Abstract
Objective
We hypothesized that compared to healthy controls, long-term abstinent substance dependent individuals (SDI) will differ in their effective connectivity between large-scale brain networks and demonstrate increased directional information from executive control to interoception-, reward-, and habit-related networks. In addition, using graph theory to compare network efficiencies we predicted decreased small-worldness in SDI compared to controls.
Methods
50 SDI and 50 controls of similar sex and age completed psychological surveys and resting state fMRI. fMRI results were analyzed using group independent component analysis; 14 networks-of-interest (NOI) were selected using template matching to a canonical set of resting state networks. The number, direction, and strength of connections between NOI were analyzed with Granger Causality. Within-group thresholds were p<0.005 using a bootstrap permutation. Between group thresholds were p<0.05, FDR-corrected for multiple comparisons. NOI were correlated with behavioral measures, and group-level graph theory measures were compared.
Results
Compared to controls, SDI showed significantly greater Granger causal connectivity from right executive control network (RECN) to dorsal default mode network (dDMN) and from dDMN to basal ganglia network (BGN). RECN was negatively correlated with impulsivity, behavioral approach, and negative affect; dDMN was positively correlated with impulsivity. Among the 14 NOI, SDI showed greater bidirectional connectivity; controls showed more unidirectional connectivity. SDI demonstrated greater global efficiency and lower local efficiency.
Conclusions
Increased effective connectivity in long-term abstinent drug users may reflect improved cognitive control over habit and reward processes. Higher global and lower local efficiency across all networks in SDI compared to controls may reflect connectivity changes associated with drug dependence or remission and requires future, longitudinal studies to confirm.
Introduction
Substance dependence is a significant public health problem with an estimated 10.3% lifetime prevalence in the United States [1]. Across substances of abuse, a generalizable pattern develops beginning with an initial stage of rewarding effects from occasional use and developing into a pathologic stage of loss of control, escalated use, compulsive drug seeking, and significant negative consequences [2]. Individuals with substance dependence have been shown to exhibit higher levels of impulsivity, behavioral approach, and negative affect [3], and these differences have been associated with structural [4] and functional [5–8] brain differences compared to healthy controls. While task-based studies using fMRI and PET have contributed significantly to our understanding of functional brain changes in specific neuroanatomical areas [9], resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) provides opportunity to explore large-scale networks and network interactions independent of task-specific neuropsychological constructs [10]. Advantages of rsfMRI include less confounding by differences in task paradigms, correlation of resting state networks (RSN) to specific tasks and neuropsychiatric constructs [11], and reproducibility due to simplified experimental design and data acquisition [12].
Stimulant dependence is characterized by complex behaviors and, like other neuropsychiatric diseases, is thought to reflect pathology at the circuit-level rather than a single brain structure [13]. Moreover, activity and connectivity differences in stimulant dependence have been demonstrated using rsfMRI across disease stages and may explain the progressive behavioral phenotype changes across the natural history of the disorder [14]. For example, active drug addiction stages include (I) binge/intoxication, (II) withdrawal/negative affect, and (III) preoccupation/anticipation [13]; involved circuits at these stages include (I) ventral tegmental area and striatum; (II) amygdala, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and ventral striatum; and (III) prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, basolateral amygdala, cingulate, and insula. Sequential cycling through these active disease stages is hypothesized to result in the neuroadaptive changes that give rise to compulsive drug-seeking and drug-taking (Fig 1).
Brain activity and connectivity at different disease stages have been correlated with individual differences in executive function, interoception, reward, and habit formation. For example, Gu et al. [15] observed decreased rsfMRI connectivity between nodes within the mesocorticolimbic reward pathway in active cocaine users compared to healthy controls. These findings are consistent with animal models, in which rats dependent upon and self-administering cocaine demonstrated decreased connectivity compared to control rats [16]; affected pathways in this sample of rats included connections between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and ventral striatum, as well as between the prelimbic cortex (homologous to anterior cingulate gyrus in humans) and entopeduncular nucleus (homologous to globus pallidus interna in humans) [16]. These active disease findings stand in contrast to findings in disease remission. In short-term abstinent cocaine dependence (≥3 days), Wilcox et al. [17] observed increased rsfMRI connectivity between the ventral striatum and ventromedial PFC. Camchong and colleagues [18] measured resting state functional connectivity amongst reward processing regions in a cohort of stimulant dependent individuals at two time points, 5 weeks abstinence and 13 weeks abstinence, with comparison to a matched healthy control group. Abstinent stimulant dependent patients demonstrated increased functional connectivity compared to controls, consistent with prior studies in patients of 1.4 years of abstinence [5] and 5.7 years of abstinence [19]. Although Camchong et al. [18] found abstinent stimulant dependent patients demonstrated increased functional connectivity compared to controls, patients who relapsed between time points demonstrated decreased connectivity compared to patients who maintained abstinence. The authors speculated that this reduction in functional connectivity from 5 to 13 weeks in relapsers compared to abstinent patients may be associated with these patients’ inability to maintain abstinence. These studies suggest that group differences in connectivity may be related to different stages of dependence/remission, possibly representing a transition from hypoconnectivity in limbic and subcortical regions during active dependence to increased top-down executive control in sustained abstinence.
Understanding differences in large-scale brain connectivity depends upon characterizing the relative activity within networks as well as between them. Two modes of functional interactions between brain regions include functional connectivity and effective connectivity. Functional connectivity is the simultaneous and temporally coherent activation of separate brain regions. Effective connectivity characterizes the directional flow of information. One method of characterizing effective connectivity is Granger causality [20], which is methodologically straightforward but requires careful application and interpretation.
To date, no study has investigated the effective connectivity differences in stimulant dependence. This is important because understanding the direction of information flow in large-scale brain networks may further elucidate mechanisms of abstinence and explain previously reported changes. To improve substance dependence treatments, a better understanding of the connectivity characteristics associated with long term remission are needed and may help to predict successful abstinence, evaluate treatment efficacy, and develop novel treatments. This study investigated the effective connectivity and graph theory characteristics of large scale networks in the resting brain in long-term abstinent SDI compared to healthy controls to provide holistic, organ-level measures of brain connectivity and organization for comparisons between groups. We hypothesized that compared to healthy controls, long-term abstinent SDI will demonstrate altered effective connectivity between large-scale brain networks and increased directional information from executive control to interoception-, reward-, and habit-related networks.
