Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2016 Apr 28;42(12):1839–1866. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000281

Semantic and Plausibility Preview Benefit Effects in English: Evidence from Eye Movements

Elizabeth R Schotter 1, Annie Jia 1
PMCID: PMC5085893  NIHMSID: NIHMS773841  PMID: 27123754

Abstract

Theories of preview benefit in reading hinge on integration across saccades and the idea that preview benefit is greater the more similar the preview and target are. Schotter (2013) reported preview benefit from a synonymous preview, but it is unclear whether this effect occurs because of similarity between the preview and target (integration), or because of contextual fit of the preview—synonyms satisfy both accounts. Studies in Chinese have found evidence for preview benefit for words that are unrelated to the target, but are contextually plausible (Yang, Li, Wang, Slattery, & Rayner, 2014; Yang, Wang, Tong, & Rayner, 2012), which is incompatible with an integration account but supports a contextual fit account. Here, we used plausible and implausible unrelated previews in addition to plausible synonym, antonym, and identical previews to further investigate these accounts for readers of English. Early reading measures were shorter for all plausible preview conditions compared to the implausible preview condition. In later reading measures, a benefit for the plausible unrelated preview condition was not observed. In a second experiment, we asked questions that probed whether the reader encoded the preview or target. Readers were more likely to report the preview when they had skipped the word and not regressed to it, and when the preview was plausible. Thus, under certain circumstances, the preview word is processed to a high level of representation (i.e., semantic plausibility) regardless of its relationship to the target, but its influence on reading is relatively short-lived, being replaced by the target word, when fixated.


A key question in reading research regards the extent to which information about words obtained from parafoveal vision influences reading behavior. It is clear from over four decades of research, since the introduction of the gaze-contingent boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975) and moving window paradigm (McConkie & Rayner, 1976), that parafoveal information is important for reading efficiency. In the moving window paradigm, the letters around the fixation location are revealed while letters beyond this visible window are masked and the size of the window is varied across experimental conditions. The general finding from these studies is that reading rate decreases as the size of the visible window becomes smaller (see Rayner, 2014 for a review). In the boundary paradigm, a particular target word in the sentence is selected to be manipulated: while the reader's eyes fixate parts of the sentence prior to that location the word is replaced by another preview stimulus (e.g., another word, a nonword, or a string of x's) and is only revealed when the reader makes an eye movement to fixate or skip over that location. The general finding from these studies is that fixation durations on the target word show a preview benefit—are shorter when the target was available as a parafoveal preview (i.e., in the identical preview condition) compared to when the reader had a parafoveal preview of a different stimulus, and there are moderate benefits for words that are not identical to the target but are similar to it in some way (e.g., visually, orthographically, phonologically; see Schotter, Angele, & Rayner, 2012 for a review).

Despite the large amounts of evidence that parafoveal information is important for reading efficiency, it is still not entirely clear how that information is used in the reading process. That is, initial explanations of parafoveal preview benefit invoked the idea of trans-saccadic integration—processing of the preview facilitates processing of the target in that, to the extent that they are similar, the access of properties of the preview allowed for a head-start on processing of the target. For example, Rayner (1975, p. 80) concluded that across saccades, “information from the two fixations is integrated… when visual or semantic discrepancies were introduced between two successive fixations, this integration failed.” Subsequently, Pollatsek, Lesch, Morris, and Rayner (1992, p. 159) concluded that word properties (i.e., phonology) are “used to preserve the `memory' of a word from one fixation to aid in its identification on the next fixation.” Thus, critical to an explanation by means of integration is the idea that greater similarity between preview and target (i.e., the more their orthographic, phonological, and/or semantic representations overlap) the greater the magnitude of preview benefit.

Initial investigations into preview benefit found data compatible with an integration account and lead to the general conclusion that semantic information was not used for integration. That is, there were several studies across many different languages demonstrating preview benefit from orthographically similar previews (Balota, Pollatsek, & Rayner, 1985; Briihl & Inhoff, 1995; Drieghe, Rayner, & Pollatsek, 2005; Inhoff, 1987, 1989a, 1989b, 1990; Inhoff & Tousman, 1990; Lima & Inhoff, 1985; Rayner, 1975; White, Johnson, Liversedge, & Rayner, 2008) and phonologically similar previews (Ashby & Rayner, 2004; Ashby, Treiman, Kessler, & Rayner, 2006; Chace, Rayner, & Well, 2005; Liu, Inhoff, Ye, & Wu, 2002; Miellet & Sparrow, 2004; Pollatsek et al., 1992; Rayner, Sereno, Lesch, & Pollatsek, 1995; Sparrow & Miellet, 2002; Tsai, Lee, Tzeng, Hung, & Yen, 2004). In contrast, initial investigations did not find any evidence for semantic preview benefit in English (Rayner, Balota, & Pollatsek, 1986, see Rayner, Schotter, & Drieghe, 2014 for a replication), but more recent studies in German and Chinese did find evidence for it (Hohenstein & Kliegl, 2014; Hohenstein, Laubrock, & Kliegl, 2010; Yan, Richter, Shu, & Kliegl, 2009; Yan, Zhou, Shu, & Kliegl, 2012; Yang, 2013; Yang, Wang, Tong, & Rayner, 2012). These differences in the findings were explained in terms of cross-language differences (see Laubrock & Hohenstein, 2012; Schotter, 2013, Schotter et al., 2012), until recent studies showed semantic preview benefit in English when the preview was a synonym of the target (e.g., start-begin: Schotter, 2013; see also Schotter, Lee, Reiderman, & Rayner, 2015). The suggested explanation for synonym preview benefit in English was attributed to the similarity of meaning between the two words (see discussions by Schotter, 2013; Rayner & Schotter, 2014) implying that meaning is what was being integrated between preview and target.

In subsequent work, Schotter et al. (2015) also found preview benefit for non-synonymous semantically related words (e.g., ready-begin) when they were embedded in semantically constraining contexts. Importantly, the constraining contexts used by Schotter et al., were not high cloze in the traditional sense in which one word form was very expected at the location of the preview/target. That is, in a modified cloze norming task (see Taylor, 1953 for the original task) in which subjects read the sentence fragment leading up to the target/preview location and had to fill in a word they thought could come next, the proportion of responses that corresponded to the target, synonym, and semantically related word were .21, .25, and .00, respectively. On traditional views of predictability, cloze values of around 25% are considered medium to low constraint (Rayner & Well, 1996). However, as Schotter et al. (2015) note, their sentences were constraining in the sense that a general meaning was highly expected, but that meaning could manifest itself as a variety of word forms, in particular both the target and synonym (i.e., the cloze probability for the shared meaning of the target, synonym preview, and other similar words was .75). They suggested that the sentence context generated expectations that whatever word would come next should be easily identifiable and, to the extent that that upcoming word had a meaning related to the event being described (e.g., the target, synonym, or a semantically related word), that preview facilitated processing and led to preview benefit. Indeed, the issue of how sentence contexts lead to linguistic prediction and how such prediction should be measured is an open area of research; for example, see preface (Hauk, 2016) to a recent special issue on prediction and language comprehension in Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, and various articles within it, as well as DeLong, Troyer, & Kutas, (2014; Schotter et al., 2015). As will be discussed below, the sentence constraint used in the current studies is slightly different than that used by Schotter et al. (2015).

From Schotter's (2013; Schotter et al., 2015) data it is unclear whether the preview benefit she observed was because of similarity between the preview and target or was due to compatibility between the preview and the sentence context. Importantly, because synonyms mean the same thing or are very similar in meaning they should fit approximately as well in the same sentence context (e.g., “The horse race will start/begin in a couple of minutes.”). Thus, an integration account and a contextual fit account can make divergent predictions. An integration account predicts that a word that is neither orthographically, phonologically, or semantically similar to the target but still fits into the sentence context should not provide preview benefit, whereas a contextual fit account predicts that such a condition should provide preview benefit. Most prior studies have not been able to address this issue because the focus has been on the relationship between the preview and target, rather than the preview and the sentence context. That is, many prior studies have not assessed the plausibility of the preview in the sentence context and many have used nonword previews, which by definition are implausible. Furthermore, the data from Schotter et al. (2015; Rayner et al., 1986, 2014; Schotter, 2013) cannot dissociate target-preview similarity from preview-context compatibility because the semantically related but non-synonymous conditions were not controlled for whether they were contextually plausible or implausible; for example, Schotter (2013) reports that, of the semantically associated previews, 17% were semantically anomalous and 13% were syntactically anomalous.

Although not designed to adjudicate between the integration and contextual fit accounts, two recent studies on reading in Chinese report patterns of data that are more compatible with the contextual fit account (Yang, Li, Wang, Slattery, & Rayner, 2014; Yang et al., 2012). Yang et al. (2012) found no benefit in reading time on the target for a semantically related preview relative to an unrelated preview when both those previews were implausible in the sentence context (Experiment 1), but they did find preview benefit for unrelated previews that were plausible in the sentence context compared to unrelated previews that were implausible (Experiment 2). Yang et al. (2014) conducted a study to determine whether the plausibility preview benefit was due to readers initially encoding the preview word rather than encoding the target word. Their study involved three preview conditions: (1) an identical word that was always plausible in the sentence context, (2) an unrelated word that was initially plausible in the preceding context but became implausible upon encountering the rest of the sentence and, (3) an unrelated word that was always implausible. They found that early reading time measures showed faster processing for the identical and initially plausible conditions compared to the implausible condition, replicating the effect of preview plausibility. However, they did not find evidence for a larger penalty to later reading (i.e., no more regressions or longer rereading times) in the initially plausible compared to the always implausible condition. They argue that these data challenge the idea that readers had encoded the initially plausible preview word (i.e., rather than the target). That is, had readers encoded and maintained the meaning of the initially plausible preview they would have eventually become confused when the sentence subsequently became implausible and would have spent more time rereading in that condition relative to the implausible condition, where readers would have been more likely to discard the implausible preview and encode the always plausible target word instead.

The results from the studies conducted by Yang and colleagues suggest that readers do obtain semantic information from a parafoveal preview because the plausibility of that word has an effect on initial reading measures on the target, but the effect is short-lived in that it does not appear in later reading measures where there is no difference between conditions with an unrelated preview that is initially plausible compared to always implausible. However, there are several crucial differences between Chinese and English that make it important to test whether a similar pattern would also be observed in English. Importantly, many of these differences have been argued to make it more likely to observe semantic preview benefit in Chinese than in English (e.g., Schotter, 2013; Yang et al., 2012). First, because many Chinese characters have radicals that encode semantics in the orthography, meaning may more readily be accessed directly from a parafoveal word. Second, because there are no spaces between characters in Chinese, upcoming words are closer to the fovea and may be processed more efficiently than upcoming words in alphabetic languages, due to higher acuity. Third, because of the lack of spaces, readers may need to allocate more attention to both the sentence context and parafoveal characters in order to properly segment characters into words so that they can properly program an eye movement to land in an optimal viewing location, which could also encourage deeper encoding of upcoming words (Yan, Kliegl, Richter, Nuthmann, & Shu, 2010; see also Li, Liu, & Rayner, 2015).

In the current studies we sought to conduct a study in English that was similar to those of Yang and colleagues, comparing unrelated preview words that were manipulated to be plausible in the sentence context or implausible. In addition, we included a comparison between synonym preview benefit and antonym preview benefit in Experiment 1 (as well as the corresponding identical, plausible unrelated and implausible unrelated conditions for the sentences in each stimulus set). In Experiment 2 we focus on the antonym set only in order to investigate whether readers had encoded the preview or target word by asking two-alternative forced choice comprehension questions after every sentence with response options that correspond to those two words.

The synonym condition is a replication of Schotter (2013; Schotter et al., 2015: e.g., used the same synonym pairs, award-prize, but with different sentences to accommodate the other preview conditions used here). Schotter (2013; Schotter et al., 2015) showed preview benefit for synonyms that was almost as large as the identical preview benefit. However, synonyms satisfy the assumptions of both accounts mentioned above in that the meanings of the two words are identical or very similar and, thus, both words are plausible in the sentence context. Therefore, if we were to assess which of those words (i.e., the target or its synonymous preview) the reader had encoded the reader might think those words were interchangeable. In contrast, antonyms (e.g., loose-tight) satisfy the constraints of the contextual fit account in that both words in the antonym pair fit well in similar contexts (for the most part in general, and always in the experiments reported below), but they do not satisfy all the assumptions of the integration account. That is, the antonym condition shares many properties to the synonym (i.e., sharing part of speech, and many semantic features), but should be less able to be integrated with the target because it represents the opposite meaning from the target whereas the synonym completely shares meaning with the target. It could be argued that antonyms share all features, except that one of them is flipped (e.g., the words large and small are both adjectives, regarding the size of an entity with the only difference being that large has a positive relative magnitude while small has a negative relative magnitude; see Hutchinson, 2003). A detailed investigation of these issues is beyond the scope of the current paper, but the antonym condition will be an important feature of Experiment 2, which assesses the reader's interpretation of the sentence (i.e., whether it includes the preview or the target) and that is easier to do by comparing antonyms than synonyms.

Finally, as noted above, we also included a condition in which the preview was completely unrelated to the target word, but was plausible in the sentence context (e.g., party-prize in the sentence “Dale's company gave him a huge party/prize for his hard work.”), replicating conditions used by Yang and colleagues, but for readers of English. If these words pattern differently from the synonym condition it would lend support for an integration account, but if they pattern similarly it would challenge such an account and support a contextual fit account.

Related to the use of the plausible unrelated preview condition, the present studies required a sentence constraint manipulation that was slightly different than that used by Schotter et al. (2015). That is, because the sentences needed to allow for the target word, synonyms of that word, as well as antonyms or a completely unrelated word, the sentence contexts necessarily have to be less constraining to a particular meaning than the 75% meaning constraint used by Schotter et al. (2015). Therefore, the cloze probability for the preview word forms in the current study (i.e., <5%) is lower than those reported by Schotter et al. (i.e., 15–35%), and the sentences used here did not converge onto one meaning to the same extent that theirs did (see normative data below).

