Abstract
The human sense of smell is often analyzed as being composed of three main components comprising olfactory threshold, odor discrimination and the ability to identify odors. A relevant distinction of the three components and their differential changes in distinct disorders remains a research focus. The present data-driven analysis aimed at establishing a cluster structure in the pattern of olfactory subtest results. Therefore, unsupervised machine-learning was applied onto olfactory subtest results acquired in 10,714 subjects with nine different olfactory pathologies. Using the U-matrix, Emergent Self-organizing feature maps (ESOM) identified three different clusters characterized by (i) low threshold and good discrimination and identification, (ii) very high threshold associated with absent to poor discrimination and identification ability, or (iii) medium threshold, i.e., in the mid-range of possible thresholds, associated with reduced discrimination and identification ability. Specific etiologies of olfactory (dys)function were unequally represented in the clusters (p < 2.2 · 10−16). Patients with congenital anosmia were overrepresented in the second cluster while subjects with postinfectious olfactory dysfunction belonged frequently to the third cluster. However, the clusters provided no clear separation between etiologies. Hence, the present verification of a distinct cluster structure encourages continued scientific efforts at olfactory test pattern recognition.
Psychophysical tests assessing a subject’s olfactory performance most frequently include the assessment of a subject’s odor identification performance addressing the ability to name or associate an odor1,2. Developments of more comprehensive test batteries led to the inclusion of the assessment of a subject’s odor threshold addressing the lowest concentration of a selected odorant at which it is still perceived3. A third test is the assessment of a subject’s odor discrimination performance addressing the ability to distinguish different smells4. Contemporary olfactory test batteries include all of these components5 or a subset of olfactory tests6,7,8,9.
A distinct importance of the three main components of the sense of smell is an active research topic. The scientific and clinical interest dedicated this question owes to the aim at deeper understanding of the pathological mechanisms via which various different etiologies may cause olfactory dysfunction and by the desire to associate an olfactory dysfunction with a specific cause, for example in a medico-legal context. The discussion is maintained by suggestions that olfactory test measure a common source of variance10 that are contrasted with clinical evidence of distinct reactions of olfactory subtests to particular conditions, such as impaired odor identification but not thresholds after focal brain excision11 or in AIDS-related dementia12, or the predominant reduction of odor thresholds by drugs such as sildenafil13, remifentanil14 or tetrahydrocannabinol15.
Several approaches were taken to address the distinct importance of olfactory subtests reaching from correlative approaches16 to factor analyses17. In the present analysis, the problem was addressed using a data driven approach in a data set comprising olfactory thresholds, odor discrimination and odor identification scores available from 10,714 subjects. The subjects had various degrees of olfactory (dys)function associated with different underlying etiologies18. Unsupervised machine-learning19 was applied to find subgroups of patients sharing similar olfactory subtest patterns. To address the effects of common causes of olfactory deficits on the olfactory subtests, the identified patterns were subsequently assessed for their association with different etiologies of altered olfactory function.
Methods
Subjects and olfactory testing
The study followed the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the TU Dresden (number EK251112006). Informed written consent was obtained from all subjects. Subjects (age: range 6–95 years, mean ± standard deviation: 52.2 ± 17 years; sex: 6,004 men, 4,710 women) were included since they had presented at the Smell & Taste Clinic, Dept. of ORL, TU Dresden with the symptom “olfactory loss”, or they had been tested in the context of a clinical standard check or they were enrolled in research projects as healthy controls. Subjects represented several different etiologies associated with olfactory performance (Table 1).
Table 1. Demographics of the enrolled subjects, separately for the subjects’ sex and the physiological or pathological (etiology) condition associated with the subjects’ olfactory functional acuity.
Etiology | Men |
Women |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
n (% within etiology) | Age (mean, SD, range) [years] | n (%) | Age (mean, SD, range) [years] | |
Healthy | 928 (44.2) | 35.9 ± 16.4, 6–90 | 1171 (55.8) | 35 ± 16.2, 7–90 |
Sinunasal disease | 852 (52) | 56.2 ± 12.7, 18–87 | 785 (48) | 54.4 ± 12.8, 14–86 |
Congenital | 79 (38.9) | 30.8 ± 16.3, 8–65 | 124 (61.1) | 28.9 ± 16.4, 7–74 |
Neurodegenerative | 91 (57.6) | 64.4 ± 10.8, 28–83 | 67 (42.4) | 64.7 ± 11.3, 37–89 |
Idiopathic | 928 (47.6) | 61.4 ± 13.6, 11–89 | 1021 (52.4) | 58.4 ± 13.9, 15–90 |
Infectious | 961 (31.5) | 58.8 ± 12.6, 11–86 | 2092 (68.5) | 59.8 ± 11.2, 12–95 |
Head trauma | 801 (53.7) | 46.9 ± 15, 9–83 | 690 (46.3) | 51.1 ± 15.1, 11–82 |
Toxic | 52 (57.8) | 60.8 ± 12.4, 19–76 | 38 (42.2) | 59.6 ± 14.6, 14–80 |
Tumor/apoplectic | 18 (52.9) | 61.1 ± 13.1, 25–79 | 16 (47.1) | 58 ± 14.7, 28–88 |
SD: standard deviation.
Olfactory function was assessed using the clinically established “Sniffin’ Sticks” test battery (Burghart, Wedel, Germany20,21). As described previously18, the test uses felt-tip pens that contain solutions of odors. Olfactory stimulation is achieved by placing the pen with removed cap for approximately 3 s at 1–2 cm before the nostrils. Odor thresholds were obtained for the rose-like odor phenyl ethyl alcohol presented in 16 successive 1:2 dilution steps starting from a 4% solution using a three-alternative forced-choice task and a staircase paradigm. The odor threshold is finally estimated as the mean of the last four out of seven staircase reversals. Odor discrimination was determined with 16 triplets of pens, two of each triplet containing the same odor and the third a different, “target” one (for names of odors see ref. 20). Odor identification was determined with 16 further odors (for names of odors see ref. 20) using a four-alternative forced-choice task with presentation of a list of four descriptors for each pen.
Data analysis
Data were analyzed using the R software package (version 3.2.3 for Linux; http://CRAN.R-project.org/22). Following statistical exploration of effects of sex23,24 and age. a sex-specific linear age correction was applied to the olfactory subtest scores. Subsequently, the probability density function (PDF) of the corrected olfactory subtest results was analyzed using the Pareto density estimation (PDE). This is a kernel density estimator particularly suitable for the discovery of groups25. The distribution of olfactory subtest results was analyzed by fitting a separate Gaussian mixture model (GMM) to the PDE of each subtest distribution. This was performed using our R package “AdaptGauss” (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/AdaptGauss/index.html) released previously26. Following this analysis, the probability for each subject to belong to a particular Gaussian of the obtained mixtures could be calculated as the Bayesian posterior27. With m = 3 Gaussians of the GMM by which the distributions of the scores of olfactory threshold, discrimination and identification were described, accommodating low, medium and high scores in each subtest, a vector of nine posterior probabilities was associated with each subject.
Unsupervised machine learning was used to identify structures within the 9 × 10,714 sized data space. Specifically, Emergent Self-organizing feature maps (ESOM) were applied in combination with the use of the U-matrix28. ESOM are based on a topology-preserving projection of high-dimensional data points xi ϵ RD onto a two dimensional self-organizing network consisting of a grid of neurons. This approach represents a topology preserving mapping of high-dimensional data points onto a two dimensional grid of neurons which was therefore favored to more common clustering algorithms such as k-means, Ward, complete- and average linkage29. The neural network consisted of a two-dimensional toroid grid of so-called neurons with 50 rows and 80 columns (n = 4,000 units). Each neuron holds a vector carrying “weights” of the same dimensions as the input dimensions, i.e., the Bayesian posteriors from GMM as described above. The weights were initially randomly drawn from the range of the data variables and subsequently adapted to the data during the learning phase that used 30 epochs respectively sweeps through the data.
A trained emergent self-organizing map (ESOM) was obtained that represented the subjects on a two-dimensional toroid map as the localizations of their respective “best matching units” (BMU), i.e., neurons on the grid that after ESOM learning carried the vector that was most similar to a subjects’ data vector. Finally, on top of this SOM grid the distance structure in the high dimensional feature space was visualized in the form of a so-called U-Matrix29,30. This was further enhanced by calculating a P-matrix that displays the point density p(x) = |{data points xi| d(xi, x) < = r}| estimated as the number of data points in a sphere with radius r around x at each grid point on the ESOM’s output grid. The U*-matrix combines distance structures (U-matrix) and density structures (P-matrix) into a single matrix28. A geographical map analogy was used to enhance the visual detection of borders of data clusters. That is, on this landscape form large heights represented large distances in the feature space while low “valleys” represented data subsets that are similar, while watersheds resembling mountain range separate data clusters. These procedures were performed using our R library “Umatrix” (M. Thrun, F. Lerch, Marburg, Germany, http://www.uni-marburg.de/fb12/datenbionik/software31; file http://www.uni-marburg.de/fb12/datenbionik/umatrix.tar.gz). The association of the identified clusters with the etiologies underlying a subject’s olfactory function was analyzed by means of χ2 statistics and by calculating the relative difference between the number of observed and the number of expected subjects in a cluster, relDiff = (nexpected – nobsered)/(0.5 (nexpected + nobsered)), as a metric for differentially represented items32. Finally, to further interpret the associations between olfactory subtests, the correlation coefficients, Spearman’s ρ33, were calculated for discrimination/identification, discrimination/threshold and threshold/identification, for the complete data set and separately for each etiological subset.
Results
Machine-learned identification of clusters of olfactory subtest results
Following projection of the vector space comprising the olfactory subtest results of each subject onto a toroid grid of 50 × 80 = 4,000 neurons and training of a self-organizing map, a U*-matrix was displayed on top of this SOM. This provided an emergent self-organizing feature map (ESOM) in which large U-heights in the U*-matrix indicated a large gap in the data space whereas low U-heights indicated that the points are close to each other in the data space indicating a distance based cluster structure in the data set. On a 3D-display of this U-matrix (Fig. 1), valleys, ridges and basins demonstrate how the distance based cluster structure could be visualized in the data set.
This cluster structure seen on the U*-matrix provided three main clusters (green, blue and magenta points in Fig. 1). The clusters were characterized (Fig. 2) by (cluster #1) a low olfactory threshold (median at dilution step #13.5) associated with good odor discrimination and identification performance, or by (cluster #2) a very high olfactory threshold (median at the first dilution step) that was associated with absent to poor odor discrimination and identification ability, or by (cluster #3) a medium olfactory threshold (median at dilution step #5). i.e., at the lower part of the mid-range of possible thresholds according to the test battery and associated with still preserved odor discrimination and identification ability.
Relationship of olfactory subtest patterns and etiologies associated with olfactory function
A projection of the etiologies noted during clinical subject enrolment onto the cluster structure obtained on the ESOM/U*-matrix discouraged a strong meaningful association. Specifically, while a clear overrepresentation of healthy subjects in the cluster with consistently good olfactory subtest results (cluster #1) was observed, the pathological etiologies seemed to be scattered across the other clusters (Fig. 3 middle). However, despite this impression the association of specific etiologies of olfactory (dys)function to the machine-leaned clusters was unequal at a high statistical significance level (χ2 = 8916.6, df = 16, p < 2.2 · 10−16) that remained significant when excluding the healthy subjects (χ2 = 396.26, df = 14, p < 2.2 · 10−16). Exploration of the representation of the etiologies among the clusters (Fig. 4) indicated (i) a clear overrepresentation of healthy subjects in cluster #1 where all pathological etiologies seemed to be underrepresented, (ii) an overrepresentation of subjects with congenital anosmia and possibly of those with head trauma in cluster #2 where healthy subjects were clearly underrepresented and (iii) an overrepresentation of subjects postinfectious olfactory dysfunction in cluster #3. However, consistent with the distribution of the subjects across the clusters on the ESOM/U-matrix, the clusters provided no clear separation between etiologies associated with the subjects’ olfactory function. The distribution of 3,326 anosmic patients, 4,664 hyposmic patients and 2,624 patients or healthy subjects with normosmia diagnosed according to the criteria of the “Sniffin’ Sticks” test battery was consistent with a known broad variability of the functional consequences of most etiologies associated with normal or altered olfactory function (Fig. 3 right). Finally, the correlations between olfactory subjects were statistically significant at the global level (discrimination/identification: ρ = 0.551, discrimination/threshold: ρ = 0.464, threshold/identification: ρ = 0.71, all p < 10−5). The correlations between olfactory subtest results also varied among the different etiologies (Fig. 5) from virtually absent (ρ < 0.2) to strong (ρ > 0.7)34, and the same analyses applied to the ESOM/U-matrix based clusters produced similar results (Fig. 5).
Discussion
Results of this analysis support the existence of structure among olfactory subtest results. This was obtained using a data-driven approach that did not predict such structure. In the unsupervised machine-learning approach, the goal was to discover structure, i.e., the first step of the present analysis was not to analyze implications of an observed or assumed cluster structure, but to establish whether or not such a structure exists in the pattern of the results of the three olfactory subtests. Only subsequently to establishing a cluster structure, the analysis aimed at associating different etiologies of possible olfactory dysfunction with the clusters identified in the data space. This differs from previous hypothesis-driven approaches to a differential importance of olfactory subtest results that used a correlational approach to test, whether the different subtests would correlate with different cognitive functions17 or analyzed similarities between subtest results without aiming at finding subgroups of patients sharing similar olfactory test pattern17. Interestingly, in one of these previous studies in 97 subjects no major difference was found between nine different tests of various aspects of olfactory function (tests of odor identification, discrimination, detection, memory, and suprathreshold intensity and pleasantness perception)10, although the number of subjects might have been too small to detect subtle differences considering the large variance typically seen with these olfactory tests.
With the observation that the different etiologies associated with different outcomes of the olfactory test battery were unevenly distributed across the clusters of olfactory subtest results, the present analysis is in line with previous reports about a different vulnerability of single subtest results to the effect of various causes of olfactory dysfunction (e.g. refs 12,35 and 36). Moreover, the different degrees of subtest correlation (Fig. 5) between the different etiologies, or clusters, also hint at distinct affections of olfactory function by diseases causing its deterioration. However, while previous reports often drew an unrestricted positive conclusion about the finding of such differences, the present results include the restriction that the observed pattern provide no clear picture of the distinct influence of etiologies of olfactory dysfunction on olfactory subtest pattern. The clusters could be only partially associated with an overrepresentation of a few etiologies, however, other etiologies seemed to produce often the same pattern. This could have been an effect of poor clustering. However, this is contrasted by previous demonstrations that the chosen ESOM/U-matrix method is able to identify clusters in data where classical clustering methods fail as on data of the Fundamental Clustering Problems Suite (FCPS) published as benchmark problems for clustering algorithms37. Popular clustering algorithms such as k-means, Ward, complete- and average linkage fail to cluster this data set correctly because these algorithms imply a geometrical model of the cluster structure. That is, k-means clustering implies a spherical shape of the clusters, while Ward hierarchical clustering implies a hyperelliptic shape. If this implicit assumption about cluster shape does not fit the cluster structure in the data, the clustering will fail. By contrast, Emergent Self-organizing feature maps (ESOM) using the U-matrix represent a topology preserving mapping of high-dimensional data points onto a two dimensional grid of neurons. Thus, the observed only weak association of etiologies with a distinct pattern of olfactory subtest results is probably a true observation. This suggests that for a complete distinctive pattern, further olfactory test results are needed in addition to the three subtests.
The most pronounced association of a particular cluster of olfactory subtest result pattern and an etiology underlying olfactory dysfunction was observed, besides for healthy subjects and cluster #1, for subjects suffering from congenital anosmia and, to a lower extent, for subjects with postinfectious olfactory dysfunction. Patients assigned to a congenital lack of olfaction were those who could not recall any perception of odors throughout their lives due to an agenesis/hypogenesis of their olfactory bulbs. They are typically diagnosed with “congenital anosmia” at an age of approximately 14–16 years; parents often (wrongly) believe that they are ill-observing and irresponsible parents. People with congenital anosmia seem to have a normal life38. Subjects with postinfectious olfactory dysfunction are typically women older than 50 years. The infection seems to damage the olfactory epithelium which causes olfactory loss. that is frequently incomplete, with a good chance for recovery (seen in about 2/3 of the patients)39.
Results encourage further pursuing pattern of olfactory function typical for certain etiologies. While three subtests probably are not enough, olfactory assessments include more options and therefore, the expectation that disease-typical olfactory pattern can be identified seems reasonable. It is in line with the increasing recognition of structure in human olfactory data18. This is likely to be of diagnostic utility. A clear pattern of olfactory test results associated with a particular etiology would also be desirable in medico-legal settings where, for example, a compensation for a claimed incident with lost olfactory function could be disputed on a basis that the pattern of olfactory test results correspond to a postinfectious dysfunction making a claimed traumatic or toxic etiology unlikely. Moreover, while present results suggest a differential modulation of olfactory subtest results by the various causes of dysfunction as reflected in the significant differences and apparent differences in the correlation levels of the three subtest among different etiologies or clusters, the results also clearly indicate that olfactory subtest results alone provide no sufficient information for a sensitive and specific association with a particular pathophysiological cause of the olfactory dysfunction. Possibly, among additional data needed to successfully approach the distinct olfactory pathologies is molecular information about the mechanisms via which different causes such as viral infection, toxic exposure, tumors or trauma influence the sense of smell at several different level exceeding the sensory perception of odors that was in the focus of the present analysis.
Conclusions
Using a data-driven approach, a cluster structure was found by applying unsupervised machine-learning19 to the results of olfactory subtests addressing olfactory thresholds, odor discrimination and odor identification. These are the olfactory dimensions implemented most frequently in the clinical testing of the human sense of smell. The observed pattern differed statistically significantly among various etiologies of olfactory dysfunction. While the three subtest results seem still insufficient to provide a distinction of the various different etiologies that can lead to olfactory dysfunction, the verification of a distinct cluster structure in the pattern of olfactory test results clearly encourages continued scientific efforts at olfactory test pattern recognition. Adding additional components of olfactory diagnostics there is a reasonable chance to succeed in a classification of olfactory dysfunction for the underlying etiology.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Lötsch, J. et al. Machine-learned pattern identification in olfactory subtest results. Sci. Rep. 6, 35688; doi: 10.1038/srep35688 (2016).
Acknowledgments
This work has been funded by the Landesoffensive zur Entwicklung wissenschaftlich - ökonomischer Exzellenz (LOEWE), LOEWE-Zentrum für Translationale Medizin und Pharmakologie (JL). Further support, in particular for the computing environment, was received from the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement no. 602919 (JL, GLORIA). We also would like to thank the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft for support (DFG HU441/18-1) to TH. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Author Contributions Conceived and designed the analysis: J.L. and A.U. Analyzed the data: J.L. and A.U. Wrote the paper: J.L. Provided data: T.H.
References
- Doty R. L., Shaman P., Kimmelman C. P. & Dann M. S. University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: a rapid quantitative olfactory function test for the clinic. Laryngoscope 94, 176–178 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain W. S. To know with the nose: keys to odor identification. Science 203, 467–470 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain W. S., Gent J. F., Goodspeed R. B. & Leonard G. Evaluation of olfactory dysfunction in the Connecticut Chemosensory Clinical Research Center. Laryngoscope 98, 83–88 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain W. S. & Krause R. J. Olfactory testing: rules for odor identification. Neurol Res 1, 1–9 (1979). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hummel T., Sekinger B., Wolf S., Pauli E. & Kobal G. ‘Sniff’ Sticks’: Olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem.Senses 22, 39–52 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain W. S., Gent J. F., Goodspeed R. B. & Leonard G. Evaluation of olfactory dysfunction in the Connecticut Chemosensory Clinical Research Center (CCCRC). Laryngoscope 98, 83–88 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas-Danguin T. et al. Development of the ETOC: a European test of olfactory capabilities. Rhinology 41, 142–151 (2003). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lam H. C., Sung J. K., Abdullah V. J. & van Hasselt C. A. The combined olfactory test in a Chinese population. J Laryngol Otol 120, 113–116 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L. & Agrawal U. The shelf life of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT). Laryngoscope 99, 402–404 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L., Smith R., McKeown D. A. & Raj J. Tests of human olfactory function: principle component analysis suggests that most measure a common source of variance. Percept. Psychophys. 56, 701–707 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones-Gotman M. & Zatorre R. J. Olfactory identification deficits in patients with focal cerebral excision. Neuropsychologia 26, 387–400 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hornung D. E. et al. The olfactory loss that accompanies an HIV infection. Physiol Behav. 64, 549–556 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gudziol V. et al. Sildenafil affects olfactory function. J Urol 177, 258–261 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lötsch J. et al. Effects of the opioid remifentanil on olfactory function in healthy volunteers. Life sciences 69, 2279–2285 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walter C. et al. Effects of 20 mg oral Δ(9) -tetrahydrocannabinol on the olfactory function of healthy volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol 78, 961–969 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedner M., Larsson M., Arnold N., Zucco G. M. & Hummel T. Cognitive factors in odor detection, odor discrimination, and odor identification tasks. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 32, 1062–1067 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lötsch J., Reichmann H. & Hummel T. Different odor tests contribute differently to the evaluation of olfactory loss. Chem Senses 33, 17–21 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lötsch J., Ultsch A. & Hummel T. A unifying data driven model of human olfactory pathology repre-senting known etiologies of dysfunction. Chem Senses (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy K. P. Machine Learning: A Probabilistic Perspective. (The MIT Press 2012). [Google Scholar]
- Hummel T., Sekinger B., Wolf S., Pauli E. & Kobal G. “Sniffin’ Sticks”: Olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses 22, 39–52 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobal G. et al. “Sniffin’ Sticks”: Screening of olfactory performance. Rhinology 34, 222–226 (1996). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (Vienna, Austria, 2008).
- Doty R. L., Applebaum S., Zusho H. & Settle R. G. Sex differences in odor identification ability: a cross-cultural analysis. Neuropsychologia. 23, 667–672 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L. & Cameron E. L. Sex differences and reproductive hormone influences on human odor perception. Physiol Behav 97, 213–228 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ultsch A. In Innovations in Classification, Data Science, and Information Systems - Proceedings 27th Annual Conference of the German Classification Society (GfKL). (eds D. Baier & K. D. Werrnecke) (Springer).
- Ultsch A., Thrun M. C., Hansen-Goos O. & Lötsch J. Identification of Molecular Fingerprints in Human Heat Pain Thresholds by Use of an Interactive Mixture Model R Toolbox (AdaptGauss). Int J Mol Sci 16, 25897–25911 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGrayne S. B. The Theory That Would Not Die: How Bayes’ Rule Cracked the Enigma Code, Hunted Down Russian Submarines & Emerged Triumphant from Two Centuries of Controversy. (Yale University Press, 2011). [Google Scholar]
- Ultsch A. In WSOM. 225–230.
- Lötsch J. & Ultsch A. In Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing Vol. 295 (eds Villmann T., Schleif F-M., Kaden M. & Lange M.) 248–257 (Springer, 2014). [Google Scholar]
- Ultsch A. & Sieman H. P. In INNC'90, Int. Neural Network Conference. 305–308 (Kluwer).
- Thrun M. C., Lerch F., Lötsch J. & Ultsch A. In Proceedings of International Conference in Central Europe on Computer Graphics, Visualization and Computer Vision.
- Ultsch A. In Classification — the Ubiquitous Challenge: Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Gesellschaft für Klassifikation e.V. University of Dortmund, March 9–11, 2004 (eds Weihs Claus & Gaul Wolfgang) 378–385 (Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2005). [Google Scholar]
- Spearman C. The proof and measurement of association between two things. The American journal of psychology 15, 72–101 (1904). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson B. & Trapp R. G. Basic and Clinical Biostatistics. 4th edn, (Lange Medical Books/McGraw-Hill, 2004). [Google Scholar]
- Klimek L., Hummel T., Moll B., Kobal G. & Mann W. J. Lateralized and bilateral olfactory function in patients with chronic sinusitis compared to healthy controls. Laryngoscope 108, 111–114 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hummel T., Heilmann S. & Murphy C. In Olfaction, taste and cognition (eds Rouby C. et al.) 441–456 (Cambridge University Press 2002). [Google Scholar]
- Ultsch A. In Workshop on Self-Organizing Maps 75–82 (Paris, 2005). [Google Scholar]
- Croy I., Negoias S., Novakova L., Landis B. N. & Hummel T. Learning about the functions of the olfactory system from people without a sense of smell. PLoS One 7, e33365 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reden J. et al. Recovery of olfactory function following closed head injury or infections of the upper respiratory tract. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 132, 265–269 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]