Materials and Methods
Sample Population
Fifty substance dependent individuals (SDI) and 50 healthy controls matched in age and sex were prospectively recruited between October 2010 and June 2013. Demographic information is reported in Table 1. SDI were recruited from a residential treatment program at the University of Colorado Denver Addiction Research Treatment Services. Inclusion criteria for SDI were lifetime DSM-IV psychostimulant dependence (methamphetamine, cocaine, or amphetamine-class substances) and abstinence from all drugs of abuse for a minimum 60 days, verified through close supervision and random urine screens. Participants were permitted to have previously met dependence criteria for substances other than psychostimulants due to the high prevalence of polysubstance use in people dependent upon psychostimulants. Average abstinence duration was 12.8 ± 12.4 months. Healthy controls were recruited from the community and excluded if dependent on alcohol or other drugs of abuse except tobacco. Exclusion criteria for all participants included major depression within the last two months, psychosis, neurological illness, prior head trauma with loss of consciousness exceeding 15 minutes, prior neurosurgery, HIV, bipolar disorder, other major medical illness, inability to tolerate MRI, positive urine or saliva screen (AccuTestTM, AlcoScreenTM), and IQ < 80. All participants provided written informed consent approved by the Colorado Multiple Institutional Review Board.
Table 1. Demographic, drug use, and behavioral characteristics of the sample population.
SDI | Control | t-value | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Demographics | ||||
N | 50 (22F/28M) | 50 (25F/25M) | 0.869 | |
Age | 34.18±7.63 | 31.6±8.57 | 1.589 | 0.115 |
Education | 12.48±1.42 | 14.66±1.47 | -7.560 | <0.001 |
Abstinence (mo) | 12.8 ± 12.4 | |||
Drug Dependence | ||||
Stimulants | 50 | |||
Nicotine | 35 | 8 | ||
Alcohol | 35 | |||
Opioids | 16 | |||
Cannabis | 20 | |||
Other | 9 | |||
Drug Use Characteristics | ||||
Stimulant Use Onset Age (yrs) | 17.3 ± 4.7 | |||
Stimulant Use Duration (yrs) | 15.7 ± 7.4 | |||
Behavioral Metrics | ||||
BIS | 20.64±3.26 | 20.10±3.56 | 0.792 | 0.430 |
BAS | 76.74±6.54 | 69.40±9.27 | 4.575 | <0.001 |
Drive | 12.80±2.17 | 10.34±2.26 | 5.551 | <0.001 |
Fun Seeking | 13.16±1.85 | 11.38±2.18 | 4.414 | <0.001 |
Reward | 17.40±1.94 | 17.10±1.75 | 0.812 | 0.419 |
BIS-11 | 72.16±11.54 | 57.30±6.71 | 7.871 | <0.001 |
Motor | 27.30±4.72 | 22.78±3.02 | 5.707 | <0.001 |
Non-Planning | 27.08±4.96 | 20.48±3.33 | 7.809 | <0.001 |
Attentional | 17.78±3.81 | 14.04±3.14 | 5.361 | <0.001 |
PANAS-X | ||||
Positive Affect | 35.32±6.09 | 35.92±6.09 | -0.488 | 0.626 |
Negative Affect | 21.54±7.98 | 14.72±3.48 | 5.540 | <0.001 |
Acronyms: BAS, Behavioral activation scale; BIS, Behavioral inhibition scale; BIS-11, Barratt impulsiveness scale version 11; PANAS-X, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule–Expanded Form.
Structured Interviews and Questionnaires
Screening Assessment
All participants received structured interviews and behavioral measures. Drug dependence was assessed using the computerized Composite International Diagnostic Interview-Substance Abuse Module (CIDI-SAM) [21]. IQ was estimated with matrix and verbal reasoning Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence subtests (WASI, Psychological Corporation, 1999) and was recorded to exclude subjects with low scores (IQ < 80). The Diagnostic Interview Schedule version IV is a computerized structured interview used to screen for psychiatric disorders. Participants completed this interview to exclude those with a history of psychiatric disorders as described above. Substance dependence severity was operationalized as the number of total substance dependence and abuse symptoms, quantified by the Diagnostic Interview Schedule version IV [22,23].
Behavioral Inhibition Scale (BIS) / Behavioral Activation Scale (BAS)
The Behavioral Inhibition and Activation Scale is a 20-item self-reported questionnaire used to measure responsiveness of motivational systems [24,25]. Behavioral approach and inhibition were operationalized as the total Behavioral Activation and Inhibition Scales, respectively.
Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11)
This is a 30-item self-reported questionnaire used to measure impulsivity; participants rated whether phrases and words describing aspects of impulsivity were self-descriptive [26]. Impulsiveness was operationalized as the total Barratt score.
Positive and Negative Affect Scales
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule–Expanded Form (PANAS-X) quantifies a participant’s positive and negative affect using a series of 60 words and phrases that are rated on a scale of self-description [27]. Positive and negative affect were operationalized as the total PANAS-X score for positive and negative attributes.
MRI Examination and Image Analysis
MRI Acquisition
Brain MRI was performed using a 3T MR scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wisconsin) and standard quadrature head coil. Head motion was minimized using a VacFix head-conforming vacuum cushion (Par Scientific A/S, Odense, Denmark). Any subjects with ≥2 mm of head motion were excluded. High resolution T1-weighted SPGR-IR sequences (TR = 45ms, TE = 20ms, flip angle = 70°, 256 × 256 matrix, 240 × 240mm2 field-of-view (0.9 × 0.9mm2 in plane), 1.7mm slice thickness, and coronal plane acquisition) and resting-state functional scans (TR = 2000ms, TE = 30ms, flip angle = 30°, axial acquisition, 64 × 64 matrix, 3.4 mm × 3.4 mm in-plane voxel size, 3mm slice thickness, 1mm gap, 150 volumes) were acquired. During fMRI acquisition, participants were instructed to close their eyes, not think of anything in particular, and not fall asleep.
Image Preprocessing
Resting fMRI images were processed using the SPM8 toolbox in MATLAB. The first four volumes of each examination were excluded to avoid saturation effects (Fig 2). Standard pre-processing steps included slice timing correction, rigid realignment and motion correction (motion >1 voxel/TR was censored), spatial normalization, and de-noising. Motion parameters (three rotation and three translation parameters) for censorship were calculated for each time-point using corresponding SPM realignment pre-processing values. Anatomical volumes were segmented into gray matter, white matter, and CSF tissue maps, and the resulting binary masks were eroded (1 isotropic voxel) to mitigate partial volume effects. CSF and white matter time series were obtained using the mean signals from voxels based on eroded CSF and white matter SPM template masks. Mask erosion and time series extraction were performed using functions contained in the CONN toolbox [28]. After linear trends were removed, time series of the motion parameters, WM signal, and CSF signal were removed from the resting-state BOLD data using linear regression, and the resultant residual BOLD time series were band-pass filtered (0.008 Hz < f < 0.15 Hz) [29]. The resultant filtered time series were spatially smoothed with a 6mm full width at half maximum Gaussian kernel.
Networks-of-interest (NOI) definition and behavioral correlations
Group independent component analysis (ICA) was performed using the GIFT toolbox as previously reported in the literature [5,30–34] in order to define the networks-of-interest (NOI). For the purposes of this study, the term resting state networks (RSN) refers to the canonical spatial maps used to define the NOI. The term NOI refers to the independent components identified in our sample population and labeled by their corresponding RSN. The dimensionality of the data from each subject was first reduced to 100 components using principal component analysis. Subsequent group-level ICA yielded 34 components, the number of which was determined using the minimum description length (MDL) algorithm [35]. Fourteen canonical RSN templates (Table 2) were provided by Stanford's Functional Imaging in Neuropsychiatric Disorders (FIND) Laboratory [36]. At the group level, the 34 identified components were spatially correlated with the canonical RSN templates. Components with the highest spatial correlation to the canonical template were labeled with the corresponding standard RSN label. These labelled components formed the set of NOI for subsequent graph analysis. All components were visually inspected by a neuroradiology fellow (N.S.) and radiology resident (M.R.) independently to confirm accuracy with the canonical RSN templates. Concordance between inspectors was 100%. Subject-specific spatial maps and time courses were estimated using the GICA back-reconstruction function in GIFT.
Table 2. Canonical RSN included in the analysis and their corresponding symbol abbreviations.
Resting State Network | Symbol |
---|---|
Auditory Network | AN |
Anterior Salience Network | aSN |
Basal Ganglia Network | BGN |
Dorsal Default Mode Network | dDMN |
High Visual Network | HVN |
Left Executive Control Network | LECN |
Language Network | LN |
Precuneus Network | PCN |
Posterior Salience Network | pSN |
Primary Visual Network | PVN |
Right Executive Control Network | RECN |
Sensorimotor Network | SMN |
Ventral Default Mode Network | vDMN |
Visuospatial Network | VSN |
For each subject, the strength (or coherence) of each NOI was operationalized as the mean beta value across spatial dimensions for that component in the mixing matrix. These values were regressed against impulsivity, approach, inhibition, and negative affect. Regressions between the NOI strength and subjects’ behavioral metrics were used to interpret the neuroimaging findings within the context of measurable behavioral characteristics.
Effective Connectivity Analysis
For each individual, the time courses corresponding to the NOI were obtained from the back-reconstruction function in group ICA. These NOI signals were linear trend removed, normalized to zero mean and unit variance, and band-pass filtered at 0.008–0.15 Hz. The resultant NOI time courses were temporally concatenated across individuals into SDI and control groups [37,38]. Effective connectivity between all 14 NOI time courses in the SDI and control graphs were calculated using Granger causality (GC) analysis implemented in the Granger Causal Connectivity Analysis (GCCA) MATLAB toolbox [20]. Significance was estimated by comparing observed group difference to a randomized null hypothesis distribution, and the test statistic was determined by the percentile position of the observed difference (i.e., the proportion of randomizations with values greater than or equal to the observed value). To determine null hypothesis distributions, subjects’ group labels were randomized and GC connectivity differences estimated for each randomization permutation until the aggregate randomization distribution achieved statistical stability. Significance level was α = 0.05 using false discovery rate (FDR) q < 0.05 to correct for multiple comparisons.
Global Network Measures
To provide global network measures, graph theory measures were used to describe the topology of the graphs of NOI. The purpose was to provide holistic, organ-level measures of brain connectivity for comparisons between groups. These measures included total weighted network density, local efficiency (derived from clustering coefficients), and global efficiency (derived from path lengths). These measures’ derivations and their justification have been previously described in detail [39]. Computational details are provided in S1 Appendix.
Results
Sample Population Demographics
There were no significant differences in age (p = 0.12) or sex (p = 0.87) between groups (Table 1). Years of education (p<0.01) and IQ (p<0.01) differed between groups. Controls had higher IQ and more years of education than SDI. All SDI met DSM-IV dependence criteria for stimulants. Drug use characteristics are also summarized in Table 1. Eight controls met dependence criteria for tobacco. No controls met dependence criteria for drugs or alcohol.
Behavioral Metrics
Behavioral characteristics are summarized in Table 1. No group difference in BIS inhibition was observed (p = 0.43). A significant group difference in behavioral approach was observed, with SDI exhibiting higher total BAS scores than controls (p<0.001). Further analysis of BAS subscales showed that SDI had higher scores on “drive” (p<0.001) and “fun-seeking” (p<0.001), but not “reward-responsiveness” (p = 0.42). As expected, SDI reported higher impulsivity than controls (p<0.001) as well as significant differences in the motor, non-planning, and attentional subscales (p<0.001). No significant difference in positive affect was observed (p = 0.626); however, a large difference in negative affect was observed (p<0.001), with SDI demonstrating greater negative affect scores than controls.
Network Analysis
Directed Connectivity Analysis
SDI network density was significantly greater compared to controls (p<0.001, Fig 3). This measure reflects increased overall mean GC causal connectivity strength between all 14 NOI compared to controls. Specifically, GC analysis results show that among 182 possible between-network pairs (Fig 4), only three pairs differed significantly across group in the FDR corrected data (Fig 5). Compared to controls, SDI showed stronger effective connectivity from the RECN to the dDMN and from the dDMN to the BGN (Fig 6). In addition, SDI showed stronger effective connectivity from the SMN to the VSN. SDI showed greater bidirectional connectivity (reciprocal GC connections) whereas controls showed more unidirectional connectivity among the 14 network components.
Network-Behavioral Correlations
The strength of the RECN correlated negatively with impulsivity (p<0.001), behavioral approach (p<0.001), and negative affect across the population (p = 0.006) (Fig 7). In contrast, the strength of the dDMN correlated positively with impulsivity (p<0.001), but not behavioral approach (p = 0.034) or negative affect (p = 0.030) after correcting for multiple comparisons (Fig 7). No NOI correlated with positive affect or educational attainment in years.
Global Graph Measures
Group comparison results for local and global efficiency across the domain of cost functions are illustrated in Fig 8. Global efficiency was significantly higher in SDI than in controls (p<0.01), suggesting greater global integration. Local efficiency was higher in controls (p<0.05), suggesting greater local specialization. These findings in conjunction suggest reduced small-worldness in SDI compared to healthy controls.
Discussion
This study revealed greater effective connectivity network patterns in abstinent substance dependent individuals compared to healthy controls. Specifically, in drug users who have been abstinent for on average over one year, effective connectivity analysis revealed increased information flow from the RECN to dDMN and dDMN to BGN compared to controls. The areas of increased effective connectivity observed in our study correspond to regions involved in executive control (RECN), interoception (dDMN), reward (BGN), and habit (BGN). The mean strength of the RECN component correlated negatively with impulsivity, behavioral approach, and negative affect. In contrast, the mean strength of the dDMN component correlated positively with impulsivity and trended towards positive correlations with behavioral approach and negative affect. Given the prolonged abstinence of our SDI sample population, these findings are consistent with the hypothesis that successful long-term abstinence is associated with increased top-down cognitive control.
Increased Effective Connectivity from RECN to dDMN
The pattern of increased effective connectivity from RECN to dDMN is consistent with increased top-down executive control in long-term abstinence. Previous work has demonstrated both task-related and resting state hyperactivity within executive control and default mode cortices in abstinent stimulant dependence associated with heightened behavioral monitoring [17,40,41]; however, this is the first study to suggest that these neural signals follow a directional flow of information from RECN to dDMN. Connolly et al. [40] conducted a cognitive control task-based fMRI study of short- (2.4 ± 1.34 weeks) and long-term (69 ± 17.49 weeks) abstinent cocaine addicts. Abstinent cocaine users demonstrated increased activity in PFC, cingulate, and inferior frontal gyri compared to healthy controls. Moreover, short-term abstinent individuals showed right dorsolateral PFC (corresponding to RECN in our study) hyperactivity positively correlating with inhibitory control. Long-term abstinent individuals showed the same finding as well as anterior and mid cingulate (corresponding to part of the dDMN in our study) hyperactivity positively correlating with cognitive errors and heightened behavioral monitoring in abstinence. The present study showed that the RECN strength was negatively correlated with subjects’ impulsiveness, while dDMN strength was positively correlated with impulsiveness. Our results advance our understanding of neural network changes during substance use disorder remission: as abstinence progresses, cortices within the RECN and dDMN may become hyperactive to exert top-down executive control in a directed fashion; this neuroadaptive change may be associated with decreases in impulsivity and increases in inhibitory control.
However, the top-down cognitive control hypothesis is not straightforward because in addition to executive function and cognitive control, affect plays an important role. Albein-Urios et al. [42] showed that short-term abstinent (2.5 ± 5.5 months) cocaine dependent individuals had increased right dorsolateral PFC and bilateral temporoparietal cortex activation during negative emotion experiences without a concomitant increase in the subjective negative experience itself, suggesting an exaggerated neural response in these regions is required to produce normal levels of emotional salience. The regions reported closely resemble by visual comparison the RECN identified by our analysis. Albein-Urios et al. posited that these areas demonstrate increased sensitization toward negative emotions in SDI. If increased RECN top-down control is a durable feature of long-term abstinence, the literature thus far suggests that its manifestations in human behavior are complex and not reducible to a single neuropsychological construct. Our finding that RECN strength is negatively correlated to negative affect while dDMN strength trended towards positive correlation with negative affect provides further evidence that the top-down control model may involve affective components as well, possibly through reciprocal connectivity with limbic areas.
Right-sided lateralization of our ECN findings is not unexpected given the asymmetric functional specialization of cerebral hemispheres in healthy humans. Cocaine dependent patients exhibit reduced resting state interhemispheric connectivity compared to healthy controls in prefrontal and parietal cortices [43], suggesting increased lateralization of function. Connolly et al. [40] reported that hyperactivity in the inferior frontal gyrus correlated with inhibitory control was greater in the left hemisphere in short-term abstinent individuals and greater in the right hemisphere in long-term abstinent individuals. They hypothesized that a shift from left to right inferior frontal gyrus for inhibitory control may reflect a transition from short-term to long-term abstinence. Our results of increased RECN effective connectivity in long-term abstinent stimulant dependence are consistent with this hypothesis, although future longitudinal studies are required for substantiation.
Increased Effective Connectivity from dDMN to BGN
Although the DMN is incompletely understood, growing evidence demonstrates its roles in internally directed tasks such as spontaneous cognition [44], self-referential [45] and autobiographical [46] thought, and social understanding of others [47]. We demonstrated increased effective connectivity from the dDMN to BGN; however, this finding must be interpreted in the context of the structures within the NOI identified as BGN (Fig 5). This NOI included basal ganglia, thalamus, amygdala, hippocampus, hypothalamus, midbrain, and pons. Thus, BGN included several key regions of the bottom-up mesocorticolimbic circuit including the ventral tegmental area, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, and striatum.
Prior studies have demonstrated hypoactivity in stimulant users in the dDMN and in BGN as well as decreased connectivity between these networks. In active cocaine users, Tomasi et al. [48] showed that cocaine cues disengaged fMRI activity in the ventral striatum, hypothalamus, and DMN in proportion to density of striatal dopamine receptors by PET. DMN activation has been shown to predict performance errors, is diminished in active cocaine dependence, and the extent of altered error-preceding activation has been reported to correlate with years of cocaine use [49]. Gu et al. [15] used a seed-based fMRI paradigm in active cocaine users and found significantly decreased functional connectivity between multiple regions of the DMN and BGN. McHugh et al. [50] showed that individuals successfully abstinent 30 days after detoxification had stronger functional connectivity between the amygdala, ventromedial PFC, and anterior cingulate cortex compared to those who had relapsed. By visual comparison, these regions correspond to structures within the NOI identified as dDMN and BGN in our study. Connolly et al. [40] demonstrated increased anterior and mid cingulate activity in long-term abstinent compared to short-term abstinent individuals, activity which correlated with heightened behavioral monitoring. Together, these prior studies suggest that DMN activity may change during abstinence. Initial hypoactivation during early abstinence may transition to hyperactivation and increased connectivity with long-term abstinence. One interpretation is that findings of increased effective connectivity from dDMN to BGN in long-term abstinence may be a compensatory mechanism related to behavioral monitoring not seen in active users. However, longitudinal studies are needed to demonstrate this.
Increased Global and Decreased Local Integration
Our findings of increased bidirectional connectivity, increased global efficiency, and decreased local efficiency in long-term abstinent SDI compared to healthy controls suggests pathologically greater global integration and lower local integration in SDI; that is, a connectomic decrease in small-worldness. Similar findings in humans have only been reported using EEG data in 1–3 week abstinent methamphetamine dependent persons. Ahmadlou et al. [51] showed that these patients demonstrated a deviation from small-worldness and increased global hypersynchronization in the gamma frequency band, the EEG band most reactive to cognitive information processing. In contrast, active cocaine users demonstrated less global connectivity compared to healthy controls during a Stroop task; however, after adjusting for individual connectivity, cocaine dependent individuals showed greater intrinsic connectivity in the ventral striatum, putamen, inferior frontal gyrus, anterior insula, thalamus and substantia nigra [52].
Several animal studies provide important context for the interpretation of our findings. Schwarz et al. [53] used a pharmacological challenge design which revealed that rats under the acute effects of amphetamine compared to a saline vehicle exhibited less clustering (small-worldness) and increased connectedness within somatosensory, motor, cingulate, prefrontal, and insular cortices. In the rhesus monkey model, active cocaine self-administration was associated with decreased global functional connectivity that selectively affected top-down prefrontal circuits and control behavior while sparing limbic and striatal areas [54]. Interestingly, impaired connectivity between prefrontal and striatal areas during abstinence predicted cocaine intake when these monkeys were again provided access to cocaine (i.e., prediction of relapse), consistent with the connectivity pattern associated with relapse in humans as reported by Camchong et al. [18].
Together these findings suggest there is globally decreased connectivity in active users and short-term abstinent with a transition to globally increased connectivity in long-term abstinent users. These findings may improve clinical management if global connectivity patterns can be used to predict abstinence success or trajectory in humans. Future longitudinal studies comparing global connectivity in active, short-term, and long-term abstinent drug users must be performed to address this question. Another approach could involve correlating abstinence duration with global connectivity across individuals, an approach we could not implement due to the group-level nature of our statistical design.
Limitations
Controversy surrounding Granger Causality
While our study provides several important novel findings, it has limitations. Influences between specialized neural systems exist on a spectrum of temporal lag. Functional connectivity using temporal correlation reflect influences with causal latencies that are below the temporal resolution of the repetition time. These influences are not truly contemporaneous in vivo, but appear so by fMRI as a result of low temporal sampling and temporal blurring induced by the hemodynamic response function. Time-lag based measures such as Granger causality reflect slower influences with greater causal latencies that occur on the order of hundreds of milliseconds, which may provide greater power in predicting cause-effect relationships at the timescale of conscious thought [55].
Neural signals between two nodes may have significantly different physiologic functions depending upon the directionality. As a result, segregating neural influences according to their directionality is necessary in order to properly examine brain function. Methods of examining effective connectivity using fMRI data include structural equation modelling [56] and dynamic causal modelling [57]. These methods require a priori hypotheses describing the theoretical connectivity structure and are limited to models consisting of a small number of nodes. We used an alternative method, Granger causality, which is based on time-lag regressions and is more data-driven.
Granger causality is increasingly used in fMRI-based neuroscience [58–62] and has been previously applied specifically to independent component analysis as in our study [37,63–70]. However, criticisms of the application of Granger causality to fMRI data have included [71]: (1) lack of evidence that Granger causality in fMRI-level time series reflects causality in neuronal-level time series, (2) insufficient temporal sampling relative to the timescale of neuronal events, and (3) the possibility that spurious findings may result from systematic differences in hemodynamic response functions. Several recent developments have provided evidence that fMRI Granger causality reliably reflects neuronal causality [60,71–73]. Seth and colleagues [74] demonstrated that Granger causality is reliably invariant to inter-regional differences in the hemodynamic response function, including the time-to-peak. However, they reported significant effects of temporal resolution on their results. Wen and colleagues [60] demonstrated that fMRI-based Granger causality is a monotonic function of neural Granger causality. Importantly, they showed that this relationship can be reliably detected using conventional fMRI temporal resolution and noise levels as was used here. However, they cautioned that differences in the hemodynamic response could lead to spurious results.
The impact of hemodynamic response variability is currently debated. Schippers and colleagues [72] demonstrated that hemodynamic response variability was minimized by multisubject group inference. Statistically, this is intuitive because population averaging will augment systematic differences (e.g., true neuronal differences) while suppressing random or pseudorandom differences (e.g., hemodynamic response variability). Some authors speculate that HRF variability could be systematic [75], and indeed this is a confound that by design exists in the majority of between-group fMRI studies using independent samples [76–78]. Accordingly, we cannot exclude that systematic differences in the neurovascular response to neural activity between groups may have contributed to our findings.
Other Limitations
Network resolution was limited by the manner in which independent component analysis identifies temporally coherent signals across the brain. For example, the network component identified as BGN included several non-basal ganglia structures, such as the thalamus, amygdala, hippocampus, and midbrain. Additionally, concatenation across individuals precluded correlation of individual psychological measures to resting state network Granger causality; as such, correlations between the strength of each NOI and behavioral metrics were used to provide psychological context for the findings. Lastly, polysubstance use and low educational attainment among psychostimulant users may be viewed as potential confounds or representation of real world clinical features. There is significant literature describing the correlation between drug use and low educational attainment; it is debated whether low educational attainment is the cause or result of drug use disorders [79–81]. More recently, however, authors have reported that this correlation is due in part to shared genetic factors [82] while others report that it is due to shared environmental or non-genetic familial risk factors [83,84]. These studies suggest that low educational attainment is a behavioral component of the pathology of substance use disorders. With regard to polysubstance use among SDI, while this limitation prevents our findings from being attributed to a single drug, it strengthens our results by providing biological and ecological validity. Epidemiologic studies have demonstrated that psychostimulant dependence does not naturally occur in isolation; rather, most patients meet dependence criteria for other drugs of abuse [85,86]. Our sample population thus reflects the real-world, clinical population of patients with stimulant dependence.
Conclusion
Increased effective connectivity in long-term abstinent drug users may reflect improved cognitive control and behavioral monitoring (ECN) over self-referential thought (DMN), habit (BGN), and reward (BGN) processes in long-term abstinent drug users. Higher global and lower local efficiency across all networks in SDI compared to healthy controls may reflect connectivity changes associated with drug dependence or remission. Future, longitudinal studies are necessary to definitively characterize connectomic changes across the natural history of substance use disorders.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA) grants DA024104 (JT), DA 027748 (JT), and DA 041011 (MFR). The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Data Availability
Data are available through the Open Science Framework, Project: “Top-Down Network Effective Connectivity in Abstinent Substance Dependent Individuals”, Contributor: Michael Regner, hyperlink: https://osf.io/49akb/. Any questions may be directed to the corresponding author.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA) (https://www.drugabuse.gov/) DA024104 (JT), DA027748 (JT), and DA041011 (MFR). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Miller TR, Hendrie D. Substance abuse prevention dollars and cents: A cost-benefit analysis. US Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Substance Abuse Prevention; 2009.
- 2.Wise RA, Koob GF. The development and maintenance of drug addiction. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;39: 254–262. 10.1038/npp.2013.261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Perry RI, Krmpotich T, Thompson LL, Mikulich-Gilbertson SK, Banich MT, Tanabe J. Sex modulates approach systems and impulsivity in substance dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;133: 222–227. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.04.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Regner MF, Dalwani M, Yamamoto D, Perry RI, Sakai JT, Honce JM, et al. Sex Differences in Gray Matter Changes and Brain-Behavior Relationships in Patients with Stimulant Dependence. Radiology. 2015;277: 801–812. 10.1148/radiol.2015142541 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Krmpotich TD, Tregellas JR, Thompson LL, Banich MT, Klenk AM, Tanabe JL. Resting-state activity in the left executive control network is associated with behavioral approach and is increased in substance dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;129: 1–7. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.01.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bell RP, Garavan H, Foxe JJ. Neural correlates of craving and impulsivity in abstinent former cocaine users: Towards biomarkers of relapse risk. Neuropharmacology. 2014;85: 461–470. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wisner KM, Patzelt EH, Lim KO, MacDonald AW. An intrinsic connectivity network approach to insula-derived dysfunctions among cocaine users. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2013;39: 403–413. 10.3109/00952990.2013.848211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hyatt CJ, Assaf M, Muska CE, Rosen RI, Thomas AD, Johnson MR, et al. Reward-related dorsal striatal activity differences between former and current cocaine dependent individuals during an interactive competitive game. PloS One. 2012;7: e34917 10.1371/journal.pone.0034917 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Jasinska AJ, Stein EA, Kaiser J, Naumer MJ, Yalachkov Y. Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues in addiction: a survey of human neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;38: 1–16. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fox MD, Greicius M. Clinical applications of resting state functional connectivity. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4: 19 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106: 13040–13045. 10.1073/pnas.0905267106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Chen S, Ross TJ, Zhan W, Myers CS, Chuang K- S, Heishman SJ, et al. Group independent component analysis reveals consistent resting-state networks across multiple sessions. Brain Res. 2008;1239: 141–151. 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.08.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;35: 217–238. 10.1038/npp.2009.110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Sutherland MT, McHugh MJ, Pariyadath V, Stein EA. Resting state functional connectivity in addiction: Lessons learned and a road ahead. NeuroImage. 2012;62: 2281–2295. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gu H, Salmeron BJ, Ross TJ, Geng X, Zhan W, Stein EA, et al. Mesocorticolimbic circuits are impaired in chronic cocaine users as demonstrated by resting-state functional connectivity. NeuroImage. 2010;53: 593–601. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lu H, Zou Q, Chefer S, Ross TJ, Vaupel DB, Guillem K, et al. Abstinence from cocaine and sucrose self-administration reveals altered mesocorticolimbic circuit connectivity by resting state MRI. Brain Connect. 2014;4: 499–510. 10.1089/brain.2014.0264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wilcox CE, Teshiba TM, Merideth F, Ling J, Mayer AR. Enhanced cue reactivity and fronto-striatal functional connectivity in cocaine use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;115: 137–144. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Camchong J, Macdonald AW, Mueller BA, Nelson B, Specker S, Slaymaker V, et al. Changes in resting functional connectivity during abstinence in stimulant use disorder: a preliminary comparison of relapsers and abstainers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;139: 145–151. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.03.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Camchong J, Stenger VA, Fein G. Resting state synchrony in long-term abstinent alcoholics with versus without comorbid drug dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;131: 56–65. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.04.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Seth AK. A MATLAB toolbox for Granger causal connectivity analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2010;186: 262–273. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cottler LB, Robins LN, Helzer JE. The reliability of the CIDI-SAM: a comprehensive substance abuse interview. Br J Addict. 1989;84: 801–814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Robins L, Cottler L, Bucholz K, Compton W, North C, Rourke K. The diagnostic interview schedule, version IV, Washington University, St. Louis MO: 1995; [Google Scholar]
- 23.Gelhorn H, Hartman C, Sakai J, Stallings M, Young S, Rhee SH, et al. Toward DSM-V: an item response theory analysis of the diagnostic process for DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence in adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47: 1329–1339. 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318184ff2e [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1994;67: 319. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Campbell-Sills L, Liverant GI, Brown TA. Psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition/behavioral activation scales in a large sample of outpatients with anxiety and mood disorders. Psychol Assess. 2004;16: 244 10.1037/1040-3590.16.3.244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995;51: 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Crawford JR, Henry JD. The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol Br Psychol Soc. 2004;43: 245–265. 10.1348/0144665031752934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2: 125–141. 10.1089/brain.2012.0073 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Braun U, Plichta MM, Esslinger C, Sauer C, Haddad L, Grimm O, et al. Test-retest reliability of resting-state connectivity network characteristics using fMRI and graph theoretical measures. NeuroImage. 2012;59: 1404–1412. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Tregellas JR, Wylie KP, Rojas DC, Tanabe J, Martin J, Kronberg E, et al. Altered default network activity in obesity. Obes Silver Spring Md. 2011;19: 2316–2321. 10.1038/oby.2011.119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tanabe J, Nyberg E, Martin LF, Martin J, Cordes D, Kronberg E, et al. Nicotine effects on default mode network during resting state. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;216: 287–295. 10.1007/s00213-011-2221-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tregellas JR, Tanabe J, Rojas DC, Shatti S, Olincy A, Johnson L, et al. Effects of an alpha 7-nicotinic agonist on default network activity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69: 7–11. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gao W, Zhu H, Giovanello KS, Smith JK, Shen D, Gilmore JH, et al. Evidence on the emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106: 6790–6795. 10.1073/pnas.0811221106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sorg C, Riedl V, Mühlau M, Calhoun VD, Eichele T, Läer L, et al. Selective changes of resting-state networks in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104: 18760–18765. 10.1073/pnas.0708803104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Li Y-O, Adali T, Calhoun VD. Estimating the number of independent components for functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28: 1251–1266. 10.1002/hbm.20359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Shirer WR, Ryali S, Rykhlevskaia E, Menon V, Greicius MD. Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cereb Cortex N Y N 1991. 2012;22: 158–165. 10.1093/cercor/bhr099 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ding X, Lee S-W. Changes of functional and effective connectivity in smoking replenishment on deprived heavy smokers: a resting-state FMRI study. PloS One. 2013;8: e59331 10.1371/journal.pone.0059331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Deshpande G, Sathian K, Hu X. Assessing and Compensating for Zero-Lag Correlation Effects in Time-Lagged Granger Causality Analysis of fMRI. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2010;57: 1446–1456. 10.1109/TBME.2009.2037808 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2010;52: 1059–1069. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Connolly CG, Foxe JJ, Nierenberg J, Shpaner M, Garavan H. The neurobiology of cognitive control in successful cocaine abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;121: 45–53. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mayer AR, Wilcox CE, Teshiba TM, Ling JM, Yang Z. Hyperactivation of the cognitive control network in cocaine use disorders during a multisensory Stroop task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;133: 235–241. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.04.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Albein-Urios N, Verdejo-Román J, Asensio S, Soriano-Mas C, Martínez-González JM, Verdejo-García A. Re-appraisal of negative emotions in cocaine dependence: dysfunctional corticolimbic activation and connectivity. Addict Biol. 2014;19: 415–426. 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00497.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Meunier D, Ersche KD, Craig KJ, Fornito A, Merlo-Pich E, Fineberg NA, et al. Brain functional connectivity in stimulant drug dependence and obsessive-compulsive disorder. NeuroImage. 2012;59: 1461–1468. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mantini D, Vanduffel W. Emerging roles of the brain’s default network. Neurosci Rev J Bringing Neurobiol Neurol Psychiatry. 2013;19: 76–87. 10.1177/1073858412446202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Vessel EA, Starr GG, Rubin N. Art reaches within: aesthetic experience, the self and the default mode network. Front Neurosci. 2013;7: 258 10.3389/fnins.2013.00258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124: 1–38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Li W, Mai X, Liu C. The default mode network and social understanding of others: what do brain connectivity studies tell us. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8: 74 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tomasi D, Wang G-J, Wang R, Caparelli EC, Logan J, Volkow ND. Overlapping patterns of brain activation to food and cocaine cues in cocaine abusers: Association to striatal D2/D3 receptors. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36: 120–136. 10.1002/hbm.22617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bednarski SR, Zhang S, Hong K-I, Sinha R, Rounsaville BJ, Li CR. Deficits in default mode network activity preceding error in cocaine dependent individuals. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;119: e51–57. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.05.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.McHugh MJ, Demers CH, Salmeron BJ, Devous MD, Stein EA, Adinoff B. Cortico-amygdala coupling as a marker of early relapse risk in cocaine-addicted individuals. Front Psychiatry. 2014;5: 16 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ahmadlou M, Ahmadi K, Rezazade M, Azad-Marzabadi E. Global organization of functional brain connectivity in methamphetamine abusers. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124: 1122–1131. 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mitchell MR, Balodis IM, Devito EE, Lacadie CM, Yeston J, Scheinost D, et al. A preliminary investigation of Stroop-related intrinsic connectivity in cocaine dependence: associations with treatment outcomes. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2013;39: 392–402. 10.3109/00952990.2013.841711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Schwarz AJ, Gozzi A, Chessa A, Bifone A. Voxel scale complex networks of functional connectivity in the rat brain: neurochemical state dependence of global and local topological properties. Comput Math Methods Med. 2012;2012: 615709 10.1155/2012/615709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Murnane KS, Gopinath KS, Maltbie E, Daunais JB, Telesford QK, Howell LL. Functional connectivity in frontal-striatal brain networks and cocaine self-administration in female rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2014; 10.1007/s00213-014-3709-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tononi G, Boly M, Massimini M, Koch C. Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17: 450–461. 10.1038/nrn.2016.44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Büchel C, Friston KJ. Modulation of connectivity in visual pathways by attention: cortical interactions evaluated with structural equation modelling and fMRI. Cereb Cortex N Y N 1991. 1997;7: 768–778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage. 2003;19: 1273–1302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Feng C, Deshpande G, Liu C, Gu R, Luo Y-J, Krueger F. Diffusion of responsibility attenuates altruistic punishment: A functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015; 10.1002/hbm.23057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Cohen Kadosh K, Luo Q, de Burca C, Sokunbi MO, Feng J, Linden DEJ, et al. Using real-time fMRI to influence effective connectivity in the developing emotion regulation network. NeuroImage. 2015;125: 616–626. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wen X, Rangarajan G, Ding M. Is Granger causality a viable technique for analyzing fMRI data? PloS One. 2013;8: e67428 10.1371/journal.pone.0067428 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Chiong W, Wilson SM, D’Esposito M, Kayser AS, Grossman SN, Poorzand P, et al. The salience network causally influences default mode network activity during moral reasoning. Brain J Neurol. 2013;136: 1929–1941. 10.1093/brain/awt066 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Zhang Y, Li Q, Wen X, Cai W, Li G, Tian J, et al. Granger causality reveals a dominant role of memory circuit in chronic opioid dependence. Addict Biol. 2016; 10.1111/adb.12390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Zhong Y, Huang L, Cai S, Zhang Y, von Deneen KM, Ren A, et al. Altered effective connectivity patterns of the default mode network in Alzheimer’s disease: an fMRI study. Neurosci Lett. 2014;578: 171–175. 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.06.043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Liu Z, Zhang Y, Bai L, Yan H, Dai R, Zhong C, et al. Investigation of the effective connectivity of resting state networks in Alzheimer’s disease: a functional MRI study combining independent components analysis and multivariate Granger causality analysis. NMR Biomed. 2012;25: 1311–1320. 10.1002/nbm.2803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Liu Z, Bai L, Dai R, Zhong C, Wang H, You Y, et al. Exploring the effective connectivity of resting state networks in mild cognitive impairment: an fMRI study combining ICA and multivariate Granger causality analysis. Conf Proc Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc Annu Conf. 2012;2012: 5454–5457. 10.1109/EMBC.2012.6347228 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Zhong C, Bai L, Dai R, Xue T, Wang H, Feng Y, et al. Modulatory effects of acupuncture on resting-state networks: a functional MRI study combining independent component analysis and multivariate Granger causality analysis. J Magn Reson Imaging JMRI. 2012;35: 572–581. 10.1002/jmri.22887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Stevens MC, Pearlson GD, Calhoun VD. Changes in the interaction of resting-state neural networks from adolescence to adulthood. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30: 2356–2366. 10.1002/hbm.20673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Londei A, D’Ausilio A, Basso D, Sestieri C, Del Gratta C, Romani GL, et al. Brain network for passive word listening as evaluated with ICA and Granger causality. Brain Res Bull. 2007;72: 284–292. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Demirci O, Stevens MC, Andreasen NC, Michael A, Liu J, White T, et al. Investigation of relationships between fMRI brain networks in the spectral domain using ICA and Granger causality reveals distinct differences between schizophrenia patients and healthy controls. NeuroImage. 2009;46: 419–431. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Diez I, Erramuzpe A, Escudero I, Mateos B, Cabrera A, Marinazzo D, et al. Information Flow Between Resting-State Networks. Brain Connect. 2015; 10.1089/brain.2014.0337 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Deshpande G, Hu X. Investigating effective brain connectivity from fMRI data: past findings and current issues with reference to Granger causality analysis. Brain Connect. 2012;2: 235–245. 10.1089/brain.2012.0091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Schippers MB, Renken R, Keysers C. The effect of intra- and inter-subject variability of hemodynamic responses on group level Granger causality analyses. NeuroImage. 2011;57: 22–36. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Deshpande G, Sathian K, Hu X. Effect of hemodynamic variability on Granger causality analysis of fMRI. NeuroImage. 2010;52: 884–896. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Seth AK, Chorley P, Barnett LC. Granger causality analysis of fMRI BOLD signals is invariant to hemodynamic convolution but not downsampling. NeuroImage. 2013;65: 540–555. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Smith SM, Bandettini PA, Miller KL, Behrens TEJ, Friston KJ, David O, et al. The danger of systematic bias in group-level FMRI-lag-based causality estimation. NeuroImage. 2012;59: 1228–1229. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Hillman EMC. Coupling mechanism and significance of the BOLD signal: a status report. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2014;37: 161–181. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.D’Esposito M, Deouell LY, Gazzaley A. Alterations in the BOLD fMRI signal with ageing and disease: a challenge for neuroimaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4: 863–872. 10.1038/nrn1246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Murphy K, Birn RM, Bandettini PA. Resting-state fMRI confounds and cleanup. NeuroImage. 2013;80: 349–359. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Beautrais AL. Cannabis and educational achievement. Addict Abingdon Engl. 2003;98: 1681–1692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Yamada T, Kendix M, Yamada T. The impact of alcohol consumption and marijuana use on high school graduation. Health Econ. 1996;5: 77–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Swaim RC, Beauvais F, Chavez EL, Oetting ER. The effect of school dropout rates on estimates of adolescent substance use among three racial/ethnic groups. Am J Public Health. 1997;87: 51–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Bergen SE, Gardner CO, Aggen SH, Kendler KS. Socioeconomic status and social support following illicit drug use: causal pathways or common liability? Twin Res Hum Genet Off J Int Soc Twin Stud. 2008;11: 266–274. 10.1375/twin.11.3.266 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Verweij KJH, Huizink AC, Agrawal A, Martin NG, Lynskey MT. Is the relationship between early-onset cannabis use and educational attainment causal or due to common liability? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;133: 580–586. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.07.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Grant JD, Scherrer JF, Lynskey MT, Agrawal A, Duncan AE, Haber JR, et al. Associations of alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, and drug use/dependence with educational attainment: evidence from cotwin-control analyses. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36: 1412–1420. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01752.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Sara G, Burgess P, Harris M, Malhi GS, Whiteford H, Hall W. Stimulant use disorders: characteristics and comorbidity in an Australian population sample. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2012;46: 1173–1181. 10.1177/0004867412461057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Stinson FS, Grant BF, Dawson DA, Ruan WJ, Huang B, Saha T. Comorbidity between DSM-IV alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;80: 105–116. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data are available through the Open Science Framework, Project: “Top-Down Network Effective Connectivity in Abstinent Substance Dependent Individuals”, Contributor: Michael Regner, hyperlink: https://osf.io/49akb/. Any questions may be directed to the corresponding author.