In these studies, we used the boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975) and focused on various preview conditions that allow us to investigate the degree to which fixation durations on a fixated word are influenced by the relationship between the parafoveal preview and foveal target information or whether the contextual fit of the preview word can facilitate reading in the absence of meaning similarity between preview and target. As is standard in boundary studies, we included an identical preview condition and an unrelated (i.e., implausible random word) preview condition, which represent two baselines with which to compare the other two preview conditions. The identical condition represents the case in which all the information is the same between preview and target and should afford the greatest opportunity for integration across saccades. The unrelated random word preview was always implausible in the sentence (i.e., syntactically or semantically anomalous) and represents the least easy to process condition, both in terms of a lack of similarity to the target and incompatibility with the sentence context. We also included a condition in which the preview was unrelated to the target word, but a plausible word in the sentence; the comparison between this condition and the implausible preview condition allows us to test the influence of the contextual fit of the preview word when it has no relationship to the target and extends the findings of Yang and colleagues to English. The final condition was either a synonym of the target (in 60% of the sentences) or an antonym of the target (in 40% of the sentences) in Experiment 1 to assess whether these conditions pattern similarly. In Experiment 2 we focus further on the antonym and plausible unrelated conditions in the set of antonym stimuli to ask specific questions about which word's meaning (i.e., the preview or the target) is ultimately represented in the reader's understanding of the sentence.

Experiment 1

Method

Subjects

Twenty-four undergraduates at the University of California, San Diego participated in the experiment for course credit. All were native English speakers, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naïve to the purpose of the experiment.

Apparatus

An SR Research Ltd. Eyelink 1000 eye tracker (with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz) was used to record the readers' eye movements. The tracker was used in the tower setup with forehead and chin rests, decreasing noise due to head movements. Viewing was binocular, but only the movements of the right eye were recorded. Subjects were seated approximately 60 cm away from a 20” HP p1230 CRT monitor with a screen resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels and a refresh rate of 150 Hz. The sentences were presented in the center of the screen with black Courier New 14-point font on a white background and were always presented in one line of text with 2.41 characters subtending 1 degree of visual angle. Following calibration, eye position errors were less than 0.3° at each calibration point. The display change was completed, on average, within 4 ms (range = 0–7 ms) of the tracker detecting a saccade crossing the boundary.

Materials and Design

Stimuli were created out of 140 pairs of target words. Of these pairs, 56 were antonyms (e.g., loose-tight) and 84 were synonyms (e.g., prize-award). Two sentence frames were created for each pair such that each member of the pair was the target (and identical preview) in its own sentence frame and the other member was used as the preview in the synonym/antonym preview condition (depending on the stimulus set). In addition to these two preview conditions we also included two more conditions that were the same across the two sentence frames/targets, a plausible unrelated word or an implausible unrelated word to create a total of 4 conditions for each target word (see Table 1a and Appendix A). Thus, each subject read a total of 280 sentences with unique target words one time each in one of 4 possible preview conditions (identical, antonym/synonym, plausible unrelated, implausible unrelated). Preview condition was counterbalanced across subjects and items in a Latin square design such that each target word was encountered only once by a given subject, but across subjects each target word was encountered in every condition. Because each version of the antonym/synonym pair was used as a target this meant that on one fourth of the trials (i.e., in the antonym/synonym preview condition) the preview word was the same as a target word from another trial. However, because the order of the trials was randomized independently for each subject and preview conditions were counterbalanced across subjects and items, this design feature is unlikely to affect the data in any systematic way. The target/preview word was always preceded and followed by a minimum of 2 words.

Table 1a.

Example stimuli used in Experiment 1. Target words are presented in boldface, but were presented normally in the experiments.

Example sentence with target word in boldface Antonym/Synonym Plausible Implausible
Synonym stimulus set
Dale's company gave him a huge prize for his hard work. award party weird
The kids were bored until the teacher offered a special award for doing well on a test. prize party weird
Antonym stimulus set
Ron's pants are still loose even after tailoring. tight short poles
The professor's coat is too tight for him. loose short poles

Normative Data

The stimuli were assessed via three norming procedures: two acceptability rating tasks (one for the sentence fragment up to and including the preview/target word and the other for the entire sentence) to assess whether the two versions of the sentence for each pair were equally sensible, and a cloze norming task to assess how predictable each of the preview/target word forms was, given the preceding sentence fragment. Results of the norming procedures and other characteristics of the stimuli are reported in Table 1b.

Table 1b.

Lexical characteristics of and normative data for target and preview words used in Experiment 1, presented separately by stimulus set. Standard errors are in parentheses.

Set Variable Preview Condition
Identical Antonym/Synonym Plausible Unrelated Implausible Unrelated
Antonym
 Length 5.46 (.13) 5.46 (.13) 5.46 (.13) 5.46 (.13)
 Log Frequency/mil (HAL) 1.72 (.07) 1.72 (.07) 1.42 (.08) 1.25 (.09)
 Acceptability Rating (fragment) 5.9 (.07) 5.7 (.10) 5.2 (.13) 2.2 (.07)
 Acceptability Rating (whole) 5.8 (.11) 5.5 (.07) 5.4 (.05) 2.0 (.10)
 Cloze Probability .04 (.01) .02 (.01) .03 (.01) .00 (.00)
Synonym
 Length 5.49 (.09) 5.49 (.09) 5.49 (.09) 5.49 (.09)
 Log Frequency/mil (HAL) 1.35 (.06) 1.35 (.06) 1.54 (.06) 1.56 (.06)
 Acceptability Rating (fragment) 5.9 (.05) 5.1 (.09) 5.3 (.08) 2.7 (.09)
 Acceptability Rating (whole) 5.2 (.08) 5.7 (.05) 5.5 (.08) 3.3 (.07)
 Cloze Probability .02 (.01) .02 (.01) .03 (.01) .00 (.00)

Forty-eight workers on Amazon's Mechanical Turk participated in one of two acceptability judgment tasks—one in which they rated the acceptability of the entire sentence, and one in which they rated the acceptability of the sentence fragment up to and including the preview/target word. In this task, subjects rated sentences or sentence fragments on a 7-point Likert scale to indicate how well they were written. The four versions of the sentence or fragment (i.e., including one of the four different preview words) were counterbalanced across four lists per task (i.e., sentence or fragment) so that each version of each sentence or fragment was rated by six independent raters and each rater saw a quarter of the stimuli in each condition (as well as 36 poorly written filler sentences or fragments). The data revealed that the words that were intended to be sensible in the sentence context were rated highly (5.1–5.9) whereas the implausible words were rated low (2.0–3.3).

A separate group of 14 subjects participated in a cloze norming task (adapted from Taylor, 1953) in which they were provided with the sentence fragment leading up to the target/preview word and were required to fill in a word they thought could come next. Seven subjects completed the task for one of the sentence frames of the pair and the other seven completed the task for the other sentence frame of the pair to avoid carry-over in the responses between pairs that contained sentence frames that were somewhat similar in meaning. These data revealed that the sentences were not constraining to the word forms of any of the previews or targets. The average cloze probabilities were .03, .02, .03, and .00 for the identical, synonym/antonym, plausible unrelated, and implausible unrelated preview, respectively. Following Schotter et al. (2015), we coded the responses to determine the predictability of the meaning shared by the target and synonym (and any other synonymous words) for the synonym stimulus set only because it was not possible to define the measure for antonyms or unrelated words. This measure revealed that the cloze probability for the meaning shared between the target and synonym was 11%, much lower than that for Schotter et al.'s sentences (75%).

Procedure

Subjects were instructed to read the sentences for comprehension and to respond to occasional comprehension questions, pressing the left or right trigger on the response controller to answer yes or no, respectively. At the start of the experiment (and during the experiment if calibration error was greater than .3 degrees of visual angle), the eye-tracker was calibrated with a 3-point calibration scheme. At the beginning of the experiment, subjects received five practice trials, each with a comprehension question, to allow them to become comfortable with the experimental procedure.

Each trial began with a fixation point in the center of the screen, which the subject was required to fixate until the experimenter started the trial. Then a fixation box appeared on the left side of the screen, located at the start of the sentence. Once a fixation was detected in this box, it disappeared and the sentence appeared. The sentence was presented on the screen until the subject pressed a button signaling they had completed reading the sentence. Following the boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975), the target replaced the preview once the subject's gaze crossed an invisible boundary located before the space before the target and took between 0 and 7 ms to complete. Subjects were instructed to look at a target sticker on the right side of the monitor beside the screen when they finished reading to prevent them from looking back to a word (in particular, the target, which was often located in the center of the sentence, near the location of the fixation point that started the next trial) as they pressed the button. Comprehension questions followed 72 (26%) of the sentences, requiring a “yes” or “no” response. Comprehension accuracy was very high (on average 91%). At the end of the experiment subjects were asked whether they noticed the screen flickering or words changing (i.e., display changes). The rate of display changes noticed was very low, between 0 and 15 trials (mean = 3.83), because subjects who reported more than this (an indication of equipment or experimenter error) were replaced without their data being analyzed, as is common in boundary paradigm experiments. The experimental session lasted approximately 40 minutes.

Results

Trials in which there was a blink or track loss on the target word during first pass reading were excluded (7.5% of the data), as were trials in which the display change was triggered by a saccade that landed to the left of the boundary or trials in which the display change was completed late (16% of the data). These data exclusions left 5335 trials (79% of the original data) available for analysis. Fixations longer than 800 ms were eliminated, as were fixations shorter than 80 ms, unless they were within one character space of a previous or subsequent fixation, in which case they were combined with that fixation. Gaze durations longer than 2,000 ms and total times longer than 4,000 ms were excluded (< 1% of fixations), as well as any fixation duration measures that were more than 2.5 standard deviations from the mean for that measure for that subject (1.4–2.2% of fixations across measures) because they are unlikely to represent normal reading (i.e., the subject may have been zoning out at the moment and had an unusually long fixation).

We report standard reading time measures (Rayner, 1998) used to investigate the time-course of word processing in reading, including first fixation duration (the duration of the first fixation on the word, regardless of how many fixations are made), single fixation duration (the duration of a fixation on a word when it is the only fixation on that word in first pass reading), gaze duration (the sum of all fixations on a word prior to leaving it, in any direction), go-past time (the sum of all fixations on a target word and all fixations on words to the left of it, starting from the first fixation on that word before going past the target word to the right), and total time (the sum of all fixations on a word, including time spent re-reading the word after a regression back to it). In addition, we analyzed three measures of fixation probability, including fixation probability (the probability that the target was fixated at least once during first-pass reading), probability of regressing out of the target (i.e., to reread words prior in the sentence), and probability of regressing into the target (i.e., from subsequent words in the sentence).

Data were analyzed using inferential statistics based on general(ized) linear mixed-effects models (GLMMs) with stimulus set entered with a centered contrast, preview condition entered with planned contrasts (see below), as well as the interaction between them entered as the fixed effects. In addition, we entered subjects and items as crossed random effects (see Baayen, Davidson, & Bates, 2008), using the maximal random effects structure: intercepts and slopes for both factors and the interaction for subjects, and intercepts and the preview contrasts for items (Barr, Levy, Scheepers & Tily, 2013)1. To assess whether there was a different pattern of effects between the antonym and synonym stimuli we entered stimulus set, centered, as a main effect and also its interaction with the preview contrasts. For the preview contrasts, the unrelated word was used as the baseline condition and three planned preview contrasts tested for preview benefit in the three other preview conditions: one tested the difference between the identical condition and the implausible unrelated condition (i.e., an identical preview benefit), another tested for a difference between the synonym/antonym condition and the implausible unrelated condition (i.e., a synonym/antonym preview benefit), and the third tested for a difference between the plausible unrelated condition and the implausible unrelated condition (i.e., a plausibility preview benefit).

In order to fit the LMMs, we used the lmer function from the lme4 package (version 1.1-8; Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015) within the R Environment for Statistical Computing (R Development Core Team, 2015). For fixation duration measures, we report linear mixed-effects regressions on the raw data: regression coefficients (b), which estimate the effect size (in milliseconds) of the reported comparison, standard error, and the (absolute) t-value of the effect coefficient. Log-transforming the dependent variable had almost no effect on the patterns of significance, so for transparency we report the results from the untransformed models and note when the results from the models on transformed data differed. For binary dependent variables (fixation probability data), logistic mixed-effects regression models were used, and regression coefficients (b), which represent effect size in log-odds space, and the (absolute) z value and p value of the effect coefficient are reported. Absolute values of the t and z statistics greater than or equal to 1.96 indicate an effect that is significant at approximately the .05 alpha level. Reading measures on the target word are shown in Table 2, results of the LMMs on fixation duration measures are reported in Table 3 and results of the fixation probability measures are reported in Table 4.

Table 2.

Means and standard errors, aggregated by subject, for reading time measures in Experiment 1.

Measure Set Preview condition
Identical Antonym/Synonym Plausible Unrelated Implausible Unrelated
First Fixation Duration Antonym 222 (5.1) 221 (5.8) 224 (4.8) 231 (6.1)
Synonym 223 (4.9) 227 (5.3) 230 (5.1) 238 (5.2)
Single Fixation Duration Antonym 224 (5.3) 222 (6.1) 226 (5.3) 237 (6.9)
Synonym 228 (5.2) 232 (6.3) 233 (5.8) 244 (6.1)
Gaze Duration Antonym 238 (6.5) 238 (6.7) 246 (6.0) 256 (8.4)
Synonym 243 (7.6) 242 (7.5) 246 (7.2) 260 (8.4)
Go-Past Time Antonym 259 (8.6) 264 (11) 280 (10) 306 (12)
Synonym 284 (13) 269 (10) 284 (11) 297 (12)
Total Time Antonym 288 (10) 306 (13) 323 (14) 325 (13)
Synonym 293 (12) 298 (14) 315 (14) 316 (12)
Fixation Probability Antonym 0.82 (.03) 0.82 (.03) 0.82 (.03) 0.85 (.03)
Synonym 0.81 (.03) 0.81 (.03) 0.81 (.03) 0.84 (.03)
Regressions out Antonym 0.08 (.01) 0.09 (.02) 0.10 (.02) 0.14 (.02)
Synonym 0.11 (.02) 0.09 (.01) 0.10 (.02) 0.12 (.02)
Regressions in Antonym 0.17 (.03) 0.21 (.03) 0.22 (.03) 0.22 (.02)
Synonym 0.14 (.02) 0.18 (.03) 0.19 (.02) 0.20 (.02)

Table 3.

