Abstract
The hypothesis that people respond to reminders of mortality with closed-minded, ethnocentric attitudes has received extensive empirical support, largely from research in the Terror Management Theory (TMT) tradition. However, the basic motivational and neural processes that underlie this effect remain largely hypothetical. According to recent neuropsychological theorizing, mortality salience (MS) effects on cultural closed-mindedness may be mediated by activity in the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), which leads to passive avoidance and decreased approach motivation. This should be especially true for people motivated to avoid unfamiliar and potentially threatening stimuli as reflected in a high need for closure (NFC). In two studies involving moderated mediation analyses, people high on trait NFC responded to MS with increased BIS activity (as indicated by EEG and the line bisection task), which is characteristic of inhibited approach motivation. BIS activity, in turn, predicted a reluctance to explore foreign cultures (Study 1) and generalized ethnocentric attitudes (Study 2). In a third study, inhibition was induced directly and caused an increase in ethnocentrism for people high on NFC. Moreover, the effect of the inhibition manipulation × NFC interaction on ethnocentrism was explained by increases in BIS-related affect (i.e., anxious inhibition) at high NFC. To our knowledge, this research is the first to establish an empirical link between very basic, neurally-instantiated inhibitory processes and rather complex, higher-order manifestations of intergroup negativity in response to MS. Our findings contribute to a fuller understanding of the cultural worldview defense phenomenon by illuminating the motivational underpinnings of cultural closed-mindedness in the wake of existential threat.
Keywords: mortality salience, behavioral inhibition system, inhibition, worldview defense, approach-avoidance motivation, ethnocentrism
We encapsulate ourselves to avoid death. And life escapes us while we huddle within the defended fortress (Becker, 1997, p. XIII).
Introduction
According to cultural anthropologist Ernest Becker, humans are uniquely preoccupied as “terrified, death-avoiding animal[s]” (Becker, 1997, p. 99). Inspired by Becker's writings, terror management theory (TMT) proposes that humans avoid the threat of death on a symbolic level by identifying with something that outlasts their individual life, such as their nation or culture (Pyszczynski et al., 1997). In support of this proposition, numerous TMT studies have demonstrated that people respond to mortality salience (MS) with increased adherence to their cultural worldviews, which often manifests in avoidance and derogation of culturally different people, customs, and ideologies (Burke et al., 2010). Hence, this research reveals a fascinating irony inherent in human efforts at self-preservation: in order to manage death-related anxiety, people develop an aversion to the cultural “other,” thereby curtailing their opportunities for psychologically enriching experiences. In other words, an unfortunate consequence of “huddling in the defended fortress” is that people avoid sampling opportunities provided by getting to know other cultures and their members.
Although it is well-documented that MS can drive cultural closed-mindedness, little is known about the basic neural and motivational processes that underlie this effect. According to the process model of threat and defense (Jonas et al., 2014), MS-induced aversion to cultural otherness may be mediated by a very basic motivation to avoid punishment. In the present research, we test this assumption using neurobehavioral measures of the activity of the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) as measured by the line bisection task (LBT; Study 1) and frontal alpha asymmetry as measured by EEG (Study 2). We also directly manipulate the proposed inhibitory motivational mediator. Specifically, we investigate whether (a) MS activates inhibitory motivational processes, (b) inhibition mediates MS effects on cultural closed-mindedness, and (c) these effects are particularly strong for persons with a dispositional inclination to avoid unfamiliar and potentially threatening stimuli.
Terror management and avoidance motivation
According to TMT, the awareness of one's inevitable demise entails the potential for paralyzing terror, which can be managed by defensive processes that are oriented toward symbolic self-preservation (Pyszczynski et al., 1997). Symbolic self-preservation is attained by identifying with entities that transcend one's individual death, such as cultural ingroups. Accordingly, there is ample evidence that MS increases cultural closed-mindedness in the form of more negative (positive) evaluations of the culturally unfamiliar (familiar; Burke et al., 2010).
Terror management theorists have generally assumed that these defenses are products of a basic motivation to avoid threatening cognitions (Pyszczynski et al., 1997, 2000, 2012). According to TMT, mortality threat is initially avoided by cognitive means, such as death-thought suppression or vulnerability denial (i.e., proximal defense; Pyszczynski et al., 1999). Second, after this active cognitive avoidance has subsided, subtle mechanisms of threat-avoidance are utilized to maintain defenses on a symbolic level (i.e., distal defense; Pyszczynski et al., 1999). These distal defenses often manifest in various forms of ethnocentric intergroup bias (e.g., avoidance and derogation of foreign people and worldviews; Burke et al., 2010). In other words, defensive responses to MS are assumed to be mediated by the motivation to avoid experiencing terror in the face of inescapable mortality (Greenberg et al., 2003). TMT argues that “when confronted with reminders of their mortality, people avoid the subjective experience of distress by increasing their commitment to the cultural worldview” (Greenberg et al., 1995b, p. 431). In sum, TMT proposes that threat leads people to shy away from cultural outgroups and cling to cultural ingroups in order to avoid the distress inherent in the existential insecurities aroused by reminders of the finitude of life.
The general process model of threat and defense
Recent neuropsychological theorizing suggests that MS-induced discomfort with cultural otherness is rooted in a basic motivation to avoid negative/punishing outcomes (Jonas et al., 2014). Specifically, the process model of threat and defense (Jonas et al., 2014) states that MS activates the BIS, an avoidance motivational system that responds to distant or anticipated threat (such as one's eventual demise), which inhibits ongoing goal approach (McNaughton and Corr, 2004). The inhibition of goal approach functions to reorient the individual's attention so that threat can be resolved and viable goal pursuit resumed (cf. McGregor et al., 2009). This dynamic is accompanied by several attentional, affective, and motivational processes, including hypervigilance, anxious arousal, and the motivation to avoid threat by inhibiting behaviors that might put the individual at risk (passive avoidance; Gray and McNaughton, 2000; McNaughton and Corr, 2004; Corr et al., 2013a)1.
Importantly, upon detecting stimuli signaling the potential for conflict or punishment, the BIS further biases negativity, which entails a tendency to avoid everything that is unfamiliar and potentially threatening (McNaughton and Corr, 2004, 2014). Accordingly, passive threat-avoidance strategies include the inhibition of exploratory approach behavior in order to maintain distance from potentially threatening stimuli (e.g., here, potentially hostile outgroup members), especially in risky or unfamiliar contexts. From an evolutionary perspective, members of cultural outgroups represent unfamiliar and potentially threatening stimuli, since hostile strangers may have been among the most serious ancestral threats to survival (McEachron and Baer, 1982; see also Kenrick et al., 2010). Accordingly, we propose that reminders of mortality activate a basic threat-avoidance system—the BIS—that subsequently inhibits the motivation to engage with cultural outgroups. This proposed process is similar to the TMT account of MS sequelae, but it differs in one key way: people's defensive responses to MS are not merely anticipatory of a negative motivational-emotional state (in TMT, the potential for anxiety; Pyszczynski et al., 1999), but instead these defenses are driven by BIS-mediated processes, such as motivational inhibition and felt anxious uncertainty, that are aroused immediately following the contemplation of one's death.
Although increased ethnocentric thinking has been frequently found following MS (Burke et al., 2010), the mediating processes proposed by the general process model (Jonas et al., 2014) are to date largely hypothetical. Advancing on this empirical gap from the general process model is all the more important given what is arguably equivocal evidence for TMT's main cognitive mediator candidate—death-thought accessibility (Das et al., 2009; Golec de Zavala et al., 2012; Trafimow and Hughes, 2012; Agroskin and Jonas, 2013; Hart, 2014). This failure has spawned a lively debate in the existential threat literature about whether MS-induced ethnocentrism might be driven by specific needs to restore meaning (Proulx and Heine, 2010), certainty (van den Bos et al., 2005), security (Hart et al., 2005), a sense of coalition (Navarrete et al., 2004), or control (Fritsche et al., 2008). However, it has also been proposed that MS-induced defensiveness may be driven by rather basic processes such as approach motivational (McGregor et al., 2013) or unconsciously vigilant (Holbrook et al., 2011) responses to any kind of threat. By contrast, Jonas et al. (2014) have suggested that MS effects on ethnocentric cognitions and behaviors are attributable to BIS-generated passive avoidance of cultural novelty. The present research tests this novel notion.
Individual differences in MS effects on cultural closed-mindedness
TMT research has demonstrated that individual differences in traits related to motivated avoidance of the unfamiliar are useful predictors of terror management processes. For instance, persons with high need for closure (NFC; Webster and Kruglanski, 1994), who strive for simple, unambiguous, and predictable environments, are accordingly reluctant to explore novel or ambiguous stimuli under threat, such as unfamiliar places (Vess et al., 2009)2. They also tend to adopt ethnocentric or xenophobic attitudes, suggesting that their discomfort with ambiguity and unfamiliarity biases their evaluations of cultural outgroups (Shah et al., 1998; Agroskin and Jonas, 2010, 2013). Accordingly, high NFC has been found to amplify MS effects on derogation of culturally different beliefs, such as differing religious worldviews (Juhl and Routledge, 2010). On a more general level, the unease with unfamiliar, unpredictable, and ambiguous stimuli following MS exhibited by people with high trait NFC can also manifest in heightened stereotypic thinking, as well as antipathy toward behaviorally inconsistent persons and even modern art (Schimel et al., 1999; Landau et al., 2004, 2006). Low NFC, conversely, is related to openness to novelty, uncertainty, and ambiguity, thus promoting increased interest in exploring unfamiliar stimuli following MS (Vess et al., 2009). In sum, high NFC appears to represent a disposition to avoidance-related cognition and behaviors, especially under threat.
This avoidance-motivational interpretation of NFC is bolstered by the fact that chronic BIS sensitivity and NFC are positively related (Corr et al., 2013b). This again suggests that high NFC may be indicative of the BIS-mediated negativity bias toward exaggerated threat perceptions and subsequent avoidance behaviors (McNaughton and Corr, 2004). The reluctance to engage with culturally-different stimuli following MS exhibited by high NFC people may reflect BIS-mediated inhibition of approach motivation under threat. On a more general level, Jonas et al. (2014) have proposed that BIS-related traits intensify and prolong BIS activity after threat. This is in line with neuroscientifically-informed theorizing regarding the hierarchical interplay between trait and state avoidance motivation (Elliot, 2006; Spielberg et al., 2013). In sum, people with high trait NFC are likely to show increased levels of BIS-mediated passive avoidance following MS, which may explain why NFC has been linked to many avoidance-related outcomes in previous TMT research.
The role of BIS activity in mortality salience effects
Although the hypothesis that MS effects on ethnocentric thinking are mediated by BIS activity has not yet been explicitly tested yet, there is some evidence suggesting an association between MS and BIS-related processes. For instance, people prefer familiar over unfamiliar products after experiencing death anxiety, suggesting a basic tendency toward novelty-avoidance (Huang and Wyer, 2015). MS also causes people to avoid stimuli that remind them of their embodied, temporal existence (Goldenberg et al., 2001; Cox et al., 2007), which suggests that MS inclines an avoidance of information that would further highlight a threat. Finally, people are even reluctant to look into mirrors and engage in self-focused writing under MS, possibly because heightened self-awareness exacerbates distressing vulnerability concerns after contemplating one's own death (Arndt et al., 1998).
In line with the process model of threat and defense (Jonas et al., 2014), these avoidance reactions are particularly likely to occur for individuals with anxious personalities. For example, MS has been found to aggravate anxious and avoidant behaviors among people especially prone to phobic or compulsive behaviors (Strachan et al., 2007), such as people high on BIS and trait anxiety (Smits and Boeck, 2006; Bijttebier et al., 2009; Erdle and Rushton, 2010). Furthermore, stimuli related to physicality and creatureliness are particularly problematic for persons high in neuroticism after MS (Goldenberg et al., 2000, 2006a,b, 2008). Finally, research from diverse labs demonstrates novelty-avoidant, risk-averse behaviors under conditions of threat and anxiety (e.g., Maner and Schmidt, 2006; Maner et al., 2007; Mortensen et al., 2010; Litt et al., 2011; Clark et al., 2012; see Kenrick et al., 2010 for review), and these reactions appear to be especially likely for people high on BIS-related traits (Landau and Greenberg, 2006; Cavallo et al., 2009; Routledge et al., 2010). Taken together, these results are consistent with the notion that BIS-sensitive people are particularly prone to passive-avoidant behaviors in the wake of threat (Jonas et al., 2014).
Measuring BIS activity
In neural terms, BIS activity has been linked to relative right-hemispheric brain activity, as reflected in prefrontal EEG alpha asymmetry. Specifically, dispositional BIS sensitivity is associated with stronger relative right-frontal brain activation, whereas BAS sensitivity is robustly related to greater left-frontal activation (Sutton and Davidson, 1997; Shackman et al., 2009). There is also evidence linking frontal asymmetric activity to prevention/promotion regulatory focus, which is conceptually related to BIS/BAS activity, respectively (Amodio et al., 2004). Importantly, relative frontal EEG activity is computed using difference scores between right- and left-frontal electrodes, implying that increased BIS-related right-frontal asymmetry is functionally equivalent to reduced BAS-related left-frontal asymmetry and vice-versa (e.g., Sutton and Davidson, 1997). This is also consistent with an inverse interrelationship between the activity of both hemispheres (e.g., Amodio et al., 2004) and the joint subsystems hypothesis of BIS/BAS activity in reinforcement sensitivity theory (Corr, 2004), which states that the two systems mutually inhibit each other. Thus, BIS-mediated inhibition of approach motivation is equivalently reflected in reduced left-frontal asymmetry as well as increased right-frontal asymmetry in the context of EEG asymmetry scores (see also Coan and Allen, 2004).
Moreover, right-frontal EEG asymmetry has been found to contribute to numerous neural and behavioral indicators of anxious or inhibitory responding. For example, relative right-frontal activity is positively associated with state anxiety (Davidson et al., 2000; Harmon-Jones et al., 2009) and hypervigilance to threat (Pérez-Edgar et al., 2013; Grimshaw et al., 2014). It also predicts the amplitude of the error-related negativity (Nash et al., 2012), which is linked to BIS sensitivity (Boksem et al., 2006, 2008). Importantly, it has also been demonstrated that right-frontal asymmetry is specifically related to the anxious inhibitory state of passive avoidance, whereas left-frontal asymmetry has been linked to the fearful state of active avoidance (Wacker et al., 2008; see also Perkins et al., 2007 for behavioral evidence on the fear/anxiety distinction). Overall, there is a substantial body of evidence to support the notion that right-frontal EEG asymmetry is associated with numerous neural and behavioral indicators of anxious or inhibitory responding (for more evidence, see Thibodeau et al., 2006; Wacker et al., 2010; McNaughton et al., 2013). Convergent evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has also demonstrated a link between right-frontal brain activity and anxiety (e.g., Dalton et al., 2005; Engels et al., 2010) and inhibition (see Aron et al., 2004 for review).
Nash et al. (2010) demonstrated that BIS/BAS activity can also be investigated using the line bisection task (LBT) as a perceptual measure of relative prefrontal hemisphericity. The LBT gauges BIS/BAS activity (or left-frontal asymmetry) by determining the extent to which people's perceptions of the midpoints of horizontal lines are biased to the right visual field, mirroring neural activity in the contralateral hemisphere (Nash et al., 2010). Accordingly, reduced rightward line bisection bias (i.e., reduced left or increased right hemisphericity) has been associated with BIS-related phenomena, such as anxious-avoidant arousal (Friedman and Förster, 2005), feelings of passivity/inhibition, and powerlessness (Drake and Myers, 2006; Wilkinson et al., 2010), as well as reduced risk taking, optimism, and goal pursuit (Drake and Ulrich, 1992; Nash et al., 2011). Notably, line bisection bias is specifically related to frontal (F7/F8) but not central, parietal, temporal, or occipital asymmetry, suggesting that the LBT represents a neurobehavioral marker specific to frontal alpha asymmetry (Nash et al., 2010). In sum, the LBT has often been used to measure motivational tendencies (see Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005 for a review of motivationally-relevant perceptual asymmetries), and has been validated as a neurobehavioral measure of asymmetric frontal brain activation (Nash et al., 2010).
The present research
In the present research, we examined the hypotheses that (a) MS causes BIS activity, evidenced by increased by greater right-hemispheric frontal asymmetry, (b) BIS activity mediates MS effects on cultural closed-mindedness, and (c) these effects are pronounced among persons with high NFC. Stated differently, we tested the moderated mediational hypothesis that MS effects on cultural closed-mindedness are mediated by BIS activity and are particularly strong for persons with high NFC. These hypotheses were examined by inducing MS (vs. a control topic), measuring NFC, measuring frontal brain asymmetry via the LBT (Study 1) and EEG (Study 2), and measuring cultural closed-mindedness in the form of reluctance to engage with cultural novelty (Study 1) and general ethnocentrism (Study 2). Moreover, in order to substantiate our claim that inhibition is the mediator, we directly manipulated the proposed mediator—inhibition—in Study 3 to test whether inhibition evokes ethnocentric thinking for high NFC persons (cf. Spencer et al., 2005).
Study 1
Study 1 was designed to test whether (a) persons with high NFC respond with increased BIS activity (as assessed by LBT-assessed right-frontal activity) to MS, and (b) their increased BIS activity mediates their increased unwillingness to explore foreign cultures.
Method
Participants and design
Seventy-seven students at the University of Salzburg participated in this paper-pencil study, which took place in class, prior to a lecture3. Three participants failed to follow instructions and were dropped prior to analyses, leaving a final sample of 74 participants (54 females and 20 males). Their mean age was 22.0 years (SD = 1.8; range: 19–31). Participant nationalities were 54 German, 18 Austrian, and 2 other. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions, MS (N = 35) or TV salience (N = 39), in a between-subjects design. The study was described as an investigation of personality and visual perception. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Salzburg. All participants signed informed consent, and could withdraw participation at any point, although no participant made use of this option.
Procedure and materials
Need for closure
Following instructions and the baseline LBT measure (described in more detail below), the NFC scale was presented. We measured NFC with three items that were taken from the German version of the NFC scale (Webster and Kruglanski, 1994; Schlink and Walther, 2007). The items were selected a priori based on two criteria: First, representativeness of the whole NFC scale, that is, corrected item-total correlations presented by Schlink and Walther (2007). Second, the items that best capture the willingness to approach/avoid novel and unpredictable stimuli should be of particular relevance (Green and Campbell, 2000), contrary to decisiveness, which is also encompassed by the NFC scale (see also Neuberg et al., 1997 for a critique of the full NFC scale's conceptual imprecision). One item was dropped prior to analyses because its corrected item-total correlation suggested it was unrelated with the other two items in the current sample (rit = 0.00). The two remaining items were: “I prefer familiar things over unfamiliar and unpredictable ones” and “I don't like unpredictable situations” (interrelation: r = 0.53, p < 0.001). A pilot study revealed this short NFC scale to correlate strongly with the conceptually related personal need for structure scale by Neuberg and Newsom (1993), r = 0.72, p < 0.001, N = 217. Responses were given on a 1 (almost never) to 6 (almost always) scale, and averaged to create a single composite score (M = 3.93, SD = 0.85).
Manipulation
After a few further personality items, included to bolster the cover story, we presented the classic MS manipulation, asking participants two open-ended questions about what will happen to them physically when they die and after they are dead, and which emotions are caused by thinking about their own death. Participants in the control condition were asked parallel questions about watching TV.
Line bisection task (inhibition of approach motivation)
Following a free-thought delay of a few minutes, the LBT was presented for a second time (the first LBT was presented right at the beginning of the study to obtain a baseline LBT measure). In each measure, participants were instructed to mark the perceived center point of eight horizontal lines that ranged from 4 to 6 (102 to 152 mm) in length with their centers offset from each other (see also Drake and Myers, 2006, who used the same LBT). Estimation errors to the left reflect an overnoticing of the left visual field characteristic of relative right cerebral hemisphericity (Jewell and McCourt, 2000). Thus, we computed the index of relative right cerebral hemisphericity by subtracting each participant's number of right-of-center ticks from left-of-center ticks (see McGregor et al., 2010, Study 1). In line with prior research, both LBT measures were correlated, r = 0.67, p < 0.001 (McGregor et al., 2010), and participants exhibited a leftward bisection bias on average (LBT 1: M = 1.92, SD = 4.12; LBT 2: M = 1.10, SD = 3.80; Jewell and McCourt, 2000).
Avoidance of cultural novelty
Passive avoidance of cultural novelty was measured with eight researcher-generated items designed to measure interest in exploring culturally foreign stimuli (e.g., people, information). Based on the conceptual view that exploration constitutes approach behavior toward novel stimuli (Green and Campbell, 2000), and evidence implicating BIS activity in reduced exploratory approach behaviors (Green and Campbell, 2000; Elliot and Reis, 2003; Kashdan et al., 2004), we recoded our cultural exploration items to obtain a measure for passive avoidance of cultural novelty. Example items include “I would like to get to know people from foreign cultures” and “I am curious to learn how people live in foreign cultures4.” Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which the items describe them at the present moment. Responses were given on a 1 (very little) to 5 (very much) scale, and averaged to create a single composite score (8 items; M = 2.40, SD = 0.80, α = 0.90).
After that, we included some newly created personality items that were unrelated to an interest in exploring culturally foreign stimuli, a newly created cross-cultural conflict resolution task that included a manipulation, and a criterion variable for explorative reasons. Finally, we collected some demographic data and measured participants' handedness (Oldfield, 1971).
Results
Threat × NFC on cultural novelty avoidance and avoidance motivation
As an initial step we sought to confirm that MS heightens avoidance of cultural otherness especially at high NFC. For this, effect-coded threat (with zero as the non-threat and one as the threat condition), NFC, and the threat × NFC term were entered as predictors of cultural avoidance. Results are displayed in Table 1. Both the main effects of threat and NFC were significant. These main effects were qualified by the predicted interaction (ΔR2 = 0.06) such that avoidance of cultural novelty was especially high under MS vs. control for people high (+1 SD) on NFC [b = 0.59, SE = 0.26, t(70) = 2.28, p = 0.025, see Figure 1]. There was no effect of threat at low NFC (−1 SD), b = −0.26, SE = 0.26, t(70) = 1.09, p = 0.277.
Table 1.
b | SE | 95% CI | t | p | β | Partial r2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Threat | 0.07 | 0.09 | −0.10 | 0.25 | 0.85 | 0.396 | 0.09 | 0.01 |
NFC | 0.38 | 0.11 | 0.15 | 0.60 | 3.34 | 0.001 | 0.40 | 0.14 |
Threat × NFC | 0.26 | 0.11 | 0.03 | 0.48 | 2.28 | 0.026 | 0.27 | 0.07 |
N = 74. Model R2 = 0.16.
Next, we tested our key hypothesis that MS causes BIS activity—as indicated by a shift in leftward bisection bias—for persons with high NFC (the mediator model). In line with prior research using the LBT and to control for hemispheric differences arising from handedness (e.g., Drake and Myers, 2006), we excluded 16 left-handed individuals from all analyses involving the LBT (Oldfield, 1971). Furthermore, the LBT baseline measure was entered as a predictor in all analyses including the LBT; thus, effects on LBT can be considered demonstrations of residual change. Baseline LBT, effect-coded threat, NFC, and the threat × NFC interaction were entered as predictors of post-threat LBT. As displayed in Table 2, there were no main effects, and the threat × NFC interaction was significant (ΔR2 = 0.060). Avoidance of cultural novelty was especially high under MS vs. control for people high on NFC, though this effect did not emerge until very high values of NFC (~+2 SD or the 95% of our observed distribution according to the Johnson-Neyman technique, 1936). There was an unpredicted effect of threat at low NFC (−1 SD), b = −2.42, SE = 1.11, t(53) = 2.17, p = 0.034. Figure 2 shows that the effect of NFC on bisection bias under MS reversed from control such that NFC predicted more avoidance under MS.
Table 2.
b | SE | 95% CI | t | p | β | Partial r2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Threat | −0.08 | 0.36 | −0.81 | 0.65 | −0.22 | 0.827 | −0.02 | 0.00 |
NFC | −0.06 | 0.50 | −1.06 | 0.95 | −0.12 | 0.907 | −0.01 | 0.01 |
Threat × NFC | 1.32 | 0.51 | 0.29 | 2.34 | 2.57 | 0.013 | 0.27 | 0.15 |
Baseline LBT | 0.56 | 0.09 | 0.38 | 0.74 | 6.29 | 0.001 | 0.61 | 0.36 |
N = 58. Model R2 = 0.52.
Test of moderated mediation
Next, we tested whether BIS activity transmits or explains the effect of the interaction of MS and NFC to cultural avoidance. Because NFC is linked to both sensitivity to threat and motivated closed-mindedness under threat, it was allowed to moderate both the path from the threat to the mediator, as well as from the mediator to the dependent variable (i.e., model 58; Hayes, 2013). Results of this analysis are displayed in Figure 3. In sum, the effect of post-threat BIS activity explained the effect of threat × NFC on cultural novelty avoidance, especially at high levels of BIS and NFC.
Brief discussion
These results provide the first evidence for the notion that (a) MS leads to increases in BIS activity, (b) BIS activity mediates MS effects on aversion to cultural novelty, and (c) these effects are particular to people high on NFC. Thus, the findings demonstrate that a basic, passive-avoidance motivational process underlies higher-order manifestations of behavioral inhibition, such as the reluctance to explore unfamiliar cultures. This dynamic was only present for high NFC individuals, suggesting that they are particularly inclined to avoid the terror of death through motivated closed-mindedness toward cultural otherness.
One limitation of the current study was that our cultural novelty-avoidance measure had not been used prior to the study, so its construct validity was questionable. We thus conducted a follow-up study to explore the relationship between this measure and a frequent operationalization of cultural worldview defense. We found that the cultural novelty-avoidance measure used in Study 1 correlated with a measure of general ethnocentrism, r(30) = 0.37, p = 0.037. This ethnocentrism measure has been used as a worldview defense measure in prior TMT research (Agroskin and Jonas, 2013), and found to be related to NFC (Agroskin and Jonas, 2010). Thus, our cultural novelty-avoidance items may at least partly tap into those destructive aspects of ethnocentric intergroup bias that are characteristic of cultural worldview defense.
Study 1 provided strong initial support for the proposition that MS activates the BIS particularly in high NFC people, which explains their post-threat closed-mindedness. However, we used a fairly indirect measure of frontal asymmetry measured via the LBT. In Study 2, we turned to a more direct measure of frontal asymmetry available via EEG. Furthermore, Study 1 employed a short NFC scale instead of the full measure. Thus, Study 2 provides an opportunity for replication and extension of the findings of Study 1.
Study 2
Building on Study 1, Study 2 employed a poem-based MS manipulation (Agroskin and Jonas, 2013), the full NFC scale (Schlink and Walther, 2007), and frontal EEG alpha asymmetry instead of the LBT as a more direct measure of BIS-related neural activity. We also used the general ethnocentrism measure from the follow-up study instead of cultural novelty-avoidance to tap into the destructive aspects of cultural closed-mindedness that often characterize worldview-defensive negativity toward cultural outgroups. Summing up, Study 2 was designed to provide multimethodological and conceptual replication for the findings of Study 1.
Method
Participants and design
The sample consisted of 33 participants (23 female) from the University of Salzburg without any reported history of neurological disorders or prior head trauma5. Their mean age was 22.1 years (SD = 3.0; range: 18–32)6. Twenty-two participants held German and eleven Austrian nationalities. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions, MS (N = 16) or TV salience (N = 17), in a between-subjects design7. All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study, which ostensibly investigated topics associated with literature and personality. They were rewarded with course credits. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Salzburg. All participants signed informed consent, and could withdraw participation at any point, although no participant made use of this option.
Procedure and materials
Need for closure
Prior to coming to the lab for the EEG measurement, participants were emailed and asked to fill out a few online questionnaires, including the whole NFC scale (Schlink and Walther, 2007) whose short form was used in Study 18. Responses were given on a 1 (totally disagree) to 6 (totally agree) scale, and averaged to create a single composite score (14 items; M = 3.23, SD = 0.70, α = 0.82)9.
Manipulation
Upon arriving at the lab, participants were asked to provide some demographic data. After that, they underwent a 90-s baseline EEG recording (see below for more detail). Then, they were presented a short poem related to death or a neutral control topic (weather). The death-related poem has been used as a MS manipulation before (Agroskin and Jonas, 2013, Study 2). Literally translated from German, it reads “The hardest and cruelest power, which man has to accept, is death.” The weather-related poem reads “Look to the sky, clouds are passing by. Some are raining, others are not. Some are flashing, others are not.” Participants were asked to read the poem and describe how they interpret it, what they associate with it (e.g., images, sounds, smells, and moods), whether they have experienced anything in their own lives that showed them that the poem was true, and whether their own existence had certain aspects reflecting the veracity of the poem. These questions were used to help ensure a deep and thorough processing of the poems and their meaning.
EEG (inhibition of approach motivation)
Right after the manipulation, participants underwent another 90-s EEG recording in order to assess post-threat avoidance motivation as a function of MS. During all EEG recordings participants were asked to fixate a small black cross in the middle of the screen. After this EEG measurement, participants filled out the 20-item Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson et al., 1988). Then, participants underwent one more 90-s EEG recording.
Ethnocentrism
Following a delay of a few minutes (involving questions about which TV programs participants would like to watch and their reactions to two press reports about crimes10), which is necessary to obtain MS effects following explicit MS inductions (Pyszczynski et al., 1999), general ethnocentrism was measured using a short form of the ethnocentrism scale by Bizumic et al. (2009). This short form has been previously used by Agroskin and Jonas (2013) as a measure of cultural worldview defense, and was found to be associated with NFC, right-wing authoritarianism, avoidance of empathy, and anti-immigration attitudes (Agroskin and Jonas, 2010). Sample items are “In most circumstances it is right and natural to favor members from one's own cultural or ethnic group over strangers or foreigners” and “We need to do what's best for our own people, and stop worrying so much about what the effect might be on other peoples.” Responses were given on a 1 (totally disagree) to 6 (totally agree) scale, and averaged to create a single composite score (4 items; M = 2.10, SD = 0.91, α = 0.71)11.
Then, for exploratory reasons, participants underwent another 90 s EEG recording trial and completed some questions about their attitudes toward the European currency. Finally, they indicated their nationality and were probed for suspicion. Our focal BIS-related hypotheses were not correctly guessed by any participant12.
EEG apparatus and data reduction
We used a 14-channel Emotiv EEG neuroheadset (Emotiv Systems Inc., San Francisco, CA, USA), recording data with Emotiv TestBench software at a sampling rate of 128 Hz. EEG data was recorded at sites AF3, AF4, F3, F4, F7, F8, FC5, FC6, P7, T7, T8, P8, O1, and O2 in accordance with the 10–20 International System. Two mastoid electrodes served as online reference. The Emotiv EEG neuroheadset employs gold-plated contact-grade hardened copper electrodes with saline-moistened felt pads to record EEG. Importantly, the Emotiv EEG device has been found to record similar suppression of alpha power (8–13 Hz) in the Left Precentral AAL (Automated Anatomical Labeling) region in response to imagined right-finger tapping trials as compared to a standard Biosemi Active-II 64-channel device (see also Stopczynski et al., 2014a,b). This suggests that the Emotiv device is suitable for investigating neural processes associated with regional changes in alpha power. Further, validation studies determined that when used no longer than about 60 min per recording session, the ERP signal quality produced by this device is comparable to (or only slightly lower than) the signal quality provided by traditional EEG systems (Badcock et al., 2013; Mayaud et al., 2013). Consequently, Emotiv EEG technology has recently been increasingly used for cognitive neuroscience and brain-computer interface (BCI) applications (Bobrov et al., 2011; Debener et al., 2012; Louwerse and Hutchinson, 2012; Choi and Jo, 2013; Khushaba et al., 2013; O'Regan et al., 2013; O'Regan and Marnane, 2013; De Vos et al., 2014; Vourvopoulos and Liarokapis, 2014; Aspinall et al., 2015; Steinhubl et al., 2015).
Using BrainVision Analyzer 2.0 (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany), EEG data were filtered with an IIR filter (high-pass cutoff: 0.1 Hz, Slope: 24 db/Oct; low-pass cutoff: 30 Hz, Slope: 24 db/Oct). Next, the three 90-s EEG recording epochs were extracted from the continuous data and were further segmented into 2s segments (1.5s overlap). Epochs were included in condition averages when a difference between two values in a moving 200 ms interval did not exceed 100 microvolt, or when the signal was below −100 or above +100 microvolt. The 2s segments were then Fourier transformed (Fast Fourier Transform, 10% Hamming Window, frequency resolution 0.5 Hz) and the resulting power spectra were averaged. Individual frontal asymmetry scores (μV2) for each epoch were calculated (log F7–log F8) in the alpha band (8–13 Hz) and exported for statistical analysis. Positive values index right-frontal hemisphericity indicative of avoidance motivation. Analyses focused on lateral frontal asymmetry (F7–F8) because these electrodes have been specifically linked to bisection bias in the LBT (Nash et al., 2010).
Results
Preliminary analyses
Frontal asymmetry scores (L – R alpha) for the proximal and distal recordings were baseline adjusted for each participant so that they reflected changes in asymmetry, following past research on state frontal alpha asymmetry (e.g., Allen et al., 2001; Ravaja et al., 2013). The resultant asymmetry scores for the proximal and distal recordings were highly correlated, r = 0.63, p < 0.001. Given that Jonas et al. (2014) assume anxious persons to show prolonged BIS activity following threat, and NFC is known to affect proximal and distal MS effects in a similar way (Juhl and Routledge, 2010), we merged the proximal and distal asymmetry scores.
Primary analyses
Analytical strategy
As in Study 1, the hypotheses were tested in several steps using moderated mediational analyses (Hayes, 2013) performed using Mplus 7 (Muthén and Muthén, 2012). The zero-order correlations are not explicitly reported because they were virtually identical (i.e., same directions and similar magnitudes and significance levels) with the respective main effects reported below13.
MS × NFC effect on EEG-inhibition
First, we tested whether individuals with high NFC showed higher right-frontal (or reduced left-frontal) asymmetry following MS (coded as MS = 1, weather = 0) by regressing the post-threat asymmetry score onto pre-threat asymmetry, threat, NFC, and the threat × NFC interaction. There was no effect of NFC in the control condition, b = −0.05, SE = 0.06, t(28) = 0.78, p = 0.443 and a significant main effect of threat such that avoidance was higher under MS than control, b = 0.69, SE = 0.31, t(28) = 2.23, p = 0.025. This effect was qualified by a significant interaction between the manipulation and NFC, b = 0.24, SE = 0.09, β = 0.43, t(28) = 2.56, p = 0.01, ΔR2 = 0.16 (see Figure 4). Simple effects analyses revealed that high NFC individuals (SD = 1) showed the predicted increase in right-frontal asymmetry following MS, b = 0.27, SE = 0.10, β = 0.69, t(28) = 2.64, p < 0.01, whereas this was not true for low NFC persons (SD = −1), b = −0.06, SE = 0.10, β = −0.16, t(28) = −0.65, p = 0.52. Thus, our hypotheses were corroborated: MS increased BIS-based inhibition of approach motivation but only for high NFC persons.
EEG-inhibition effect on ethnocentrism
Right-frontal asymmetry was entered as a predictor in addition to the manipulation and NFC. The interaction between the manipulation and NFC (i.e., direct interactive effect) was fixed to zero because we assumed a complete mediation model (Shrout and Bolger, 2002). As expected, right-frontal asymmetry predicted ethnocentrism, b = 2.49, SE = 0.69, β = 0.54, t(29) = 3.59, p < 0.001. No other relationships attained significance (ps > 0.34). In contrast to Study 1, adding the interaction between right-frontal asymmetry and NFC did not reveal moderation by NFC, t(27) = −0.62, p = 0.53. Thus, BIS activity affected ethnocentrism regardless of NFC levels. We thus fixed the effect of NFC to zero in accordance with Hayes (2013, Model 7) for the moderated mediational analyses. Summing up, the more participants had right-frontal asymmetry, the more they adopted ethnocentric attitudes. Our hypotheses were therefore supported.
Bootstrap estimation of the indirect effects
As per bias-corrected bootstrap procedure using 75000 bootstrap samples (Hayes, 2013, Model 7, see Figure 5), MS significantly increased ethnocentrism through right-frontal asymmetry for high NFC persons, point estimate (b) = 0.67, 95% CI [0.02, 1.43]. In contrast, there was no conditional indirect effect for low NFC persons, b = −0.15, 95% CI [−0.84, 0.33].
Finally, the overall model test was consistent with our hypothesis of complete mediation (Hayes, 2013, Model 7), = 0.65, p = 0.72, CFI = 1.00. Thus, our key mediational hypothesis for high NFC individuals was supported: MS amplified ethnocentric thinking via the BIS-based inhibition of approach motivation14.
Discussion
These results replicate and expand the findings from Study 1 in several important ways. First, they provide a more direct measure of right-frontal asymmetric activity in the prefrontal cortex via EEG. Second, they involve another operationalization of motivated closed-mindedness in the cultural domain—generalized ethnocentric attitudes—which suggests that a similar process may underlie various forms of worldview defense. All in all, the findings are clearly supportive of the view that high NFC persons' approach motivation is strongly inhibited following mortality reminders, which underlies their inclination to engage in ethnocentric thinking.
In Study 3, we aimed at conceptually replicating the effect of BIS-related inhibition on ethnocentrism by directly manipulating inhibition. Further, this test provides another means of testing for mediation by manipulating the proposed mediator (Spencer et al., 2005).
Study 3
In Study 3, we employed an experimental approach to testing mediation by manipulating the assumed mediator—inhibition. Building on research on disinhibition by van den Bos et al. (2009), we developed a novel manipulation of inhibition to test whether bringing people into a state of inhibition produces the same ethnocentric bias as threat and BIS activity. Thus, we measured NFC, induced inhibition, and measured ethnocentrism with the same instrument as in Study 2. In addition, we included a BIS affect measure as a manipulation check. We expected inhibition to promote ethnocentric thinking for persons with high NFC.
Method
Participants and design
Seventy-six students at the University of Salzburg participated in this paper-pencil study, which took place in class, after a lecture15. Participants were 58 females, 15 males, and 3 undisclosed. Their mean age was 21.6 years (SD = 2.0; range: 19–30). Fifty-five participants held German, 17 Austrian, and 1 another nationality (3 undisclosed). Participants were randomly assigned to one of two conditions, inhibition (N = 36), or control (N = 40), in a between-subjects design. The study was described as an investigation of participants' personalities. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Salzburg. All participants signed informed consent, and could withdraw participation at any point, although no participant made use of this option.
Procedure and materials
Need for closure
Because the two-item NFC measure used in Study 1 was shown to be sensitive to MS, we measured NFC with same two items (M = 3.77, SD = 0.85; r = 0.44, p < 0.001).
Manipulation
After a few further personality items, included to bolster the cover story, we presented a novel manipulation of inhibition. Building on the disinhibition manipulation from van den Bos et al. (2009), we asked participants three open-ended questions about behaving with inhibitions: “Please briefly describe a situation out of your own life in which you felt and behaved in an inhibited way like you typically do.” In the next two questions, we asked participants how they behaved and felt in this situation. In the control condition, we posed the same three questions referring to their behaving in a normal way (cf. van den Bos et al., 2009): “Please briefly describe a situation out of your own life in which you felt and behaved in a normal way like you typically do.” The other two questions referred to participants' behavior and feelings again.
Ethnocentrism
Ethnocentrism was measured in the same way as in Study 2 (4 items; M = 1.94, SD = 0.77, α = 0.60)16.
BIS affect
Following Schumann et al. (2014), we included a retrospective manipulation check asking about how participants felt when responding to the inhibition/control condition-related questions. We presented five items measuring BIS-related affect, including four adjectives from the German translation of the Carver and White (1994) BIS sensitivity scale (Strobel et al., 2001), namely nervous (German: nervös), anxious (ängstlich), worked up (unruhig), and worried (besorgt), along with the face-valid adjective inhibited (gehemmt; five items; M = 2.32, SD = 1.45, α = 0.94). Participants responded on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Finally, participants completed the PANAS (Watson et al., 1988) and some demographic questions17.
Results
Inhibition × NFC on ethnocentrism
To examine whether high NFC persons respond with increased ethnocentrism to the inhibition recall-task, we used moderated regression analyses18. Whereas, there were no significant main effects (ps > 0.14), the interaction between the manipulation (coded as inhibition = 1, control = 0) and NFC was significant, b = 0.49, SE = 0.21, β = 0.27, t(69) = 2.38, p < 0.02, ΔR2 = 0.07 (see Figure 6). As predicted, high NFC individuals (SD = 1) showed significantly increased ethnocentrism following MS, b = 0.59, SE = 0.24, β = 0.39, t(69) = 2.41, p < 0.05, whereas this was not true for low NFC persons (SD = −1), b = −0.24, SE = 0.25, β = −0.16, t(69) = −0.98, p = 0.33. Thus, our hypotheses were corroborated: inhibition caused ethnocentrism in high NFC persons, whereas low NFC persons were not affected by inhibition in this way.
BIS affect (manipulation check)
As expected, BIS affect was significantly higher in the inhibition condition (M = 3.63, SD = 1.24) than in the control condition (M = 1.32, SD = 0.47), t(35.40) = 9.65, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.6319.
PANAS
Mood was also affected by inhibition. Negative affect was significantly increased and positive affect was significantly decreased by inhibition, ts > 2.91, ps < 0.01. Therefore, we reanalyzed the inhibition effect on BIS affect including the PANAS scales as covariates. Inhibition still increased BIS affect notwithstanding the PANAS, F(1, 65) = 49.64, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.43, although BIS affect was highly correlated with negative mood, r = 0.79, p < 0.001. There was no correlation between BIS affect and positive mood, r = −0.16, p = 20.
Likewise, the interactive effect of inhibition and NFC on ethnocentrism was virtually unaltered after controlling for the PANAS subscales, b = 0.44, SE = 0.22, t(63) = 1.95, p = 0.051. Persons with high NFC still showed increased ethnocentrism after inhibition, b = 0.67, SE = 0.31, t(63) = 2.14, p < 0.05, contrary to low NFC persons, b = −0.07, SE = 0.29, t(63) = −0.26, p = 0.80. The PANAS scales did not affect ethnocentrism (ps > 0.21).
Exploratory analyses: BIS affect × NFC on ethnocentrism
For the sake of convergent validity, we explored whether BIS affect elicited ethnocentrism for high NFC persons similar to the inhibition manipulation. We also controlled for the PANAS scales to rule out the possibility that the effect of BIS affect could be explained by mood more generally. There was a marginally significant main effect of BIS affect, b = 0.17, SE = 0.10, β = 0.32, t(65) = 1.69, p < 0.10, all other ps > 0.22. Moreover, a significant interaction between BIS affect and NFC occurred, b = 0.24, SE = 0.09, β = 0.39, t(65) = 2.73, p < 0.01, ΔR2 = 0.09 (see Figure 7). As predicted, high NFC individuals showed significantly increased ethnocentrism following MS, b = 0.36, SE = 0.12, β = 0.66, t(65) = 3.04, p < 0.01, whereas this was not true for low NFC persons, b = −40.05, SE = 0.13, β = −0.11, t(65) = −0.43, p = 0.67. Again, the PANAS scales did not affect ethnocentrism (ps > 0.21). Thus, BIS affect evoked ethnocentrism for high NFC persons in the same way as inhibition, and this effect was independent of changes in mood.
Discussion
These results constitute an important conceptual replication of the relationship between BIS activity and ethnocentric thinking. Manipulating inhibition, the proposed mediator of MS effects on ethnocentrism, provided further support for the proposition that inhibition plays a causal role in generating ethnocentrism following MS. Persons with high NFC responded with increased ethnocentrism to not only induced inhibition but also experiences of BIS-related affect. These results provide important multimethod evidence for the notion that not only perceptual and neural (Studies 1–2) but also cognitive-affective indicators of BIS-mediated inhibition can provoke ethnocentric thinking, and these effects are particularly strong for persons with high NFC.
General discussion
In this paper, we have presented evidence from three studies that shed light on several previously uninvestigated questions that have recently gathered much attention in the existential threat literature (Tritt et al., 2012; Jonas et al., 2014). We found that (a) MS causes BIS activity, as indexed by right-hemispheric asymmetric activation in the frontal cortex (measured by the LBT in Study 1 and EEG in Study 2), (b) BIS activity mediates MS effects on cultural closed-mindedness (operationalized as reluctance to explore foreign cultures in Study 1 and ethnocentrism in Studies 2 and 3), and (c) these effects are particularly strong for persons inclined to experience discomfort when faced with unfamiliar and potentially threatening stimuli (measured by NFC in all studies). In addition, Study 3 conceptually replicated the first two studies by showing that directly manipulating inhibition promotes ethnocentric thinking for high NFC persons in a manner similar to right-frontal asymmetry. To our knowledge, these results are the first to demonstrate that MS-based aversion to cultural otherness is driven by a very basic, neurally-instantiated inhibitory process.
Theoretical implications
The findings have important implications for understanding the neural and motivational underpinnings of ethnocentric attitudes in response to existential threat. They provide the first direct support for the hypothesis that inhibitory processes, as indicated by relative right-hemispheric activity, underlie MS effects on ethnocentric thinking, which is a key aspect of worldview defense. This mediational finding is consistent with the process model of threat and defense, which states that defensive responses to threat are driven by passive avoidance motivation, for which the BIS is primarily responsible (Jonas et al., 2014). This is also in line with the assumption of TMT that ethnocentric responses to MS constitute “avoidant defenses,” which are aimed at avoiding experiencing existential anxiety (Greenberg et al., 2003, p. 519). Moreover, our results are exactly in line with Greenberg et al.'s (2003) speculation that these defenses might be mediated by “a relative increase in right-hemisphere frontal lobe activity” (Greenberg et al., 2003, p. 519). On a general level, our findings may advance and refine TMT research in that they show that MS can not only promote various BIS-related behaviors (Landau and Greenberg, 2006; Routledge et al., 2010; Huang and Wyer, 2015), but also that cultural closed-mindedness can be explained through very basic inhibitory processes.
To be clear, we are not suggesting that all manifestations of worldview defense in response to MS are driven by inhibitory processes based on the current observations. Although, this would follow from the general process model (Jonas et al., 2014), our results can only speak to cultural closed-mindedness. Thus, it largely remains an empirical question to what extent inhibition processes drive other forms of worldview defense given that inhibition has yet to be thoroughly tested as a mediator in this regard. Future research should sample the conceptual space of worldview defense more broadly to examine the generality of the process we present On a related note, it also remains to be seen whether approach-related cognition can alleviate the passive avoidance observed here and prevent negative effects of MS on desirable outcomes, like creativity (Sligte et al., 2013). The findings may also advance research on threat and defense in that they suggest that various responses to threat that are not obviously or intuitively related to a lack of approach motivation may be attributed to the approach-inhibitory activity of the BIS.
These advances also have important implications for inspiring research on the defensive function of BIS anxiety, which originates from ethological research on animals' responses to threat (Gray and McNaughton, 2000). It has been argued that “there are long-standing concerns that rodent models of psychological processes are too simple to apply in humans (Matthews, 2008): For example, there is no evidence that rodents experience anxiety of an abstract type related to existential issues, whereas historical and literary accounts abundantly point to the existence of such angst in humans. Concerns of this type have raised a need for studies of human defense that can test the validity of the defensive explanation for anxiety in humans” (Perkins and Corr, 2014, pp. 42–43). Our findings show that rodent models of BIS anxiety can indeed be applied to defensive behaviors in the wake of threat in humans and point toward the possibility of a more consilient science of motivation.
On the whole, our findings provide valuable insight into contextual, interindividual, motivational, and neural determinants of cultural closed-mindedness. To our knowledge, this research is the first to establish an empirical link between very basic, neurally instantiated inhibitory processes and rather complex, higher-order manifestations of intergroup negativity. By investigating the neural and motivational underpinnings of ethnocentric attitudes in the wake of existential threat, this research might contribute to a fuller understanding of cultural worldview defensive phenomena, among the most investigated outcomes in social psychology over the last 25 years (Burke et al., 2010).
Limitations and future directions
We used an indirect measure of BIS activity in Studies 1 and 2. Following theoretical and empirical research relating reduced left-frontal asymmetry (or increased right-frontal asymmetry) to BIS-mediated inhibition of approach motivation, we used perceptual and neural indicators of frontal asymmetry to gauge BIS activity (e.g., Davidson et al., 2000; Coan and Allen, 2004; Corr, 2004; Harmon-Jones et al., 2009; Nash et al., 2010). To provide more direct evidence for the role of inhibition in the emergence of ethnocentric thinking, we directly manipulated inhibition in Study 3, finding increased ethnocentrism for high NFC persons following experiences of inhibition and BIS-related affect. In combination, these studies yield support for BIS-driven ethnocentrism in the wake of existential threat. Further, we have other lines of converging evidence for the notion that perceptual and neural indicators of right-frontal asymmetry reflect BIS activity (Agroskin and Jonas, 2015; Agroskin et al., 2015). For instance, we have found in four EEG and LBT studies that directly boosting/blocking BIS anxiety leads to increased/reduced right-frontal asymmetry following MS (and also without MS). Mirroring increased BIS affect following induced inhibition (Study 3), we have also found amplified BIS affect following MS. In summary, accumulating, multi-method evidence supports the notion that threat-induced anxious inhibition (BIS activity) is a cause of ethnocentric thinking.
Future research might consider an intriguing existential interpretation of the avoidance—approach motivational systems. According to TMT, avoidance—approach conflicts may reflect the duality between the core motives for self-protective defense (avoidance motivation) and self-expansive growth (approach motivation; Greenberg et al., 1995a; Pyszczynski et al., 2000, 2012). From this perspective, closed-minded persons engage in novelty-avoidant defense of the “familiar” in response to mortality awareness, whereas open-minded individuals approach the “unfamiliar,” expanding the self by gaining new experiences. Future research may investigate to what extent these motivational tendencies mediate MS effects on various behavioral manifestations of the defense and growth motives, such as support of extreme military action against cultural outgroups (defense motive), and commitment to important life goals that convey a sense of existential meaning (growth motive; Greenberg et al., 1995a; Pyszczynski et al., 2012; see Vail et al., 2012 for a review of growth-oriented MS effects).
Another avenue for future research might be to investigate further neural underpinnings of ethnocentric attitudes following MS. Along with the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the BIS may be subserved by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; McNaughton and Corr, 2004). In addition to generating neural signals of error-related distress (ERN) and anxiety (see Shackman et al., 2011 for a review), the ACC contributes to risk aversion in decision making (Brown and Braver, 2007). This suggests that the ACC may also support risk-avoidant behaviors after MS, such as the ethnocentric avoidance of cultural outgroups. Future research might shed some more light on previously unexamined neural mediators of terror management mechanisms.
Conclusion
Becker (1997) saw man as a “terrified, death-avoiding animal” (p. 99) that, by seeking to avoid death, kills off “so large a spectrum of his action-world that he is actually isolating and diminishing himself and becomes as though dead” (Becker, 1997, p. 181). Psychiatrist Joseph Rheingold arrived at the same conclusion: “The common denominator of all negative ways of dealing with anxiety is a shrinking of the area of awareness and of activity. (…) We are afraid to die, and therefore we are afraid to live, (…) we avoid non-being by avoiding being. The avoidance of anxiety then means a kind of death in life” (Rheingold, 1967, pp. 204–205). These insightful analyses shed light on a fascinating irony inherent in human striving for self-preservation, or in terms of terror management, a life free from consciously experienced death anxiety (Greenberg et al., 2003). In trying to avoid death, people actually avoid life, given that exploring (cultural) novelty contributes to various indicators of well-being, including subjective vitality and satisfaction with life (Kashdan et al., 2004). Our findings represent the first neural evidence, to our knowledge, that this existential ironical twist is driven by an ancient motivational system, oriented toward passively avoiding punishment from the environment. By symbolically avoiding the punishment of death, people in reality obtain the punishment of an unlived life.
Author contributions
DA and EJ conceptualized the project and collected the data. JK advised the EEG data collection and analysis and assisted manuscript preparation. DA analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. MP assisted with data analysis and manuscript preparation.
Funding
The research reported in this article has been financed by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF-P27457). The first author of this article was also financially supported by the Doctoral College “Imaging the Mind” of the Austrian Science Fund (FWF-W1233).
Conflict of interest statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
1Corr et al. (2013a) propose a distinction between a system for passive avoidance, generated by the BIS (e.g., inhibition of approach motivational behavior toward unfamiliar and potentially threatening people or places), and a system for active avoidance, produced by the fight-flight-freeze system (FFFS; e.g., running away from a predator). Contrary to the BIS that responds to more distant, abstract, and ambiguous threats, the FFFS responds to more proximate, concrete, and unequivocal threats, such as immediate attackers. The present line of research pertains to existential threats, which are more distant, abstract, and ambiguous (e.g., the time and way of one's death is usually unknown; see also Huang and Wyer, 2015). Therefore, we focus on BIS-mediated passive avoidance motivational responses to threat in this paper.
2In some studies discussed in this paper, NFC was measured with the personal need for structure (PNS) scale (Neuberg and Newsom, 1993), which has been used as an alternative operationalization of NFC in prior TMT research (e.g., Dechesne et al., 2000).
3Regarding sample size considerations, MS has an expected effect size of MS at d = 0.75 on ethnocentrism and related outcomes (Burke et al., 2010). Thus, power for a main effect is at 80% with a sample of n = 60. Given that a personality moderator should add prediction to a model, power was expected to be well over 80% for the current studies. For example in the current research domain, Vess et al. (2009) observed NFC × MS interaction effects on meaning in life in three studies (1a–1c) with an average d = 0.90.
4In contrast to Vess et al. (2009) who used Green and Campbell's (2000) exploration scale, we developed new items for cultural novelty-avoidance because their exploration scale largely lacks explicit reference to culturally foreign stimuli.
5Due to the intensive nature of EEG data collection and analysis, collection was stopped after reaching an n of 15 per cell plus 10% for anticipated data loss.
6Age was unequally distributed across experimental conditions. Participants were significantly younger in the MS condition (M = 20.4, SD = 1.93) than in the control condition (M = 23.7, SD = 2.91), t(31) = 3.78, p < 0.001. We included age as a covariate in all analyses reported below. Running the analysis without this covariate did not impact the status of the significance tests or the substance of the conclusions.
7This study was part of a larger project that included other experimental conditions for exploratory reasons. Specifically, conditions not reported here were presented with other poems about existential topics that did not refer to the subject of death and that were not designed as neutral control conditions either. Given that this study was primarily designed as a conceptual replication of Study 1, we present only the analyses that concern the MS and neutral control conditions.
8In addition to the 33 participants reported above, nine more persons participated in the EEG study but failed to complete the personality questionnaires. They were dropped from all analyses.
9Two items were dropped prior to analyses because of very low corrected item-total correlations (rits < 0.20).
10The press reports described crimes committed in Austria (or by Austrians). Then, participants' intentions to punish the perpetrators were measured. Given that 66.7% of our participants were Germans, we could not analyze their punishment intentions in response to Austrian crimes as indicators of their ethnocentric, worldview-defensive attitudes (cf. Rosenblatt et al., 1989). We also included four items measuring worldview-defensive prejudice toward immigrants (Agroskin and Jonas, 2010) right after the focal ethnocentrism measure (see below), which were not analyzed for the same reason (i.e., most of our participants were immigrants themselves).
11Two items were dropped prior to analyses because of very low corrected item-total correlations (rits < 0.20).
12Two participants mentioned death-related thoughts and ethnocentric attitudes, though. Dropping these two participants did not substantially change the results reported below, which is why we kept them in the final sample.
13Four participants had missing data in either the proximal or the distal EEG measurement. All reported analyses have been run with the whole sample using Full-Information-Maximum Likelihood (FIML) parameters in Mplus 7 (Muthén and Muthén, 2012). However, excluding the participants with missing data using listwise deletion did not change the results substantially (see also footnote 15).
14All critical effects stayed significant using non-bootstrapped coefficients, including the conditional indirect effect for high NFC, b = 0.69, SE = 0.33, t = 2.10, p < 0.05. However, although the point estimate for the indirect effect was even somewhat increased, the bootstrap estimates became less robust, b = 0.69, 90% CI [0.05, 1.54], 95% CI [−0.11, 1.73]. This may be due to reduced degrees of freedom. Note, that the same pattern occurred when computing the model without the PANAS using listwise deletion instead of FIML, which likewise reduced degrees of freedom, b = 0.62, SE = 0.31, t = 2.04, p < 0.05 (non-bootstrap test), b = 0.62, 90% CI [0.04, 1.23], 95% CI [−0.11, 1.34] (bootstrap test; see also footnote 16). The results were virtually unchanged after controlling for the PANAS scales.
15With respect to the stopping rule for data collection in this study, we aimed at recruiting the same amount of participants as in Study 1, distributing questionnaires to all students who attended a lecture that is usually attended by about 80 people.
16After the ethnocentrism items, we included four items measuring prejudice toward immigrants (Agroskin and Jonas, 2010). These items were included as another measure of an ethnocentric, worldview-defensive attitude but were not analyzed because 75% of participants of this study were technically immigrants themselves (i.e., Germans studying in Austria).
17The negative mood scale included only eight instead of ten items because the items anxious and nervous were included in the BIS affect measure.
18Three participants failed to complete the ethnocentrism measure.
19Seven participants failed to complete the BIS affect and PANAS measures. We also used the adjusted degrees of freedom because variances were not equal as indicated by a significant Levene's test (p < 0.001).
References
- Agroskin D., Jonas E. (2010). Out of control: how and why does perceived lack of control lead to ethnocentrism? Rev. Psychol. 17, 79–90. [Google Scholar]
- Agroskin D., Jonas E. (2013). Controlling death by defending ingroups — mediational insights into terror management and control restoration. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1144–1158. 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.05.014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Agroskin D., Jonas E. (2015). Boosting and Blocking the Behavioral Inhibition System Following Mortality Awareness. Unpublished manuscript, University of Salzburg, Salzburg. [Google Scholar]
- Agroskin D., Klackl J., McGregor I., Prentice M., Lechinger J., Jonas E. (2015). From Avoidance to Approach via Ethnocentrism: Temporal Dynamics of Avoidance-Approach Motivation following Two Existential Threats. Unpublished manuscript, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria. [Google Scholar]
- Allen J. J. B., Harmon-Jones E., Cavender J. H. (2001). Manipulation of frontal EEG asymmetry through biofeedback alters self-reported emotional responses and facial EMG. Psychophysiology 38, 685–693. 10.1111/1469-8986.3840685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amodio D. M., Shah J. Y., Sigelman J., Brazy P. C., Harmon-Jones E. (2004). Implicit regulatory focus associated with asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 225–232. 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00100-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Arndt J., Greenberg J., Simon L., Pyszczynski T., Solomon S. (1998). Terror management and self-awareness: evidence that mortality salience provokes avoidance of the self-focused state. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 24, 1216–1227. 10.1177/01461672982411008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Aron A. R., Robbins T. W., Poldrack R. A. (2004). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 170–177. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aspinall P., Mavros P., Coyne R., Roe J. (2015). The urban brain: analysing outdoor physical activity with mobile EEG. Br. J. Sports Med. 49, 272–276. 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091877 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badcock N. A., Mousikou P., Mahajan Y., de Lissa P., Thie J., McArthur G. (2013). Validation of the Emotiv EPOC® EEG gaming system for measuring research quality auditory ERPs. PeerJ 1:e38. 10.7717/peerj.38 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker E. (1997). The Denial of Death. New York, NY: Free Press Paperbacks. [Google Scholar]
- Bijttebier P., Beck I., Claes L., Vandereycken W. (2009). Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory as a framework for research on personality–psychopathology associations. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 421–430. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bizumic B., Duckitt J., Popadic D., Dru V., Krauss S. (2009). A cross-cultural investigation into a reconceptualization of ethnocentrism. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 39, 871–899. 10.1002/ejsp.589 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bobrov P., Frolov A., Cantor C., Fedulova I., Bakhnyan M., Zhavoronkov A. (2011). Brain–computer interface based on generation of visual images. PLoS ONE 6:e20674. 10.1371/journal.pone.0020674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boksem M. A. S., Tops M., Kostermans E., De Cremer D. (2008). Sensitivity to punishment and reward omission: evidence from error-related ERP components. Biol. Psychol. 79, 185–192. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boksem M. A. S., Tops M., Wester A. E., Meijman T. F., Lorist M. M. (2006). Error-related ERP components and individual differences in punishment and reward sensitivity. Brain Res. 1101, 92–101. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown J. W., Braver T. S. (2007). Risk prediction and aversion by anterior cingulate cortex. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 266–277. 10.3758/CABN.7.4.266 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke B. L., Martens A., Faucher E. H. (2010). Two decades of terror management theory: a meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 14, 155–195. 10.1177/1088868309352321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carver C. S., White T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment. J. Pers. Soc. Psych. 67, 319–333. [Google Scholar]
- Cavallo J. V., Fitzsimons G. M., Holmes J. G. (2009). Taking chances in the face of threat: romantic risk regulation and approach motivation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 35, 737–751. 10.1177/0146167209332742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi B., Jo S. (2013). A low-cost EEG system-based hybrid brain-computer interface for humanoid robot navigation and recognition. PLoS ONE 8:e74583. 10.1371/journal.pone.0074583 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark L., Li R., Wright C. M., Rome F., Fairchild G., Dunn B. D., et al. (2012). Risk-avoidant decision making increased by threat of electric shock. Psychophysiology 49, 1436–1443. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01454.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coan J. A., Allen J. J. B. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biol. Psychol. 67, 7–50. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corr P. J. (2004). Reinforcement sensitivity theory and personality. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 317–332. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corr P. J., DeYoung C. G., McNaughton N. (2013a). Motivation and personality: a neuropsychological perspective. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 7, 158–175. 10.1111/spc3.12016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Corr P. J., Hargreaves-Heap S., Tsutsui K., Russell A., Seger C. (2013b). Personality and social attitudes: evidence for positive-approach motivation. Pers. Individ. Diff. 55, 846–851. 10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cox C. R., Goldenberg J. L., Pyszczynski T., Weise D. (2007). Disgust, creatureliness and the accessibility of death-related thoughts. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 494–507. 10.1002/ejsp.370 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dalton K. M., Kalin N. H., Grist T. M., Davidson R. J. (2005). Neural-cardiac coupling in threat-evoked anxiety. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 969–980. 10.1162/0898929054021094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Das E., Bushman B. J., Bezemer M. D., Kerkhof P., Vermeulen I. E. (2009). How terrorism news reports increase prejudice against outgroups: a terror management account. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 453–459. 10.1016/j.jesp.2008.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson R. J., Marshall J. R., Tomarken A. J., Henriques J. B. (2000). While a phobic waits: regional brain electrical and autonomic activity in social phobics during anticipation of public speaking. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 85–95. 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00222-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Debener S., Minow F., Emkes R., Gandras K., de Vos M. (2012). How about taking a low-cost, small, and wireless EEG for a walk? Psychophysiology 49, 1617–1621. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01471.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dechesne M., Janssen J., van Knippenberg A. (2000). Derogation and distancing as terror management strategies: the moderating role of need for closure and permeability of group boundaries. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 923–932. 10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.923 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vos M., Gandras K., Debener S. (2014). Towards a truly mobile auditory brain–computer interface: Exploring the P300 to take away. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 91, 46–53. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drake R. A., Myers L. (2006). Visual attention, emotion, and action tendency: feeling active or passive. Cogn. Emot. 20, 608–622. 10.1080/0269993050036810527723477 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Drake R. A., Ulrich G. (1992). Line bisecting as a predictor of personal optimism and desirability of risky behaviors. Acta Psychol. 79, 219–226. 10.1016/0001-6918(92)90058-L [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliot A. J. (2006). The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance motivation. Motiv. Emot. 30, 111–116. 10.1007/s11031-006-9028-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Elliot A. J., Reis H. T. (2003). Attachment and exploration in adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 317–331. 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.317 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engels A. S., Heller W., Spielberg J. M., Warren S. L., Sutton B. P., Banich M. T., et al. (2010). Co-occurring anxiety influences patterns of brain activity in depression. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 141–156. 10.3758/CABN.10.1.141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erdle S., Rushton J. P. (2010). The general factor of personality, BIS–BAS, expectancies of reward and punishment, self-esteem, and positive and negative affect. Pers. Individ. Diff. 48, 762–766. 10.1016/j.paid.2010.01.025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman R. S., Förster J. (2005). Effects of motivational cues on perceptual asymmetry: implications for creativity and analytical problem solving. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 263–275. 10.1037/0022-3514.88.2.263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritsche I., Jonas E., Fankhänel T. (2008). The role of control motivation in mortality salience effects on ingroup support and defense. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 524–541. 10.1037/a0012666 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg J. L., Hart J., Pyszczynski T., Warnica G. M., Landau M., Thomas L. (2006a). Ambivalence toward the body: death, neuroticism, and the flight from physical sensation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 1264–1277. 10.1177/0146167206289505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg J. L., Heflick N. A., Cooper D. P. (2008). The thrust of the problem: bodily inhibitions and guilt as a function of mortality salience and neuroticism. J. Pers. 76, 1055–1080. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00513.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg J. L., Kosloff S., Greenberg J. (2006b). Existential underpinnings of approach and avoidance of the physical body. Motiv. Emot. 30, 127–134. 10.1007/s11031-006-9023-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg J. L., Pyszczynski T., Greenberg J., Solomon S. (2000). Fleeing the body: a terror management perspective on the problem of human corporeality. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 4, 200–218. 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0403_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goldenberg J. L., Pyszczynski T., Greenberg J., Solomon S., Kluck B., Cornwell R. (2001). I am not an animal: mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human creatureliness. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 427–435. 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golec de Zavala A., Cichocka A., Orehek E., Abdollahi A. (2012). Intrinsic religiosity reduces intergroup hostility under mortality salience. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42, 451–461. 10.1002/ejsp.1843 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gray J. A., McNaughton N. (2000). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg J., Martens A., Jonas E., Eisenstadt D., Pyszczynski T., Solomon S. (2003). Psychological defense in anticipation of anxiety. Psychol. Sci. 14, 516–519. 10.1111/1467-9280.03454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg J., Pyszczynski T., Solomon S. (1995a). Toward a dual-motive depth psychology of self and human behavior, in Efficacy, Agency, and Self-Esteem, ed Kernis M. H. (New York, NY: Plenum; ), 73–99. [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg J., Simon L., Harmon-Jones E., Solomon S., Pyszczynski T., Lyon D. (1995b). Testing alternative explanations for mortality salience effects: terror management, value accessibility, or worrisome thoughts? Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 25, 417–433. 10.1002/ejsp.242025040619570581 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Green J. D., Campbell W. K. (2000). Attachment and exploration in adults: chronic and contextual accessibility. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 452–461. 10.1177/0146167200266004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grimshaw G. M., Foster J. J., Corballis P. M. (2014). Frontal and parietal EEG asymmetries interact to predict attentional bias to threat. Brain Cogn. 90, 76–86. 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmon-Jones E., Peterson C. K., Harris C. R. (2009). Jealousy: novel methods and neural correlates. Emotion 9, 113–117. 10.1037/a0014117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart J. (2014). Toward an integrative theory of psychological defense. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 19–39. 10.1177/1745691613506018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart J., Shaver P. R., Goldenberg J. L. (2005). Attachment, self-esteem, worldviews, and terror management: evidence for a tripartite security system. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 999–1013. 10.1037/0022-3514.88.6.999 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis a Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Holbrook C., Sousa P., Hahn-Holbrook J. (2011). Unconscious vigilance: worldview defense without adaptations for terror, coalition, or uncertainty management. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 451–466. 10.1037/a0024033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang Z. (Tak)., Wyer R. S., Jr. (2015). Diverging effects of mortality salience on variety seeking: the different roles of death anxiety and semantic concept activation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 58, 112–123. 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.01.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jewell G., McCourt M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia 38, 93–110. 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00045-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson P. O., Neyman J. (1936). Tests of certain linear hypotheses and their application to some educational problems. Stat. Res. Mem. 1, 57–93. [Google Scholar]
- Jonas E., McGregor I., Klackl J., Agroskin D., Fritsche I., Holbrook C., et al. (2014). Threat and defense: from anxiety to approach, in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 49, eds Olson J. M., Zanna M. P. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 219–286. [Google Scholar]
- Juhl J., Routledge C. (2010). Structured terror: further exploring the effects of mortality salience and personal need for structure on worldview defense. J. Pers. 78, 969–990. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00639.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kashdan T. B., Rose P., Fincham F. D. (2004). Curiosity and exploration: facilitating positive subjective experiences and personal growth opportunities. J. Pers. Assess. 82, 291–305. 10.1207/s15327752jpa8203_05 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenrick D. T., Griskevicius V., Neuberg S. L., Schaller M. (2010). Renovating the pyramid of needs contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5, 292–314. 10.1177/1745691610369469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khushaba R. N., Wise C., Kodagoda S., Louviere J., Kahn B. E., Townsend C. (2013). Consumer neuroscience: assessing the brain response to marketing stimuli using electroencephalogram (EEG) and eye tracking. Expert Syst. Appl. 40, 3803–3812. 10.1016/j.eswa.2012.12.095 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Landau M. J., Greenberg J. (2006). Play it safe or go for the gold? A terror management perspective on self-enhancement and self-protective motives in risky decision making. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 1633–1645. 10.1177/0146167206292017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau M. J., Greenberg J., Solomon S., Martens A., Pyszczynski T. (2006). Windows into nothingness: terror management, meaninglessness, and negative reactions to modern art. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 879–892. 10.1037/0022-3514.90.6.879 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau M. J., Johns M., Greenberg J., Pyszczynski T., Martens A., Goldenberg J. L., et al. (2004). A function of form: terror management and structuring the social world. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 190–210. 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Litt A., Reich T., Maymin S., Shiv B. (2011). Pressure and perverse flights to familiarity. Psychol. Sci. 22, 523–531. 10.1177/0956797611400095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louwerse M., Hutchinson S. (2012). Neurological evidence linguistic processes precede perceptual simulation in conceptual processing. Front. Cogn. Sci. 3:385. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00385 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maner J. K., Schmidt N. B. (2006). The role of risk avoidance in anxiety. Behav. Ther. 37, 181–189. 10.1016/j.beth.2005.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maner J. K., Richey J. A., Cromer K., Mallott M., Lejuez C. W., Joiner T. E., et al. (2007). Dispositional anxiety and risk-avoidant decision-making. Pers. Individ. Diff. 42, 665–675. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews G. (2008). Reinforcement sensitivity theory: a critique from cognitive science, in The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality, ed Corr P. J. (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; ), 482–507. [Google Scholar]
- Mayaud L., Congedo M., Van Laghenhove A., Orlikowski D., Figère M., Azabou E., et al. (2013). A comparison of recording modalities of P300 event-related potentials (ERP) for brain-computer interface (BCI) paradigm. Neurophysiol. Clin. 43, 217–227. 10.1016/j.neucli.2013.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEachron D. L., Baer D. (1982). A review of selected sociobiological principles: application to hominid evolution: II. The effects of intergroup conflict. J. Soc. Biol. Struct. 5, 121–139. 10.1016/S0140-1750(82)91789-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor I., Nash K., Mann N., Phills C. E. (2010). Anxious uncertainty and reactive approach motivation (RAM). J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 133–147. 10.1037/a0019701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor I., Prentice M. S., Nash K. A. (2009). Personal uncertainty management by reactive approach motivation. Psychol. Inq. 20, 225–229. 10.1080/10478400903333460 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McGregor I., Prentice M., Nash K. (2013). Anxious uncertainty and reactive approach motivation (RAM) for religious, idealistic, and lifestyle extremes. J. Soc. Issues 69, 537–563. 10.1111/josi.12028 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McNaughton N., Corr P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 285–305. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNaughton N., Corr P. J. (2014). Approach, avoidance, and their conflict: the problem of anchoring. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:124. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNaughton N., Swart C., Neo P., Bates V., Glue P. (2013). Anti-anxiety drugs reduce conflict-specific theta—A possible human anxiety-specific biomarker. J. Affect. Dis. 148, 104–111. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.11.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mortensen C. R., Becker D. V., Ackerman J. M., Neuberg S. L., Kenrick D. T. (2010). Infection breeds reticence the effects of disease salience on self-perceptions of personality and behavioral avoidance tendencies. Psychol. Sci. 21, 440–447. 10.1177/0956797610361706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén L. K., Muthén B. O. (2012). Mplus User's Guide, Vol. 7 Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén. [Google Scholar]
- Nash K., Inzlicht M., McGregor I. (2012). Approach-related left prefrontal EEG asymmetry predicts muted error-related negativity. Biol. Psychol. 91, 96–102. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nash K., McGregor I., Inzlicht M. (2010). Line bisection as a neural marker of approach motivation. Psychophysiology 47, 979–983. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00999.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nash K., McGregor I., Prentice M. (2011). Threat and defense as goal regulation: from implicit goal conflict to anxious uncertainty, reactive approach motivation, and ideological extremism. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 1291–1301. 10.1037/a0025944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navarrete C. D., Kurzban R., Fessler D. M. T., Kirkpatrick L. A. (2004). Anxiety and intergroup bias: terror management or coalitional psychology? Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 7, 370–397. 10.1177/136843020404614423280148 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Neuberg S. L., Newsom J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: individual differences in the desire for simple structure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 113–131. 10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.113 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Neuberg S. L., Nicole T., West S. G. (1997). What the need for closure scale measures and what it does not: toward differentiating among related epistemic motives. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 1396–1412. 10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1396 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Oldfield R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Regan S., Faul S., Marnane W. (2013). Automatic detection of EEG artefacts arising from head movements using EEG and gyroscope signals. Med. Eng. Phys. 35, 867–874. 10.1016/j.medengphy.2012.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Regan S., Marnane W. (2013). Multimodal detection of head-movement artefacts in EEG. J. Neurosci. Methods 218, 110–120. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Edgar K., Kujawa A., Nelson S. K., Cole C., Zapp D. J. (2013). The relation between electroencephalogram asymmetry and attention biases to threat at baseline and under stress. Brain Cogn. 82, 337–343. 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins A. M., Corr P. J. (2014). Anxiety as an adaptive emotion, in The Positive Side of Negative Emotions, ed Parrott W. G. (New York, NY: Guilford Press; ), 37–54. [Google Scholar]
- Perkins A. M., Kemp S. E., Corr P. J. (2007). Fear and anxiety as separable emotions: an investigation of the revised reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality. Emotion 7, 252–261. 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proulx T., Heine S. J. (2010). The frog in Kierkegaard's beer: finding meaning in the threat-compensation literature. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 4, 889–905. 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00304.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pyszczynski T., Greenberg J., Arndt J. (2012). Freedom versus fear revisited: an integrated analysis of the dynamics of the defense and growth of self, in Handbook of Self and Identity, 2nd Edn., eds Leary M. R., Tangney J. P. (New York, NY: Guilford Press; ), 378–404. [Google Scholar]
- Pyszczynski T., Greenberg J., Solomon S. (1997). Why do we need what we need? A terror management perspective on the roots of human social motivation. Psychol. Inq. 8, 1–20. 10.1207/s15327965pli0801_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pyszczynski T., Greenberg J., Solomon S. (1999). A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: an extension of terror management theory. Psychol. Rev. 106, 835–845. 10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.835 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pyszczynski T., Greenberg J., Solomon S. (2000). Toward a dialectical analysis of growth and defensive motives. Psychol. Inq. 11, 301–305. [Google Scholar]
- Ravaja N., Somervuori O., Salminen M. (2013). Predicting purchase decision: the role of hemispheric asymmetry over the frontal cortex. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 6, 1–13. 10.1037/a0029949 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rheingold J. C. (1967). The Mother, Anxiety, and Death: The Catastrophic Death Complex. Boston, MA: Little, Brown. [Google Scholar]
- Rosenblatt A., Greenberg J., Solomon S., Pyszczynski T., Lyon D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory I: the effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 681–690. 10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.681 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Routledge C., Ostafin B., Juhl J., Sedikides C., Cathey C., Liao J. (2010). Adjusting to death: the effects of mortality salience and self-esteem on psychological well-being, growth motivation, and maladaptive behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 897–916. 10.1037/a0021431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schimel J., Simon L., Greenberg J., Pyszczynski T., Solomon S., Waxmonsky J., et al. (1999). Stereotypes and terror management: evidence that mortality salience enhances stereotypic thinking and preferences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 77, 905–926. 10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlink S., Walther E. (2007). Kurz und gut: eine deutsche Kurzskala zur Erfassung des Bedürfnisses nach kognitiver Geschlossenheit [Short and good: a German short scale for the measurement of the need for cogntive closure]. Z. Sozialpsychol. 38, 153–161. 10.1024/0044-3514.38.3.153 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schumann K., McGregor I., Nash K. A., Ross M. (2014). Religious magnanimity: reminding people of their religious belief system reduces hostility after threat. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 107, 432–453. 10.1037/a0036739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shackman A. J., McMenamin B. W., Maxwell J. S., Greischar L. L., Davidson R. J. (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1500–1506. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shackman A. J., Salomons T. V., Slagter H. A., Fox A. S., Winter J. J., Davidson R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154–167. 10.1038/nrn2994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shah J. Y., Kruglanski A. W., Thompson E. P. (1998). Membership has its (epistemic) rewards: need for closure effects on in-group bias. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 383–393. 10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shrout P. E., Bolger N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. 10.1037/1082-989X.7.4.422 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sligte D. J., Nijstad B. A., De Dreu C. K. W. (2013). Leaving a legacy neutralizes negative effects of death anxiety on creativity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39, 1152–1163. 10.1177/0146167213490804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smits D. J. M., Boeck P. D. (2006). From BIS/BAS to the big five. Eur. J. Pers. 20, 255–270. 10.1002/per.583 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer S. J., Zanna M. P., Fong G. T. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 845–851. 10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberg J. M., Heller W., Miller G. A. (2013). Hierarchical brain networks active in approach and avoidance goal pursuit. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:284. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00284 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinhubl S. R., Wineinger N. E., Patel S., Boeldt D. L., Mackellar G., Porter V., et al. (2015). Cardiovascular and nervous system changes during meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:145. 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stopczynski A., Stahlhut C., Larsen J. E., Petersen M. K., Hansen L. K. (2014a). The smartphone brain scanner: a portable real-time neuroimaging system. PLoS ONE 9:e86733. 10.1371/journal.pone.0086733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stopczynski A., Stahlhut C., Petersen M. K., Larsen J. E., Jensen C. F., Ivanova M. G., et al. (2014b). Smartphones as pocketable labs: visions for mobile brain imaging and neurofeedback. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 91, 54–66. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strachan E., Schimel J., Arndt J., Williams T., Solomon S., Pyszczynski T., et al. (2007). Terror mismanagement: Evidence that mortality salience exacerbates phobic and compulsive behaviors. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 1137–1151. 10.1177/0146167207303018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strobel A., Beauducel A., Debener S., Brocke B. (2001). Eine deutschsprachige Version des BIS/BAS-Fragebogens von Carver und White. Z. Diff. Diagnostische Psychol. 22, 216–227. 10.1024//0170-1789.22.3.216 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton S. K., Davidson R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: a biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychol. Sci. 8, 204–210. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00413.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thibodeau R., Jorgensen R. S., Kim S. (2006). Depression, anxiety, and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: a meta-analytic review. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 715–729. 10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.715 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trafimow D., Hughes J. S. (2012). Testing the death thought suppression and rebound hypothesis: death thought accessibility following mortality salience decreases during a delay. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 3, 622–629. 10.1177/1948550611432938 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tritt S. M., Inzlicht M., Harmon-Jones E. (2012). Toward a biological understanding of mortality salience (and other threat compensation processes). Soc. Cogn. 30, 715–733. 10.1521/soco.2012.30.6.715 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vail K. E., III, Juhl J., Arndt J., Vess M., Routledge C., Rutjens B. T. (2012). When death is good for life considering the positive trajectories of terror management. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 303–329. 10.1177/1088868312440046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G., Rogers L. J. (2005). Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 575–589. 10.1017/S0140525X05000105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Bos K., Müller P. A., van Bussel A. A. L. (2009). Helping to overcome intervention inertia in bystander's dilemmas: behavioral disinhibition can improve the greater good. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 873–878. 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- van den Bos K., Poortvliet P. M., Maas M., Miedema J., van den Ham E.-J. (2005). An enquiry concerning the principles of cultural norms and values: the impact of uncertainty and mortality salience on reactions to violations and bolstering of cultural worldviews. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 91–113. 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vess M., Routledge C., Landau M. J., Arndt J. (2009). The dynamics of death and meaning: the effects of death-relevant cognitions and personal need for structure on perceptions of meaning in life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 728–744. 10.1037/a0016417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vourvopoulos A., Liarokapis F. (2014). Evaluation of commercial brain–computer interfaces in real and virtual world environment: a pilot study. Comput. Electrical Eng. 40, 714–729. 10.1016/j.compeleceng.2013.10.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wacker J., Chavanon M.-L., Leue A., Stemmler G. (2008). Is running away right? The behavioral activation-behavioral inhibition model of anterior asymmetry. Emotion 8, 232–249. 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wacker J., Chavanon M.-L., Leue A., Stemmler G. (2010). Trait BIS predicts alpha asymmetry and P300 in a Go/No-Go task. Eur. J. Pers. 24, 85–105. 10.1002/per.740 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Watson D., Clark L. A., Tellegen A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webster D. M., Kruglanski A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 1049–1062. 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson D., Guinote A., Weick M., Molinari R., Graham K. (2010). Feeling socially powerless makes you more prone to bumping into things on the right and induces leftward line bisection error. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 17, 910–914. 10.3758/PBR.17.6.910 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]