Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Aug 22.
Published in final edited form as: J Health Commun. 2016 Aug 22;21(SUP2):141–154. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2016.1179368

Dimensions of Deaf/Hard-of Hearing and Hearing Adolescents’ Health Literacy and Health Knowledge

Scott R Smith a,b, Vincent J Samar a
PMCID: PMC5073377  NIHMSID: NIHMS821152  PMID: 27548284

Abstract

Deaf and hard-of-hearing (D/HH) adults have lower health literacy compared to hearing adults but it is unclear if this disparity also occurs in adolescence. We used the Health Literacy Skills Instrument-Short Form (HLSI-SF), Short Form of the Test of Functional Health Literacy (S-TOFHLA), Comprehensive Heart Disease Knowledge Questionnaire (CHDKQ) and newly constructed interactive and critical health literacy survey items to quantify D/HH and hearing adolescents’ health literacy. We adapted and translated survey materials into sign language and spoken English to reduce testing bias due to variable English language skills. Participants were 187 D/HH and 94 hearing college-bound high school students. Adjusting for age, gender, race/ethnicity, school grade, and SES, D/HH adolescents demonstrated weaker general and functional health literacy and cardiovascular health knowledge than hearing adolescents on the HLSI, S-TOFHLA, and CHDKQ (all p’s<.0001). Standard health literacy or knowledge scores were associated with several interactive and critical health literacy skills (all p’s<.05). D/HH adolescents who reported greater hearing-culture identity, having hearing aids, experiencing better hearing with assistive devices, having good quality of communication with parents, and attending hearing schools at least half of the time had higher functional health literacy (all p’s<.025). Those who reported English as their best language and attending hearing schools at least half the time had higher cardiovascular health knowledge scores (all p’s< .03). Results suggest that interventions to improve D/HH adolescents’ health literacy should target their health-related conversations with their families, access to printed health information, and access to health information from other people, especially health care providers and educators.

Keywords: adolescent health literacy; functional, interactive, and critical health literacy; cardiovascular health knowledge; deaf health literacy

Introduction

Growing evidence indicates that D/HH people have substantial health knowledge and health literacy weaknesses. Of 203 D/HH adults in Chicago, 40% could not name any heart attack symptoms and over 60% could not name any stroke symptoms (Margellos-Anast, Estarziau, & Kaufman, 2006). McKee, Paasche-Orlow, et al. (2015) reported that 48% of 166 Deaf participants given a sign language version of the Newest Vital Sign (ASL-NVS) had inadequate health literacy, a nearly 7 fold difference compared to 239 hearing participants.

These disparities in health literacy may be relevant to disparities in the health outcomes of D/HH people (Moreland, Atcherson, Zazove, & McKee, 2015; McKee & Paasche-Orlow, 2012; McKee, Schlehofer, et al., 2011; Pollard & Barnett, 2009). Deaf or Hard-of-Hearing (D/HH) adults report more health problems, including obesity, intimate partner violence, and suicidal behaviors than the general adult population (Barnett et al., 2011). They also utilize health care differently (McKee, Moreland, Atcherson, & Zazove, 2015; McKee, Barnett, Block, & Pearson, 2011). For example, D/HH individuals are more likely to use the emergency department over the prior 36 months than hearing individuals for complex cultural and experiential reasons (McKee, Winters, Sen, Zazove, & Fiscella, 2015).

Recent work supports the hypothesis that D/HH people’s health literacy deficits are present in adolescence. Smith, Kushalnagar, and Hauser (2015) found that D/HH adolescents have weak cardiovascular health literacy, similar to D/HH adults, lacking knowledge about basic cardiovascular pathophysiology, their family cardiovascular health history, and important common medical terms like “cholesterol”. To date, no studies have systematically quantified D/HH adolescents’ health literacy and general knowledge.

Because health literacy is a multidimensional construct, it is a challenge to measure (Nutbeam, 2008), especially in populations whose best language is typically not English (McKee & Paasche-Orlow, 2012; Sentell & Braun, 2012). Parker, Baker, Williams, & Nurss (1995) define functional health literacy as “being able to apply literacy skills to health related materials such as prescriptions, appointment cards, medicine labels, and directions for home health care”. An individual’s ability to read and understand printed English and numerical health information has been the essential competency underlying functional health literacy measurement in the US. Traditional assessments include the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine (Davis et al., 1991), the Short Form of the Test of Functional Health Literacy (Baker, Williams, Parker, Gazmararian, & Nurss, 1999), Newest Vital Sign (Weiss et al., 2005), and Medical Term Recognition Test (Rawson et al., 2010). These measures often provide meaningful information about health literacy and health outcomes (Marrie, Salter, Tyry, Fox, & Cutter, 2014; Sharif & Blank, 2010), but represent health literacy constructs biased toward printed and spoken literacy (Chinn & McCarthy, 2012; Berkman, Davis, & McCormack, 2010; Nutbeam, 2009; Nutbeam, 2008).

A broader health literacy construct encompassing dimensions such as interactive and critical health literacy skills and experiences (Nutbeam, 2009) and health knowledge (Al Sayah, Majumdar, Egede, & Johnson, 2015), in addition to functional health literacy, provides more meaningful information (Haun, Valerio, McCormack, Sorensen, & Paasche-Orlow, 2014), especially about people with limited English proficiency (McKee & Paasche-Orlow, 2012). Interactive health literacy involves the use of advanced social, cognitive, and communication skills to interact with and obtain health knowledge from others, such as parents, teachers, and health care providers and adapt to changing circumstances (Nutbeam, 2008). Critical health literacy involves judgment and evaluation skills necessary to analyze health information and better control life circumstances to be healthy (Nutbeam, 2008). Health literacy instruments that capture some interactive and critical literacy skills include the e-Health Literacy Scale (Norman & Skinner, 2006); Communicative and Critical Health Literacy Measure (Ishikawa, Nomura, Sato, & Yano, 2008); Media Health Literacy (Levin-Zamir, Lemish, & Gofin, 2011); and All Aspects of Health Literacy Scale (AAHLS; Chinn & McCarthy, 2012). Other tools capture a broader construct of “overall health literacy” that incorporates elements of interactive and critical health literacy, e.g., Health Literacy Skills Instrument (McCormack et al., 2010). Furthermore, several health literacy measures have been developed for adolescents including Adolescent Health Literacy (Wu et al, 2010); Adolescent Media Health literacy (Levin-Zamir, Lemish, & Gofin, 2011); e-Health Literacy Scale (Norman & Skinner., 2006), and REALM-Teen (Davis, et al., 2006). However, none of these measures has been adapted for D/HH adolescents.

The complexity of the health literacy construct complicates health literacy measurement in D/HH people. Functional health literacy instruments are intended to measure an individual’s ability to understand and use conventional printed health materials and may therefore provide some useful information about the ability of D/HH individuals with limited English language skills to access conventional English-dependent health information in the real world. However, functional literacy in people with limited English proficiency is discipline-specific (Fang & Schleppegrell, 2010), and, as a result, D/HH individuals’ performance on functional health literacy measures may not accurately reflect their true functional health literacy in specific health contexts.

Furthermore, interactive and critical health literacy constructs extend beyond English-dependent access because they do not require English language and reading skills per se, but rather access to effective communication within the health care system and information environment. Nevertheless, interactive and critical health literacy assessment instruments in the US typically do rely on spoken or printed English language skills and cultural-specific fund-of-information that are not appropriate for the D/HH population (Pollard & Barnett, 2009). Therefore, using such English-based instruments can introduce construct-irrelevant linguistic and cultural testing biases into items meant to assess health literacy skills that, in principle, do not fundamentally require English language skills.

Appropriate linguistic and cultural translation and adaptation of measures of health literacy and knowledge dimensions other than English-based functional health literacy per se are necessary to ensure construct-relevant assessment. Such measures will provide more accurate information to guide future research and interventions to improve D/HH adolescents’ health literacy and ultimately to reduce and eliminate D/HH people’s health disparities.

Study Purpose and Aims

Previous health literacy studies of D/HH people (e.g., McKee, Paasche-Orlow, et al., 2015; McKee & Paasche-Orlow, 2012; McKee, Schlehofer, et al., 2011; Pollard & Barnett, 2009; Margellos-Anast, Estarziau, & Kaufman, 2006) have not adequately controlled for the potential confounding influence of English print literacy on health literacy measures based on underlying constructs that do not essentially depend on English language skill. Construct-irrelevant sources of variance in such measures may specifically include, for example, the use of English for administering the test instructions of standard measures or the use of English survey questions to query individuals with limited English about their actual access to health care or their health knowledge. Therefore, our first aim was to quantitatively test whether evidence of disparities in D/HH and hearing adolescents’ health literacy and health knowledge remain on a set of gold standard health literacy measures after translating and adapting those measures for both populations to eliminate any test-instrument dependence on English that is irrelevant to health literacy measurement.

Since various common demographic factors such as age, gender, race/ethnicity, grade-level, and socioeconomic status may be associated with group differences in health literacy and health knowledge (Nolke, Mensing, Kramer, & Hornberg, 2015; Freedman, Kouri, West, & Keating, 2015; Green & Cavanaugh, 2015), we controlled these factors in comparisons of D/HH and hearing adolescents. Furthermore, we conducted analyses to determine whether D/HH adolescents’ health disparities persist on each standard health literacy measure even after controlling for the other standard health literacy measures. These analyses evaluate for evidence of distinct domains of health literacy and health knowledge in D/HH adolescents.

Our second aim was to quantitatively compare D/HH adolescents’ versus hearing adolescents’ interactive and critical health literacy skills and experiences for the first time. Because other interactive and critical health literacy measures were not suitable for use by D/HH and hearing adolescents, we used qualitative findings from focus groups (Smith et al., 2015) to create several original items to measure their interactive and critical health literacy skills and experiences.

Our third aim was to identify subgroups of D/HH adolescents who might have stronger health literacy than others. This information will help guide future research and specific interventions for those with weaker health literacy.

Methods

Participants

D/HH participants were 187 high school students who attended one of two 2013 Explore Your Future summer programs at the National Technical Institute for the Deaf (NTID) at the Rochester Institute of Technology (RIT). Hearing participants were 94 high school students who attended one of several 2014 summer medical career orientation programs at the University of Rochester Medical Center (URMC). D/HH and hearing participants in these programs are "college-bound" students. Program acceptance requires students to demonstrate performance in core academic subjects that strongly suggest they will pass high school graduation exit exams and gain admission to competitive colleges or universities. Program staff carefully evaluated applicants academically. No member of the research study team was involved in screening applicants.

We established relationships with pre-college summer career exploration programs at both institutions to recruit study participants. The survey was presented to campers as part of a public health workshop designed to inform them about research and health literacy in general. No personal identifying information was collected. Therefore, formal documentation of parental consent was unnecessary. All participants’ parents were notified that their child would be invited to voluntarily participate in a health literacy and health knowledge survey. All students could decline survey participation.

Participants had 40 to 55 minutes to complete their survey, depending on class schedules. There was no penalty for skipping questions or not completing the survey within the time limit. The Institutional Review Boards at URMC and NTID/RIT approved all procedures.

Measures

Standard health literacy measures

We conducted a comprehensive literature review of health literacy measures, assessments, and tools to identify relevant and potentially adaptable measures for D/HH adolescents. We identified 12 candidate health literacy and health knowledge measures. However, some measures were not appropriate for use with deaf adolescents because they required speech, or contained culturally inappropriate content. Three standard health literacy measures were accepted for adaptation for D/HH adolescents to capture different aspects of health literacy and health knowledge.

General health literacy

Health Literacy Skills Instrument-Short Form (HLSI-SF; Bann, McCormack, Berkman, & Squiers, 2012). The 10-item HLSI-SF assesses overall health literacy, probing a wide range of skills related to accessing and interpreting health-related information from various sources (e.g., print, charts, maps, brochures, videos, and websites). These skills are necessary to understand and apply health information presented in formats including and beyond printed words and numbers. The HLSI-SF is valid with acceptable internal consistency reliability for group-level comparisons, and is usually completed within ten minutes (Bann et al., 2012).

We intentionally did not translate text within the existing standard materials that the HLSI-SF requires participants to interpret and answer questions about because these materials are intended to represent real-world health-related materials conventionally offered to consumers of health information and health care, including D/HH people. That is, the HLSI contains a functional health literacy component to assess overall health literacy. However, while an individual’s ability to interpret these materials is relevant to their overall health literacy, their ability to interpret the specifically formatted language of the test instrument itself is construct-irrelevant. Therefore, we adapted the HLSI-SF by translating the English instructions, questions, answers, and phone-tree menu into American Sign Language (ASL) and Conceptually Accurate Sign English (CASE)2 and included captions of health-related video narrations to eliminate this source of assessment error. Such multi-lingual adaptations restrict the influence of English knowledge to the construct-relevant functional health literacy component of this measure.

Functional health literacy

Short Form of the Test of Functional Health Literacy (S-TOFHLA; Baker, Williams, Parker, Gazmararian, & Nurss, 1999). The S-TOFHLA is a gold-standard measure of functional health literacy, a 36-item cloze assessment of an individual’s ability to read and understand printed health information. Participants choose one of four word options that best continue sentences within two health-related passages. Since functional health literacy by definition depends upon printed text comprehension, we administered the S-TOFHLA without any translation or adaptation. The S-TOFHLA was programmed as an interactive computer application, as has been done in other studies without any significant changes to its psychometric properties (Chesser, Keene-Woods, Wipperman, Wilson, & Dong, 2014).

Health knowledge

The Comprehensive Heart Disease Knowledge Questionnaire (CHDKQ; Bergman, Reeve, Moser, Scholl, & Klein, 2011). The CHDKQ is a 30-item test of general cardiovascular knowledge that has good convergent and divergent validity with other cardiovascular and general health literacy measures, respectively, and acceptable internal reliability (Bergman, et al., 2011). We translated the CHDKQ into ASL and CASE and also produced a spoken language version, to respectively assess D/HH and hearing adolescents’ cardiovascular knowledge independent of their reading skills. We focused on cardiovascular health knowledge because cardiovascular disease is a critical concern for D/HH people (Jones, Renger, & Firestone, 2005).

Original interactive and critical health literacy measures

Based on the major themes that emerged from formative interviews with 20 D/HH high school students (Smith et al., 2015), we developed several original self-report items about specific interactive and critical health literacy skills (see Table 4). These items were then translated into ASL and CASE as well as spoken English. Participants rated all items on a 1–5 Likert scale. An example item is “How easy is it for you to decide if health information that you get from other people is true or false? (1=very difficult, 2=difficult, 3=somewhat difficult, 4= easy, 5=very easy)”

Table 4.

Interactive and Critical Health Literacy Skills and Experiences of D/HH and Hearing Adolescents Adjusted for Age, Grade, Gender, Race/Ethnicity, and Childhood Socioeconomic Status

Interactive and
Critical Health
Literacy Skills
and
Experiences
Level Hearing Status Statistics

D/HH Hearing chi2(df), p

N % N %
Frequency of
family
discussions
about family
health history
Never to
Sometimes
86 93.48 82 90.80 1.7 (1), ns
Often to Very
Often
6 6.52 9 9.89
Ease of
creating a
healthy
environment
for self
Hard 50 57.47 35 38.46 12.0 (1), .0005
Easy 37 42.53 56 61.54
Ease of
deciding truth
of printed
health
information
Hard 60 68.97 58 64.44 1.0 (1), ns
Easy 27 31.03 32 35.56
Ease of
deciding truth
of health
information
from other
people
Hard 68 79.07 59 64.84 8.0 (1), .0048
Easy 18 20.93 32 35.16
Ease of
deciding
which foods
are healthy
Hard 23 26.44 16 17.58 1.7 (1), ns
Easy 64 73.56 75 82.42
Ease of
deciding
amount of
exercise
Hard 34 39.53 29 32.22 3.1 (1), .0762
Easy 52 60.47 61 67.78
Ease of
deciding when
to talk to
doctor about
family medical
history
Hard 47 55.29 43 47.25 0.1 (1), ns
Easy 38 44.71 48 52.75
Frequency of
events that
make you think
about health
Never to
sometimes
54 63.53 60 67.42 0.0 (1), ns
Often to very
often
31 36.47 29 32.58
Ease of
deciding when
to see a doctor
Hard 41 50.00 24 26.97 6.3 (1), .0120
Easy 41 50.00 65 73.03

Demographics

We developed items about general, social, and deaf-related demographic characteristics that influence language, educational, and cognitive development potentially important for D/HH and hearing adolescents’ health literacy (See Table 1). Childhood SES was assessed based on participants’ parents’ education levels, one of three traditional SES proxies for adolescents (Lien, Friestad, & Klepp, 2001). Participants reporting both parents had at most a high school education or attended vocational/technical school were classified as low childhood SES. Participants reporting one or more parent had some college education or greater were classified as high childhood SES. All items were translated into ASL and CASE as well as spoken language to test D/HH and hearing adolescents, respectively, independent of their reading skills.

Table 1.

Demographics of D/HH and Hearing Study Participants

Demographic Level Hearing Status

D/HH Hearing


N M (SD) or % N M (SD) or %
Age**** 154 17.0 (.84) 89 15.8 (1.1)
Gender** Male 83 53.2 30 33
Female 73 46.8 61 67
Grade**** 8th 8 8.8
9th 14 15.4
10th 31 20 27 29.7
11th 86 55.5 36 39.5
12th 38 24.5 5 5.5
13th 1 1.1
Hispanic
ethnicity
Yes 31 20.3 12 13.2
No 122 79.7 79 86.8
Race! White 95 63.3 47 51.6
African American 13 8.7 17 18.7
Asian 12 8 9 9.9
Native Hawaiian or other PI 1 0.7
Native American, American
Indian, Alaskan Native
8 5.3 1 1.1
Other 21 14 17 18.7
Childhood
socioeconomic
status****
Low 41 29.7 6 6.8
High 97 70.3 82 93.2
Cultural identity Culturally
Deaf
25 17.4
Deaf 63 43.8
Hard of
hearing
35 24.3
Hearing
impaired
16 11.1
Hearing 3 2.1 91 100
Other 2 1.4
Best language ASL/Sign
Language
38 26.8
English 49 34.5 88 96.7
Both ASL
and English
55 38.7
Other
Language
3 3.3
Age onset of
deafness
At birth 77 53.9
By 3 years 34 23.8
After 3 years 22 15.4
Are Hearing 2 1.4 91 100
Don’t know 8 5.6
Deaf parents One or more
deaf
20 14.2
Hearing 121 85.8
Deaf immediate
family
One or more
deaf
35 24.8
Hearing 106 75.2
Deaf extended
family
One or more
deaf
45 31.9
Hearing 98 68.1
Have hearing aid Yes 93 64.1
No 52 35.9
Have cochlear
implant
Yes 52 37.1
No 88 62.9
Hearing without
hearing assistance
1 = You hear
everything
8 5.7
2 12 8.5
3 40 28.4
4 28 19.9
5 = You hear
nothing
53 37.6
Hearing with
hearing assistance
1 = You hear
everything
31 27.2
2 29 25.4
3 19 16.7
4 17 14.9
5 = You hear
nothing
18 15.8
!

p<.1,

*

p<.05,

**

p<.01,

****

p<.0001

Procedure

Translation and video production

We extensively pre-tested and revised the initial written version of all survey measures with 18 individuals, including content-expert and content-naïve deaf and hearing colleagues, teachers, interpreters, and students, to improve the items’ comprehension ease and face validity. To respect the diversity of sign languages within the Deaf community, we followed basic protocols for translation, adaptation, back translation, cognitive interview, and studio production procedures described in other health survey research with deaf people (Graybill et al., 2010; Pollard, Dean, O’Hearn, & Haynes, 2009). We convened 5 bilingual ASL/English experts and community members to translate by consensus the written English of the instrument’s instructions and content into ASL. A second group of 5 bilingual English/CASE experts and community members translated by consensus the same materials into Conceptually Accurate Sign English. All materials were back translated into English by three independent bilingual community members to validate the faithfulness of these translations. Four D/HH students produced the material in sign language on camera. These models varied in gender, race, and sign language type to provide a diverse choice of survey presenters.

To provide more comparable testing conditions for D/HH and hearing adolescents and to control for the possibility that some hearing adolescents might have poor reading skills, we created a spoken English version of all signed measures. Four hearing models that matched the D/HH models in gender and racial diversity were video-recorded speaking the items.

Cognitive testing and survey construction

We programmed all measures on touch-screen laptops. We developed a new computer interface to present the HLSI-SF and S-TOFHLA and presented the remaining survey items using a video-enabled interface developed by the National Center for Deaf Health Research (Samar, Barnett, Oyzon, Mowl, & Sutter, 2012; Barnett et al., 2011; Graybill et al., 2010). In addition to the signed or spoken videos, these interfaces also presented the printed text of all measures. We named the full collection of measures the “Health Literacy Survey for Young People” (HLSYP). We pilot-tested the HLSYP with in-depth cognitive interviews of four female and two male sign language-using deaf college students, which led to minimal changes in the survey instructions as well as some adjustments in the survey format and item order to improve the comprehensibility and flow of the survey.

Data collection

Participants took the HLSYP anonymously on laptop computers at their own pace in small groups of 12–16 seated around large classroom tables prior to a scheduled workshop on public health issues and research methods.

