Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Am Geriatr Soc. 2016 Sep 9;64(10):1981–1987. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14308

Age-Related Sensory Impairments and Risk of Cognitive Impairment

Mary E Fischer a, Karen J Cruickshanks a,b, Carla R Schubert a, Alex A Pinto a, Cynthia M Carlsson c,d, Barbara EK Klein a, Ronald Klein a, Ted S Tweed a
PMCID: PMC5073029  NIHMSID: NIHMS781834  PMID: 27611845

Abstract

Background/Objectives

To evaluate the associations of sensory impairments with the 10-year risk of cognitive impairment. Previous work has primarily focused on the relationship between a single sensory system and cognition.

Design

The Epidemiology of Hearing Loss Study (EHLS) is a longitudinal, population-based study of aging in the Beaver Dam, WI community. Baseline examinations were conducted in 1993 and follow-up exams have been conducted every 5 years.

Setting

General community

Participants

EHLS members without cognitive impairment at EHLS-2 (1998–2000). There were 1,884 participants (mean age = 66.7 years) with complete EHLS-2 sensory data and follow-up information.

Measurements

Cognitive impairment was a Mini-Mental State Examination score of < 24 or history of dementia or Alzheimer’s disease. Hearing impairment was a pure-tone average of hearing thresholds (0.5, 1, 2 and 4 kHz) of > 25 decibel Hearing Level in either ear. Visual impairment was Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity of < 1.55 log units in the better eye and olfactory impairment was a San Diego Odor Identification Test score of < 6.

Results

Hearing, visual, and olfactory impairment were independently associated with cognitive impairment risk [Hearing: Hazard Ratio (HR) = 1.90, 95% Confidence Interval (C.I.) = 1.11, 3.26; Vision: HR = 2.05, 95% C.I. = 1.24, 3.38; Olfaction: HR = 3.92, 95% C.I. = 2.45, 6.26]. However, 85% with hearing impairment, 81% with visual impairment, and 76% with olfactory impairment did not develop cognitive impairment during follow-up.

Conclusion

The relationship between sensory impairment and cognitive impairment was not unique to one sensory system suggesting sensorineural health may be a marker of brain aging. The development of a combined sensorineurocognitive measure may be useful in uncovering mechanisms of healthy brain aging.

Keywords: cognitive impairment, hearing impairment, visual impairment, olfactory impairment, population-based

INTRODUCTION

With aging comes an increase in the incidence of sensory loss and cognitive decline, each having a considerable impact on public health and quality of life.14 In cross-sectional and prospective studies, significant associations of hearing, visual, and olfactory impairment with cognitive decline or impairment, including Alzheimer’s disease and all cause dementia, have been observed.516 Among the prospective studies, in a recent investigation in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging cohort, with a median follow-up of nearly 12 years, an increased risk of incident dementia associated with hearing loss was observed.14 In a 6-year follow-up investigation in the Maastricht Aging Study, poorer baseline hearing ability and a decline in hearing ability over the 6 years were both related to declining cognitive test scores including tests measuring memory and processing speed.8 Visual impairment has also been reported to be significantly related to cognitive function. For example, in a study of older individuals with an average age of over 80, the mean Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score was modestly but significantly lower in those with visual acuity impairment.9 In another study with several thousand women 65 years of age and older, visual impairment (corrected binocular vision worse than 20/40) was related to cognitive decline over a 4 year follow-up.7

Prospective investigations of the risk of cognitive decline or impairment associated with olfactory dysfunction have been conducted.1012 In the Epidemiology of Hearing Loss Study (EHLS), a significantly elevated risk of developing cognitive impairment over a 5 year follow-up period was observed in those with olfactory impairment at baseline.11 Olfactory function was also significantly related to the incidence of mild cognitive impairment over a 5 year period in the Rush Memory and Aging Project10 and with 5-year decline in the MMSE score in the Betula Study.12

Previous work has been primarily focused on sensory-specific relationships. Although a few investigations, including early studies, have considered dual impairment,6,7,9 no longitudinal study has considered the inter-relationship of hearing, visual, and olfactory impairment, and the risk of cognitive impairment or decline. Studying the association between cognition and sensory loss in multiple systems may provide useful information in the consideration of possible mechanisms and interventions. The objective of the present study was to determine whether hearing, visual, and olfactory impairment were independently associated with an increased 10 year risk of cognitive impairment. The validity and predictive value of using sensory measures as screening tools for future development of cognitive impairment was evaluated.

METHODS

Study Population

The study population included participants in the EHLS, a population-based, prospective investigation of age-related sensory loss. The EHLS is based in Beaver Dam, Wisconsin and participants in the baseline (1987–1988) Beaver Dam Eye Study were eligible. The baseline EHLS examination was conducted in 1993–1995 and follow-up examinations have been done on a 5-year basis. In the current study, the first 5-year follow-up (EHLS-2, 1998–2000) was used as the baseline with cognitive impairment incidence data coming from the 10-year (EHLS-3, 2003–2005) and 15-year (EHLS-4, 2009–2010) examinations. Additional details of the EHLS and Beaver Dam Eye Study have been reported.1719 Institutional Review Board approval by the Health Sciences Institutional Review Board of the University of Wisconsin and informed consent were obtained.

EHLS participants were eligible for inclusion in the current study if they were examined at EHLS-2, had a MMSE20 score of 24 or greater, had no self- or proxy-reported history of dementia, and were examined again during at least one of the follow-ups. There were 2015 eligible participants and 1884 had complete baseline sensory impairment information. Of these 1884 participants, 258 were examined in EHLS-3 but died before EHLS-4. Among the remaining 1626 participants, 156 (9.6%) participated in EHLS-3 only, and 1470 (90.4%) participated in EHLS-4 (1433 were examined in EHLS-3 and EHLS-4 whereas 37 were examined in EHLS-4 only).

Measurements

Outcomes

Cognitive Impairment

Cognitive impairment was defined as an MMSE score of < 24/30 or a self- or proxy-reported history of dementia or Alzheimer’s Disease.11 The MMSE and Alzheimer’s Disease/dementia history were ascertained at all phases included in this report.

Sensory Impairments

Hearing

Examinations included audiometric testing conducted according to the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association guidelines21 and in compliance with American National Standards Institute (ANSI) standards.22,23 Clinical audiometers with TDH-50P earphones and ER-3A insert earphones were used in sound-treated booths. Portable audiometers with insert earphones were used for testing in homes or group facilities when the participant was unable to visit the examination office. Pure-tone air-conduction thresholds were obtained at 0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, and 8 kHz, and bone-conduction thresholds were obtained at 0.5, 2, and 4 kHz. Testing was performed for both ears and masking was done when necessary.18,19 Hearing impairment was defined as a pure-tone average (PTA) at 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz of > 25 decibel Hearing Level in either ear.18

Vision

Vision was assessed in the Beaver Dam Eye Study examination concurrent with the EHLS study.24 Contrast sensitivity was measured in both eyes using Pelli-Robson letter charts.25 The measurement unit was log contrast sensitivity units based on triplet scores and visual impairment as defined as contrast sensitivity < 1.55 log units in the better eye.26

Olfaction

Olfactory function was determined using the San Diego Odor Identification Test (SDOIT), a standardized test of the ability to correctly identify 8 common odors.27 The odorants were presented in a random order and at 45 second intervals. A picture array with illustrations of the 8 odorants and 12 distractors was available during the test to aid in identification and allow for a non-verbal response. After presentation of an odor, participants could respond either verbally or point to the picture of the odorant. If an odor was not identified correctly, participants were told the name of the odor and it was presented a second time later in the test sequence. Olfactory impairment was defined as correctly identifying < 6 odors after 2 trials. This cut point was based on a previous evaluation of scores which found that 95% of the youngest EHLS participants had scores of 6 or better.27

Covariates

Covariate information from the current study’s baseline (EHLS-2) was used.

Measurements

Blood pressure was measured following the Hypertension Detection and Follow-up Program protocol.28 Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure >=140 mmHg or diastolic blood pressure >= 90 mmHg or a diagnosis of high blood pressure with current anti-hypertensive medication use. Glycosylated hemoglobin was measured using the Gly-Affin assay (Isolab, Inc., Akron OH) and diabetes was defined as Glycosylated hemoglobin > 8% (Hemoglobin A1C >= 6.5%) or a diagnosis of diabetes or suspected diabetes with current treatment. Reflectance spectrophotometry was used to determine cholesterol levels in non-fasting samples and non-high-density (non-HDL) cholesterol (mg/dl) was calculated as total minus HDL. High resolution B-mode carotid artery ultrasound images were obtained with a Biosound AU4 (Indianapolis, IN, USA). A modified Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities protocol29 was followed to measure the carotid intima-media thickness (IMT) of the near and far walls of the common carotid artery, internal carotid artery and the bifurcation on the left and right sides. The mean of the 12 sites was used in analyses.

High sensitivity C-reactive protein (CRP) and interleukin-6 (IL-6) were measured in non-fasting samples. CRP was measured using a latex particle-enhanced immunoturbidimetric assay kit (Roche Diagnostics, Indianapolis, IN) and a quantitative sandwich enzyme assay technique (QuantiKine High Sensitivity Kit, R&D Systems, Minneapolis, MN) was used to measure IL-6. The interassay coefficient of variation was 4.5% for CRP and 11.7% for IL-6. The CRP and IL-6 values were dichotomized according to highest tertile versus the combined middle and low tertiles of the respective distributions; the cutpoint was 3.53 mg/L for CRP and 2.11 pg/mL for IL-6. A summary variable was created which was a count of the number of inflammatory markers in the highest tertile (range 0–2).

Height and weight were measured and body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters and was categorized as < 26.0, 26.0–29.9, and 30.0+ kg/m2. A frailty index was obtained by giving a 0–1 score to 4 frailty measures and summing.30 The 4 measures included gait time (time to walk 10 feet at usual pace), the number of attempts to rise from a seated position without using arms, the peak expiratory flow rate using the mini-Wright meter,24 and handgrip strength in the dominant hand measured with a dynamometer.24 A score of 1 was assigned if gait time was in the 4th quartile, peak expiratory flow rate was in the 1st quartile, handgrip strength was in the 1st quartile, or the participant could not stand from a seated position in the first attempt.30

Self-Reported Information

Interviewer-administered questions gathered information on birthdate, sex, education, occupational history (longest held job), exercise (at least once per week long enough to sweat), smoking status, number of servings of beer, wine or hard liquor consumed during an average week in the past year (converted to grams of ethanol), and history of hypertension, diabetes, recent cold (today or past week), nasal polyps, deviated septum, allergy, head injury, stroke, or epilepsy.

Statistical Methods

All analyses were completed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc. Cary, NC).

Kaplan-Meier (product-limit) survival analysis produced the 10-year cumulative incidence of cognitive impairment.31 Cox discrete time proportional hazards analyses were performed to model the relationship between sensory impairment and the incidence of cognitive impairment.32 Hazard ratios and 95% confidence intervals were calculated from the parameter estimates and standard errors. Covariates previously reported as associated with sensory or cognitive impairment were included in multivariable models.

Sensitivity was calculated as the percentage of the cognitively impaired at follow-up who displayed sensory impairment at baseline (EHLS-2). Specificity was calculated as the percentage of the non-cognitively impaired at follow-up who did not display sensory impairment at baseline. The positive predictive value is the percentage that developed cognitive impairment in those who had baseline sensory impairment whereas the negative predictive value is the percentage that did not develop cognitive impairment in the participants who did not have baseline sensory impairment.

RESULTS

The mean age of the 1884 study subjects was 66.7 years (standard deviation = 8.4 years), 40.9% were male, and 37% had some college or more education (Table 1). Slightly over 10% were current smokers at baseline and less than half exercised long enough to work up a sweat at least once per week. Approximately 56% of the participants had 1 or more sensory impairments (43.9% had hearing impairment, 21.1% had visual impairment, and 17.2% had olfactory impairment). There were 90 participants (4.8%) with all 3 impairments.

Table 1.

Baseline (EHLS-2) Characteristics of Participants With Complete Sensory Measurement Data

Baseline Characteristic N or Mean % or S.D.1
Overall 1884 100.0
Demographic
Age (Mean, S.D.) 66.7 8.4
Male 771 40.9
Education
 < 12 yrs 245 13.0
 12 yrs 941 50.0
 13–15 yrs 329 17.5
 16+ yrs 368 19.5
Behavioral
Smoking Status
 Never 894 47.5
 Former 790 41.9
 Current 200 10.6
Alcohol Consumption2 (Mean, S.D.) 43.3 83.8
Exercise at Least 1x/Wk 838 44.5
Health History
Body Mass Index (kg/m2)
 < 25.0 333 17.7
 25.0 – 29.9 649 34.6
 30.0+ 896 47.7
Hypertension 1066 56.6
Diabetes Mellitus 208 11.0
# High Inflammatory Markers
 0 935 51.9
 1 545 30.3
 2 321 17.8
Stroke 84 4.5
Head Injury 510 27.1
Non-HDL Cholesterol (mg/dL) (Mean, S.D.) 163.0 38.8
Mean Intima-Media Thickness (IMT) (mm) (Mean, S.D.) 0.86 0.22
Frailty Score
 0 1144 62.3
 1 440 24.0
 2 177 9.6
 3 64 3.5
 4 11 0.6
Sensory Impairment3
No Impairment 833 44.2
One Impairment 645 34.2
 Hearing Only 449 23.8
 Vision Only 114 6.0
 Olfaction Only 82 4.4
Two Impairments 316 16.8
 Hearing & Vision 165 8.8
 Hearing & Olfaction 122 6.5
 Vision & Olfaction 29 1.5
Three Impairments 90 4.8
1

S.D. = Standard Deviation

2

Alcohol consumption measured in grams of ethanol per week

3

Hearing impairment: Pure-tone average 0.5, 1, 2,4 kHz, either ear > 25decibel Hearing Level; Vision impairment: Contrast sensitivity better eye < 1.55 log units; Olfaction impairment: San Diego Odor Identification Test score < 6 odors

The 10-year cumulative incidence of cognitive impairment was 9.9%. Participants with sensory impairment at baseline had higher cognitive impairment incidence rates than those without sensory impairment. The 10-year, unadjusted cumulative incidence of cognitive impairment for subjects with hearing, visual, and olfactory impairment was 17.3%, 23.5%, and 30.0%, respectively (Table 2).

Table 2.

Baseline (EHLS-2) Sensory Impairment and the 10-year Cumulative Incidence of Cognitive Impairment Hazard Ratios, 95% Confidence Intervals

Baseline Sensory Impairment1 Unadjusted Cumulative Incidence (%) Hazard Ratio (95% Confidence Interval)
Age-sex-education Adjusted Multivariable Adjusted2
Hearing 2.11 (1.30, 3.40) 2.09 (1.29, 3.39)
 Yes 17.3
 No 4.7
Vision 1.92 (1.25, 2.96) 1.96 (1.25, 3.07)
 Yes 23.5
 No 6.7
Olfaction 4.18 (2.68, 6.51) 4.04 (2.54, 6.43)
 Yes 30.0
 No 6.3
1

Hearing impairment: Pure-tone average 0.5, 1, 2,4 kHz, either ear > 25decibel Hearing Level; Vision impairment: Contrast sensitivity better eye < 1.55 log units; Olfaction impairment: San Diego Odor Identification Test score < 6 odors

2

Hearing and vision impairment models adjusted for age, sex, education, smoking status, BMI, exercise, alcohol consumption, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, number of high inflammatory markers, non-HDL cholesterol, mean IMT, and frailty score. Olfaction impairment model adjusted for age, sex, education, smoking status, BMI, exercise, alcohol consumption, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, number of high inflammatory markers, non-HDL cholesterol, mean IMT, frailty score, longest held job, cold or stuffy nose, nasal polyps, deviated septum, allergies, head injury, stroke/TIA, and epilepsy.

With adjustment for age, sex, and education, the risk of cognitive impairment was significantly higher among those with baseline hearing impairment [Hazard ratio (HR) = 2.11, 95% Confidence Interval (C.I.) = 1.30, 3.40)], visual impairment (HR = 1.92, 95% C.I. = 1.25, 2.96), or olfactory impairment (HR = 4.18, 95% C.I. = 2.68, 6.51) compared to those without impairment (Table 2). With adjustment for additional covariates, including vascular-related measures, the association between sensory impairment and 10-year risk of cognitive impairment continued to be statistically significant (hearing: HR = 2.09, 95% C.I. = 1.29, 3.39; vision: HR = 1.96, 95% C.I. = 1.25, 3.07; olfaction: HR = 4.04, 95% C.I. = 2.54, 6.43). When the 3 sensory impairments were included in the model simultaneously, each displayed a significant, independent association with cognitive impairment risk. The strength of the associations were similar in the sensory-specific impairment models (Table 2) and the multiple impairment models (Table 3) (Multivariable adjusted: hearing: HR = 1.90, 95% C.I. = 1.11, 3.26; vision: HR = 2.05, 95% C.I. = 1.24, 3.38; olfaction: HR = 3.92, 95% C.I. = 2.45, 6.26).

Table 3.

Multiple Baseline (EHLS-2) Sensory Impairments and the 10-year Cumulative Incidence of Cognitive Impairment1 Hazard Ratios, 95% Confidence Intervals

Baseline Sensory Impairment2 Hazard Ratio (95% Confidence Interval)
Age-sex-education Adjusted Multivariable Adjusted3
Hearing 1.96 (1.16, 3.29) 1.90 (1.11, 3.26)
Vision 1.85 (1.15, 2.97) 2.05 (1.24, 3.38)
Olfaction 4.02 (2.58, 6.28) 3.92 (2.45, 6.26)
1

Models include hearing, vision, and olfactory impairment

2

Hearing impairment: Pure-tone average 0.5, 1, 2,4 kHz, either ear > 25decibel Hearing Level; Vision impairment: Contrast sensitivity better eye < 1.55 log units; Olfaction impairment: San Diego Odor Identification Test score < 6 odors

3

Model adjusted for age, sex, education, smoking status, BMI, exercise, alcohol consumption, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, number of high inflammatory markers, non-HDL cholesterol, mean IMT, frailty score, longest held job, cold or stuffy nose, nasal polyps, deviated septum, allergies, head injury, stroke/TIA, and epilepsy.

A sensitivity analysis assessing the impact of cataract surgery on the contrast sensitivity–cognitive impairment relationship was performed. Participants with no cataract surgery history at baseline were selected and results showed the cognitive impairment risk associated with contrast sensitivity impairment in the sub-sample was very similar (HR multivariable adjusted = 2.14, 95% C.I. = 1.30, 3.52) to that observed in the entire study population. In addition, the relationship between contrast sensitivity in the worse eye and cognitive impairment risk was found to be not significant (HR multivariable adjusted = 1.46, 95% C.I. = 0.89, 2.40).

The use of sensory measures as screening tools for future cognitive impairment was assessed. Sensitivity ranged from 21.7% for having all 3 sensory impairments to 72.9% for hearing impairment (Table 4). Correspondingly, specificity was low for hearing impairment (59.0%) but very high for the combination of all 3 sensory impairments (96.9%). Positive predictive value was 14.6% for hearing impairment meaning 85.4% of the participants with baseline hearing impairment did not develop cognitive impairment during follow-up. The highest positive predictive value, 40.0%, was associated with having all 3 impairments.

Table 4.

Performance Characteristics of Sensory Impairments as Screening Tools For Development of Cognitive Impairment

Sensory Impairment1 Sensitivity (%) Specificity (%) Positive Predictive Value (%) Negative Predictive Value (%)
Hearing 72.9 59.0 14.6 95.7
Vision 46.4 81.3 19.3 94.0
Olfaction 47.6 85.8 24.5 94.4
Hearing, Vision, and Olfaction2 21.7 96.9 40.0 92.8
1

Hearing impairment: Pure-tone average 0.5, 1, 2, 4 kHz, either ear > 25 decibel Hearing Level; Vision impairment: Contrast sensitivity better eye < 1.55 log units; Olfaction impairment: San Diego Odor Identification Test score < 6 odors

2

All 3 sensory impairments are present

DISCUSSION

Hearing, visual, and olfactory impairment demonstrated significant effects on the 10 year risk of cognitive impairment independent of one another. The results from the joint model suggested that an individual with hearing, visual, and olfactory impairment had a risk of developing cognitive impairment which was 15 times that of an individual without baseline sensory impairment.

These results are consistent with the extensive previous literature reporting that hearing, vision, and olfaction individually are associated with cognition. But the current study extends the research and models the joint effect of the 3 sensory impairments on the risk of cognitive impairment showing that each sensory impairment has an independent effect on the risk of cognitive impairment. The association between sensory impairment and cognitive impairment was not unique to one sensory system.

The finding of a significant relationship between sensory impairment and cognitive impairment is possibly expected since neurologic functioning is being assessed when sensorineural health is measured as well as when cognitive health is measured. For example, although pure-tone audiometry relies on the functioning of the auditory periphery, it also relies on central auditory processing and decoding of the signals along with some cognitive decision-making in forming a response to the signal. The sensorineural system needs the brain for processing. Audiometric loss may not be indicative of simply peripheral changes; it may also be indicative of auditory system-wide changes. Cognitive processing, judgment and decision-making are also involved in contrast sensitivity and olfaction testing. Therefore, in models using sensory measures to predict cognitive impairment or decline, one contributory explanation for the observed relationships may be that essentially there are measures of neurologic health on both the dependent (outcome) and the independent (predictors) sides of the model.

Alternatively, more than 20 years ago, the common-cause theory was introduced suggesting that one or more underlying factors exist which are contributing to the development of both sensory and cognitive impairment.6 One such factor or influence may be a generalized aging effect. Previous work has found that a very high proportion of the age-related variance in cognitive measures is shared with sensory measures.6,33 In the Australian Longitudinal Study of Aging, close to 80% of the age-related variance in cognition was shared with hearing and vision.33 Sensory declines may be early signs of age-related changes in neurologic functioning, neuropathology or neurodegeneration, before measures of cognitive function show impairment. Sensorineural health may serve as a marker of healthy brain aging.

Another common cause which has been investigated is vascular disease. Many reports have described an association of cardiovascular disease (CVD) and related risk factors with cognitive decline/impairment.3438 In addition, cardiovascular-related factors or conditions have also been found to be related to hearing, vision and olfaction function.18,19,3947 For example, early ecologic studies suggested an association between CVD and hearing loss and subsequent cross-sectional and prospective studies observed similar findings for CVD, atherosclerotic and microvascular changes, and CVD risk factors.18,19,39,40,42,44,45 In a recent investigation, a significant association of subclinical atherosclerosis, measured as carotid IMT and plaque, with the 5-year incidence of hearing impairment was reported.45 Therefore, there is evidence that vascular changes may be associated with both sensory and cognitive health. In the present study, there was only slight attenuation of the estimated risks of cognitive impairment after further adjustment for CVD-related factors and conditions, including a measure of subclinical atherosclerosis. This could mean that strong CVD influences on the sensory-cognitive relationship were not present or that the CVD measures used in the adjustment did not fully represent the shared vascular component, for example, microvascular changes in the brain, so that residual confounding was present.

Sensory and cognitive impairments very likely share more than age and vascular disease as common causes. Sensorineural impairment may be a surrogate for poor health in general and it is highly unlikely that an adequate set of covariates accounting for all facets of poor health can be measured and included in modeling.

It is important to note that there may also be independent mechanisms involved in specific sensory-cognitive associations. With olfaction the independent process may involve shared neuropathology such as neurofibrillary tangles in areas of the brain involved in olfactory processes.48 With hearing, it has been suggested that there may be a causal relationship between hearing loss and cognitive decline/impairment involving either increased cognitive load, resource allocation changes, sensory deprivation, and/or social isolation14 but the evidence is not clear. For example, while some studies have suggested a positive relationship between social engagement and cognitive function,49 a review of observational studies found that support for a link between social support/network and cognitive decline was low.50

Even though the underlying mechanism for the association between sensory and cognitive loss is not clearly understood, there is interest in being able to use sensory measures as screening tools for cognitive impairment risk. This study found the highest sensitivity when using baseline hearing impairment as the screening tool. But the sensitivity was only 72.9% so more than 1 out of 4 of those who developed cognitive impairment were not identified as being at high risk for it. In addition, 41.0% of those who did not develop cognitive impairment were identified as being likely to develop impairment, i.e. false positives. The positive predictive value of baseline hearing impairment was 14.6% which means 85.4% of the participants who had a baseline hearing impairment did not develop cognitive impairment during follow-up. The greatest positive predictive value was associated with having all 3 sensory impairments (40.0%) but even then 60.0% of the participants who had the 3 sensory impairments did not develop cognitive impairment. Interventions for the treatment of sensory loss in a large segment of the older population are reasonable for enriching lives and maintaining good quality of life, but are probably not warranted as a means to prevent cognitive impairment. Sensory impairments, particularly hearing, are highly prevalent in an aged population but cognitive impairment is relatively uncommon. The likelihood of developing cognitive impairment must be considered and not only the elevated relative risk associated with sensory loss.

The strengths of this study included the prospective design, large population, and extensive information related to sensory and cognitive measures and demographic, behavioral, and health factors. Hearing, vision, and olfaction were measured during the same phase by trained and certified examiners using standardized methods. The MMSE, used to define cognitive impairment, was also administered by certified examiners at each phase. The prevalence of the sensory impairments and incidence of cognitive impairment were high enough to provide adequate power to estimate the independent effect of each sensory impairment. More extensive adjustment for potential confounders was possible, particularly for inflammation and vascular-related factors, such as IMT, a measure of subclinical atherosclerosis. Among the limitations of the study is that the outcome of cognitive impairment included a mix of dementias including Alzheimer’s Disease. As a result there are very likely multiple etiologies associated with the one comprehensive outcome of cognitive impairment. Also, in the modeling, it was not possible to consider duration of sensory impairment prior to measurement; EHLS-1 could not serve as the baseline because olfaction and the MMSE were first measured at EHLS-2. Finally, because EHLS-2 served as baseline, only incidents of cognitive impairment occurring within a 10 year follow-up period and not those occurring at a later point, could be included in the analyses. The observed results apply only to the 10-year risk of cognitive impairment.

In the current study, the relationship between sensory impairment and cognitive impairment risk was not limited to a single sensory system but rather a significant, independent association of cognitive impairment risk with hearing, vision, and olfaction impairment was observed. These findings suggest that sensory measures may be markers of brain aging. Future studies are needed to extend this work to cognitive change and decline. In addition, efforts should be made to develop a combined measure of sensorineurocognitive function to more fully represent underlying neural health and to help in elucidating related factors and mechanisms of healthy brain aging.

Acknowledgments

Funding source: The project described was supported by Award Number R37AG011099 from the National Institute on Aging (to K.J. Cruickshanks), EY06594 from the National Eye Institute (to R. Klein and B.E.K. Klein), and an unrestricted grant from Research to Prevent Blindness.

The authors thank the participants for their continued commitment to the study.

The authors report grants with the National Institutes of Health and Research to Prevent Blindness. CMC also reports grant support from the VA and being the site PI for a study co-sponsored by NIH and Lilly (the A4 Study). CMC received an honorarium in 3/2015 for participation in an expert panel on a Dementia Care Pathway co-sponsored by UCSF and Quest Diagnostics.

Footnotes

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the official views of the National Institute on Aging or the National Institutes of Health.

The findings included in this paper were presented in preliminary form at the International Conference on Cognitive Hearing Science for Communication in Linköping, Sweden.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures:

Elements of Financial/Personal Conflicts MEF KJC CRS AAP
Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No
Employment or Affiliation X X X X
Grants/Funds X X X X
Honoraria X X X X
Speaker Forum X X X X
Consultant X X X X
Stocks X X X X
Royalties X X X X
Expert Testimony X X X X
Board Member X X X X
Patents X X X X
Personal Relationship X X X X
Elements of Financial/Personal Conflicts CMC BEKK RK TST
Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes No
Employment or Affiliation X X X X
Grants/Funds X X X X
Honoraria X X X X
Speaker Forum X X X X
Consultant X X X X
Stocks X X X X
Royalties X X X X
Expert Testimony X X X X
Board Member X X X X
Patents X X X X
Personal Relationship X X X X
*
Authors can be listed by abbreviations of their names.
For “yes” x mark(s): give brief explanation below:

Author Contributions:

Fischer, Cruickshanks, Schubert, Pinto: study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data. Fischer, Cruickshanks, Schubert, Klein BEK, Klein R, Tweed: acquisition of subjects and/or data. All authors: preparation of manuscript.

Sponsor’s Role:

The funding agencies had no role in the design, methods, subject recruitment, data collection, analysis and preparation of the paper. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the official views of the National Institute on Aging or the National Institutes of Health.

References

  • 1.Dalton DS, Cruickshanks KJ, Klein BE, et al. The impact of hearing loss on quality of life in older adults. Gerontologist. 2003;43:661–668. doi: 10.1093/geront/43.5.661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wilson RS, Boyle PA, Segawa E, et al. The influence of cognitive decline on well-being in old age. Psychol Aging. 2013;28:304–313. doi: 10.1037/a0031196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Christ SL, Zheng DD, Swenor BK, et al. Longitudinal relationships among visual acuity, daily functional status, and mortality: the Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2014;132:1400–1406. doi: 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2014.2847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Croy I, Nordin S, Hummel T. Olfactory disorders and quality of life – an updated review. Chem Senses. 2014;39:185–194. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjt072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Uhlmann RF, Larson EB, Rees TS, et al. Relationship of hearing impairment to dementia and cognitive dysfunction in older adults. JAMA. 1989;261:1916–1919. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lindenberger U, Baltes P. Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychol Aging. 1994;9:339–355. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.9.3.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lin MY, Gutierrez PR, Stone KL, et al. Vision impairment and combined vision and hearing impairment predict cognitive and functional decline in older women. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52:1996–2002. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52554.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Valentijn SA, van Boxtel MP, van Hooren SA, et al. Change in sensory functioning predicts change in cognitive functioning: results from a 6-year follow-up in the maastricht aging study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:374–380. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53152.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tay T, Wang JJ, Kifley A, et al. Sensory and cognitive association in older persons: findings from an older Australian population. Gerontology. 2006;52:386–394. doi: 10.1159/000095129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wilson RS, Schneider JA, Arnold SE, et al. Olfactory identification and incidence of mild cognitive impairment in older age. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:802–808. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schubert CR, Carmichael LL, Murphy C, et al. Olfaction and the 5-year incidence of cognitive impairment in an epidemiological study of older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1517–1521. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01826.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Olofsson JK, Ronnlund M, Nordin S, et al. Odor identification deficit as a predictor of five-year global cognitive change: interactive effects with age and ApoE-epsilon4. Behav Genet. 2009;39:496–503. doi: 10.1007/s10519-009-9289-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Karpa MJ, Gopinath B, Rochtchina E, et al. Prevalence and neurodegenerative or other associations with olfactory impairment in an older community. J Aging Health. 2010;22:154–168. doi: 10.1177/0898264309353066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lin FR, Metter EJ, O’Brien RJ, et al. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol. 2011;68:214–220. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Fischer ME, et al. Odor identification and cognitive function in the Beaver Dam Offspring Study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2013;35:669–676. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2013.809701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lin FR, Yaffe K, Xia J, et al. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173:293–299. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Klein R, Klein BEK, Linton KLP, et al. The Beaver Dam Eye Study: Visual acuity. Ophthalmology. 1991;98:1310–1315. doi: 10.1016/s0161-6420(91)32137-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cruickshanks KJ, Wiley TL, Tweed TS, et al. Prevalence of hearing loss in older adults in Beaver Dam, Wisconsin: the Epidemiology of Hearing Loss Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1998;148:879–886. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Cruickshanks KJ, Tweed TS, Wiley TL, et al. The 5-year incidence and progression of hearing loss: the epidemiology of hearing loss study. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2003;129:1041–1046. doi: 10.1001/archotol.129.10.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini-mental state’ A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (AHSA) Guidelines for Manual Pure-Tone Threshold Audiometry. Vol. 20. ASHA; 1987. pp. 297–301. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.American National Standards Institute. Maximum Permissible Ambient Noise Levels for Audiometric Test Rooms. New York, NY: ANSI; 1999. ANSI S3.1-1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.American National Standards Institute. Specification for Audiometers. New York, NY: ANSI; 2010. ANSI S3.6-2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Klein R, Klein BEK. Beaver Dam Eye Study III. Springfield, VA: US Dept of Commerce; 1999. Manual of Operations. NTIS accession No. PB99-137861. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pelli DG, Robson JG, Wilkins AJ. The design of a new letter chart for measuring contrast sensitivity. Clin Vis Sci. 1988;2:187–199. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Elliott DB, Sanderson K, Conkey A. The reliability of the Pelli-Robson contrast sensitivity chart. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1990;10:21–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Murphy C, Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, et al. Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA. 2002;288:2307–2312. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Borhani NO, Kass EH, Langford HG, et al. The hypertension detection and follow-up program: Hypertension detection and follow-up program cooperative group. Prev Med. 1976;5:207–215. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(76)90039-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bond MG, Barnes RW, Riley WA, et al. High resolution B-mode ultrasound scanning methods in the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study (ARIC) J Neuroimaging. 1991;1:68–73. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Klein BE, Klein R, Knudtson MD, et al. Relationship of measures of frailty to visual function: the Beaver Dam Eye Study. Trans Am Ophthalmol Soc. 2003;101:191–196. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kaplan EL, Meier P. Nonparametric estimation from incomplete observations. J Am Statist Assoc. 1958;53:457–81. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Cox DR. Regression models and life tables. J Royal Stat Soc. 1972;B34:187–220. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA, Sanchez L. A reevaluation of the common factor theory of shared variance among age, sensory function, and cognitive function in older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2001;56:P3–P11. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.1.p3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Anstey KJ, von Sanden C, Salim A, et al. Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;166:367–378. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kaffashian S, Dugravot A, Nabi H, et al. Predictive utility of the Framingham general cardiovascular disease risk profile for cognitive function: evidence from the Whitehall II study. Eur Heart J. 2011;32:2326–2332. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehr133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Zhong WJ, Cruickshanks KJ, Schubert CR, et al. Carotid atherosclerosis and 10-year changes in cognitive function. Atherosclerosis. 2012;224:506–510. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2012.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dregan A, Stewart R, Gulliford MC. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in adults aged 50 and over: a population-based cohort study. Age Ageing. 2013;42:338–345. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gottesman RF, Schneider AL, Albert M, et al. Midlife hypertension and 20-year cognitive change: the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71:1218–1227. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rosen S. Hearing studies in selected urban and rural populations. Trans NY Acad Sci. 1966;29:9–21. doi: 10.1111/j.2164-0947.1966.tb02246.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Gates GA, Cobb JL, D’Agostino RB, et al. The relation of hearing in the elderly to the presence of cardiovascular disease and cardiovascular risk factors. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 1993;119:156–161. doi: 10.1001/archotol.1993.01880140038006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bergman B, Nilsson-Ehle H, Sjöstrand J. Ocular changes, risk markers for eye disorders and effects of cataract surgery in elderly people: a study of an urban Swedish population followed from 70 to 97 years of age. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2004;82:166–174. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0420.2004.00182.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Curhan SG, Eavey R, Wang M, et al. Body mass index, waist circumference, physical activity, and risk of hearing loss in women. Am J Med. 2013;126:1142e1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.amjmed.2013.04.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Klein R, Lee KE, Gangnon RE, et al. Relation of smoking, drinking, and physical activity to changes in vision over a 20-year period: the Beaver Dam Eye Study. Ophthalmology. 2014;121:1220–1228. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2014.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cruickshanks KJ, Nondahl DM, Dalton DS, et al. Smoking, central adiposity, and poor glycemic control increase risk of hearing impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63:918–924. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Fischer ME, Schubert CR, Nondahl DM, et al. Subclinical atherosclerosis and increased risk of hearing impairment. Atherosclerosis. 2015;238:344–349. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2014.12.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Fischer ME, et al. Carotid intima media thickness, atherosclerosis, and 5-year decline in odor identification: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2015;70:879–884. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Fischer ME, et al. Inflammatory and vascular markers and olfactory impairment in older adults. Age Ageing. 2015;44:878–882. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afv075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Wilson RS, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, et al. The relationship between cerebral Alzheimer’s disease pathology and odour identification in old age. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78:30–35. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.099721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CR, Wilson RS, et al. Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology. 2004;63:2322–2326. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000147473.04043.b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Plassman BL, Williams JW, Jr, Burke JR, et al. Systematic review: factors associated with risk for and possible prevention of cognitive decline in later life. Ann Intern Med. 2010;153:182–193. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-153-3-201008030-00258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES