with a period of 25.1 h in combination with one rhythm that is synchronized to the external 24 h rhythm (masking). After transfer from LDim to DD, rhythmic strength was lower in the initial 10 days (with three animals being virtually arrhythmic), compared to the second 10 days in DD, which supports the notion that the two rhythms may be two desynchronized endogenous rhythms that gradually resynchronize over time in DD.
In a control experiment we verified whether the occurrence of two interacting free running rhythms was specific for LDim and not due to continuous exposure to minimally 5 lux. Indeed, under continuous dim light (DimDim) or continuous light (LL), no rhythm splitting was observed. Instead, in DimDim, rats showed a single free running rhythm with a period duration of 24.9 hr, which was comparable to the period duration of the free running component under LDim. The increased free running period duration in continuous dim light compared to the free running period in continuous darkness after LD is in line with Aschoff’s rule that predicts an increase of period duration with increasing intensity of light exposure in nocturnal animals35,36,37. The complete loss of rhythmicity that we observed in LL has been reported before under high light intensities37,38,39.
The interaction within one animal of two rhythms in locomotor activity with a different period, also called internal desynchronization, has been described previously in various desynchronization paradigms in the vole40, the mouse41,42, and the rat43. However, to the best of our knowledge, two interacting rhythms in locomotor activity have never been described in a 12:12 L:D lighting schedule. Thus, our present data, for the first time, indicate that internal desynchronization can occur in a situation when the light-dark cycle adheres to the natural cycle duration of 24 hr. This finding may have translational consequences, since humans usually adhere to a 24-hr cycle.
The rodent and human circadian timing system share many common features. Both in rodents and humans, the circadian effects of light are mediated by ipRGCs. In both species, these ipRGCs detect light with the photopigment melanopsin that has maximal sensitivity for blue light with a wavelength of approximately 480 nm, and integrate this information with input from the classical photoreceptors in retinal rods and cones44. Subsequently, ipRGCs transfer the integrated light information to hypothalamic areas including the SCN27,44. The SCN rhythm of firing activity and neuropeptide expression is largely similar between nocturnal and diurnal mammalian species45. Despite these similarities, there are differences between the rodent and human circadian timing system in the sensitivity to light44: In humans for example, light with an intensity of 1 lux is barely sufficient to entrain the circadian pacemaker46, whereas in rodents, approximately 10-fold lower light levels clearly affect SCN neuronal firing47 and circadian period48. In the present paper we used 5 lux as a light stimulus in order to replicate commonly used dim light conditions18,24,25. We are aware that lux is a measure defined by a weighting function specifically designed for the spectral sensitivity of the human image-forming visual system. Unfortunately, at present there is no validated alternative weighting function for the circadian effects of light44, although there have been some first attempts49,50.
The translational value of our findings is limited to the effects of dim light at night on the circadian timing system, since the direct effects of light at night is opposite between species: in rodents bright light induces sleep16 whereas in humans bright light induces waking51.
With these caveats in mind, we believe that it is important to realize that internal desynchronization due to light at night as described in the present study, might be responsible for the association between light at night and insomnia in humans. It is interesting to note that the light intensity of 5 lux is frequently encountered in human bedrooms13, but keeping in mind the differences in sensitivity to light between the rodents and the human circadian timing system, it will be important to investigate if these light levels also cause internal desynchronization in diurnal species.
It is well known that the rodent SCN contains multiple cell groups that can function as separate pacemakers with a different period length in terms of clock gene expression43,52 and neuropeptide secretion53,54,55. In Wistar rats subjected to a 22-h LD cycle, two interacting rhythms in behavior were coupled to two separate subdivisions of the SCN with the dorsomedial SCN representing the rhythm with a period of 24.9 h, and the ventrolateral SCN representing the rhythm with a period of 22 h43. In order to investigate the representation within the SCN of the two rhythmic components that we observed in locomotor behavior, we analyzed SCN clock gene expression at the time when the two rhythmic components in behavior were aligned or misaligned. LDim caused an overall reduction of the amplitude of the daily rhythm of Per1 and Arntl expression, but no significant desynchronization between ventrolateral and dorsomedial SCN could be detected. Possibly, the sensitivity of the two time-point analysis is insufficient to detect two independent rhythms in our model. Alternatively, the spatiotemporal organization of the two presumed pacemakers within the SCN may be less pronounced in our model compared to the 22-h day paradigm43. Future studies with transgenic clock-gene luciferase rats56 may elucidate the anatomical representation of the free running rhythm.
Thus, we introduce a rat model for the effects of dim light at night on the circadian control of sleep. In contrast to the Swiss Webster mouse model25, we found profound disturbances of vigilance state distribution and sleep architecture. Based on the mouse model18,24, we expected that the circadian disruptions would have adverse metabolic consequences. However, despite increased daytime food intake during LDim on both chow and HFD, we did not observe effects of LDim on body weight, adiposity or glucose tolerance. On the chow diet, animals exposed to LDim did show a slightly smaller increase of food intake over the weeks compared to animals exposed to LD. Since body weight gain was not affected by the light schedule, this suggests that LDim may induce slightly more weight gain per g of food consumed (i.e. higher feeding efficiency). However, when the animals were induced to become obese with a high fat diet, this effect was not reproduced. The absence of metabolic effects contrasts to the Swiss Webster mouse model, where increased daytime food intake due to dim light at night causes obesity and decreased glucose tolerance18,24. The differences between the mouse model and our rat model may be attributable to species differences in the susceptibility to sleep disturbance or metabolic disease57,58 or to the difference in light phase duration since the Swiss Webster mouse model utilized a 16:818 or 14:1024,25 L:Dim cycle whereas we used a 12:12 L:Dim cycle.
In conclusion, we present the first rodent model for disrupted circadian control of sleep-wake rhythms due to light at night. In male Wistar rats, LDim strongly reduced the daily amplitude of sleep-wake and feeding-fasting behavior. This amplitude reduction was due to a direct sleep promoting effect combined with reduced SCN output strength due to internal desynchronization between the environmentally entrained 24 h rhythm and the LDim induced free running rhythm of ~25 h. We show that internal desynchronization is possible in a 12:12 L:D cycle, and therefore identified a potential mechanism for the association between light at night and insomnia in humans. Taken together, the detrimental effects of artificial light at night on the circadian control of sleep warrant attention from policy makers, researchers, and anyone with the control over a light switch.
Methods
Animals
Male Wistar WU rats (Charles River, Germany) were used for all experiments. Before the start of experimental procedures rats were given at least one week time for accommodation to the animal facility with standard chow and water available ad libitum. Experimental protocols were approved by the Animal Experiments Ethical Committees of the Leiden University Medical Center and/or the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, and the study was performed in accordance with Dutch laws.
Light schedules
During accommodation, rats were exposed to a 12-h light (150–200lux):12-h dark (LD) schedule. To investigate the effect of dim light at night, rats were exposed to a 12 h light (150–200 lux):12 h dim white light (5 lux) (LDim) schedule, while control animals continued exposure to the LD schedule. During vigilance state and circadian activity experiments, dim light was emitted by white fluorescent tubes placed above the cage. During SCN clock gene expression and metabolic experiments, dim light was emitted by white LED lights placed around the cage. All light intensities were verified in the animal gaze direction with an AvaSpec 2048-SPU (Avantus BV, the Netherlands) light meter. The light source spectral power distributions are shown in Supplementary Figure 5.
Vigilance state experiment
Rats (n = 13; bodyweight 283 ± 5 g) were anesthetized with Ketamine/Xylazine/Atropine, fixed in a stereotaxic frame and implanted with 2 electroencephalogram (EEG) screw electrodes (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA, USA; 1.4 mm diameter) and 2 electromyogram (EMG) patch electrodes (Plastics One; 0.35 mm diameter) as described previously26,31,32,33. One EEG electrode was placed over the right hemisphere above the sensory cortex, while the other was placed above the cerebellum. The EMG electrodes were inserted between the neck muscles and the skin. The wires of the electrodes were set in a plastic pedestal (Plastics One) which was fixed to the skull with dental cement and 3 additional support screws.
After 7–10 days recovery from surgery in individual housing, rats were divided into two bodyweight-matched groups and exposed to either LDim (n = 8) or LD (n = 5). Twenty-four h EEG/EMG recordings were performed at baseline (day 0, prior to the LDim schedule), day 1, day 7 and day 14. Recordings on day 0 and day 1 started at ZT12, recordings on day 7 and day 14 started at ZT0. The EEG and EMG were recorded with a portable system (Institute of Pharmacology and Toxicology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland) as described previously59,60. Before each recording, a calibration signal (10 Hz sine wave, 300 μV peak-to-peak) was recorded on the EEG and EMG channel. Both signals were amplified (amplification factor ~2000), conditioned by analogue filters (high-pass filter −3 dB at 0.16 Hz) and sampled at 512 Hz. The signals were filtered through a digital Finite Impulse Response filter: EEG low-pass filter at 30 Hz and EMG band-pass filter between 20 and 40 Hz.
The vigilance states (waking, NREM sleep and REM sleep) were scored off-line in 4-s epochs by visual inspection of the raw EEG and EMG signal, as well as EEG power density in the slow-wave range (0.75–4.0 Hz) according to standardized criteria for rats26,61. Artifact free EEG power density values were computed for 4-s epochs in the range between 0.25–25.0 Hz59,60,62. Between 0.25 and 5.0 Hz, values were collapsed into 0.5 Hz bins, between 5.25–25.0 Hz in 1-Hz bins. Average values of the amount of each vigilance state and the corresponding spectra, and changes over the day in vigilance states and EEG power density, were computed for each vigilance state and for each recording day. EEG power density data were standardized relative to the individual 24-h mean baseline value in NREM sleep (=100%) and subsequently averaged over all animals.
Circadian experiments
Rats (n = 8, bodyweight 254 ± 1 g) were individually housed in transparent plastic cages and locomotor activity was monitored by passive infrared detectors using Actimetrics software (Wilmette, Il, USA). In circadian experiment A, animals were first exposed to 10 days LD and subsequently to 30 days LDim, 20 days constant darkness (DD), re-entrainment during 30 days LD and finally 10 days DD. In circadian experiment B we investigated the effect of continuous light intensity on circadian activity by exposing the rats (n = 8, bodyweight 251 ± 2 g) consecutively to at least 10 days LD, 16 days constant dim light (DimDim), 22 days constant light (LL, 150–200 lux), 32 days DimDim, 14 days LD and 16 days DimDim.
SCN clock gene expression experiment
Individually housed rats (n = 42, bodyweight 254 ± 4 g) were exposed to either LDim or LD and locomotor activity was measured using pressure-sensitive baseplates5. For LDim animals, the moments were determined when the activity phase of the free running rhythm and the activity phase of the 24-h rhythm were either aligned or misaligned, as determined by visual inspection of the actogram combined with F-periodogram analysis63,64. Rats were divided into 6 groups (n = 7 per group) and sacrificed accordingly: 1) LDim aligned ZT6, 2) LDim aligned ZT18, 3) LDim misaligned ZT6, 4) LDim misaligned ZT18, 5) LD ZT6 and 6) LD ZT18. Animals were briefly anesthetized with 80% CO2 and decapitated. Brains were frozen on dry ice and stored at −80 °C. Coronal 20 μm slices of the SCN were obtained with a cryostat. In situ hybridization of Per1 and Arntl (Bmal1) were performed using riboprobes of rPer1 (kindly provided by Dr. H. Okamura, Kyoto University, Japan) and mArntl (mBmal1)65 genes. Antisense RNA probes were generated with an in vitro transcription kit (Maxiscript; Ambion, Austin, TX, USA). Hybridization was carried out as described previously66. Slices and radioactive standards were exposed to an autoradiographic film (Biomax MS-1 Kodak, Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO, USA). Quantitative analysis of the autoradiograms was performed using the ImageJ software (W. Rasband, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA). A Nissl staining was performed on the same slides and the section containing the mid SCN was selected. The optical density of the whole SCN, the ventromedial SCN and the dorsolateral SCN were measured by a researcher (JMen) who was blind for treatment, Relative optical density was determined by subtracting the background intensity (measured in the anterior hypothalamic area) from the signal for each animal. Left and right sided relative optical densities were averaged and the dorsomedial/ventrolateral ratio43 was calculated.
Metabolic experiments
The first metabolic experiment was performed with 8 rats (bodyweight 201 ± 2 g) on a regular chow diet (Harlan Teklad Global Diet 2918, Harlan Laboratories Inc, Madison, Wisconsin, USA). Individually housed rats were divided into two bodyweight-matched groups and exposed to either LDim or LD. For the assessment of food intake and energy expenditure, rats were placed in PhenoMaster/LabMaster calorimetric cages (TSE Systems GmbH, Bad Homburg, Germany) in week 0 (baseline, before start of dim light exposure), and 1, 2 and 7 weeks after the start of dim light exposure. Rats were sacrificed after 8 weeks. The second metabolic experiment was performed with 16 rats (bodyweight 196 ± 1 g) on a high fat diet (HFD) (D12451 high fat diet, Research Diets, Inc, New Brunswick, New Jersey, USA). Again, rats were divided into two bodyweight-matched groups and exposed to either LDim or LD. HFD animals were placed in the calorimetric cages in week 0 (baseline, before start of dim light exposure), and 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 8 and 9 weeks after the start of dim light exposure, and HFD animals were sacrificed after 11 weeks. Rats were given at least 2 hours to accommodate to the calorimetric cages before the 48-hr recordings started.
After sacrificing the animals, the mesenteric, epidydimal, perirenal and subcutaneous WAT depots, adrenals and thymus from the right side of the rat were collected and weighed.
Glucose tolerance experiment
Individually housed rats were divided into two bodyweight-matched groups (n = 7 per group) and exposed to either LDim or LD for at least 7 days before surgery. Locomotor activity was measured using pressure-sensitive baseplates5. The rats (bodyweight 320 ± 11 g at surgery) were anesthetized with fentanyl/fluanisone/midazolam, a silicone catheter was inserted into the right atrium via the jugular vein and attached to the skull with 4 screws and dental cement5. Rats were given at least 1 week to recover, and glucose tolerance was assessed at ZT6 twice in every rat: at the time when the activity phases of the free running and 24 h rhythms were aligned and when the activity phases were misaligned (as determined by visual inspection of the actogram combined with F-periodogram analysis63,64). One day before the intravenous glucose tolerance test (IVGTT)5 rats were attached to a head stage that allows infusion and blood sampling in freely moving animals. On the day of the experiment, food was removed 4 h before the glucose injection. A 1000 mg/kg bodyweight glucose bolus (Sigma-Aldrich) was injected at t = 0 and blood samples were obtained from the catheter at t = −1, 5, 10, 20, 30 and 60 min. Glucose was measured immediately with a FreeStyle Freedom Lite glucose measurement device (Abott Diabetes Care, Alameda, CA, USA).
Statistical analysis
Normally distributed variables are expressed as mean and standard error of the mean (SEM). Vigilance state and EEG data were analyzed with repeated measures ANOVA with the factors Phase (Light, Dark/Dim), Day (0, 1, 7, 14) and their Interaction. Power density spectra of NREM sleep were analyzed with ANOVA with the factors Frequency (0.25–25.0 Hz), Day (0, 1, 7, 14) and their Interaction. For circadian locomotor activity data, the rhythm period and strength were determined with F-periodogram analysis63 as previously described64. Rhythm strengths were analyzed by ANOVA with the factor Light schedule (LD, LDim, DD). SCN gene expression data were analyzed with ANOVA using the factors Time (ZT6, ZT18), Group (LD, LDim-aligned, LDim-misaligned) and their Interaction.
Data from the calorimetric cages were analyzed with a linear mixed model in order to correctly incorporate missing data, with Light schedule (LD, LDim), Week (week 0–9) and their Interaction as fixed effects. The covariance structure ‘Compound Symmetry’ was selected based on the Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC).
If a significant main effect was detected in ANOVA or linear mixed model analyses, post hoc tests were performed with an independent samples two-tailed student’s t-test (for 2 groups with independent samples), a related samples two-tailed student’s t-test (for 2 groups with related samples) or Duncan’s test (for >2 groups).
For glucose tolerance data, the iAUC was calculated for individual animals as described previously67. The effect of LDim exposure on body weight, organ weight and glucose tolerance was assessed with a student’s independent samples two-tailed t-test or a student’s related samples two-tailed t-test where appropriate.
All statistical analysis were performed with SPSS statistics (v21, SPSS, Inc) using a significance level of 0.05.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Stenvers, D. J. et al. Dim light at night disturbs the daily sleep-wake cycle in the rat. Sci. Rep. 6, 35662; doi: 10.1038/srep35662 (2016).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to M. Stok for assistance with animal experiments, Dr. J.W. Stenvers and Dr. R. de Roode for technical assistance, Dr. N. van Geloven for statistical advice, Dr. H. Okamura for providing the Per1 plasmid and Dr. H. Dardente for providing the Arntl clone. D.J.S. is supported by ZonMW Agiko stipend 92003592. J.M. is supported by the Agence National de la Recherche (ANR-14-CE13-0002-01 ADDiCLOCK JCJC). A.L.O. is supported by STW grant “OnTime” (#12195).
Footnotes
Author Contributions Conceptualization, D.J.S., P.H.B., A.K. and T.D.; Investigation, D.J.S., R.v.D., E.F., J.M. and A.L.O.; Writing-Original Draft, D.J.S. and T.D.; Writing-Review & Editing, E.F., J.M., J.H.M. and A.K.; Supervision, A.K. and T.D.; Funding Acquisition D.J.S., P.H.B., A.K. and T.D.
References
- Navara K. J. & Nelson R. J. The dark side of light at night: physiological, epidemiological, and ecological consequences. J.Pineal Res. 43, 215–224 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cinzano P., Falchi F. & Elvidge C. D. The first World Atlas of the artificial night sky brightness. Monthly Notices of the Royal Astronomical Society 328, 689–707 (2001). [Google Scholar]
- Saper C. B., Scammell T. E. & Lu J. Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian rhythms. Nature 437, 1257–1263 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stenvers D. J., Jonkers C. F., Fliers E., Bisschop P. H. & Kalsbeek A. Nutrition and the circadian timing system. Progress in brain research 199, 359–376 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- La Fleur S. E., Kalsbeek A., Wortel J., Fekkes M. L. & Buijs R. M. A daily rhythm in glucose tolerance: a role for the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Diabetes 50, 1237–1243 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spiegel K., Tasali E., Leproult R. & Van Cauter E. Effects of poor and short sleep on glucose metabolism and obesity risk. Nature Reviews Endocrinology 5, 253–261 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skomro R. P., Ludwig S., Salamon E. & Kryger M. H. Sleep complaints and restless legs syndrome in adult type 2 diabetics. Sleep Med 2, 417–422 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh J. K. et al. Nighttime insomnia symptoms and perceived health in the America Insomnia Survey (AIS). Sleep 34, 997–1011 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danaei G. et al. National, regional, and global trends in fasting plasma glucose and diabetes prevalence since 1980: systematic analysis of health examination surveys and epidemiological studies with 370 country-years and 2.7 million participants. Lancet 378, 31–40 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonken L. K. & Nelson R. J. The Effects of Light at Night on Circadian Clocks and Metabolism. Endocr.Rev. 35, 648–670 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obayashi K., Saeki K. & Kurumatani N. Association between light exposure at night and insomnia in the general elderly population: the HEIJO-KYO cohort. Chronobiology international 31, 976–982 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFadden E., Jones M. E., Schoemaker M. J., Ashworth A. & Swerdlow A. J. The relationship between obesity and exposure to light at night: cross-sectional analyses of over 100,000 women in the Breakthrough Generations Study. American journal of epidemiology 180, 245–250 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obayashi K. et al. Exposure to Light at Night, Nocturnal Urinary Melatonin Excretion, and Obesity/Dyslipidemia in the Elderly: A Cross-Sectional Analysis of the HEIJO-KYO Study. JCEM 98, 337–344 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho C. H. et al. Exposure to dim artificial light at night increases REM sleep and awakenings in humans. Chronobiology international 33, 117–123 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cho J. R., Joo E. Y., Koo D. L. & Hong S. B. Let there be no light: the effect of bedside light on sleep quality and background electroencephalographic rhythms. Sleep Med. 14, 1422–1425 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borbely A. A. Sleep and motor activity of the rat during ultra-short light-dark cycles. Brain Res 114, 305–317 (1976). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plata-Salaman C. R. & Oomura Y. Food intake dependence on acute changes in light schedule. Physiol Behav. 41, 135–140 (1987). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonken L. K. et al. Light at night increases body mass by shifting the time of food intake. PNAS 107, 18664–18669 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coomans C. P. et al. Detrimental effects of constant light exposure and high-fat diet on circadian energy metabolism and insulin sensitivity. FASEB journal 27, 1721–1732 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephenson R., Lim J., Famina S., Caron A. M. & Dowse H. B. Sleep-wake behavior in the rat: ultradian rhythms in a light-dark cycle and continuous bright light. Journal of biological rhythms 27, 490–501 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raap T., Pinxten R. & Eens M. Light pollution disrupts sleep in free-living animals. Scientific reports 5, 13557 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dauchy R. T. et al. Dark-phase light contamination disrupts circadian rhythms in plasma measures of endocrine physiology and metabolism in rats. Comp Med. 60, 348–356 (2010). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dauchy R. T. et al. The influence of red light exposure at night on circadian metabolism and physiology in Sprague-Dawley rats. J Am Assoc Lab Anim Sci 54, 40–50 (2015). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fonken L. K., Lieberman R. A., Weil Z. M. & Nelson R. J. Dim light at night exaggerates weight gain and inflammation associated with a high-fat diet in male mice. Endocrinology 154, 3817–3825 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borniger J. C., Weil Z. M., Zhang N. & Nelson R. J. Dim light at night does not disrupt timing or quality of sleep in mice. Chronobiology International 30, 1016–1023 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deboer T., Vansteensel M. J., Detari L. & Meijer J. H. Sleep states alter activity of suprachiasmatic nucleus neurons. Nature neuroscience 6, 1086–1090 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gooley J. J., Lu J., Fischer D. & Saper C. B. A broad role for melanopsin in nonvisual photoreception. J Neurosci 23, 7093–7106 (2003). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey D. M. et al. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature 433, 749–754 (2005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch H. J., Deng M. H. & Wurtman R. J. Light intensities required to suppress nocturnal melatonin secretion in albino and pigmented rats. Life sciences 35, 841–847 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huber R., Deboer T., Schwierin B. & Tobler I. Effect of melatonin on sleep and brain temperature in the Djungarian hamster and the rat. Physiol Behav 65, 77–82 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deboer T. Sleep and sleep homeostasis in constant darkness in the rat. Journal of sleep research 18, 357–364 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deboer T., Detari L. & Meijer J. H. Long term effects of sleep deprivation on the mammalian circadian pacemaker. Sleep 30, 257–262 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yasenkov R. & Deboer T. Circadian regulation of sleep and the sleep EEG under constant sleep pressure in the rat. Sleep 33, 631–641 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yasenkov R. & Deboer T. Interrelations and circadian changes of electroencephalogram frequencies under baseline conditions and constant sleep pressure in the rat. Neuroscience 180, 212–221 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferraro J. S. & McCormack C. E. Minimum duration of light signals capable of producing the Aschoff effect. Physiol Behav 38, 139–144 (1986). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aschoff J. Exogenous and endogenous components in circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 25, 11–28 (1960). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Summer T. L., Ferraro J. S. & McCormack C. E. Phase-response and Aschoff illuminance curves for locomotor activity rhythm of the rat. The American journal of physiology 246, R299–R304 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honma K. I. & Hiroshige T. Endogenous ultradian rhythms in rats exposed to prolonged continuous light. The American journal of physiology 235, R250–R256 (1978). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenwasser A. M. Circadian drinking rhythms in SHR and WKY rats: effects of increasing light intensity. Physiol Behav 53, 1035–1041 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Veen D. R., Saaltink D. J. & Gerkema M. P. Behavioral responses to combinations of timed light, food availability, and ultradian rhythms in the common vole (Microtus arvalis). Chronobiology international 28, 563–571 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ono D., Honma S. & Honma K. Postnatal constant light compensates Cryptochrome1 and 2 double deficiency for disruption of circadian behavioral rhythms in mice under constant dark. PloS one 8, e80615 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casiraghi L. P., Oda G. A., Chiesa J. J., Friesen W. O. & Golombek D. A. Forced desynchronization of activity rhythms in a model of chronic jet lag in mice. Journal of biological rhythms 27, 59–69 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de la Iglesia H. O., Cambras T., Schwartz W. J. & Diez-Noguera A. Forced desynchronization of dual circadian oscillators within the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. Current biology 14, 796–800 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucas R. J. et al. Measuring and using light in the melanopsin age. Trends Neurosci 37, 1–9 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smale L., Lee T. & Nunez A. A. Mammalian diurnality: some facts and gaps. Journal of biological rhythms 18, 356–366 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright K. P. Jr., Hughes R. J., Kronauer R. E., Dijk D. J. & Czeisler C. A. Intrinsic near-24-h pacemaker period determines limits of circadian entrainment to a weak synchronizer in humans. PNAS 98, 14027–14032 (2001). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meijer J. H., Watanabe K., Schaap J., Albus H. & Detari L. Light responsiveness of the suprachiasmatic nucleus: long-term multiunit and single-unit recordings in freely moving rats. J Neurosci. 18, 9078–9087 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cambras T. & Diez-Noguera A. Effects of Forward and Backward Transitions in Light Intensities in Tau-Illuminance Curves of the Rat Motor Activity Rhythm Under Constant Dim Light. Chronobiology international 29, 693–701 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rea M. S., Figueiro M. G., Bullough J. D. & Bierman A. A model of phototransduction by the human circadian system. Brain Res Rev 50, 213–228 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen M., Mardaljevic J. & Lockley S. W. A framework for predicting the non-visual effects of daylight - Part I: photobiology-based model. Lighting Res Technol 44, 37–53 (2012). [Google Scholar]
- Zeitzer J. M., Ruby N. F., Fisicaro R. A. & Heller H. C. Response of the Human Circadian System to Millisecond Flashes of Light. PloS one 6 e22078 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans J. A., Leise T. L., Castanon-Cervantes O. & Davidson A. J. Dynamic interactions mediated by nonredundant signaling mechanisms couple circadian clock neurons. Neuron 80, 973–983 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinohara K., Honma S., Katsuno Y., Abe H. & Honma K. Two distinct oscillators in the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus in vitro. PNAS 92, 7396–7400 (1995). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noguchi T. & Watanabe K. Regional differences in circadian period within the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Brain Res 1239, 119–126 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura W., Honma S., Shirakawa T. & Honma K. Regional pacemakers composed of multiple oscillator neurons in the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. The European journal of neuroscience 14, 666–674 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He P. J., Hirata M., Yamauchi N., Hashimoto S. & Hattori M. A. Gonadotropic regulation of circadian clockwork in rat granulosa cells. Mol Cell Biochem 302, 111–118 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fellmann L., Nascimento A. R., Tibirica E. & Bousquet P. Murine models for pharmacological studies of the metabolic syndrome. Pharmacol Ther 137, 331–340 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opperhuizen A. L., van Kerkhof L. W., Proper K. I., Rodenburg W. & Kalsbeek A. Rodent models to study the metabolic effects of shiftwork in humans. Front Pharmacol 6, 50 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vyazovskiy V. V., Ruijgrok G., Deboer T. & Tobler I. Running wheel accessibility affects the regional electroencephalogram during sleep in mice. Cerebral cortex 16, 328–336 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deboer T., Ruijgrok G. & Meijer J. H. Short light-dark cycles affect sleep in mice. The European journal of neuroscience 26, 3518–3523 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franken P., Dijk D. J., Tobler I. & Borbely A. A. Sleep deprivation in rats: effects on EEG power spectra, vigilance states, and cortical temperature. The American journal of physiology 261, R198–R208 (1991). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deboer T., Fontana A. & Tobler I. Tumor necrosis factor (TNF) ligand and TNF receptor deficiency affects sleep and the sleep EEG. Journal of neurophysiology 88, 839–846 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dörrscheidt G. J. & Beck L. Advanced methods for evaluating characteristic parameters (tau, alpha, rho) of circadian rhythms. J. Math. Biology 2, 107–121 (1975). [Google Scholar]
- Jenni O. G., Deboer T. & Achermann P. Development of the 24-h rest-activity pattern in human infants. Infant behavior & development 29, 143–152 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendoza J., Graff C., Dardente H., Pevet P. & Challet E. Feeding cues alter clock gene oscillations and photic responses in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of mice exposed to a light/dark cycle. J Neurosci 25, 1514–1522 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feillet C. A., Mendoza J., Albrecht U., Pevet P. & Challet E. Forebrain oscillators ticking with different clock hands. Mol Cell Neurosci 37, 209–221 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stenvers D. J. et al. Breakfast replacement with a low-glycaemic response liquid formula in patients with type 2 diabetes: a randomised clinical trial. British Journal of Nutrition 112, 504–512 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.