Data analysis

We used analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) for the standard health literacy outcome measures and logistic regressions for the interactive and critical health literacy measures with hearing status (D/HH vs. hearing) as a between subjects factor to test for group performance disparities. We also conducted within group ANCOVAs using interactive and critical health literacy categories and deaf-related demographic categories as between-subjects factors to predict the three standard health literacy measures. All ANCOVAS and logistic regressions included covariates to adjust for age, grade, gender, race/ethnicity, and childhood socioeconomic status. Race/ethnicity was recoded as White/non-Hispanic and Other to provide a dichotomous covariate.

Results

Demographics

Table 1 summarizes the participant groups’ demographic characteristics. Approximately 39% of the D/HH participants did not complete the survey, while 100% of the hearing participants completed the survey. In addition, some students skipped various questions. Therefore, the group numbers in Table 1 vary. D/HH participants were significantly older, more frequently male, in higher grades, from lower childhood socioeconomic homes and tended to be more frequently white non-Hispanic compared with hearing participants.

D/HH participants reported a very broad range of hearing loss, with and without assistive devices. Approximately two-thirds reported having hearing aids and one-third reported having cochlear implants. D/HH participants described a wide range of cultural identities including individuals who described themselves as “culturally deaf” or “deaf “to individuals who described themselves as “hard-of-hearing”, “hearing impaired”, or “hearing”. They also reported a range of best languages ranging from ASL, to equivalent competence in ASL and English, and to English. Approximately 78% reported hearing loss onset before age 3 and the large majority reported having hearing parents and family members.

Group Comparisons on Standard Health Literacy Measures

Table 2 compares the D/HH and hearing participants on the three standard health literacy measures (HLSI-SF, S-TOFHLA, and CHDKQ). Controlling for general demographics, D/HH adolescents had significantly lower scores than hearing adolescents on all three measures (all p’s<0.001).

Table 2.

Mean Performance Scores on Standard Health Literacy Measures of D/HH and Hearing Adolescents Adjusted for Age, Grade, Gender, Race/Ethnicity, and Childhood Socioeconomic Status

Standard
Health
Literacy
Measure
Hearing Status Statistics

D/HH Hearing F(df1,df2), p

N M (CI) N M (CI)
HLSI 186 5.0
(4.5–5.6)
93 7.6
(6.9–8.2)
73.9(1,207), .0001
S-TOFHLA 185 25.6
(23.7–27.4)
91 31.5
(29.4–33.6)
38.5(1,205), .0001
CHDKQ
(%correct)
104 30.6
(25.0–36.3)
90 48.4
(42.2–54.6)
39.2(1,167), .0001

We further adjusted the regression equations predicting each health literacy measure by including the two remaining health literacy measures as covariates. This procedure allowed us to evaluate for group disparities in each health literacy measure that were specific to that measure. Table 3 reveals that D/HH adolescents continued to demonstrate lower scores on each health literacy measure after adjusting simultaneously for demographic variables and for performance on the other two health literacy measures.

Table 3.

D/HH Versus Hearing Group Comparisons on the HLSI, S-TOFHLA, and CHDKQ Adjusted for Age, Grade, Gender, Race/Ethnicity, Childhood Socioeconomic Status, and Different Health Literacy Measures

Standard Health
Literacy Measure
Group Statistics

D/HH Hearing F(df1,df2), p

N M
(CI)
N M
(CI)
HLSI 186 6.1
(5.5–6.7)
93 7.6
(7.0–8.2)
2
0.5 (1,161), .001
S-TOFHLA 185 29.0
(27.2–30.9)
91 3
0.9
(29.1–34.7)
3.6 (1,161), .060
CHDKQ
(%correct)
104 35.6
(29.5–41.6)
90 45.4
(39.2–51.6)
8.8 (1,161), .004

Group Comparisons on Interactive and Critical Health Literacy Skills and Experiences

Table 4 shows that D/HH adolescents differed significantly from hearing adolescents with more trouble: 1) creating healthy environment for themselves; 2) determining the accuracy of health information obtained from other people; 3) deciding how much exercise is needed to stay healthy; and 4) deciding when they needed to go see a doctor. D/HH and hearing adolescents reported similar frequency of family discussion about family health history, and similar ability to determine the accuracy of printed health information, what foods are healthy to eat, and when to talk to their doctors about their family health history.

Relationships between Interactive and Critical Health Literacy Skills/Experiences and Standard Health Literacy Measures

Table 5 reveals that D/HH adolescents who reported having more frequent family discussions about their family health history had higher HLSI-SF and S-TOFHLA scores than D/HH adolescents who had fewer such discussions. Furthermore, D/HH adolescents who reported having an easier time determining the truth of printed health information had higher HLSI-SF and CHDKQ scores than those who reported having trouble making such decisions. In addition, those who reported an easier time determining the accuracy of health information obtained from other people had higher CHDKQ scores. These relationships were not significant for hearing adolescents.

Table 5.

Mean Performance Scores on Standard Health Literacy Measures of Deaf/HH and Hearing Adolescents as a Function of Interactive and Critical Health Literacy Skills and Experiences Adjusted for Age, Grade, Gender, Race/Ethnicity, and Childhood Socioeconomic Status

Hearing Status

D/HH Hearing


Interactive
and Critical
Health
Literacy
Skills and
Experiences
Level N HLSI
Adj. M
(SE)
S-
TOFHLA
Adj. M
(SE)
CHDKQ
Adj. M
(SE)
N HLSI
Adj. M
(SE)
S-
TOFHLA
Adj. M
(SE)
CHDKQ
Adj. M
(SE)
Frequency of
family
discussions
about family
medical
history
Never to
Sometimes
86 5.02a
(.27)
27.14b
(.88)
36.96
(2.30)
82 7.74
(.38)
33.79
(.44)
52.04
(4.21)
Often to
Very
Often
6 7.04a
(1.00)
34.28b
(3.19)
34.30
(8.35)
9 7.54
(.64)
33.89
(.76)
44.06
(7.44)
Ease of
creating a
healthy
environment
for self
Easy 37 4.81
(.34)
27.05
(1.18)
35.11
(2.89)
56 7.62
(0.43)
33.74
(0.52)
51.36
(4.99)
Hard 50 5.48
(.36)
27.48
(1.24)
40.90
(3.03)
35 7.81
(0.40)
33.81
(0.46)
52.40
(4.44)
Ease of
deciding
truth of
printed
health
information
Hard 60 4.83c
(.31)
26.47
(1.07)
35.02d
(2.61)
58 7.57
(0.46)
33.75
(0.51)
50.76
(4.81)
Easy 27 5.74c
(.41)
28.83
(1.41)
43.50d
(3.44)
32 7.6
(0.45)
33.8
(0.53)
55.27
(5.09)
Ease of
deciding
truth of
health
information
from other
people
Hard 68 5.13
(.28)
27.02
(0.98)
36.22aa
(2.32)
59 7.62
(0.41)
33.48
(0.5)
47.12
(4.75)
Easy 18 5.09
(.60)
28.88
(2.08)
49.65aa
(4.93)
32 7.98
(0.43)
33.6
(0.52)
53.45
(4.97)
Ease of
deciding
which foods
are healthy
Hard 23 4.47!
(.45)
27.02
(1.57)
28.20bb
(3.62)
16 7.65
(0.48)
32.81e
(.59)
52.09
(5.66)
Easy 64 5.42!
(.31)
27.39
(1.08)
42.00bb
(2.50)
75 7.78
(0.40)
34.06e
(0.44)
52.05
(4.39)
Ease of
deciding
amount of
exercise
Hard 34 4.32cc
(.36)
26.35
(1.31)
31.92dd
(3.09)
29 7.76
(0.41)
33.13f
(.49)
46.77g
(4.80)
Easy 52 5.74cc
(.31)
27.95
(1.15)
42.20dd
(2.72)
61 7.72
(0.4)
34.17f
(0.45)
55.25g
(4.41)
Ease of
deciding
when to talk
to doctor
about family
medical
history
Hard 47 4.88
(.34)
26.19
(1.15)
34.20h
(2.81)
90 7.87
(0.41)
33.77
(0.47)
50.17
(4.51)
Easy 38 5.45
(.38)
28.45
(1.28)
42.10h
(3.12)
86 7.62
(0.41)
33.8
(0.49)
54.46
(4.68)
Frequency of
events that
make you
think about
health
Never to
Sometimes
54 5.17
(.34)
27.66
(1.12)
37.14
(2.78)
60 7.68
(0.38)
33.81
(0.37)
50.99
(4.11)
Often to
Very
Often
31 5.26
(.45)
26.66
(1.48)
39.34
(3.68)
29 7.86
(0.45)
33.46
(0.46)
53.98
(4.99)
Ease of
deciding
when to see a
doctor
Hard 41 4.65!
(.38)
25.56ee
(1.21)
33.47i
(3.23)
24 7.72
(0.56)
34.33
(0.54)
43.65!
(6.11)
Easy 41 5.49!
(.36)
29.69ee
(1.13)
41.38i
(3.01)
65 7.86
(0.45)
34.91
(0.42)
51.02!
(4.75)
!

p<.1; paired single superscript letters (e.g., a through i) denote significant differences between pairs of means at the .05 level; paired double superscript letters (e.g., aa through ee) denote significant differences between pairs of means at the .01 level.

D/HH adolescents who reported having an easier time deciding when they needed to see a doctor had higher HLSI, S-TOFHLA, and CHDKQ scores. Those who reported having an easier time deciding when they needed to talk to their doctors about their family medical history, how much exercise they need to stay healthy, and which foods are healthy to eat had higher CHDKQ scores. Those who reported having an easier time deciding how much exercise they need to stay healthy and which foods are healthy to eat had higher HLSI scores. Hearing adolescents generally demonstrated similar patterns in these relationships.

Relationships between D/HH Demographic Factors and Standard Health Literacy Measures

Table 6 shows that D/HH adolescents who had higher S-TOFHLA scores included those who: 1) described themselves as being hearing/hearing impaired/hard-of-hearing; 2) reported having better hearing with assistive devices; 3) reported having hearing aids; 4) reported wearing their hearing aids frequently; 5) described English as their best language; 6) reported good quality of communication with their parents; and 7) reported attending hearing schools at least half of the time. D/HH adolescents who had higher cardiovascular health knowledge scores included those who reported wearing their hearing aids frequently, described English as their best language, and reported attending hearing schools at least half of the time.

Table 6.

Mean Health Literacy Scores of D/HH Adolescents with Various Deaf-Related Demographics Adjusted for Age, Grade, Gender, Race/Ethnicity, and Childhood Socioeconomic Status

Deaf-Related
Demographic
Level N HLSI S-TOFHLA CHDKQ
Adj.
M
(SE)
F
(df)
p
Adj. M
(SE)
F
(df)
p
Adj. M
(SE)
F
(df)
p
Cultural identity Deaf 88 4.96
(.26)
24.74
(.92)
5.4
(1,113)
.025
36.26
(2.52)
Hard-of-hearing
Hearing impaired
Hearing
56 5.28
(.31)
27.73
(1.08)
37.68
(2.91)
Hearing without
assistive devices
Mild loss 20 5.05
(.52)
27.91
(1.83)
34.71
(4.60)
Moderate to
profound loss
121 5.22
(.24)
25.66
(.85)
36.46
(2.27)
Hearing with
assistive devices
Mild loss 60 5.38
(.30)
28.50
(.94)
8.2
(1,93)
.006
37.29
(2.51)
Moderate to
profound loss
54 4.84
(.32)
25.05
(.99)
33.16
(2.78)
Have hearing aid Yes 93 5.17
(.26)
26.89
(.90)
5.4
(1,114)
.022
34.76
(2.37)
No 52 4.90
(.33)
23.79
(1.13)
38.78
(3.12)
How often use
hearing aid
Infrequently 37 4.88
(.37)
25.30
(1.170
3.67
(1,72)
.059
30.49
(3.28)
2.95
(1,53)
.092
Frequently 56 5.13
(.36)
28.24
(1.13)
37.33
(2.84)
Have cochlear
implant
Yes 53 5.49
(.34)
26.59
(1.24)
37.78
(3.48)
No 88 4.88
(.25)
25.36
(.89)
35.57
(2.29)
How often use
cochlear implant
Infrequently 14 4.34
(.63)
3.39
(1,33)
.071
25.05
(1.79)
28.25
(8.81)
Frequently 39 5.89
(.52)
24.32
(1.48)
36.29
(6.71)
Age of onset of
deafness
Before 3 111 5.00
(.25)
26.00
(0.88)
34.40
(2.34)
After 3 32 5.25
(.38)
25.13
(1.36)
40.46
(3.56)
Best language American Sign
Language
38 4.95
(.38)
23.47
(1.33)
2.39
(2,111)
.096
37.91
(3.50)
4.43
(2,81)
.015
English 49 5.19
(.34)
27.22
(1.20)
41.53
(3.11)
Both 55 5.06
(.32)
26.32
(1.15)
30.75
(2.80)
Socialize Deaf 30 5.24
(.45)
25.85
(1.59)
34.53
(3.89)
Hearing 23 4.96
(.46)
27.6
(1.61)
40.79
(4.02)
Both 87 5.06
(.26)
25.3
(.93)
34.77
(2.51)
Deaf mother Yes 19 5.76
(.51)
26.99
(1.83)
37.86
(4.26)
No or don't know 122 4.99
(.23)
25.65
(.84)
35.77
(2.19)
Deaf father Yes 16 5.51
(.55)
25.64
(1.99)
36.61
(4.75)
No or don't know 126 5.01
(.23)
25.77
(.82)
36.03
(2.16)
Deaf siblings Yes 29 5.16
(.42)
25.48
(1.55)
38.51
(3.73)
No or don't know 93 5.05
(.26)
25.91
(.98)
36.03
(2.39)
Deaf extended
family
Yes 16 5.35
(.43)
24.27
(1.52)
36.23
(3.96)
No or don't know 126 5.03
(.24)
26.26
(.86)
36.07
(2.22)
Quality of
communication
with parents
Poor 42 4.80
(.35)
22.00
(1.17)
18.29
(1,107)
.0001
34.65
(3.17)
Good 92 5.16
(.28)
28.25
(.95)
36.91
(2.52)
Type of school Mostly deaf 4.91
(.31)
23.95
(1.09)
5.36
(1,105)
.0225
32.52
(2.64)
4.92
(1,82)
.0293
Hearing at least
half of the time
5.07
(.29)
27.09
(1.03)
40.11
(2.73)

Having a cochlear implant (CI) per se was not related to D/HH adolescents’ HLSI, S-TOFHLA, or CHDKQ scores. However, of the D/HH adolescents who reported having a CI, those who used their CI more frequently showed a trend toward higher HLSI scores (p=0.071). Otherwise, D/HH adolescents’ reports of their degree of hearing loss without assistive devices, their age of onset of deafness, their preferences for socializing with deaf versus hearing friends, and the presence or absence of deaf family members were not related to their HLSI, S-TOFHLA, and CHDKQ scores.

Discussion

Using culturally and linguistically appropriate measures of health literacy adapted for both D/HH and hearing adolescents, our findings indicate that D/HH adolescents have significantly weaker general health literacy, functional health literacy, and cardiovascular health knowledge than their hearing adolescent peers. These health literacy disparities emerged despite controlling for common demographic variables that might influence health literacy in general populations, including age, grade, gender, race/ethnicity, and socioeconomic status. Importantly, D/HH adolescents’ disparities on each of the three standard health literacy measures persisted even when adjusted for the two other measures. The fact that a significant portion of D/HH participants were slower than hearing participants in responding to survey items is consistent with our finding of their broad health literacy and knowledge weaknesses.

Our results agree with contemporary views that health literacy is a multidimensional construct encompassing functional, interactive, and critical health literacy skills and experiences as well as health knowledge. Our results also provide the first evidence that this view also applies to D/HH adolescents. The fact that controlling for functional health literacy did not eliminate the disparities between D/HH and hearing adolescents on general health literacy and cardiovascular health knowledge measures supports the hypothesis that D/HH people’s health literacy challenges extend beyond the narrow construct captured by functional health literacy measures. This, in turn, suggests that measures of functional health literacy are insufficient to characterize the full range of health literacy challenges faced by D/HH people. Therefore, even D/HH adolescents who have stronger English language skills may be at increased risk for weaker health literacy.

Having frequent family discussions about family health history was positively associated with greater general health literacy (HLSI) and greater functional health literacy (S-TOFHLA) for D/HH but not hearing adolescents. Since the HSLI scores were broadly distributed for both D/HH and hearing groups, the absence of this effect for hearing adolescents was not due to any relatively greater range restriction on the hearing adolescent’s HLSI scores because of their overall superior health literacy (interquartile range: Hearing, 7–9; D/HH, 4–7). Rather, we speculate that family conversations play a more critical role for deaf children than for hearing children in their development of health literacy skills. Family conversations and other incidental sources of health knowledge are crucial for developing strong health literacy skills (Smith et al., 2015). Hearing adolescents who do not directly discuss health issues with their families nevertheless might overhear conversations about family health among immediate or extended family members and friends or through the media, whereas D/HH adolescents generally have limited access to such incidental sources. Our finding that the relationship occurred for D/HH adolescents but not for hearing adolescents agrees with these considerations.

The frequency of family health conversations was also positively related to functional health literacy in D/HH but not hearing adolescents. Hearing adolescents did show a highly restricted range of S-TOFHLA scores compared with D/HH adolescents (interquartile range: Hearing, 34–36; D/HH, 21–33), perhaps because the S-TOFHLA is heavily dependent on reading ability, which does not vary greatly in hearing adolescents. Since reading skill is the primary determinant of S-TOFHLA scores, the failure of frequent family discussion to be associated with S-TOFHLA for hearing adolescents might therefore be explained by the severe restriction on variability in reading ability in our hearing sample. Nevertheless, it remains possible that frequency of family health discussion might relate significantly to functional health literacy in a general-population sample of hearing adolescents with a greater expected range of reading skill. Nevertheless, our finding of this relationship for academically select D/HH adolescents underscores the pervasiveness of functional health literacy deficits in the D/HH population regardless of their education level.

D/HH adolescents who reported that they could easily evaluate the accuracy of printed health information had greater general health literacy (HLSI) and greater cardiovascular health knowledge (CHDKQ) than other D/HH adolescents. Furthermore, D/HH adolescents who reported that they could easily evaluate health information obtained from other people and who could easily decide when to talk to a doctor about their family’s medical history had greater cardiovascular heath knowledge than other D/HH adolescents. In contrast, hearing adolescents did not show these relationships. Again, these results could not be explained by any selective restriction on score ranges for hearing adolescents for the HLSI or the CHDKQ (interquartile range: Hearing, 26.7–50; D/HH, 41.0–63.3). Rather, these results are consistent with the claim that due to their greater communication skills and social access during childhood, hearing adolescents’ health literacy skills are shaped by redundant access to multiple alternative sources of common health information. Collectively, these results suggest that critical and interactive measures of health-related behaviors and experiences are more significant and informative markers of overall health literacy in deaf adolescents than in hearing adolescents.

Other interactive and critical health literacy behaviors and experiences shown in Table 5 were related to standard health literacy measures in both deaf and hearing participant groups. Ease of deciding which foods are healthy, ease of deciding the amount of exercise to stay healthy, and ease of deciding when to see a doctor were related to one or more of the three standard health literacy measures in both D/HH and hearing participants. These relationships underscore the potential value of including specific direct interactive and critical health literacy measures in comprehensive health literacy assessments of both D/HH and hearing adolescents.

Analyses of deafness-related demographic subgroups revealed relationships primarily related to functional health literacy. D/HH adolescents who reported severe hearing loss even with assistive devices or who reported not having or not frequently using hearing aids had lower S-TOFHLA scores than other deaf adolescents. However, having or frequently using a cochlear implant was not clearly related to D/HH adolescents’ health literacy.

D/HH adolescents who reported a deaf cultural identity, having sign language as their best language, having poor quality of communication with their parents, or having attended mostly deaf schools had lower functional health literacy and cardiovascular health knowledge than other D/HH adolescents. These results suggest that cultural, social and family communication factors may influence deaf adolescents’ health literacy development. However, the reported hearing status of parents, siblings, extended family, or social group members did not significantly relate to any standard health literacy measures. It is possible that survey items such as those represented in Table 1 that measure hearing status of family and peers may be less reliable proxies for communication quality than direct questions about childhood communication experiences. Further research is necessary to verify and reconcile these results.

As adolescence is a critical developmental period for health education inventions, we should increase our efforts to develop targeted health education and interventions for this population. Such efforts should include educating families of D/HH children to include their children and adolescents in more conversations about their health history, which might help to compensate for the barriers they experience in gaining access to indirect incidental family health information and general health knowledge from health care professionals and media sources.

D/HH adolescents would also benefit from improved access to printed health information in general. Printed health information designed for D/HH adolescents should use simpler English grammar and vocabulary, and more visual information (pictures, models, etc.). Further research and effort is needed to make the production of simplified printed health information more efficient and effective, such as automated text simplification programs (Leroy, Endicott, Kauchak, Mouradi, & Just, 2013). Such materials, tailored to the English language skills and visual learning competencies of D/HH adolescents would help to compensate for the health information that they otherwise miss in their environments. Moreover, increasing the number of well-qualified healthcare sign language interpreters and promoting the use of these interpreters for D/HH children’s interactions with health care providers would also help to improve D/HH adolescents’ access to health information from health professionals who do not use sign language (Smith et al., 2015).

This study used cross-sectional survey data and therefore causal inferences about correlated findings cannot be made. In addition, random uncontrollable factors such as strict survey time limits as dictated by program schedules caused some participants not to complete their surveys, weakening the statistical power of our comparisons. The strict time limit also did not allow us to obtain an independent standardized measure of reading level from our participants. However, this limitation is mitigated by the fact that the S-TOFHLA is generally recognized as a reading comprehension measure, since it uses a cloze procedure and is based specifically on the reading comprehension part of the TOFHLA (Hoffman, et al., 2013). That reading comprehension part has been demonstrated to have good convergent validity with the WRAT as a literacy measure for adolescents (Chisolm & Buchanan, 2007). Therefore, using the S-TOFHLA as a covariate in regression analyses that predict group disparities in the HLSI and CHDKQ measures allowed us to essentially account for variance associated with reading comprehension.

The use of a national convenience sample of only college-bound adolescents limits the generalizability of study results and findings. Although the D/H and hearing samples were well match academically, they clearly do not represent the range of academic and communication skills present in the general D/HH or hearing adolescent populations. Studies that compare D/HH and hearing groups inevitable face many potentially uncontrollable sampling biases, such as biases due to geographically local sampling, differences in developmental, socioeconomic, and academic attainment variables. A positive aspect of our particular convenience sample is that by partnering with college exploration programs, we were able to compare national samples of academically successful D/HH and hearing individuals across multiple social strata.

A factor that might have contributed to the observed group differences is potential differences in the general interest of the two participant groups in health-related issues. The hearing participants were attending a summer program for adolescents who had a general interest in pursuing health careers, whereas the D/HH participants attended a broader career exploration camp. It is possible that the hearing participants might have been more likely to have sought out information about various health-related topics. This could cause them to do better on health literacy measures regardless of their long history of better communication and access to formal and incidental information in social and health care environments. If a general interest in health-related topics caused the hearing participants to have generally greater health literacy and health-related knowledge, we might expect the size of the group disparity to be correlated across our three standard health literacy and knowledge measures, since greater interest should motivate specific individuals to seek more or less broad exposure to information across several domains of health literacy and knowledge. However, our results indicate that the D/HH versus hearing group disparity on each of the three health literacy and knowledge measures remained even after statistically controlling scores on the other two measures. These results suggest that the group disparities were not correlated, but were domain-specific and likely stemmed from distinct underlying deaf-related factors rather than from a general sample difference in the overall level of interest in health topics. However, we cannot completely rule out a contribution of differences in the overall level of interest to the group disparities and future research should more rigorously control for this potential factor.

Although we have controlled for several demographic variables, we note that childhood SES and race/ethnicity are multidimensional constructs and our measures only crudely controlled for these social determinants of health literacy and knowledge. Future research should include additional measures and categories (Lien et al, 2001).

Our adapted HLSI version eliminated artificial influences of test format-related language on overall health literacy measurement, but retained significant English content in the test’s health-related materials. This procedure maintained the test’s component sensitivity to functional health literacy given real-world health information sources, an important issue for deaf adolescents (Smith et al., 2015). Limited ability of the D/HH participants to process the English content could have partially but not fully depressed HLSI scores because the HLSI disparity remained even after controlling statistically for the effect of functional health literacy using the S-TOFHLA. Furthermore, this possibility cannot explain the overall pattern of depressed health literacy and health knowledge, which was broadly expressed in our other translated measures. Future research should explore the effect of fully translating all reference materials in the HLSI to eliminate the functional health literacy contribution on its measurement outcomes.

Traditional methods such as the standard health literacy measures used in this study, even if linguistically and culturally adapted for D/HH populations, may not adequately assess the health literacy and health-related knowledge of D/HH individuals. The D/HH community prefers to acquire and share information through dialogic interactions between community members, as opposed to the typical monologic format of educational presentations or survey tools. (Pollard et al., 2009). Dialogic assessment protocols and novel interactive tools that respect the sociolinguistic norms of the D/HH community should be developed and explored in future research.

Conclusion

D/HH adolescents, especially those who use sign language to communicate with professionals and educators who do not use sign language, are at increased risk for weak overall, interactive, and critical health literacy, and health knowledge that extend beyond their functional health literacy deficits. Interventions to improve D/HH adolescents’ health literacy should target family-oriented activities to improve their access to conversations about health, adaptation of printed health information to match their English language and visual learning competencies, and interpreter-aided access to health information from health care providers and educators who do not use sign language.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the following individuals and organizations for their participation in and contribution to this study: Jacqueline Pransky, Lorne Farovitch, Pratik Mehta, Poonam Waral, Peter Hauser, Robert Pollard, Thomas Pearson, National Center for Deaf Health Research, Deaf Studies Laboratory, NTID Pre-College Outreach Program, and University of Rochester Campus Programs.

Footnotes

1

We use the abbreviation “D/HH” here to refer to individuals with childhood onset of deafness who typically, but not always, use American Sign Language or Signed English as their primary language

2

Conceptually accurate sign English refers to the use of simultaneously communicated conceptually accurate sign choices and ASL features that follow English word order

References

  1. Al Sayah F, Majumdar SR, Egede LE, Johnson JA. Associations between health literacy and health outcomes in a predominantly low-income African American population with type 2 diabetes. Journal of Health Communication. 2015;20(5):581–588. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2015.1012235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baker DW, Williams MV, Parker RM, Gazmararian JA, Nurss J. Development of a brief test to measure functional health literacy. Patient Education and Counseling. 1999;38(1):33–42. doi: 10.1016/s0738-3991(98)00116-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bann CM, McCormack LA, Berkman ND, Squiers LB. The Health Literacy Skills Instrument: a 10-item short form. Journal of Health Communication. 2012;17(3):191–202. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2012.718042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barnett S, Klein JD, Pollard RQ, Samar VJ, Schlehofer D, Starr M, Sutter E, Yang H, Pearson TA. Community participatory research with deaf sign language users to identify health inequities. American Journal of Public Health. 2011;101(12):2235–2238. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bergman HE, Reeve BB, Moser RP, Scholl S, Klein WM. Development of a Comprehensive Heart Disease Knowledge Questionnaire. American Journal of Health Education. 2011;42(2):74–87. doi: 10.1080/19325037.2011.10599175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berkman ND, Davis TC, McCormack L. Health literacy: what is it? Journal of Health Communication. 2010;15(2):9–19. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2010.499985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chesser AK, Keene Woods N, Wipperman J, Wilson R, Dong F. Health literacy assessment of the STOFHLA: paper versus electronic administration continuation study. Health Education and Behavior. 2014;41(1):19–24. doi: 10.1177/1090198113477422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chinn D, McCarthy C. All Aspects of Health Literacy Scale (AAHLS): developing a tool to measure functional, communicative and critical health literacy in primary healthcare settings. Patient Education and Counseling. 2012;90(2):247–253. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2012.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chisolm DJ, Buchanan L. Measuring adolescent functional health literacy: A pilot validation of the test of functional health literacy in adults. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2007;41:312–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Davis TC, Crouch MA, Long SW, Jackson RH, Bates P, George RB, Bairnsfather LE. Rapid assessment of literacy levels of adult primary care patients. Family Medicine. 1991;23(6):433–435. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fang Z, Schleppegrell MJ. Disciplinary literacies across content areas: Supporting secondary reading through functional language analysis. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy. 2010;53(7):587–597. [Google Scholar]
  12. Freedman RA, Kouri EM, West DW, Keating NL. Racial/ethnic disparities in knowledge about one’s breast cancer characteristics. Cancer. 2015;121(5):724–732. doi: 10.1002/cncr.28977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Graybill P, Aggas J, Dean RK, Demers S, Finigan E, Pollard RQ. A community-participatory approach to adapting survey items for deaf individuals and American Sign Language. Field Methods. 2010;22(4):1–20. [Google Scholar]
  14. Green JA, Cavanaugh KL. Understanding the influence of educational attainment on kidney health and opportunities for improved care. Advances in Chronic Kidney Disease. 2015;22(1):24–30. doi: 10.1053/j.ackd.2014.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Haun JN, Valerio MA, McCormack LA, Sorensen K, Paasche-Orlow MK. Health literacy measurement: an inventory and descriptive summary of 51 instruments. Journal of Health Communication. 2014;19(2):302–333. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2014.936571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hoffman S, Trout AL, Nelson TD, Huscroft-D’Angelo J, Sullivan J, Epstein MH, Gibbons C. A psychometric assessment of health literacy measures among youth in a residential treatment setting. Journal of Studies in Social Sciences. 2013;5:288–300. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ishikawa H, Nomura K, Sato M, Yano E. Developing a measure of communicative and critical health literacy: a pilot study of Japanese office workers. Health Promotion International. 2008;23(3):269–274. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dan017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Jones EG, Renger R, Firestone R. Deaf community analysis for health education priorities. Public Health Nursing. 2005;22(1):27–35. doi: 10.1111/j.0737-1209.2005.22105.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Leroy G, Endicott JE, Kauchak D, Mouradi O, Just M. User evaluation of the effects of a text simplification algorithm using term familiarity on perception, understanding, learning, and information retention. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2013;15(7):e144. doi: 10.2196/jmir.2569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Levin-Zamir D, Lemish D, Gofin R. Media Health Literacy (MHL): development and measurement of the concept among adolescents. Health Education Research. 2011;26(2):323–335. doi: 10.1093/her/cyr007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lien N, Friestad C, Klepp KI. Adolescents’ proxy reports of parents’ socioeconomic status: How valid are they? Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2001;55:731–737. doi: 10.1136/jech.55.10.731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Margellos-Anast H, Estarziau M, Kaufman G. Cardiovascular disease knowledge among culturally deaf patients in Chicago. Preventive Medicine. 2006;42(3):235–239. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Marrie RA, Salter A, Tyry T, Fox RJ, Cutter GR. Health literacy association with health behaviors and health care utilization in multiple sclerosis: a cross-sectional study. Interactive Journal of Medical Research. 2014;3(1):e3. doi: 10.2196/ijmr.2993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. McCormack L, Bann C, Squiers L, Berkman ND, Squire C, Schillinger D, Ohene-Frempong J, Hibbard J. Measuring health literacy: a pilot study of a new skills-based instrument. Journal of Health Communication. 2010;15(2):51–71. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2010.499987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. McKee MM, Barnett SL, Block RC, Pearson TA. Impact of communication on preventive services among deaf American Sign Language users. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2011;41(1):75–79. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. McKee M, Schlehofer D, Cuculick J, Starr M, Smith S, Chin NP. Perceptions of cardiovascular health in an underserved community of deaf adults using American Sign Language. Disability and Health Journal. 2011;4(3):192–197. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2011.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. McKee MM, Paasche-Orlow MK. Health literacy and the disenfranchised: the importance of collaboration between limited English proficiency and health literacy researchers. Journal of Health Communication. 2012;17(3):7–12. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2012.712627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McKee M, Thew D, Starr M, Kushalnagar P, Reid JT, Graybill P, Velasquez J, Pearson T. Engaging the Deaf American Sign Language community: lessons from a community-based participatory research center. Progress in Community Health Partnerships: Research, Education, and Action. 2012;6(3):321–329. doi: 10.1353/cpr.2012.0037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. McKee MM, Moreland C, Atcherson SR, Zazove P. Hearing loss: communicating with the patient who is Deaf or Hard of Hearing. FP Essentials. 2015;434:24–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. McKee MM, Winters PC, Sen A, Zazove P, Fiscella K. Emergency Department utilization among Deaf American Sign Language users. Disability and Health Journal. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2015.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Moreland C, Atcherson SR, Zazove P, McKee MM. Hearing loss: issues in the Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing communities. FP Essentials. 2015;434:29–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Nolke L, Mensing M, Kramer A, Hornberg C. Sociodemographic and health-(care-) related characteristics of online health information seekers: a cross-sectional German study. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:31. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-1423-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Norman CD, Skinner HA. eHEALS: The eHealth Literacy Scale. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2006;8(4):e27. doi: 10.2196/jmir.8.4.e27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Nutbeam D. The evolving concept of health literacy. Social Science and Medicine. 2008;67(12):2072–2078. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.09.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nutbeam D. Defining and measuring health literacy: what can we learn from literacy studies? International Journal of Public Health. 2009;54(5):303–305. doi: 10.1007/s00038-009-0050-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. The test of functional health literacy in adults: a new instrument for measuring patients’ literacy skills. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 1995;10(10):537–541. doi: 10.1007/BF02640361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pollard RQ, Barnett S. Health-related vocabulary knowledge among deaf adults. Rehabilitation Psychology. 2009;54(2):182–185. doi: 10.1037/a0015771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pollard RQ, Dean RK, O’Hearn A, Haynes SL. Adapting health education material for deaf audiences. Rehabilitation Psychology. 2009;54(2):232–238. doi: 10.1037/a0015772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rawson KA, Gunstad J, Hughes J, Spitznagel MB, Potter V, Waechter D, Rosneck J. The METER: a brief, self-administered measure of health literacy. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2010;25(1):67–71. doi: 10.1007/s11606-009-1158-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Samar VJ, Barnett S, Oyzon E, Mowl C, Sutter E. Modality-Independent Survey Tool: Imagine the potential. NTID Research Bulletin. 2012;15(1):1–5. [Google Scholar]
  41. Sentell T, Braun KL. Low health literacy, limited English proficiency, and health status in Asians, Latinos, and other racial/ethnic groups in California. Journal of Health Communication. 2012;17(3):82–99. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2012.712621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Sharif I, Blank AE. Relationship between child health literacy and body mass index in overweight children. Patient Education and Counseling. 2010;79(1):43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2009.07.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Smith SR, Kushalnagar P, Hauser PC. Deaf adolescents’ learning of cardiovascular health information: sources and access challenges. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. 2015;20(4):408–418. doi: 10.1093/deafed/env021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Weiss BD, Mays MZ, Martz W, Castro KM, DeWalt DA, Pignone MP, Mockbee J, Hale FA. Quick assessment of literacy in primary care: the newest vital sign. Annals of Family Medicine. 2005;3(6):514–522. doi: 10.1370/afm.405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wu AD, Begoray DL, Macdonald M, Wharf Higgins J, Frankish J, Kwan B, Fung W, Rootman I. Developing and evaluating a relevant and feasible instrument for measuring health literacy of Canadian high school students. Health Promotion International. 2010;25(4):444–452. doi: 10.1093/heapro/daq032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES