Significance
Many large-scale parent interventions turn out to be ineffective, particularly for socioeconomically disadvantaged families—possibly because some parents believe that their children’s reading skills are relatively fixed and unresponsive to practicing. This study shows large and consistent effects on both reading and writing skills of second-grade children whose parents received a few children’s books and information about the value of supporting children when learning to read. Effects are at least as large for children with immigrant background or low-educated mothers as for other children—and biggest for those children whose parents before the intervention believed reading abilities to be relatively fixed. The study thereby shows a direction for effective parent interventions.
Keywords: education, parent intervention, randomized controlled trial, reading intervention, parental beliefs
Abstract
Laboratory experiments have shown that parents who believe their child’s abilities are fixed engage with their child in unconstructive, performance-oriented ways. We show that children of parents with such “fixed mindsets” have lower reading skills, even after controlling for the child’s previous abilities and the parents’ socioeconomic status. In a large-scale randomized field trial (Nclassrooms = 72; Nchildren = 1,587) conducted by public authorities, parents receiving a reading intervention were told about the malleability of their child’s reading abilities and how to support their child by praising his/her effort rather than his/her performance. This low-cost intervention increased the reading and writing achievements of all participating children—not least immigrant children with non-Western backgrounds and children with low-educated mothers. As expected, effects were even bigger for parents who before the intervention had a fixed mindset.
Across the world, family background explains a substantial variation in children’s abilities (1), and there is substantial variation in how and how often parents spend time with their children (2). There may, therefore, be a large potential in supporting parents in helping their children to learn, especially compared with the effect of increasing the time that children spend with teachers in school, where evidence is mixed (3). Unfortunately, many large-scale parent interventions turn out to be ineffective, particularly for socioeconomically disadvantaged families (4–6).
One reason for the ineffectiveness of some parent interventions may be that some parents do not believe that they can make much of a difference to their child’s abilities. They may, therefore, interact with the child in unconstructive ways. Laboratory experiments show that parents who tend to believe that their child’s ability to learn is innate [that is, parents with a “fixed mindset” (7, 8)] interact with their child in a more controlling way, focusing on the performance rather than the effort of the child, compared with parents with a more incremental or “growth mindset” (9). Furthermore, parents praising performance rather than effort induce a fixed mindset in the child (10–12), and parent praise to 1 to 3 y olds predicts the child’s motivational framework 5 y on (13).
Only recently have social–psychological or academic mindset interventions been tested in ways that are potentially scalable (14–17). However, growth approach interventions—that explain to parents that reading abilities are malleable and reward effort rather than performance—may be very effective in large scale. They address the problem that parents with a fixed mindset may not comply with ordinary interventions, precisely because they do not believe that it will make much of a difference.
We show that a reading intervention with a growth mindset approach delivered by public authorities had large average intention to treat effects on childrens’ reading achievements in three domains and childrens’ skills in writing their own narrative. As expected, effects were strongest for children whose parents had a more fixed mindset before the intervention.
Materials and Methods
A large-scale classroom-randomized trial included 72 classrooms with 1,587 second-grade children in Aarhus Municipality in Denmark (Fig. S1). The randomized, controlled trial was approved by the municipality. The Danish Data Protection Agency approved the collection and treatment of all data for the project (approval no. 2013-41-1793). All schools were informed about the trial and data collection before enrolling on a voluntary basis. It was voluntary for parents and children to read together and to use the books provided (see below). Parents were informed in five different languages that it was voluntary to participate in the survey. They were also informed that if they participated in the survey their responses would be treated anonymously and confidentially.
We rank-ordered the classrooms on mean child language skills, and then, we created strata of four classrooms and randomized two to treatment and two to control within each stratum. Table S1 shows baseline characteristics and balance between treatment and control groups.
Table S1.
Variable | Control group | Treatment group | P value |
Child is a boy | 0.51 | 0.51 | 0.95 |
Child’s age in 2013, y | 8.12 | 8.12 | 0.95 |
Child immigrant | 0.23 | 0.19 | 0.04** |
Child living with both parents | 0.76 | 0.73 | 0.20 |
Child living with single parent | 0.22 | 0.22 | 0.85 |
Child living with parent in new relationship | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.04** |
Child not living with own parents | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.16 |
Missing information about living arrangement | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.41 |
No. of children in family | 2.45 | 2.40 | 0.33 |
Mother compulsory education | 0.19 | 0.17 | 0.29 |
Mother upper secondary education | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.30 |
Mother vocational education | 0.24 | 0.24 | 0.89 |
Mother short-cycle education | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.40 |
Mother medium-cycle education | 0.23 | 0.22 | 0.80 |
Mother long-cycle education | 0.20 | 0.21 | 0.44 |
Mother no data on education | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.84 |
Mother employed | 0.68 | 0.70 | 0.49 |
Mother unemployed | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.30 |
Mother outside labor market | 0.28 | 0.25 | 0.18 |
Mother no data on employment | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.46 |
Mother’s total income | 224,663 | 239,544 | 0.13 |
Missing on mother total income | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.46 |
Mother’s age in 2012, y | 38.23 | 38.27 | 0.88 |
Missing on mother’s age | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.32 |
Mother is teenager at date of birth | 0.02 | 0.01 | 0.19 |
Father compulsory education | 0.15 | 0.15 | 0.90 |
Father upper secondary education | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.94 |
Father vocational education | 0.27 | 0.25 | 0.34 |
Father short-cycle education | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.56 |
Father medium-cycle education | 0.15 | 0.18 | 0.12 |
Father long-cycle education | 0.21 | 0.21 | 0.69 |
Father no data on education | 0.08 | 0.08 | 0.66 |
Father employed | 0.76 | 0.76 | 0.96 |
Father unemployed | 0.03 | 0.05 | 0.29 |
Father outside labor market | 0.16 | 0.14 | 0.26 |
Father no data on employment | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.40 |
Father’s total income | 335,907 | 346,477 | 0.53 |
Missing on father total income | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.48 |
Father’s age in 2012, y | 41.34 | 40.83 | 0.09* |
Missing on father’s age | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.40 |
Father is teenager at date of birth | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.96 |
Observations | 1,587 |
Note that P values are based on t test to test the difference between the two groups. Register data are from 2012. Two-sided test (*P < 0.1; **P < 0.05).
The treatment was designed based on the mindset research showing links between (i) parents’ growth mindsets, (ii) constructive, mastery-oriented interaction with the child, and (iii) praising child effort rather than performance and results. Recent research also shows that parents do not automatically pass on their growth mindsets to their children (12), which suggests that interventions should not only cultivate growth mindsets in parents but also, provide scaffolding for parents so that they learn how to put their growth mindset into practice. Parents in the reading intervention group were, therefore, provided with a booklet and access to an online video (all information translated into 10 languages) that underpin each of these three components. (i) The information emphasized a growth theory of abilities by explaining to the parents that their child’s reading ability can be improved, no matter whether the child is already good or bad at reading (7, 8). (ii) The material encouraged parents to take a constructive, mastery-oriented approach, supporting the child’s autonomous engagement with the books (4, 9) by asking the parent to talk to the child about the content before, during, and after reading it; pose open questions to the child; take time to answer the child’s questions; and make sure that it was an enjoyable experience. The parents were encouraged not to correct their child’s reading mistakes unless they affected the child’s understanding of what had been read. (iii) To encourage parents to praise their child’s effort rather than performance (10, 11), parent and child could use a logbook, noting down every reading session. The logbook thereby endorsed child effort, not performance or results (not the speed or accuracy of the reading). After 10 reading sessions, children could bring the logbooks to their school teacher, and the class would get a sticker. The class with the most stickers received a prize.
It was not mandatory for teachers to use the logbook system; 13 of 36 classrooms in the treatment group made use of the logbooks. According to the logbooks, parents were, on average, reading 89.2 times with their children during the intervention period. (The available data only recorded the number of reading sessions at the classroom level.) The class competition might, on the one hand, have crowded out some intrinsic motivation for some children—or directly demotivated children who felt that they could not match their classmates. On the other hand, the competition was not based on how fast children were reading but on how often they were reading. This design should ideally motivate all or most students to contribute. Also, the logbooks were an aspect of the intervention that was voluntary for the teachers to use to not interfere with teachers’ existing work plans and cooperation with parents. This fact may imply that only teachers who found the competition beneficial to their particular students used it. Future research should examine the isolated effect of using classroom competition to motivate children’s reading.
Children in the treatment group also received three books to get them started and information on how to find other reading material at the library, at the school, in the newspaper, etc. School authorities and schools implemented the treatment without any researcher involvement.
To estimate the effect of this combined reading intervention with a growth mindset approach compared with treatment as usual in the control group, we use regression analyses with SEs clustered at the classroom level to account for the hierarchical structure of the dataset (children within classrooms). We obtain similar results using a hierarchical linear model. Additional information on materials and methods is in SI Materials and Methods.
SI Materials and Methods
We worked with the City of Aarhus, Denmark to recruit 72 second-grade classrooms, including 1,587 children (28 schools). The sample represents considerable variety in terms of immigrants and nonimmigrants, high- and low-educated parents, and high- and low-income parents (Table S1). This study was part of a larger project that tests another intervention in third-grade classrooms. The intervention materials are publically accessible (www.aarhus.dk/read).
Using estimates from prior studies within education research (20), power calculations showed that, because of the hierarchical structure of the data, we should aim for 90 classrooms (about 30 schools) to be able to detect an effect size of 0.2 SDs with power of 80% and a significance level of 5%; 72 classrooms were enrolled. To increase power, classrooms were stratified based on available data on language proficiency of the children. Within each stratum, classrooms were randomly assigned to either the treatment or control group. We used classroom cluster randomization to reduce potential spillover effects between the treatment and control groups.
Fig. S1 illustrates the progress through the phases of the stratified, cluster-randomized trial. Initially, 74 classrooms (clusters) signed up for the trial. To make 18 strata of four classrooms each, two classrooms were randomly selected and placed in a separate stratum (named “stratum 19”). Based on available data on language proficiency of the students, the remaining 72 classrooms were rank-ordered and divided into strata (blocks) of four classrooms each. This strategy follows the recommendation of having relatively many and relatively small strata rather than either complete randomization or paired randomization (21). Within each stratum, two classrooms were randomly allocated to the treatment group, and two classrooms were randomly allocated to the control group.
After the initial randomization but before the intervention began, six classrooms were merged into four classrooms (because of an amalgamation of two schools with four classrooms and the merging of two classrooms into one school). The number of pupils was not affected by this administrative change, but it reduced the total number of classrooms enrolled in the trial from 74 to 72. The four merged classrooms were placed in stratum 19. The six classrooms from stratum 19 (initially two classrooms + four merged classrooms) were randomly allocated to either the treatment (three classrooms) or the control group (three classrooms).
The number of lessons per week varies between schools. On average, it is around 18 lessons a week. The marginal cost of one additional child attending school (that is, treatment as usual in the control group) is US$4,800. The cost of the treatment per child is US$77, representing an increase in marginal cost of 1.6%.
By using the personal identification numbers of the Danish civil registration system, we are able to combine test scores for the children with information on their parental background and survey data on the parents. To measure children’s reading achievements, we use a standardized, adaptive, self-scoring (hence, also blinded) electronic reading test. The test is used for all children in the country and has not been developed specifically for this study. It measures three reading domains separately: decoding (from letters to words), language comprehension (vocabulary), and text comprehension (drawing meaning and information from a text). The test system also computes a total test score based on the results in the three domains. This test was taken 2 as well as 7 mo after the intervention began. To measure children’s expressive language skills, we used a writing test presenting the children with writing prompts consisting of four pictures representing steps in a narrative structure. The third picture was a question mark, allowing the pupils to make up their own twist of the plot at a pivotal place in the story. The writing test was conducted in school. All pupils had one lesson (45 min) to write the story. Afterward, the children’s narratives were coded by two trained coders using the Narrative Assessment Protocol (22). The intercoder reliability was at least as good as in the validation study (22). The writing test was used as a pretest (before intervention) and a posttest 7 mo after the intervention began.
Information on parents was obtained by combining administrative register data and a survey administered to parents before the intervention. The register data provided detailed information on parents’ education, country of origin, income, and other information (Table S1). The survey included a version of the Parents’ Beliefs about Abilities questionnaire (23) adapted to parents of children in primary school. It included six questions about parents’ beliefs about the fixedness of their child’s ability to learn to read. Two items were reversed. All six items load predominantly on one factor in an exploratory factor analysis. Only one factor had an eigenvalue above one. The factor scores are used to measure parents’ fixedness beliefs (Table S2).
Table S2.
Presurvey | Factor 1 | Factor 2 |
My child’s ability to learn how to read is innate and will never change | 0.522 | −0.041 |
My child’s ability to learn how to read can change significantly from birth (RC) | −0.399 | 0.170 |
After a certain point in childhood, my child’s ability to learn how to read cannot improve | 0.607 | 0.019 |
My child can always improve his/her ability to learn how to read, no matter how old he/she is (RC) | −0.490 | 0.161 |
My child’s ability to learn how to read can only be substantially improved during a specific period in his/her development | 0.467 | 0.155 |
My child is past the age at which he/she can substantially improve his/her ability to learn how to read | 0.431 | 0.194 |
Eigenvalue | 1.445 | 0.119 |
Cronbach’s alpha | 0.650 |
Adapted from ref. 23. RC, reversed code.
To estimate the effect of the intervention compared with treatment as usual in the control group, we use regression analyses with SEs clustered at the classroom level to account for the hierarchical structure of the dataset (children within classrooms). We obtain similar results using a hierarchical linear model. Estimation is based on the following model:
[S1] |
Yi j k denotes the outcome of interest for student i in classroom j of stratum k; α1 represents the intention to treat effect. We estimate the intention to treat effect, because we aim to estimate the effect of the intention to support parents in a realistic large-scale setting that takes into account variability in implementation by school teachers and acceptance by parents. To increase precision of the estimates, we include indicators of 19 strata used for randomization (Stratumk), the pretest writing scores Pre_testi, and the child and family covariates (represented by the vector xi), which are listed in Table 1. Results are similar without the inclusion of these covariates.
Table 1.
Sample | Reading test, 2 mo | Reading test, 7 mo | Writing test, 7 mo: NAP | ||||||
Total score | Language comprehension | Decoding | Text comprehension | Total score | Language comprehension | Decoding | Text comprehension | ||
All children | 0.257*** (0.0687) | 0.187*** (0.0684) | 0.231*** (0.0676) | 0.272*** (0.0661) | 0.121** (0.0575) | 0.0418 (0.0523) | 0.153** (0.0619) | 0.127** (0.0565) | 0.158* (0.0837) |
Subgroups | |||||||||
Danish background | 0.242*** (0.0751) | 0.144* (0.0738) | 0.222*** (0.0749) | 0.284*** (0.0731) | 0.104* (0.0623) | 0.0306 (0.0600) | 0.152** (0.0655) | 0.0940 (0.0626) | 0.144 (0.0896) |
Immigrant background | 0.335*** (0.113) | 0.413*** (0.119) | 0.268** (0.106) | 0.218* (0.115) | 0.203* (0.111) | 0.105 (0.122) | 0.162 (0.122) | 0.274** (0.110) | 0.250** (0.116) |
Mother high education | 0.223** (0.0902) | 0.179* (0.0904) | 0.193* (0.0978) | 0.225** (0.0847) | 0.0565 (0.0728) | −0.00901 (0.0644) | 0.104 (0.0847) | 0.0551 (0.0774) | 0.142 (0.105) |
Mother low education | 0.320*** (0.0870) | 0.223*** (0.0814) | 0.288*** (0.0839) | 0.346*** (0.0911) | 0.204*** (0.0746) | 0.119 (0.0758) | 0.222*** (0.0725) | 0.201*** (0.0730) | 0.193** (0.0816) |
SEs are clustered at the classroom level. Covariates and constants are included in all models. Two-sided test (*P < 0.1; **P < 0.05; ***P < 0.01). NAP, Narrative Assessment Protocol.
In total, 1,587 children were assigned to either the treatment or control group. Of 41 variables measuring baseline characteristics of the child, the mother, and the father, only two were significantly (at the 10% level) differently distributed between treatment and control groups using multiple t tests (Table S1). The response rate for the reading test after 2 mo and the writing test after 7 mo was 74%. The response rate for the reading test after 7 mo, which was mandatory for all students in the country, was 94%. There were no significant differences in the response rates of the treatment and control groups for the second tests (Table S3). The response rate of the pretest parent survey was 55%, with higher response rates for the treatment group. Although respondents and nonrespondents in general are similar on background characteristics, results including the parent survey should be interpreted with caution, because they apply only to parents responding to the survey.
Table S3.
Data collection | Control group, % | Treatment group, % | P value |
Pretest, writing | 78.9 | 81.6 | 0.18 |
Writing test, 8 mo | 74.6 | 69.5 | 0.02 |
Reading test, 3 mo | 82.3 | 78.4 | 0.05 |
Reading test, 7 mo | 94.2 | 94.5 | 0.80 |
Presurvey, parents* | 47.1 | 56.5 | 0.00 |
Parents who responded to all of the beliefs questions are classified as responding to the presurvey (n = 1,587).
Results
The treatment improved reading in three domains (language comprehension, decoding, and text comprehension) after 3 mo and again, after 7 mo—although with smaller changes after 7 mo (Table 1). The treatment not only improved the children’s achievements in reading and understanding a text, it also improved their expressive language skills as measured in the writing test. Looking at subgroups (Table 1), effects are at least as strong for immigrant children with non-Western backgrounds and children with low-educated mothers (less than medium-cycle higher education). Differences between subgroups are not statistically significant but presented to show that the average intention to treat effect is not driven only by children with high socioeconomic status. The reading intervention with a growth approach thereby succeeded in supporting groups of children who normally spend less time with their parents (2).
The children in the control group progressed, on average, 0.12 SD per month between the first and second reading tests. The effect on the total reading scores after 2 mo of intervention, therefore, corresponds to about 2 mo of additional gain in reading score in the treatment group. After 7 mo of intervention, the effect is reduced to about 1 mo of additional gain.
Based on mindset research, we expected the effect of the treatment to be higher the more fixed the parents’ mindset was before the intervention, because the potential for improvement is higher in these families. The treatment may make them not only read more with their children but also, do this in more constructive ways. We examine this by combining the treatment indicator with the measure of parental beliefs about child ability fixedness collected before the intervention. (Table S2 shows the operationalization of parental beliefs. Table S3 shows that response rates of the parent survey differ between treatment and control groups. These results apply only to parents responding to the survey.)
Table 2 presents the results. First, we note that, in this interaction model, the variable “fixedness beliefs, preintervention” estimates the association in the control group between fixedness beliefs and the outcomes when controlling for all covariates, including the pretest writing score, parents’ level of education, and income. The results show that children whose parents had higher fixedness beliefs had lower reading skills after the intervention than children with similar writing skills before the intervention and otherwise similar parental backgrounds. Although causal inference cannot be made from these observational data without additional assumptions, we do conclude that this supports the theory that parents with fixed mindset are less able to support the academic progress of their children.
Table 2.
Variable | Reading test, 2 mo | Reading test, 7 mo | Writing test, 7 mo: NAP | ||||||
Total score | Language comprehension | Decoding | Text comprehension | Total score | Language comprehension | Decoding | Text comprehension | ||
Treatment | 0.202*** (0.0695) | 0.150** (0.0725) | 0.174** (0.0671) | 0.216*** (0.0684) | 0.0990 (0.0613) | 0.0235 (0.0663) | 0.163** (0.0653) | 0.0765 (0.0595) | 0.168* (0.0889) |
Fixedness beliefs, preintervention | −0.126* (0.0642) | −0.157** (0.0716) | −0.0659 (0.0661) | −0.114* (0.0595) | −0.0837* (0.0490) | −0.126** (0.0512) | −0.0699 (0.0516) | −0.0269 (0.0532) | −0.0508 (0.0439) |
Treatment × fixedness beliefs | 0.205** (0.0842) | 0.232*** (0.0873) | 0.114 (0.0903) | 0.205** (0.0809) | 0.186*** (0.0625) | 0.136* (0.0792) | 0.193*** (0.0656) | 0.167** (0.0686) | 0.144** (0.0677) |
Observations | 686 | 686 | 686 | 686 | 791 | 791 | 791 | 791 | 625 |
R2 | 0.343 | 0.281 | 0.313 | 0.300 | 0.325 | 0.258 | 0.308 | 0.273 | 0.450 |
SEs are clustered at the classroom level. Covariates and constants are included in all models. Two-sided test (*P < 0.1; **P < 0.05; ***P < 0.01). NAP, Narrative Assessment Protocol.
Second, the interaction variable in Table 2 shows—as expected—that the treatment had greater effect for parents with higher fixedness beliefs. Effect sizes are substantial. Parents with fixedness beliefs 1 SD above the mean showed an estimated effect of the reading intervention of about 0.3 SD (0.106 + 0.193) after 7 mo of intervention, which corresponds to 2.4 mo of additional progression in reading.
Discussion
The results support the notion that the reading intervention with a growth approach—which explains to parents that they can make a difference to their child’s reading abilities and shows how to do so—has a large potential for supplementing schools’ efforts to teach children to read well and express themselves in writing. This extra potential is especially present for parents who do not already have a growth mindset. The growth approach intervention in this study was combined with delivery of books and encouragement to read together with the child. We cannot isolate the effect of the growth mindset approach from the other elements of the intervention. Furthermore, the results do not show whether the heterogeneous effects for parents with high and low fixedness beliefs are caused by these beliefs or whether they are related to other parental characteristics associated with fixedness beliefs. However, we note that we do not find the same degree of heterogeneous effects for the other parental characteristics in Table 1 (ethnic background or education).
The effect of the reading materials may be age-dependent. For preschool children, parents may have to read aloud to their child. Older children may read themselves. Therefore, activities may be age-specific, but the parental growth approach will most likely be relevant across a broad span of age groups.
The fact that effects are smaller after 7–8 mo than after 3 mo may be taken to suggest that a growth approach intervention may be even more effective if it is combined with interventions that make parents sustain their efforts.
From the perspective of public expenditures, engaging parents in reading with their child directly is much cheaper than increasing the time that the child spends with teachers in school. Two recent randomized trials that use the same reading test as the outcome but increased the time that children spent with adults by either increasing the number of lessons per week or having two adults in the classroom (coteachers) found effect sizes of similar magnitude; however, public expenditures were at least twice as high (18, 19). This observation supports the efficiency of a growth mindset parental reading intervention—even when implemented in realistic settings without full compliance.
Acknowledgments
We thank James Heckman, Stephen Raudenbush, Meredith Rowe, and seminar participants for valuable inputs and TrygFonden’s Centre for Child Research, Aarhus University and Center for the Economics of Human Development, The University of Chicago. We thank Mette Kjærgaard Thomsen, Morten Hjortskov Larsen, Allan Aachmann, Stine Wium Olesen, Astrid Jæger, and Simon Bodilsen for great research assistance. We also thank the City of Aarhus and VIA University College for very fruitful collaboration. The project was funded by the nonprofit organization TrygFonden as part of a larger grant for TrygFonden’s Centre for Child Research. Part of the data reported in the paper from administrative registers is provided by Statistics Denmark and the Danish Ministry for Children, Education, and Gender Equality. Because the data include confidential information on individual citizens, they are not publicly available but placed on a secured server hosted by Statistics Denmark. Independent researchers can apply to Statistics Denmark for access.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1607946113/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.OECD . PISA 2012 Results: Excellence Through Equity. Vol 2 Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development; Paris: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Guryan J, Hurst E, Kearney M. Parental education and parental time with children. J Econ Perspect. 2008;22:23–46. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Raudenbush SW, Eschmann RD. Does schooling increase or reduce social inequality? Annu Rev Sociol. 2015;41:443–470. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Pomerantz EM, Moorman EA, Litwack SD. The how, whom, and why of parents’ involvement in children’s academic lives: More is not always better. Rev Educ Res. 2007;77:373–410. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Castro M, et al. Parental involvement on student academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Educ Res Rev. 2015;14:33–46. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kalil A. Families in an era of increasing inequality. In: Amato PR, Booth A, McHale SM, Hook JV, editors. Proceedings of the National Symposium on Family Issues. Springer; Berlin: 2015. pp. 63–82. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dweck CS. Self-Theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personality, and Development. Psychology Press; New York: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Dweck C. Mindset: The New Psychology of Success. Random House; New York: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Moorman EA, Pomerantz EM. Ability mindsets influence the quality of mothers’ involvement in children’s learning: An experimental investigation. Dev Psychol. 2010;46(5):1354–1362. doi: 10.1037/a0020376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mueller CM, Dweck CS. Praise for intelligence can undermine children’s motivation and performance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75(1):33–52. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.1.33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kamins ML, Dweck CS. Person versus process praise and criticism: Implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Dev Psychol. 1999;35(3):835–847. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.3.835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Haimovitz K, Dweck CS. What predicts children’s fixed and growth intelligence mind-sets? Not their parents’ views of intelligence but their parents’ views of failure. Psychol Sci. 2016;27(6):859–869. doi: 10.1177/0956797616639727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gunderson EA, et al. Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later. Child Dev. 2013;84(5):1526–1541. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Miu AS, Yeager DS. Preventing symptoms of depression by teaching adolescents that people can change effects of a brief incremental theory of personality intervention at 9-month follow-up. Clin Psychol Sci. 2015;3:726–743. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Paunesku D, et al. Mind-set interventions are a scalable treatment for academic underachievement. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(6):784–793. doi: 10.1177/0956797615571017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Yeager DS, et al. Using design thinking to improve psychological interventions: The case of the growth mindset during the transition to high school. J Educ Psychol. 2016;108(3):374–391. doi: 10.1037/edu0000098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yeager DS, et al. Teaching a lay theory before college narrows achievement gaps at scale. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113(24):E3341–E3348. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1524360113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Andersen SC, Humlum MK, Nandrup AB. Increasing instruction time in school does increase learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113(27):7481–7484. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1516686113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Andersen SC, Beuchert L, Nielsen HS, Thomsen MK. 2015 The Effect of Teacher’s Aides in the Classroom: Evidence from a Randomized Trial. SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 2626677. Available at papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2626677. Accessed April 10, 2016.
- 20.Schochet PZ. Statistical power for random assignment evaluations of education programs. J Educ Behav Stat. 2008;33:62–87. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Imbens GW. 2011 Experimental Design for Unit and Cluster Randomized Trials. Available at cyrussamii.com/wp-content/uploads/2011/06/Imbens_June_8_paper.pdf. Accessed April 10, 2016.
- 22.Justice LM, Bowles R, Pence K, Gosse C. A scalable tool for assessing children’s language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol) Early Child Res Q. 2010;25:218–234. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Muenks K, Miele DB, Ramani GB, Stapleton LM, Rowe ML. Parental beliefs about the fixedness of ability. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2015;41:78–89. [Google Scholar]