Results of the linear mixed effects models for reading time measures on the target across preview conditions (i.e., identical, antonym/synonym, plausible unrelated, and implausible unrelated) and stimulus sets (i.e., the antonym set and the synonym set), including the interaction term. The left hand column represents the average effects across stimulus sets (because stimulus set was entered as a centered predictor) and the right hand column represents the tests for interactions between the preview contrasts and stimulus set. In the left hand columns, the intercept represents the implausible unrelated condition and the subsequent rows test for preview benefit effects—the difference between the named condition and the intercept. Significant effects are indicated by boldface.

Measure Contrast Overall Preview Benefit Interaction with Stimulus Set
b SE |t| b SE |t|
First Fixation Duration
Intercept 234.84 5.23 44.89 4.77 5.38 0.89
Identical −12.01 4.21 2.85 −3.60 5.95 0.60
Antonym/Synonym −12.02 3.64 3.30 2.49 6.46 0.39
Plausible −7.35 3.56 2.06 0.37 7.01 0.05
Single Fixation Duration
Intercept 240.01 5.94 40.40 4.76 5.48 0.87
Identical −13.44 4.63 2.90 −1.75 6.20 0.28
Antonym/Synonym −13.67 4.23 3.23 4.94 6.45 0.77
Plausible −9.98 4.03 2.48 0.24 7.47 0.03
Gaze Duration
Intercept 258.10 8.06 32.01 3.79 5.97 0.63
Identical −18.27 6.21 2.94 1.13 6.73 0.17
Antonym/Synonym −19.22 5.21 3.69 1.14 7.36 0.16
Plausible −11.53 5.24 2.20 −5.21 7.47 0.70
Go-Past Time
Intercept 301.16 11.71 25.73 −8.37 9.70 0.86
Identical −30.18 8.09 3.73 33.81 11.07 3.05
Antonym/Synonym −33.67 7.72 4.36 13.16 12.68 1.04
Plausible −19.08 7.51 2.54 11.48 12.77 0.90
Total Time
Intercept 320.56 12.08 26.54 −7.50 10.34 0.73
Identical −32.06 7.86 4.08 14.98 12.19 1.23
Antonym/Synonym −18.29 7.57 2.42 1.44 12.60 0.11
Plausible −1.12 6.43 0.17 1.58 15.40 0.10

Table 4.

Results of the linear mixed effects models for reading probability measures on the target across conditions. The left hand column represents the average effects across stimulus sets (because stimulus set was entered as a centered predictor) and the right hand column represents the tests for interactions between the preview contrasts and stimulus set. In the left hand columns, the intercept represents the implausible unrelated condition and the subsequent rows test for preview benefit effects—the difference between the named condition and the intercept. Significant effects are indicated by boldface.

Measure Contrast Overall Preview Benefit Interaction w/ Set
b |z| p b |z| p
Fixation Probability
Intercept 2.27 7.77 < .001 0.18 0.69 .49
Identical −0.39 2.13 < .05 −0.14 0.44 .66
Antonym/Synonym −0.33 1.88 .06 −0.44 1.26 .21
Plausible −0.21 1.15 .25 −0.51 1.52 .13
Regressions Out
Intercept −2.16 12.12 < .001 −0.22 1.14 .25
Identical −0.35 2.26 < .05 0.63 2.39 < .05
Antonym/Synonym −0.42 2.23 < .05 0.21 0.78 .44
Plausible −0.32 1.66 .10 0.23 0.89 .38
Regressions In
Intercept −1.49 11.22 < .001 −0.13 0.75 .45
Identical −0.66 3.55 < .001 −0.20 0.71 .48
Antonym/Synonym −0.14 0.99 .32 −0.14 0.55 .58
Plausible −0.003 0.02 .99 −0.04 0.19 .85

Fixation duration measures

None of the interactions between stimulus set and preview benefit contrast were significant2, suggesting that the effects were similar for synonyms and antonyms (i.e., for the synonym/antonym contrast) and across different types of stimuli (for the identical and plausible unrelated preview benefit). Therefore, while the analyses and tables reported below maintain the interaction term in the models, we discuss the preview benefit effects together for brevity and simplicity. For all reading time measures, there was a significant identical preview benefit (all ts > 2.84) and a significant antonym/synonym preview benefit (all ts > 2.41), replicating the effect reported by Schotter (2013; Schotter et al., 2015) for synonyms and extending the effect to antonyms. The preview benefit provided by the plausible unrelated word was also significant in all early reading time measures (all ts > 2.05), replicating Yang and colleagues' findings in English and suggesting that it is unlikely that integration between the preview and target can completely explain the other preview benefit effects observed here. That is, because there is no relationship between the preview and target, it is unclear what information would be integrated in order to facilitate processing once the target word was fixated.

The plausibility preview benefit was not significant in total time (t = 0.17), which includes any time spent re-reading the target after moving past it. Furthermore, the antonym/synonym preview condition produced reading times closer to the identical preview condition in early reading time measures (e.g., gaze duration) than in total time (Figure 1), suggesting that any benefits from having a plausible but non-identical preview were relatively short-lived and diminished once the reader had moved past the word (leading to a higher likelihood of making a regression back to the target, see below).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Reading time on the target word in Experiment 1 as a function of preview condition and stimulus set (antonyms or synonyms), across 2 reading time measures (gzd = gaze duration, tvt = total time).

Fixation probability measures

None of the interactions with stimulus set were significant, indicating the same pattern of effects for both sets3 so we discuss the patterns of preview effects together. Moreover, the lack of interaction between the intercept and stimulus set indicates that there was no effect of stimulus set in the implausible unrelated condition, and the additional lack of interactions with the preview contrasts suggests that the overall main effect of stimulus set was not significant overall.

For fixation probability, only the contrast for the identical preview was significant (p < .05)4, indicating that the identical preview was fixated less often than the implausible unrelated word. The contrast for the antonym/synonym preview was marginally significant (p = .06), also indicating that these previews were fixated slightly less often than implausible unrelated preview. The contrast for the plausible unrelated previews was not significant (p > .24), indicating that those previews were fixated as frequently as the implausible unrelated preview.

For regressions out of the target word, both the contrasts for the identical preview and the antonym/synonym were significant (both ps < .05), indicating that those preview conditions produced fewer regressions out of the target than the implausible unrelated preview condition. The contrast for the plausible unrelated preview was not significant (p = .10), indicating an approximately equivalent rate of regressions out as in the implausible unrelated preview condition.

For regressions into the target word, only the contrast for the identical preview condition was significant (p < .001.), indicating fewer regressions in the identical preview condition than in the implausible unrelated preview condition, which did not differ from the other preview conditions (both ps > .31).

Discussion

There are several important aspects of the data from Experiment 1. The lack of interactions between the antonym/synonym contrast and stimulus set suggests that the preview benefit for synonyms observed by Schotter (2013; Schotter et al., 2015) cannot exclusively be explained by similarity and trans-saccadic integration of meaning between the preview and target. Instead, it is possible that, since both the synonym and antonym preview were plausible in the sentence context, the contextual fit of the preview itself (i.e., post-lexical integration of the word in the sentence context) might explain the observed preview benefit, as suggested by Yang and colleagues. Moreover, this interpretation is supported by the data from the plausible unrelated preview condition, which provided preview benefit in all reading time measures except for total time, which includes regressions back into the target. Because the preview in this condition did not share any meaningful relationship with the target it is unlikely that a similarity-based or integration-based account can explain these data. Furthermore, this condition replicates the plausibility effects observed by Yang and colleagues and extends the effect from Chinese to English. The preview benefit for the plausible unrelated condition likely disappeared in total time because, after the reader had fixated and processed the target word, discrepancies between its orthographic and/or semantic form and that of the preview lead to increased re-reading time. The total time data suggest that there is more of a benefit for antonym and synonym previews, which are semantically similar to the target word and thus may mostly carry a cost of difference in orthography, than for plausible unrelated previews, which may impose costs for both orthographic and semantic discrepancies between representations (see General Discussion).

In light of these data, instead of an account that invokes integration, we favor an account whereby preview benefit for first-pass measures can be explained in terms of ease of processing the preview per se via its compatibility with the sentence context (in addition to lexical features like word frequency and predictability; see Schotter et al., 2015; Schotter, Reichle, & Rayner, 2014). We will elaborate on this account in the General Discussion, but note here that the three conditions in Experiment 1 that lead to the fastest first-pass reading times were those where the preview was plausible in the sentence context. As noted by Yang and colleagues (2012, 2014), this finding suggests that readers were able to access a linguistically high level of information from the preview word—not only its meaning but also the extent to which that meaning was appropriate in the sentence context. This then raises the question of whether the readers had obtained so much information from the preview that they had actually encoded that word rather than the target word when the preview was a plausible word in the sentence. We explore this possibility in Experiment 2.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 was designed to assess which of the two words—the preview or the target—the subject had interpreted by the time he or she had finished reading the sentence. We took an approach first reported by Blanchard, McConkie, Zola, and Wolverton (1984), in which there was a comprehension question after each sentence that, in display change conditions, could be answered with either the preview or the target (see details in the method section, below). For the identical (i.e., non display change) condition there was a comprehension question that asked about parts of the sentence other than the target in order to encourage the subjects to read for comprehension and not merely recognize and remember individual words. We analyzed the responses to the questions in relation to the eye movement behavior to assess (1) whether subjects ever reported the preview word, and (2) what experimental conditions and eye movement behaviors made reporting the preview word more likely.

Method

Subjects

Twenty-four UCSD students participated in the experiment for course credit. None had participated in Experiment 1, but they were selected using the same inclusion criteria.

Apparatus

The apparatus was the same as in Experiment 1.

Materials and design

Because it would be confusing for subjects to choose between two synonyms in the comprehension questions, this experiment used only the antonym sentences from Experiment 1. Additionally, two sentences from the antonym stimulus set were excluded because it was not possible to create a comprehension question that satisfied our constraints, yielding 110 experimental sentences. Every sentence was followed by a question, which differed depending on the experimental condition. In the identical preview condition, the question was a general comprehension question that did not refer to the target word. For both the antonym and plausible unrelated preview conditions, the question was designed such that either the preview or target could be plausible answers. For the unrelated preview condition the question directly asked which word the subject read (see Table 5 and Appendix B). The response options were presented in all capital letters whereas the preview and target words in the sentence were presented normally in lower case letters so that subjects could not rely on visual memory of the words in the sentence to make their response.

Table 5.

Example questions across condition in Experiment 2 for the example sentence “Ron's pants are still loose/tight/short/poles even after tailoring.”

Preview Condition Question Option 1 Option 2
Identical Did Ron get his pants tailored? Yes No
Antonym How are Ron's pants now? LOOSE TIGHT
Plausible How are Ron's pants now? LOOSE SHORT
Implausible Which of these words did you read? LOOSE POLES

Procedure

The procedure was the same as in Experiment 1 except questions followed 100% of the sentences and differed depending on experimental condition (see above) and the experimental session lasted approximately 30 minutes. The rate of display changes noticed was very low, between 0 and 15 trials (mean = 2.42). Comprehension accuracy in the identical preview condition (i.e., the general comprehension questions) was high (on average 95%). Responses to the questions that probed whether the subjects had maintained the preview or target representation are reported below.

Results

Data processing procedures were the same as for Experiment 1, leaving 2156 trials for analysis (82% of the original data: 7.5% of trials were excluded for blinks and track loss, 13.5% of trials were excluded for late or inappropriate display changes; < 1% of long or short fixations were excluded, and 1.4–2.4% of fixations were excluded based on standard deviation by subject). Data analysis procedures were mostly the same except only the contrasts to test for preview benefit were used as fixed and random effects because there was only one stimulus set: the intercept of the model was the implausible unrelated word and the preview contrasts tested for the difference of each of the other preview conditions compared to the baseline, independently5. Reading measures on the target word are shown in Table 6, results of the LMMs on fixation duration measures are reported in Table 7 and results of the fixation probability measures are reported in Table 8.

Table 6.

Means and standard errors, aggregated by subject, for reading time measures in Experiment 2.

Measure Preview condition
Identical Antonym Plausible Implausible
First Fixation Duration 217 (5.7) 218 (6.0) 227 (6.3) 236 (6.4)
Single Fixation Duration 222 (5.9) 223 (6.6) 231 (7.5) 245 (6.5)
Gaze Duration 235 (7.6) 241 (7.7) 245 (8.2) 266 (8.8)
Go-Past Time 267 (11) 275 (13) 273 (10) 313 (13)
Total Time 293 (12) 333 (17) 341 (16) 350 (16)
Fixation Probability .85 (.03) .87 (.03) .85 (.03) .86 (.03)
Regressions out .10 (.02) .09 (.02) .09 (.01) .15 (.02)
Regressions in .16 (.02) .23 (.02) .25 (.03) .23 (.03)

Table 7.

Results of the linear mixed effects models for reading time measures on the target across conditions in Experiment 2. Significant effects are indicated by boldface.

Measure Contrast b SE |t|
First Fixation Duration
Intercept 235.58 6.65 35.43
Identical −18.31 4.39 4.17
Antonym −17.17 5.11 3.36
Plausible −8.50 4.50 1.89
Single Fixation Duration
Intercept 244.87 6.77 36.19
Identical −24.32 4.99 4.87
Antonym −20.49 5.57 3.68
Plausible −13.17 5.68 2.32
Gaze Duration
Intercept 265.54 9.10 29.17
Identical −31.66 5.72 5.54
Antonym −23.47 6.31 3.72
Plausible −20.43 5.88 3.47
Go-Past Time
Intercept 313.83 14.00 22.41
Identical −49.48 11.06 4.47
Antonym −37.39 9.55 3.92
Plausible −39.89 9.65 4.13
Total Time
Intercept 349.52 16.12 21.68
Identical −60.07 10.95 5.49
Antonym −14.46 10.08 1.44
Plausible −8.12 11.03 0.74

Table 8.

Results of the linear mixed effects models for reading probability measures on the target across conditions in Experiment 3. Significant effects are indicated by boldface.

Measure Contrast b |z| p
Fixation Probability
Intercept 2.51 7.64 < .001
Identical −0.31 1.04 .30
Antonym −0.05 0.16 .87
Plausible −0.47 1.71 .09
Regressions Out
Intercept −2.02 9.10 < .001
Identical −0.64 1.84 .07
Antonym −0.75 2.39 < .05
Plausible −0.31 1.20 .23
Regressions In
Intercept −1.47 7.58 < .001
Identical −0.33 1.42 .16
Antonym 0.08 0.34 .73
Plausible 0.33 1.64 .10

Fixation duration measures

The fixation data from Experiment 2 mostly replicate the patterns of data from Experiment 16. For all duration measures, the identical preview benefit was significant (all ts > 4.16). For all early duration measures the antonym preview benefit was significant (all ts > 3.35), but the effect was not significant in total time (t = 1.44) whereas it was significant in Experiment 1. It is likely that the high occurrence of word-specific comprehension questions caused the readers to be more cautious about their understanding of the sentences, which eradicated the effect in the antonym condition. That is, because antonyms do not have identical meanings, if readers had encoded both the preview from parafoveal vision and the target, once it was directly fixated, these two representations might compete in the comprehension system and cause the reader to make a regression to double check the meaning of the sentence. This is more likely to happen when they encounter questions after every sentence that require a precise understanding of the meaning of the sentence (as in Experiment 2) than when the questions appear less often and only probe a general understanding of the text (as in Experiment 1). The plausible unrelated preview benefit was significant in single fixation duration, gaze duration, and go-past time (all ts > 2.31), marginally significant in first fixation duration (t = 1.89) and was not significant in total time (t < 1). The non-significant preview benefit in total time for the plausible unrelated condition was not observed in Experiment 1 either. We suspect that this is also because of the difference in meaning between the sentences with the preview and target, as noted above. However, because the meanings are so different, this was observed even in the first experiment, with lower comprehension demands.

Fixation probability measures

There were no significant differences across preview condition for the probability of making a first-pass fixation on the target (all ps > .08)7. For regressions out of the target, numerically all plausible conditions had lower regression rates than the implausible condition, but only the difference for the antonym condition was significant (p < .05). For the probability of making a regression into the target, none of the contrasts were significant (all ps > .09), even though numerically, there was a lower rate of regressions in the identical preview condition than in the other preview conditions.

Comprehension analyses

In order to investigate whether readers had identified and maintained the preview information, we analyzed the readers' responses to the questions. Critically, the questions in the display change conditions allowed us to probe which of the words the reader had interpreted by the end of the sentence. For the most part, readers reported the target rather than the preview word, suggesting that the information from a directly fixated word was maintained more strongly than (or replaced) the information obtained only from a parafoveally viewed word.

The only fixation related measure that showed a relationship with the reader's response was the measure of skipping. To assess this relationship we conducted a mixed effects logistic regression analysis with the reader's response on each trial (i.e., target coded as 1 vs. preview coded as 0) as the dependent measure and two variables (including their interaction) as predictors. The first predictor was the probability of skipping the preview/target word, entered as a centered predictor, and the second variable was preview condition entered with successive differences contrasts that tested for (1) a difference between the implausible and plausible condition and (2) a difference between the plausible and antonym condition. We included random slopes only for preview condition since there was very little variability in skipping the preview/target word. The identical preview condition was excluded from the analyses because the comprehension question did not assess whether the subjects remembered the preview versus the target.

The analysis revealed a significant effect of skipping on the readers' responses; readers were more likely to respond that they had read the target word when they had fixated it than when they had skipped it (p < .001). There were also differences across experimental conditions; in the antonym preview condition, subjects reported the preview 14% of the time, more often than in the plausible unrelated preview condition (11% of the time; p < .01), which was more likely than in the implausible unrelated preview condition (only 3% of the time; p < .01). Furthermore, there was one significant interaction between skipping and experimental condition whereby the effect of skipping the word (i.e., increased likelihood of reporting the preview if the reader had skipped it) was more pronounced in the antonym preview condition (an average effect size of 22 percentage points) than in the plausible unrelated preview condition (an average effect size of 9 percentage points; interaction p < .05). The effect of skipping in the plausible unrelated condition did not differ from the effect of skipping on the probability of reporting the target in the implausible unrelated preview condition (an average effect of 4 percentage points; interaction p > .13; Figure 2). In addition, this pattern held (and was slightly more pronounced) for trials in which the reader did not make a regression (Figure 3, left panel) but did not hold when the reader made a regression and therefore directly fixated the target word (Figure 3, right panel). Therefore, the effect seems to be completely dissolved if the target word is fixated at all during reading of the sentence.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Proportion of trials in which the reader selected the response that indicates that he or she had maintained the representation of the target as a function of whether the target was fixated (filled circles) or skipped (open triangles) and preview condition in Experiment 2.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Proportion of trials in which the reader selected the response that indicates that he or she had maintained the representation of the target as a function of whether the target was fixated (filled circles) or skipped (open triangles) and preview condition in Experiment 2, split by trials in which the reader did not make a regression back to the target (left panel) or did make a regression (right panel).

Discussion

The data from Experiment 2 replicate the main pattern of eye movement measures from Experiment 1. In addition, analysis of the comprehension questions that were designed to assess which of the words (i.e., the preview or target) the reader had encoded by the end of the sentence shows an interesting, but not necessarily surprising finding. Mainly, readers almost always maintained the representation of the target word by the end of the sentence, leading to rates of reporting the target rather than the preview of around 90%. Importantly, however, the probability of reporting the preview increased when the reader had skipped the target and never regressed to fixate it, suggesting that words that are directly fixated are more likely to be encoded than words that are only viewed in parafoveal vision. However, in the absence of direct fixation on the target word, a parafoveal preview was sufficient for the preview to be encoded and maintained in the reader's interpretation of the sentence, except when the preview word was implausible.

These data might help to explain why Yang et al. (2014) did not find evidence in their regression data that Chinese readers encoded the plausible preview word. In their study readers only skipped the preview/target word 4% of the time and they did not report an analysis comparing regression behavior between trials in which the target word was fixated compared to when it was skipped. Therefore, it might be that readers in their study did not maintain the meaning of the preview word because they had fixated the target word and its representation overrode the representation of the target word.

General Discussion

The results from these studies add to a growing body of literature (Rayner & Schotter, 2014; Schotter et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2012, 2014) that suggest that preview benefit may not necessarily operate through integration of preview and target information, as was initially assumed. Instead, these data suggest that, to the extent that information can be obtained from the preview to cause the upcoming word to be skipped, preview information may be sufficient to support efficient reading. In such cases, readers are likely to encode and maintain the representation of the preview and may be completely unaware that the stimulus had actually changed to something else as they were reading, leading to a higher likelihood of reporting the preview on skipping trials in Experiment 2. Indeed, this proposition is not new and researchers have posited that skipping of words does indicate that readers had obtained a significant amount of meaningful information from (if not completely identified) the target (see Rayner, 2009).

However, the pattern of data for early eye movement measures in both Experiments 1 and 2, in which we observed shorter reading time on directly fixated target words following any plausible parafoveal preview regardless of its relationship to the target (i.e., synonym, antonym, or completely unrelated), suggests that the influence of the preview's meaning is not completely confined to the measure of skipping. Importantly, the fact that all of the plausible conditions, regardless of the preview's relationship to the target, yielded a preview benefit in early fixation duration measures challenges an integration account in favor of a contextual fit account. However, the effect of preview plausibility is relatively short-lived and the benefit of an unrelated yet plausible preview is not seen in later reading time measures. What might be driving the effects of the preview on early fixations on the target if not integration across saccades, and why do those effects differ from the pattern of data in later measures?

It may be the case that the contextual fit account is most appropriate to explain the first-pass reading data since the preview information is available earlier (i.e., from parafoveal vision) than the target information. Target information is only available once the word is fixated, after the delay for transmission of information from the retina to the brain (i.e., the 50–60 ms eye-brain lag; see Reichle & Reingold, 2013 for a review,) and then after some amount of time required for linguistic processing of the content of that word. To explain how reading behavior on a fixated target word can be primarily influenced by properties of a different, unfixated (i.e., parafoveal preview) word we turn to an explanation suggested by Schotter et al. (2014; see also Schotter et al., 2015) regarding the relationship between linguistic processing and eye movement programming in first-pass reading as described by the framework of the E-Z Reader model (e.g., Reichle, 2011). The full details of the model are beyond the scope of this paper, but it is sufficient for the following discussion to know that both word identification and saccade programming are divided into two stages in the model. With respect to word identification, the completion of the first stage (L1) triggers both the second stage of word identification (L2) and the first stage of saccade programming (M1) away from that word. The completion of the second stage of word identification causes attention to shift to the upcoming word and for the first stage of word identification to start for that word. Therefore, covert attention and eye movement control are often dissociate in the model, with the shift of attention toward a word preceding the eye movement toward it. With respect to the saccade programming stages, the first stage is labile, meaning that the saccade program can be cancelled and a new one can be programmed whereas the second stage (M2) is non-labile, meaning that it cannot be cancelled.

The above description of the E-Z Reader model has remained mostly unchanged since it was first proposed (Reichle, Pollatsek, Fisher, & Rayner, 1998). The idea regarding preview benefit effects introduced by Schotter et al. (2014) is that there is a subset of cases (8% of the time in the simulation of Schotter's (2013) data that they conducted) in which L1 for the upcoming preview word completes and the model enters the L2 stage for that word while the eyes are still fixating the prior word. As noted by Schotter et al. (2015), a consequence of the completion of the L1 stage is not only the start of the L2 stage, but also the initiation of a saccade program (M1). If the completion of the L1 stage that initiates the M1 stage for the upcoming word occurs during the current saccade program's labile (M1) stage, then the saccade to the preview/target is cancelled and the system programs a skip instead (as noted above, word skipping is uncontroversially influenced by properties of the preview; Rayner, 2009). If, however, completion of the L1 stage that initiates the M1 stage for the upcoming word occurs during the current saccade program's non-labile (M2) stage, the system cannot skip the preview word and instead pre-initiates the upcoming saccade program (i.e., from the preview/target word to the following word) prior to fixation on the target word. This feature of parallel saccade programs was introduced in Morrison's (1984) model of oculomotor control in reading and was incorporated into the E-Z Reader framework. This pre-initiation shortens the subsequent fixation duration on the target word and appears as a preview benefit effect. Thus, there are some fixations on the target whose duration is determined by properties of the preview rather than the target because the saccade toward that word was in the non-labile (M2) stage when the completion of the L1 stage for that word completed. Importantly, because the completion of L1 had happened while the eyes were still fixating the prior word, the completion of L1 (and therefore initiation of M1 and concomitant shortening of the subsequent fixation duration) was based on the preview, rather than the target word.

This account also predicts that fixation durations on the target word should be lengthened if the L1 stage does not complete early enough during parafoveal preview to cause a skip or trigger pre-initiation of the upcoming saccade (i.e., if it is a nonword as in many prior boundary paradigm experiments). In this case, more information is needed and the target word must be fixated and processed to some degree in order to initiate saccade programming, leading to a long fixation on the word. In the model, the duration of the L1 stage is determined by three lexical characteristics: word length, frequency, and word-form cloze predictability. However, in the present study, we observed longer first pass times on actual words (i.e., as opposed to nonwords) when the preview was an implausible word. At the moment, this effect cannot be explained within the E-Z Reader framework because plausibility is considered a post-lexical characteristic and does not exert an influence on reading behavior until after the L1 stage. Without conducting simulations with the model it is not possible make concrete predictions, but to accommodate this finding we can imagine (at least) two possibilities. First, plausibility of the preview may also be a feature of words that influences L1 and could be incorporated as an additional parameter in determining its duration. Second, plausibility of the preview word may be processed shortly after L1 (after fixation on the target word) and, at that point, inhibits or cancels the saccade program, which would similarly predict longer fixation durations. Either of these accounts could explain why the implausible unrelated preview condition yielded longer first-pass fixation durations than any of the plausible preview conditions even though it was similar in length and frequency to the other preview words, and why it yielded the lowest proportion of non-target responses in the questions in Experiment 2.

One note we must make with respect to preview benefit effects from the prior literature is that some studies have actually found significant preview benefit in first-pass fixation duration measures from orthographically related nonword previews (e.g., Rayner et al., 1986; Drieghe et al., 2005). As mentioned in the introduction, the explanation for the effects was by means of integration across saccades, an account against which we argue here. These data may be accommodated by the account we describe above by means of misperception of the nonword preview for a similar real word form (see Slattery, 2009 for a discussion of misperception). Particularly, the nonword preview could have been misperceived as the visually/orthographically similar target word, which in the context of the sentences used in those studies was a plausible and sensible word form. This seems to be a reasonable account since visual acuity is poor in the parafovea and especially for early studies that used low quality computer monitors that would make it difficult to precisely determine letter identities (see Drieghe et al., 2005 for a similar argument about monitor quality).

The above account only describes eye movement behavior during first-pass reading. The total time and regression data might be better accommodated by an integration account since the target information would have had more time to be processed and the relationship between it and the preview might therefore be more pertinent to eye movement behavior. We must, however, be more specific in what we mean by “integration” in this case. In fact, the E-Z Reader framework incorporated an integration stage (Reichle, Warren, & McConnell, 2009), which refers to the post-lexical integration of a given word with the sentence context, rather than the trans-saccadic integration of the preview and the target. Perhaps the idea of “memory codes” suggested by Pollatsek et al. (1992) operates on a longer time scale than across saccades and at a higher linguistic level than between the individual word forms of the preview and target. That is, it is possible that the total time data in our experiments could be explained by the reader entertaining two simultaneous representations of the event being described by the two versions of the sentence, one including the preview word and the other including the target word. Indeed, prior research has suggested that readers maintain uncertainty about the identities of previously read words until they can be resolved by subsequent input (Levy, Bicknell, Slattery, & Rayner, 2009). This notion could easily extend to two representations of a word or described event that the reader has encountered—one prior to fixation and the other upon direct fixation. To the extent that these two representations are the same (i.e., when the words are synonyms) or very similar (i.e., when they are antonyms) not much distinction or adjudication between the events is necessary in order for the reader to settle on a reasonable general understanding. However, in the plausible unrelated preview condition this is not the case and two unrelated but equally plausible events are now both entertained and the reader needs to select one of them. This would lead the reader to make a regression and check which word was actually there, increasing total time and leading to the elimination of the early preview benefit that was afforded by the contextual fit of the preview, despite the fact that the norming procedures showed that the complete sentences with the plausible unrelated word were still perfectly sensible.

The lack of a preview benefit in total time for the plausible unrelated condition, which contrasted with the significant preview benefit in first-pass measures for that condition, was observed in both experiments. In contrast, two different patterns of preview benefit in total time were observed for the antonym condition between the two experiments. This difference could be explained by the above account because of the different comprehension demands imposed by the nature of the questions in the two experiments. In the first experiment, questions probed a general understanding of the sentence (i.e., did not distinguish between the preview/target words) and only occurred on 26% of the trials. In contrast, in the second experiment questions occurred after every sentence and, in addition to general comprehension questions on non-display change trials, included detailed comprehension questions that probed whether the reader had encoded the preview or the target. It is likely that the increased demand on the comprehension system to have a precise representation of the sentence made the readers more cautious and less willing to accept an incomplete or ambiguous understanding of the event.

The account of early preview effects based on the timing of parafoveal word identification relative to saccade programming predicts that the patterns of data observed in these studies are due to a mixture of cases—skip trials, shortened fixations that are due to pre-initiated saccades influenced primarily by the parafoveal preview, and long fixations influenced primarily by the foveal target. Thus, the preview benefits observed in all of the plausible preview conditions in the studies reported here are likely to have a greater proportion of shortened fixations than the implausible preview conditions. Because this is a probabilistic account, future research will be necessary to determine on an individual trial basis whether a fixation should be considered a shortened fixation or a long fixation, given variability across both subjects and items in terms of fixation durations. Using the computational models of oculomotor control in reading, with which we can probe the time course of word identification stages relative to saccade programming stages, could be quite useful in simulating and estimating these probabilities, which can only be inferred from the empirical data. Relatedly, recent research has shown that the skill level of the reader affects the pattern of semantic (Veldre & Andrews, 2016a) and plausibility (Veldre & Andrews, 2016b) preview benefits observed in boundary paradigm studies. Thus, modeling work simulating different proportions of shortened and long fixation cases, as well as individual differences across readers' skill levels, will be useful in supporting or disconfirming these hypotheses.

The account of the later reading time data via competing sentence representations is admittedly quite speculative and future work will be needed to more precisely determine the reader's internal representation of the event being described in the text, particularly with respect to skipping and display change paradigms in which there may be two parallel events being represented. One thing that is clear from the current data is that initial eye movement behavior on a word, not only the likelihood of skipping but also fixation durations on the word when it is not skipped, can be influenced by properties of the parafoveal preview (e.g., its plausibility) irrespective of its relationship to the target, as demonstrated by the first-pass reading data in both Experiments 1 and 2. However, the ultimate understanding of the sentence is most likely to be based on the target word if that has been fixated either during first pass reading or after a regression, as demonstrated by the comprehension data in Experiment 2. This dissociation suggests that the engine driving the eyes forward in the text is a quick and relatively risky local word identification processes, to a large part determined by properties of the parafoveal preview. However, the ultimate understanding of the sentence is the consequence of a more sluggish and careful process that requires regressions to ensure accurate understanding and which is mostly determined by fixated target information and the relationship between the preview and target in the case in which they are different.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Atkinson Family Endowment Fund, grant HD065829 from the National Institutes of Health, and a gift from the Microsoft Corporation. Portions of these data were presented at the European Conference on Eye Movements, Vienna, Austria, August, 2015. We thank Denis Drieghe and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Appendix A.1.

Set of antonym stimuli used in the experiments. Target words (identical previews) are presented in boldface (not in boldface in the experiments). Columns to the right represent the antonym, plausible unrelated, and implausible unrelated previews. Items with asterisks were excluded from Experiment 2.

Item Sentence Antonym Plausible Unrelated Implausible Unrelated
1 Jane will travel south on her trip to Los Angeles next week. north coach house
2 Every year, the family flies north on their way to a fabulous vacation. south coach house
3 His friend advised him to graciously accept the generous bribe. refuse report device
4 Jane would never refuse a bonus from her boss. accept report device
5 The ghost suddenly appeared in the upper window of the house. vanished screamed nominate
6 The young girl vanished from around the corner suddenly. appeared screamed nominate
7 Jenny prefers to purchase large bottles of lotion. small fancy whale
8 My aunt liked the very small earrings in the display. large fancy whale
9 George wore a new black suit to the party. white shiny years
10 Deb test drove a fancy white car yesterday with her friend. black shiny years
11 The new student answered the question boldly when no one else raised their hand. meekly easily killer
12 Karen jumped over the stream meekly before everyone else. boldly easily killer
13 The children came up with a very clever way to fix the sink. stupid clumsy roamer
14 My nephew is very stupid when doing housework. clever clumsy roamer
15 Somehow, the food stayed cool after the long trip. warm soft nose
16 David likes to eat extremely warm potato salad. cool soft nose
17 The teacher always seemed too kind to his students. mean dull luck
18 Jen's classmate is always mean when they go on trips. kind dull luck
19 Sam's train arrived just before dusk and we were able to give her a ride home. dawn ours year
20 Mindy's dog always wakes up after dawn and we are always waiting for them. dusk ours year
21 The final seemed extremely easy after the last midterm. hard long pens
22 The hike up the mountain was hard but very fun. easy long pens
23 Kevin's brother ate all their fresh bread in the apartment. stale baked place
24 The supermarket stopped selling stale pastries on the weekends. fresh baked place
25 Everyone looks like a skinny giant standing next to Henry. dwarf model peels
26 Yesterday I saw a famous dwarf at the beach. giant model peels
27 The box gradually became heavier the longer he carried it. lighter dirtier checker
28 Betty wondered if she looked lighter when she spent more time at the gym. heavier dirtier checker
29 At the restaurant, the girl wanted less dressing in her salad. more tart bats
30 John should have baked more apples into the pie. less tart bats
31 Ron's pants are still loose even after tailoring. tight short poles
32 The professor's coat is too tight for him. loose short poles
33 The dogs are always noisy when people visit the house. quiet eager ocean
34 The children are very quiet during storytime. noisy eager ocean
35 The train has to run under the bridge before reaching the town. above along bland
36 Visitors have to drive above the cliff to get to Sarah's house. under along bland
37 Dana didn't realize his tattoo was permanent until almost four months later. temporary offensive schoolbag
38 Everyone wanted Jeff to quit his temporary job as soon as possible. permanent offensive schoolbag
39 Nobody could agree if the jewel was real or if it wasn't. fake pink bars
40 Zoe insisted her hair color wasn't fake but no one believed her. real pink bars
41 Everyone knew the girl was very rich because of the neighborhood she lived in. poor lazy mall
42 Her previous roommate was extremely poor and had no friends. rich lazy mall
43 Kristy realized that she guessed the answer right on yesterday's exam. wrong badly flute
44 Olga set up the equipment wrong the first time. right badly flute
45 All the vegetables have to be kept separate on the boy's plate or he will be upset. together lukewarm discover
46 Employees are told to store the chemicals together or there will be an explosion. separate lukewarm discover
47 There were so many serious stories in the news today. trivial strange already
48 The accident was considered trivial by most of the bystanders. serious strange already
49 The new event was such a great success that people were talking about it for weeks. failure scandal kittens
50 The TV show became a famous failure after the actress joined the cast. success scandal kittens
51 The man claimed that the dogs became tame after they were given to him. wild loud ball
52 The circus had many wild lions in the show. tame loud ball
53 Jessica always drives too fast on the highway. slow long yelp
54 The boat trip was so extremely slow that I couldn't enjoy it. fast long yelp
55 The grandmother can only sleep on soft mattresses or she complains of backaches. hard many jump
56 Jason likes to eat hard candies while doing homework. soft many jump
57 The special fruits were recently exported from the neighboring country. imported snatched believer
58 The people suddenly imported all the goods they could before the embargo. exported snatched believer
59 Joshua is always drunk outside of school. sober angry gauge
60 Henry didn't allow anyone to be visibly sober at his party last week. drunk angry gauge
61 There was a woman walking three tiny dogs at the park today. huge gray what
62 Dale's favorite horse at the stable was huge and lively. tiny gray what
63 Ian put the containers above the cabinet with the dishes. below aside hairy
64 Alex was surprised her glasses were below the dresser in her brother's room. above aside hairy
65 My aunt likes to occasionally give apples to her neighbors' children. take show cape
66 The student will reluctantly take his research paper to the professor. give show cape
67* Adam's younger brothers hate eating junk food when depressed. love stop deep
68* The high school students always love running when they are tired. hate stop deep
69 The tools Robert has are more inferior compared to the ones we have. superior delicate absently
70 The bakery's pastries are truly superior compared to the other place's. inferior delicate absently
71 Bob was forced to repaint the building's exterior three times before getting it right. interior insignia bathrobe
72 We noticed that the company's interior was very fancy. exterior insignia bathrobe
73 The number of cookies in the jar seems decreased from the last time I looked. increased unchanged afterglow
74 The amount of classwork has become increased after the first few weeks. decreased unchanged afterglow
75 Every week I always find my keys behind the couch. lose toss blue
76 Darren managed to somehow lose the wedding ring down the storm drain. find toss blue
77 The family left the garage door closed when they went on vacation. opened broken banana
78 The storefront was unexpectedly opened over the weekend. closed broken banana
79 Her plane will arrive later in the afternoon than expected. depart refuel turtle
80 Gary didn't expect to have to suddenly depart but he pulled it off without a hitch. arrive refuel turtle
81 The team expected a huge victory after seeing the competition. failure quarrel polygon
82 We watched an interesting failure on TV last night. victory quarrel polygon
83 Ted thinks the singer's loud voice doesn't fit this song. soft cute leap
84 Jeffrey didn't like the really soft music playing at the party. loud cute leap
85 Amanda likes her soup salty and her desserts sweet. bland clear shine
86 This restaurant's broth is too bland and spicy for my taste. salty clear shine
87 Melissa found a very sharp knife in her mother's closet. blunt shiny chair
88 The swords in the museum were blunt and covered with jewels. sharp shiny chair
89 My roommate's furniture is very fancy compared to mine. plain dirty drink
90 Anne didn't feel comfortable in the plain restaurant and wanted to leave. fancy dirty drink
91 The company requires workers to slowly raise the crane to prevent accidents. lower drive table
92 Jacob was told to promptly lower the plow but he forgot. raise drive table
93 Even five men could not easily pull the boulder off the road. push drop blue
94 The directions state that you should push the lever when the red light is on. pull drop blue
95 The chemical laboratory's danger area is marked by a large sign. safety office easily
96 Steve's mother is concerned about safety issues and won't stop talking about them. danger office easily
97 The class noticed the repeated angel motifs in the paintings. devil daisy crawl
98 Jenna decided to dress in a sexy devil costume and won the prize. angel daisy crawl
99 Amy's little brother never argues with his parents. agrees drives dancer
100 The couple always agrees with each other and has a reputation. argues drives dancer
101 The kids always ignore when their shoes are untied. notice scream laptop
102 By all accounts, the janitor shouldn't notice that the toilet was overflowing. ignore scream laptop
103 The court officials cannot enter before the emperor allows them to. leave drink tiger
104 The restaurant patrons are urged to kindly leave by the side door. enter drink tiger
105 The artist loved to paint rural landscapes as well as portraits. urban green stare
106 The newlyweds decided to move somewhere urban for a change. rural green stare
107 Everyone driving by stops to see the damage done on the bridge. repair dances stifle
108 The PTA was eager to talk about the expensive repair happening at the school. damage dances stifle
109 Lucy thinks her parents will forbid her going to the dance. permit escort jagged
110 David would never permit a girl going out on the town. forbid escort jagged
111 The kids were excited to leave for their summer vacation and could barely sleep. winter school change
112 The children played all day during winter inside but there was no choice. summer school change

Appendix A.2.

Set of antonym stimuli used in Experiment 1. Target words (identical previews) are presented in boldface (not in boldface in the experiment). Columns to the right represent the synonym, plausible unrelated, and implausible unrelated previews.

Item Sentence Synonym Plausible Unrelated Implausible Unrelated
113 The dishes are stored below the sink in the kitchen. under aside order
114 Bob carefully put the glassware under the counter to make more room for other things. below aside order
115 The sons were quite lousy at doing their chores before dinner. awful happy rated
116 The neighbor's kids were too awful to remember to clean up their mess. lousy happy rated
117 Tim really wanted to become buddies with more people at his school. friends popular towards
118 Anna didn't want to remain friends with the weird kids in her class. buddies popular towards
119 At the zoo, Kim saw the giant adult panda eating bamboo leaves. older quiet album
120 There were many older men on the bus this morning. adult quiet album
121 Ian auctioned an antique clock to raise money for a charity. watch chair peace
122 Harry bought a broken watch to repair for fun. clock chair peace
123 Max had to have the teacher clarify when the homework assignment was due. explain restate captain
124 The lawyer asked the witness to carefully explain what he saw on the day of the crime. clarify restate captain
125 Everyone was pleased that the talented chef prepared such a wonderful meal. cook kids acid
126 The couple went to try what the famous cook had made for the food festival. chef kids acid
127 The new hairdresser has never placed curlers in another person's hair before. rollers flowers suffice
128 Jane had forgotten to take out several rollers from her hair and everyone stared. curlers flowers suffice
129 After working out, Shelley felt a sudden acute pain in her calves. sharp weird strip
130 Johnny had noticed sharp pains in his tooth and called the dentist. acute weird strip
131 The Johnson family loved the beautiful vast backyard at their new home. huge lush dogs
132 Hanna pleasantly looked out across huge stretches of ocean and contemplated life. vast lush dogs
133 The little girl complained about her upset tummy and asked to skip soccer practice. belly ankle darts
134 The puppy exposed his soft belly and enjoyed when it was rubbed. tummy ankle darts
135 Tammy noticed many items were still blank when checking over her exam. empty wrong imply
136 Dina gave Manny a cold empty stare when he asked a stupid question. blank wrong imply
137 Cops need to be aware of a possible ambush while on the job. attack murder effort
138 The building was closed due to a recent attack that occurred inside. ambush murder effort
139 Sara's friends watch the same movie every week because they can't agree on another one. video dance water
140 Jennifer went to watch the exciting video at the new theater. movie dance water
141 Betty enjoys going to the nearby town to have fun on the weekends. city lake only
142 It was Kara's dream to live near the beautiful city after retiring from her job. town lake only
143 Frank always sits in the exact middle of the classroom every day. center corner member
144 Sal lost his notes somewhere in the messy center of his bedroom. middle corner member
145 Greta's dog would always select the bowl with the treats inside. choose ignore chance
146 Lisa always manages to easily choose the correct answer, even if she isn't sure. select ignore chance
147 Lana's roommate would never scrub the dishes enough and they were always dirty. clean break alone
148 The boy tried to quickly clean the car window before rushing to his meeting. scrub break alone
149 Steven liked to make mean quips about his sister's boyfriends. jokes songs gates
150 Teresa's grandmother doesn't like to hear any jokes about the war. quips songs gates
151 The man was a notorious murderer and stories about him can be found in many papers. assassin lecturer enormous
152 The people feared that a dangerous assassin would come into town in the night. murderer lecturer enormous
153 The ring had beautiful jewels around the band. stones shapes cleans
154 The crown was inlaid with nice stones all over and was known throughout the land. jewels shapes cleans
155 We received a warning that a dangerous tornado might hit the town this week. twister tsunami booster
156 Sandra's grandmother is scared that a freak twister would suddenly come. tornado tsunami booster
157 After a while, Kelly noticed a weird scent coming from the trash can. smell sound vault
158 We ran away from the overpowering smell of the stink bomb exploding. scent sound vault
159 After dinner, Wendy always rinsed the dishes right away. washed forgot socket
160 Howard's chemistry lab partner washed the beakers in the sink. rinsed forgot socket
161 After witnessing the theft, many guards chased the thief. police people palace
162 Jason was nervous walking past police who were standing outside the embassy. guards people palace
163 Buying a car seems very essential for most college students. necessary expensive remaining
164 The sign stated that it is absolutely necessary that no garbage is left behind. essential expensive remaining
165 Although the apartment décor was too drab the owners felt it suited their needs. dull busy hulk
166 Jean says that dull clothing is all the rage these days. drab busy hulk
167 Andrew enjoyed the interesting tome he borrowed from the library. book tape jump
168 Pam almost forgot to return the heavy book in time. tome tape jump
169 Brad thought his project idea was incredibly ingenious and wanted to tell everyone. brilliant efficient fortified
170 Everybody applauded his brilliant gadget at the meeting. ingenious efficient fortified
171 Jim's children are often obdurate when it is time to clean up. stubborn restless stitches
172 The puppy was so extremely stubborn that it took three people to get him into the car. obdurate restless stitches
173 Chris is always told that he should relax after he gets back from work. sleep drink cheap
174 All Calvin wanted to do was sleep after getting home at midnight. relax drink cheap
175 Dan needed to have his molar replaced after the accident. tooth joint tenth
176 Peggy thought she cracked her tooth when she fell off the wall. molar joint tenth
177 Dave admired his well kept turf while driving home. lawn tree lava
178 George planned to get a new lawn for his front yard. turf tree lava
179 Every year the children wish for new toys. hope yell days
180 As December approaches, the students hope for Christmas vacation. wish yell days
181 Felix likes to wear clean boots to his line dancing party. shoes pants chess
182 Heidi always packs too many shoes when she goes on vacation. boots pants chess
183 Gary thought if he put on a costume he could excite the children in the class. thrill baffle thrift
184 Hailey was trying to subtly thrill her students by taking them to Disneyland. excite baffle thrift
185 George was afraid of stepping on a possibly lethal part of the abandoned building. deadly flimsy kindle
186 Shane jumped when he thought about deadly snakes that had been seen nearby. lethal flimsy kindle
187 Alice's perfume was very aromatic and caught the attention of many men. fragrant specific linguist
188 Angie bent down to the pleasantly fragrant rose and took a deep breath. aromatic specific linguist
189 Peg thought Jen would be somewhat envious about her engagement ring and kept it hidden. jealous excited gardens
190 Howard was extremely jealous because of Sam's new game boy. envious excited gardens
191 As the family approached their cabin they noticed some things were out of place. house place known
192 Ann always stays at the same house in Tahoe for vacation. cabin place known
193 Bo got a really cool gadget for his seventeenth birthday. device outfit drives
194 Jamie begged his parents to get him a nifty device but they didn't have the money. gadget outfit drives
195 She noticed that there was a small stone spire on top of the church. tower eagle horns
196 Despite hearing many stories, Jan didn't notice a single tower on her trip to Prague. spire eagle horns
197 Dale's company gave him a huge prize for his hard work. award party weird
198 The kids were bored until the teacher offered a special award for doing well on a test. prize party weird
199 Sheena wrote down the incorrect avenue and got lost on her way to the restaurant. street number clears
200 The police searched every street for the suspect. avenue number clears
201 Jack heard of more unusual sightings in the woods last week. strange raccoon storage
202 Marsi pondered the recent strange events and wondered how to make them stop. unusual raccoon storage
203 James agreed to meet at the large foyer in the hotel before dinner. lobby bench fifty
204 There were renovations on the outdated lobby and hotel patrons need to use a back door. foyer bench fifty
205 Joel made a rapid halt when the light turned red. stop turn ship
206 When Betty couldn't stop the car fast enough she ran into the median. halt turn ship
207 Last night my dreams were very lucid so I wrote about them in my journal. clear weird class
208 Austin peered down toward clear ponds in the distance. lucid weird class
209 The man took out his scary satan costume for Halloween. devil ghost trend
210 The family put an evil looking devil decoration on their front lawn. satan ghost trend
211 Anna's nanny made her a bracelet with string for her fifth birthday. thread silver threat
212 She thought that the bright thread was perfect for her art project. string silver threat
213 Nadine goes to the gym because she wants to look lean in a swimsuit at the beach. thin good kiss
214 The tall man looked very thin wearing his new tuxedo. lean good kiss
215 Peter was asked to point out on the large globe where Antarctica was. world print tried
216 He looked over the used world to find the country he was looking for. globe print tried
217 Samantha was very prudent about not making a mistake in her drawing. careful excited invited
218 The young boy was always careful when he received any money. prudent excited invited
219 Sheldon could not hear their answers over the loud music. replies screams replace
220 Rachel's new parrot replies whenever you ask it a question. answers screams replace
221 Some animals eat from very tall trees in the zoo. high rare kept
222 His stature was quite high for a boy of his age. tall rare kept
223 Some people think a heavy brick could break a window. stone punch clean
224 He was injured by the solid stone that was thrown at him. brick punch clean
225 Some people thought the parrot was mute but it just did not want to talk. dumb dead pile
226 The doctor said the man would be nearly dumb when the operation was finished. mute dead pile
227 The chemist did not realize the reaction could arise without a spark. occur erupt seven
228 A riot was certain to quickly occur if the people were not helped. arise erupt seven
229 The church received a beautiful piano from an anonymous donor. organ bible argue
230 She received a well used organ from her great grandmother. piano bible argue
231 The class complained about the boring exam to the professor. test book kind
232 The students thought the long test was rather tedious. exam book kind
233 The dog would always sniff the grass in front of the house. smell shred vault
234 Hillary was asked to quickly smell the leftover scented paper. sniff shred vault
235 The losing team's rebuttal was so legendary that it went viral on YouTube. response insanity congress
236 The angry mob's growing response could not be controlled. rebuttal insanity congress
237 The loving couple looked at the peaceful shore while on vacation. beach hills trust
238 No one lived in the house by the secluded beach for many years. shore hills trust
239 The notorious gang defaced the statue in front of city hall. damaged painted foreign
240 An unknown person damaged the wall of the museum defaced painted foreign
241 The police were alert after they got a call from dispatch. ready early table
242 Roger made sure he was always ready for his exams so that he would do well. alert early table
243 After the party the couch felt grimy from all the guests sitting on it. dirty comfy lunge
244 The hotel room was dirty when the maids came to clean it. grimy comfy lunge
245 The salesman said the sculpture would hold its original worth for many years. value shape sites
246 It was unfortunate that the great value of the house had diminished. worth shape sites
247 The sisters could not name all their favorite movies because there were too many. list take best
248 They were able to quickly list everything they needed. name take best
249 Everyone thought the student was very astute after meeting him. clever smelly cheese
250 Martin was known for being quite clever when he was younger. astute smelly cheese
251 The students always save all their homework when the quarter is over. keep burn long
252 Mark thought it was best to carefully keep his old personal documents. save burn long
253 The teacher always finds a relevant topic for the students to write about. issue movie musty
254 Danny thought that the unclear issue was pointless to discuss. topic movie musty
255 The teacher thought the reports were too brief and didn't give anyone a good grade. short awful stand
256 There was a very short news story on tv last night. brief awful stand
257 The team captains tried to establish concord between the two teams. harmony rivalry forming
258 The two cities lived in complete harmony for many years. concord rivalry forming
259 The most experienced scout led the group along the cliff. guide hiker quote
260 George was a skilled guide and had explored the trail many times. scout hiker quote
261 Tommy decided he would fling the stone in the pond later that day. throw leave floor
262 Billy was asked to just throw the frisbee on the table. fling leave floor
263 Will keeps a large knife in his backpack when he goes hiking. blade phone forge
264 Liam made sure to have a backup blade in case of emergencies. knife phone forge
265 The horse race will begin in a couple minutes. start close check
266 Registration for the event will start at the end of the month. begin close check
267 Kenny told his longtime rival to meet him outside after school. enemy tutor array
268 Andrew gained a major enemy at his new school. rival tutor array
269 Last week, Alexander totaled his newly bought car. wrecked painted awaited
270 They had heard that he fully wrecked his garage last Saturday. totaled painted awaited
271 Callie and her coworker must evade the office because their boss is mad at them. avoid block round
272 The criminal tried to stealthily avoid the cops in the building. evade block round
273 In kindergarten the kids would loudly notify the teacher when someone cut in line. inform bother actors
274 The junk mail would always inform its recipients with new offers. notify bother actors
275 In the morning Jessica tallied all the sales from last weekend to her boss. counted emailed existed
276 Peter made sure that he promptly counted the numbers to his friend. tallied emailed existed
277 The frogs in the pond can leap around the lily pads to get to the log. jump swim goat
278 Katie watched the kids jump in the pool at the rec center. leap swim goat
279 Sally forgot the specific tune she made up while walking home. song poem warp
280 Lisa taught the children a familiar song to keep them entertained. tune poem warp

Appendix B.

Experimental questions and response options for the comprehension questions used in Experiment 2. Item numbers correspond to those used in Experiment 1 (antonym stimulus set; note that items 67 and 68 were excluded for Experiment 2 and therefore there are no questions with those item numbers). Question version a was presented in the identical condition, version b was presented in the antonym condition, version c was presented in the plausible unrelated condition, and version d was presented in the implausible unrelated condition.

Item Question Option 1 Option 2
1 a Is Jane traveling to San Francisco? YES NO
b How will Jane travel to Los Angeles? SOUTH NORTH
c How will Jane travel to Los Angeles? SOUTH COACH
d Which of these words did you read? SOUTH HOUSE
2 a Does the family take a plane for their vacation? YES NO
b How does the family get to their vacation? NORTH SOUTH
c How does the family get to their vacation? NORTH COACH
d Which of these words did you read? NORTH HOUSE
3 a Did his friend give him advice? YES NO
b What was he advised to do with the bribe? ACCEPT REFUSE
c What was he advised to do with the bribe? ACCEPT REPORT
d Which of these words did you read? ACCEPT DEVICE
4 a Was Jane contemplating a bonus? YES NO
b What would Jane never do with a bonus? REFUSE ACCEPT
c What would Jane never do with a bonus? REFUSE REPORT
d Which of these words did you read? REFUSE DEVICE
5 a Was there a dog in the window? YES NO
b What did the ghost just do? APPEARED VANISHED
c What did the ghost just do? APPEARED SCREAMED
d Which of these words did you read? APPEARED NOMINATE
6 a Was the girl at the mall? YES NO
b What did the girl just do? VANISHED APPEARED
c What did the girl just do? VANISHED SCREAMED
d Which of these words did you read? VANISHED NOMINATE
7 a Does Jenny buy lotion? YES NO
b What lotion bottles does Jenny prefer? LARGE SMALL
c What lotion bottles does Jenny prefer? LARGE FANCY
d Which of these words did you read? LARGE WHALE
8 a Were there earrings on display? YES NO
b What kind of earrings did she like? SMALL LARGE
c What kind of earrings did she like? SMALL FANCY
d Which of these words did you read? SMALL WHALE
9 a Did George wear a T-shirt? YES NO
b What type of suit did George wear? BLACK WHITE
c What type of suit did George wear? BLACK SHINY
d Which of these words did you read? BLACK YEARS
10 a Did Deb drive a motorcycle? YES NO
b What type of car did Deb test drive? WHITE BLACK
c What type of car did Deb test drive? WHITE SHINY
d Which of these words did you read? WHITE YEARS
11 a Did the new student answer the question? YES NO
b How did the student answer the question? BOLDLY MEEKLY
c How did the student answer the question? BOLDLY EASILY
d Which of these words did you read? BOLDLY KILLER
12 a Was Karen the last person to jump over the stream? YES NO
b How did she jump? MEEKLY BOLDLY
c How did she jump? MEEKLY EASILY
d Which of these words did you read? MEEKLY KILLER
13 a Did the children fix the toilet? YES NO
b How are the children? CLEVER STUPID
c How are the children? CLEVER CLUMSY
d Which of these words did you read? CLEVER ROAMER
14 a Does my nephew have homework? YES NO
b How is the nephew with housework? STUPID CLEVER
c How is the nephew with housework? STUPID CLUMSY
d Which of these words did you read? STUPID ROAMER
15 a Did the food heat up on the drive? YES NO
b After the trip, how was the food? COOL WARM
c After the trip, how was the food? COOL SOFT
d Which of these words did you read? COOL NOSE
16 a Does David have a temperature preference for his potato salad? YES NO
b What kind of salad does David like? WARM COOL
c What kind of salad does David like? WARM SOFT
d Which of these words did you read? WARM NOSE
17 a Does the teacher have students? YES NO
b How does the teacher's personality seem? KIND MEAN
c How does the teacher's personality seem? KIND DULL
d Which of these words did you read? KIND LUCK
18 a Did Jen have classmates? YES NO
b What is Jen's classmate like? MEAN KIND
c What is Jen's classmate like? MEAN DULL
d Which of these words did you read? MEAN LUCK
19 a Were we able to give Sam a ride? YES NO
b When did the train arrive? DUSK DAWN
c When did the train arrive? DUSK OURS
d Which of these words did you read? DUSK YEAR
20 a Are we always waiting for Mindy and her dog? YES NO
b When did Mindy's dog wake up? DAWN DUSK
c When did Mindy's dog wake up? DAWN OURS
d Which of these words did you read? DAWN YEAR
21 a Did we have a final for the class? YES NO
b How was the final compared to the midterm? EASY HARD
c How was the final compared to the midterm? EASY LONG
d Which of these words did you read? EASY PENS
22 a Was the hike down a canyon? YES NO
b What was the mountain hike like? HARD EASY
c What was the mountain hike like? HARD LONG
d Which of these words did you read? HARD PENS
23 a Did Kevin's brother eat all the fruit? YES NO
b What kind of bread did he eat? FRESH STALE
c What kind of bread did he eat? FRESH BAKED
d Which of these words did you read? FRESH PLACE
24 a Did the supermarket sell pastries on the weekend? YES NO
b What kind of pastries was the supermarket selling? STALE FRESH
c What kind of pastries was the supermarket selling? STALE BAKED
d Which of these words did you read? STALE PLACE
25 a Does everyone look skinny next to Henry? YES NO
b What would someone next to Henry look like? GIANT DWARF
c What would someone next to Henry look like? GIANT MODEL
d Which of these words did you read? GIANT PEELS
26 a Was there someone famous at the beach? YES NO
b Who did I see at the beach yesterday? DWARF GIANT
c Who did I see at the beach yesterday? DWARF MODEL
d Which of these words did you read? DWARF PEELS
27 a Was he carrying a bag? YES NO
b What did the box become? HEAVIER LIGHTER
c What did the box become? HEAVIER DIRTIER
d Which of these words did you read? HEAVIER CHECKER
28 a Did Betty go to the gym a lot? YES NO
b How does Betty think she looks? LIGHTER HEAVIER
c How does Betty think she looks? LIGHTER DIRTIER
d Which of these words did you read? LIGHTER CHECKER
29 a Did the girl want less salad? YES NO
b What did the girl ask for regarding the dressing? LESS MORE
c What did the girl ask for regarding the dressing? LESS TART
d Which of these words did you read? LESS BATS
30 a Did John need more pears? YES NO
b What should he have baked into the pie? MORE LESS
c What should he have baked into the pie? MORE TART
d Which of these words did you read? MORE BATS
31 a Did Ron get his pants tailored? YES NO
b How are Ron's pants now? LOOSE TIGHT
c How are Ron's pants now? LOOSE SHORT
d Which of these words did you read? LOOSE POLES
32 a Does the coat fit well? YES NO
b How does the coat fit on the professor? TIGHT LOOSE
c How does the coat fit on the professor? TIGHT SHORT
d Which of these words did you read? TIGHT POLES
33 a Are there dogs at the house? YES NO
b How do the dogs behave when there are visitors? NOISY QUIET
c How do the dogs behave when there are visitors? NOISY EAGER
d Which of these words did you read? NOISY OCEAN
34 a Are the dogs at story time? YES NO
b How are the children at story time? QUIET NOISY
c How are the children at story time? QUIET EAGER
d Which of these words did you read? QUIET OCEAN
35 a Is there a bridge before the town? YES NO
b How does the train pass the bridge? UNDER ABOVE
c How does the train pass the bridge? UNDER ALONG
d Which of these words did you read? UNDER BLAND
36 a Are visitors trying to get to Sarah's house? YES NO
b How do visitors get past the cliff? ABOVE UNDER
c How do visitors get past the cliff? ABOVE ALONG
d Which of these words did you read? ABOVE BLAND
37 a Did Dana get a tattoo yesterday? YES NO
b What kind of tattoo does Dana have? PERMANENT TEMPORARY
c What kind of tattoo does Dana have? PERMANENT OFFENSIVE
d Which of these words did you read? PERMANENT SCHOOLBAG
38 a Did Jeff quit his job yet? YES NO
b What type of job did Jeff have? TEMPORARY PERMANENT
c What type of job did Jeff have? TEMPORARY OFFENSIVE
d Which of these words did you read? TEMPORARY SCHOOLBAG
39 a Are people disagreeing over a jewel? YES NO
b What could nobody agree on about the jewel? REAL FAKE
c What could nobody agree on about the jewel? REAL PINK
d Which of these words did you read? REAL BARS
40 a Did people believe Zoe? YES NO
b What does Zoe deny about her hair color? FAKE REAL
c What does Zoe deny about her hair color? FAKE PINK
d Which of these words did you read? FAKE BARS
41 a Did everyone know about the girl? YES NO
b What was the girl's neighborhood like? RICH POOR
c What was the girl's neighborhood like? RICH LAZY
d Which of these words did you read? RICH MALL
42 a Was the previous roommate popular? YES NO
b What was the roommate like? POOR RICH
c What was the roommate like? POOR LAZY
d Which of these words did you read? POOR MALL
43 a Did Kristy have an exam? YES NO
b How was her answer written? RIGHT WRONG
c How was her answer written? RIGHT BADLY
d Which of these words did you read? RIGHT FLUTE
44 a Was Olga setting up equipment? YES NO
b How did she set up the equipment? WRONG RIGHT
c How did she set up the equipment? WRONG BADLY
d Which of these words did you read? WRONG FLUTE
45 a Does the boy want his vegetables separately? YES NO
b How does the boy like his vegetables? SEPARATE TOGETHER
c How does the boy like his vegetables? SEPARATE LUKEWARM
d Which of these words did you read? SEPARATE DISCOVER
46 a Are they storing food? YES NO
b How must the chemicals be stored? TOGETHER SEPARATE
c How must the chemicals be stored? TOGETHER LUKEWARM
d Which of these words did you read? TOGETHER DISCOVER
47 a Were there many stories in the news? YES NO
b What kind of stories were in the news? SERIOUS TRIVIAL
c What kind of stories were in the news? SERIOUS STRANGE
d Which of these words did you read? SERIOUS ALREADY
48 a Was there an accident? YES NO
b What kind of accident was it? TRIVIAL SERIOUS
c What kind of accident was it? TRIVIAL STRANGE
d Which of these words did you read? TRIVIAL ALREADY
49 a Was there a new event? YES NO
b What was the event considered as? SUCCESS FAILURE
c What was the event considered as? SUCCESS SCANDAL
d Which of these words did you read? SUCCESS KITTENS
50 a Did the actress join a TV show? YES NO
b What was the TV show considered as? FAILURE SUCCESS
c What was the TV show considered as? FAILURE SCANDAL
d Which of these words did you read? FAILURE KITTENS
51 a Was the man given cats? YES NO
b What were the dogs like, to cause his complaint? TAME WILD
c What were the dogs like, to cause his complaint? TAME LOUD
d Which of these words did you read? TAME BALL
52 a Did the circus have lions? YES NO
b What kind of lions were at the circus? WILD TAME
c What kind of lions were at the circus? WILD LOUD
d Which of these words did you read? WILD BALL
53 a Does Jessica drive on the highway? YES NO
b How does Jessica drive? FAST SLOW
c How does Jessica drive? FAST LONG
d Which of these words did you read? FAST YELP
54 a Was the boat trip enjoyable? YES NO
b How was the boat trip? SLOW FAST
c How was the boat trip? SLOW LONG
d Which of these words did you read? SLOW YELP
55 a Does she like to sleep on the floor? YES NO
b What kind of mattresses does she prefer? SOFT HARD
c What kind of mattresses does she prefer? SOFT MANY
d Which of these words did you read? SOFT JUMP
56 a Does Jason like eating fruit while doing homework? YES NO
b What kind of candy does he eat? HARD SOFT
c What kind of candy does he eat? HARD MANY
d Which of these words did you read? HARD JUMP
57 a Are the fruits special? YES NO
b What happened to the special fruit? EXPORTED IMPORTED
c What happened to the special fruit? EXPORTED SNATCHED
d Which of these words did you read? EXPORTED BELIEVER
58 a Was there an embargo? YES NO
b What did people do with the goods? IMPORTED EXPORTED
c What did people do with the goods? IMPORTED SNATCHED
d Which of these words did you read? IMPORTED BELIEVER
59 a Does Joshua go to school? YES NO
b What is Joshua always like? DRUNK SOBER
c What is Joshua always like? DRUNK ANGRY
d Which of these words did you read? DRUNK GAUGE
60 a Did Henry have a party last week? YES NO
b How were the people at Henry's party? SOBER DRUNK
c How were the people at Henry's party? SOBER ANGRY
d Which of these words did you read? SOBER GAUGE
61 a Was the woman walking her cat? YES NO
b What kind of dogs was she walking? TINY HUGE
c What kind of dogs was she walking? TINY GRAY
d Which of these words did you read? TINY WHAT
62 a Does Dale like horses? YES NO
b What kind of horse does Dale like? HUGE TINY
c What kind of horse does Dale like? HUGE GRAY
d Which of these words did you read? HUGE WHAT
63 a Did he put them with the dishes? YES NO
b Where did Ian put the containers? ABOVE BELOW
c Where did Ian put the containers? ABOVE ASIDE
d Which of these words did you read? ABOVE HAIRY
64 a Was Alex looking for her earring? YES NO
b Where were her glasses? BELOW ABOVE
c Where were her glasses? BELOW ASIDE
d Which of these words did you read? BELOW HAIRY
65 a Does she like giving candy to the children? YES NO
b What does she do with the apples? GIVE TAKE
c What does she do with the apples? GIVE SHOW
d Which of these words did you read? GIVE CAPE
66 a Did the student forget to do the research paper? YES NO
b What did he do with the paper? TAKE GIVE
c What did he do with the paper? TAKE SHOW
d Which of these words did you read? TAKE CAPE
69 a Are Robert's tools the best? YES NO
b How are Robert's tools? INFERIOR SUPERIOR
c How are Robert's tools? INFERIOR DELICATE
d Which of these words did you read? INFERIOR ABSENTLY
70 a Does the bakery sell pastries? YES NO
b How are the pastries at this bakery? SUPERIOR INFERIOR
c How are the pastries at this bakery? SUPERIOR DELICATE
d Which of these words did you read? SUPERIOR ABSENTLY
71 a Did Bob paint the building more than once? YES NO
b What did Bob have to repaint? EXTERIOR INTERIOR
c What did Bob have to repaint? EXTERIOR INSIGNIA
d Which of these words did you read? EXTERIOR BATHROBE
72 a Is the company shoddy-looking? YES NO
b Which part of the company is fancy? INTERIOR EXTERIOR
c Which part of the company is fancy? INTERIOR INSIGNIA
d Which of these words did you read? INTERIOR BATHROBE
73 a Was the jar filled with candies? YES NO
b How did the cookie level change? DECREASED INCREASED
c How did the cookie level change? DECREASED UNCHANGED
d Which of these words did you read? DECREASED AFTERGLOW
74 a Was there classwork? YES NO
b How did the classwork load change? INCREASED DECREASED
c How did the classwork load change? INCREASED UNCHANGED
d Which of these words did you read? INCREASED AFTERGLOW
75 a Did I lose my keys in the garden? YES NO
b What did I do to the keys? FIND LOSE
c What did I do to the keys? FIND TOSS
d Which of these words did you read? FIND BLUE
76 a Did Darren lose his watch? YES NO
b What did he do to the ring? LOSE FIND
c What did he do to the ring? LOSE TOSS
d Which of these words did you read? LOSE BLUE
77 a Was the front door left open? YES NO
b How did they leave the garage door? CLOSED OPENED
c How did they leave the garage door? CLOSED BROKEN
d Which of these words did you read? CLOSED BANANA
78 a Is the store normally closed on the weekend? YES NO
b Over the weekend, how was the storefront? OPENED CLOSED
c Over the weekend, how was the storefront? OPENED BROKEN
d Which of these words did you read? OPENED BANANA
79 a Is she arriving in a train? YES NO
b What will her plane be doing this afternoon? ARRIVE DEPART
c What will her plane be doing this afternoon? ARRIVE REFUEL
d Which of these words did you read? ARRIVE TURTLE
80 a Did Gary have any problem with the sudden change in plans? YES NO
b What did Gary have to do? DEPART ARRIVE
c What did Gary have to do? DEPART REFUEL
d Which of these words did you read? DEPART TURTLE
81 a Did the team see the competition? YES NO
b What did the team expect? VICTORY FAILURE
c What did the team expect? VICTORY QUARREL
d Which of these words did you read? VICTORY POLYGON
82 a Did we watch TV last night? YES NO
b What was on TV last night? FAILURE VICTORY
c What was on TV last night? FAILURE QUARREL
d Which of these words did you read? FAILURE POLYGON
83 a Was Ted listening to a singer? YES NO
b What kind of voice did the singer have? LOUD SOFT
c What kind of voice did the singer have? LOUD CUTE
d Which of these words did you read? LOUD LEAP
84 a Was Jeffrey at his office? YES NO
b What kind of music was playing at the party? SOFT LOUD
c What kind of music was playing at the party? SOFT CUTE
d Which of these words did you read? SOFT LEAP
85 a Does Amanda like sour dessert? YES NO
b What kind of soup does Amanda prefer? SALTY BLAND
c What kind of soup does Amanda prefer? SALTY CLEAR
d Which of these words did you read? SALTY SHINE
86 a Is the broth spicy? YES NO
b What was the broth like? BLAND SALTY
c What was the broth like? BLAND CLEAR
d Which of these words did you read? BLAND SHINE
87 a Was there a knife in the closet? YES NO
b What type of knife did she find? SHARP BLUNT
c What type of knife did she find? SHARP SHINY
d Which of these words did you read? SHARP CHAIR
88 a Were the swords plain? YES NO
b What were the museum swords like? BLUNT SHARP
c What were the museum swords like? BLUNT SHINY
d Which of these words did you read? BLUNT CHAIR
89 a Is my roommate's furniture different from mine? YES NO
b How was the roommate's furniture? FANCY PLAIN
c How was the roommate's furniture? FANCY DIRTY
d Which of these words did you read? FANCY DRINK
90 a Did Anne like the restaurant? YES NO
b What was the restaurant like? PLAIN FANCY
c What was the restaurant like? PLAIN DIRTY
d Which of these words did you read? PLAIN DRINK
91 a Does the company try to prevent accidents? YES NO
b What are the workers supposed to do with the crane? RAISE LOWER
c What are the workers supposed to do with the crane? RAISE DRIVE
d Which of these words did you read? RAISE TABLE
92 a Did Jacob remember the instructions? YES NO
b What was he supposed to do with the plow? LOWER RAISE
c What was he supposed to do with the plow? LOWER DRIVE
d Which of these words did you read? LOWER TABLE
93 a Was the boulder on the road? YES NO
b What are they trying to do with the boulder? PULL PUSH
c What are they trying to do with the boulder? PULL DROP
d Which of these words did you read? PULL BLUE
94 a Is the light green? YES NO
b What is supposed to be done with the lever? PUSH PULL
c What is supposed to be done with the lever? PUSH DROP
d Which of these words did you read? PUSH BLUE
95 a Is there a large sign in the lab? YES NO
b Which area in the lab was marked? DANGER SAFETY
c Which area in the lab was marked? DANGER OFFICE
d Which of these words did you read? DANGER EASILY
96 a Is his mother concerned? YES NO
b What issues is his mother concerned with? SAFETY DANGER
c What issues is his mother concerned with? SAFETY OFFICE
d Which of these words did you read? SAFETY EASILY
97 a Was the class looking at sculptures? YES NO
b What was repeated in the paintings? ANGEL DEVIL
c What was repeated in the paintings? ANGEL DAISY
d Which of these words did you read? ANGEL CRAWL
98 a Did Jenna win a prize? YES NO
b What costume did Jenna wear? DEVIL ANGEL
c What costume did Jenna wear? DEVIL DAISY
d Which of these words did you read? DEVIL CRAWL
99 a Does Amy have a brother? YES NO
b What doesn't he do with his parents? ARGUES AGREES
c What doesn't he do with his parents? ARGUES DRIVES
d Which of these words did you read? ARGUES DANCER
100 a Is the couple well known? YES NO
b What does the couple always do with each other? AGREES ARGUES
c What does the couple always do with each other? AGREES DRIVES
d Which of these words did you read? AGREES DANCER
101 a Were the kids wearing sandals? YES NO
b What did the kids do about their untied laces? IGNORE NOTICE
c What did the kids do about their untied laces? IGNORE SCREAM
d Which of these words did you read? IGNORE LAPTOP
102 a Did the sink overflow? YES NO
b What shouldn't the janitor do about the toilet? NOTICE IGNORE
c What shouldn't the janitor do about the toilet? NOTICE SCREAM
d Which of these words did you read? NOTICE LAPTOP
103 a Does the emperor give the orders? YES NO
b What does the emperor allow court officials to do? ENTER LEAVE
c What does the emperor allow court officials to do? ENTER DRINK
d Which of these words did you read? ENTER TIGER
104 a Are the patrons urged rudely? YES NO
b How do patrons use the side door? LEAVE ENTER
c How do patrons use the side door? LEAVE DRINK
d Which of these words did you read? LEAVE TIGER
105 a Does the artist hate landscapes? YES NO
b What kind of landscapes does the artist paint? RURAL URBAN
c What kind of landscapes does the artist paint? RURAL GREEN
d Which of these words did you read? RURAL STARE
106 a Are the newlyweds moving? YES NO
b What kind of place did they move to? URBAN RURAL
c What kind of place did they move to? URBAN GREEN
d Which of these words did you read? URBAN STARE
107 a Is everyone looking at a bridge? YES NO
b What is everyone looking at on the bridge? DAMAGE REPAIR
c What is everyone looking at on the bridge? DAMAGE DANCES
d Which of these words did you read? DAMAGE STIFLE
108 a Is the PTA talking about a zoo? YES NO
b What is happening at school? REPAIR DAMAGE
c What is happening at school? REPAIR DANCES
d Which of these words did you read? REPAIR STIFLE
109 a Does Lucy want to go to the dance? YES NO
b What would her parents do about Lucy going to the dance? FORBID PERMIT
c What would her parents do about Lucy going to the dance? FORBID ESCORT
d Which of these words did you read? FORBID JAGGED
110 a Does the girl want to go to a museum? YES NO
b What would David do about the girl going out? PERMIT FORBID
c What would David do about the girl going out? PERMIT ESCORT
d Which of these words did you read? PERMIT JAGGED
111 a Are the kids going on vacation? YES NO
b Which vacation were the kids leaving for? SUMMER WINTER
c Which vacation were the kids leaving for? SUMMER SCHOOL
d Which of these words did you read? SUMMER CHANGE
112 a Did they have to play inside? YES NO
b When did the children play inside? WINTER SUMMER
c When did the children play inside? WINTER SCHOOL
d Which of these words did you read? WINTER CHANGE

Footnotes

1

When models did not converge with the maximum random effects structure we removed the random effect that accounted for the least amount of variance until convergence and we report the results of the largest model that converged. For the model on gaze duration this was the random slopes for item in the model on the raw and log transformed data. For the model on log-transformed first fixation data this was the slope for the interaction for subjects.

2

The interaction between stimulus set and the identical preview benefit was significant in go-past time. However, because this is the only measure in which this occurred we consider this to be an anomaly and did not pursue it further.

3

There was a significant interaction between stimulus set and the contrast for the identical preview condition in the probability of regressions out of the target, which likely contributed to the interaction in go-past time. As with go-past time, we did not pursue this effect further.

4

The reason that the statistics show differences across conditions that are not apparent in the table of means is due to aggregation across subjects when reporting the means but not in the GLMMs, as well as rounding to two decimal places when reporting the means but using the raw binary outcome as the dependent variable in the GLMMs.

5

The random effects that were removed were the following. For the model on raw single fixation duration we removed the slope for the identical preview contrast for subjects. For the model on raw gaze duration we removed the correlation between the slope for the plausible preview contrast from the other effects for items. For the model on log-transformed go-past time we removed the correlation between the intercept and slopes for items. For the model on raw total time we removed the correlation between the intercept and slopes for items.

6

In addition to the differences noted in the text, the plausible preview benefit was only marginally significant in first fixation duration whereas it was fully significant in Experiment 1.

7

The lack of differences in fixation probability is likely because the increased rate of questions (i.e., one after every sentence) made readers more cautious when reading the sentence and increased fixation probabilities overall (e.g., 81–85% in Experiment 1 compared to 85–87% in Experiment 2).

References

  1. Ashby J, Rayner K. Representing syllable information during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2004;19:391–426. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ashby J, Treiman R, Kessler B, Rayner K. Vowel processing during silent reading: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2006;32:416–424. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.2.416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baayen RH, Davidson DH, Bates DM. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language. 2008;59:390–412. [Google Scholar]
  4. Balota DA, Pollatsek A, Rayner K. The interaction of contextual constraints and parafoveal visual information in reading. Cognitive Psychology. 1985;17:364–390. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(85)90013-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language. 2013;68:255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
  7. Blanchard HE, McConkie GW, Zola D, Wolverton GS. Time course of visual information utilization during fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1984;10:75–89. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Briihl D, Inhoff AW. Integrating information across fixations during reading: The use of orthographic bodies and of exterior letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1995;21:55–67. [Google Scholar]
  9. Chace KH, Rayner K, Well AD. Eye movements and phonological parafoveal preview: Effects of reading skill. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2005;59:209–217. doi: 10.1037/h0087476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. DeLong KA, Troyer M, Kutas M. Pre-Processing in Sentence Comprehension: Sensitivity to Likely Upcoming Meaning and Structure. Language and Linguistics Compass. 2014;8:631–645. doi: 10.1111/lnc3.12093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Drieghe D, Rayner K, Pollatsek A. Eye movements and word skipping during reading revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2005;31:954–969. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hauk O. Preface to special issue “prediction in language comprehension and production”. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience. 2016;31:1–3. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1102299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hohenstein S, Kliegl R. Semantic preview benefit during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2014;40:166–190. doi: 10.1037/a0033670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hohenstein S, Laubrock J, Kliegl R. Semantic preview benefit in eye movements during reading: A parafoveal fast- priming study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2010;36:1150–1170. doi: 10.1037/a0020233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hutchinson KA. Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2003;10:785–813. doi: 10.3758/bf03196544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Inhoff AW. Parafoveal word perception during eye fixations in reading: Effects of visual salience and word structure. In: Coltheart M, editor. Attention and Performance XII. Erlbaum; Hillsdale: 1987. pp. 403–418. [Google Scholar]
  17. Inhoff AW. Lexical access during eye fixations in reading: Are word access codes used to integrate lexical information across interword fixations? Journal of Memory and Language. 1989a;28:444–461. [Google Scholar]
  18. Inhoff AW. Parafoveal processing of words and saccade computation during eye fixations in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1989b;15:544–555. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.15.3.544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Inhoff AW. Integrating information across eye fixations in reading: The role of letter and word units. Acta Psychologica. 1990;73:281–297. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(90)90027-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Inhoff AW, Tousman S. Lexical priming from partial- word previews. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1990;16:825–836. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.16.5.825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Laubrock J, Hohenstein S. Orthographic consistency and parafoveal preview benefit: A resource-sharing account of language differences in processing of phonological and semantic codes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2012;35:292–293. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Levy R, Bicknell K, Slattery T, Rayner K. Eye movement evidence that readers maintain and act on uncertainty about past linguistic input. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:21086–21090. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907664106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Li X, Liu P, Rayner K. Saccade target selection in Chinese reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2015;22:524–530. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0693-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lima SD, Inhoff AW. Lexical access during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word-initial letter sequence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1985;11:272–285. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.11.3.272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Liu W, Inhoff AW, Ye Y, Wu C. Use of parafoveally visible characters during the reading of Chinese sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2002;28:1213–1227. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.28.5.1213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. McConkie GW, Rayner K. Asymmetry of the perceptual span in reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 1976;8:365–368. [Google Scholar]
  27. Miellet S, Sparrow L. Phonological codes are assembled before word fixation: Evidence from boundary paradigm in sentence reading. Brain and Language. 2004;90:299–310. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00442-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Morrison RE. Manipulation of stimulus onset delay in reading: evidence for parallel programming of saccades. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1984;10:667–682. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.5.667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Pollatsek A, Lesch M, Morris RK, Rayner K. Phonological codes are used in integrating information across saccades in word identification and reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1992;18:148–162. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.18.1.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. R Core Team . R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. URL https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  31. Rayner K. The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading. Cognitive Psychology. 1975;7:65–81. [Google Scholar]
  32. Rayner K. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;124:372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rayner K. The thirty fifth Sir Frederick Bartlett lecture: Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2009;62:1457–1506. doi: 10.1080/17470210902816461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rayner K. The gaze-contingent moving window in reading: Development and review. Visual Cognition. 2014;22:242–258. [Google Scholar]
  35. Rayner K, Balota DA, Pollatsek A. Against parafoveal semantic preprocessing during eye fixations in reading. Canadian Journal of Psychology. 1986;40:473–483. doi: 10.1037/h0080111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Rayner K, Schotter ER. Semantic preview benefit in reading English: The effect of initial letter capitalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2014;40:1617–1628. doi: 10.1037/a0036763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rayner K, Schotter ER, Drieghe D. Lack of semantic parafoveal preview benefit in reading revisited. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2014;21:1067–1072. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0582-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rayner K, Sereno SC, Lesch MF, Pollatsek A. Phonological codes are automatically activated during reading: Evidence from an eye movement priming paradigm. Psychological Science. 1995;6:26–32. [Google Scholar]
  39. Rayner K, Well AD. Effects of contextual constraint on eye movements in reading: A further examination. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 1996;3:504–509. doi: 10.3758/BF03214555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Reichle ED. Serial attention models of reading. In: Liversedge SP, Gilchrist ID, Everling S, editors. Oxford handbook on eye movements. Oxford University Press; Oxford: 2011. pp. 767–786. [Google Scholar]
  41. Reichle ED, Pollatsek A, Fisher DL, Rayner K. Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review. 1998;105:125–157. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.105.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Reichle ED, Reingold EM. Neurophysiological constraints on the eye-mind link. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Reichle ED, Warren T, McConnell K. Using EZ Reader to model the effects of higher level language processing on eye movements during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2009;16:1–21. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.1.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schotter ER. Synonyms provide semantic preview benefit in English. Journal of Memory and Language. 2013;69:619–633. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schotter ER, Angele B, Rayner K. Parafoveal processing in reading. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 2012;74:5–35. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0219-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schotter ER, Lee M, Reiderman M, Rayner K. The effect of contextual constraint on parafoveal processing in reading. Journal of Memory and Language. 2015;83:118–139. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2015.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Schotter ER, Reichle ED, Rayner K. Rethinking parafoveal processing in reading: Serial attention models can account for semantic preview benefit and n + 2 preview effects. Visual Cognition. 2014;22:309–333. [Google Scholar]
  48. Slattery TJ. Word misperception, the neighbor frequency effect, and the role of sentence context: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2009;35:1969–1975. doi: 10.1037/a0016894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Sparrow L, Miellet S. Activation of phonological codes during reading: Evidence from errors detection and eye movements. Brain and Language. 2002;81:509–516. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Taylor WL. “Cloze procedure”: A new tool for measuring readability. Journalism Quarterly. 1953;30:415–433. [Google Scholar]
  51. Tsai JL, Lee CY, Tzeng OJL, Hung DL, Yen NS. Use of phonological codes for Chinese characters: Evidence from processing of parafoveal preview when reading sentences. Brain and Language. 2004;91:235–244. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Veldre A, Andrews S. Semantic Preview Benefit in English: Individual Differences in the Extraction and Use of Parafoveal Semantic Information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2016a doi: 10.1037/xlm0000212. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Veldre A, Andrews S. Is semantic preview benefit due to relatedness or plausibility? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2016b doi: 10.1037/xhp0000200. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. White SJ, Johnson RL, Liversedge SP, Rayner K. Eye movements when reading transposed text: The importance of word-beginning letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2008;34:1261–1276. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Yan M, Kliegl R, Richter EM, Nuthmann A, Shu H. Flexible saccade-target selection in Chinese reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2010;63:705–725. doi: 10.1080/17470210903114858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Yan M, Richter EM, Shu H, Kliegl R. Readers of Chinese extract semantic information from parafoveal words. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2009;16:561–566. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.3.561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Yan M, Zhou W, Shu H, Kliegl R. Lexical and sublexical semantic preview benefits in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2012;38:1069–1075. doi: 10.1037/a0026935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Yang J. Preview effects of plausibility and character order in reading Chinese transposed words: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Research in Reading. 2013;36:S18–S34. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2013.01558.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Yang J, Li N, Wang S, Slattery TJ, Rayner K. Encoding the target or the plausible preview word? The nature of the plausibility preview benefit in reading Chinese. Visual Cognition. 2014;22:193–213. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2014.890689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Yang J, Wang S, Tong X, Rayner K. Semantic and plausibility effects on preview benefit during eye fixations in Chinese reading. Reading and Writing. 2010;25:1031–1052. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010-9281-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES