Abstract
Frequency tuning within the auditory papilla of most non-mammalian species is electrical, deriving from ion-channel resonance within their sensory hair cells. In contrast, tuning within the mammalian cochlea is mechanical, stemming from active mechanisms within outer hair cells that amplify the basilar membrane travelling wave. Interestingly, hair cells in the avian basilar papilla demonstrate both electrical resonance and force-generation, making it unclear which mechanism creates sharp frequency tuning. Here, we measured sound-induced vibrations within the apical half of the chicken basilar papilla in vivo and found broadly-tuned travelling waves that were not amplified. However, distortion products were found in live but not dead chickens. These findings support the idea that avian hair cells do produce force, but that their effects on vibration are small and do not sharpen tuning. Therefore, frequency tuning within the apical avian basilar papilla is not mechanical, and likely derives from hair cell electrical resonance.
The avian auditory papilla has many similarities to the mammalian cochlea but whether force generation by hair cells amplifies the travelling wave, as it does in mammals, remains unknown. Here the authors show that the chicken basilar papilla does not have a ‘cochlear amplifier' and that sharp frequency tuning does not derive from mechanical vibrations.
The inner ear transduces the mechanical energy of sound into electrochemical signals that are encoded within the auditory nerve. This originates when sound-induced pressure waves deflect stereociliary bundles at the apical poles of sensory hair cells. Ionic flow through mechanoelectrical transduction channels generates receptor potentials that modulate synaptic release from the basal poles of the hair cells. However, hair cells do not only sense mechanical force: they also produce force. The stereociliary bundle can produce force that appears to be associated with opening of the transduction channel1,2 and/or fast adaptation associated with closing of the transduction channel3,4. Stereociliary bundle force production is likely present to some degree in every type of hair cell across all vertebrates and may aid frequency tuning5. In mammals, outer hair cells (OHCs) also produce force by changing their cell length in response to changes in their transmembrane potential, a process that requires prestin protein, termed electromotility6,7. Together, these forces produced by hair cells give rise to otoacoustic emissions, sounds that emanate from the inner ear. Otoacoustic emissions have been found in all vertebrate classes8.
However, there are obvious anatomical and physiological differences between the inner ears of different species that argue for substantial differences in how they transduce sound9. For example, within the auditory papillae of commonly studied vertebrates such as frogs, lizards and turtles, the sensory hair cells are anatomically quite similar and are clustered with minimal organization over a plane. In frogs, there is no basilar membrane (BM)10. In most lizards and turtles, the hair cells sit on a vibratory BM that is not mechanically tuned11,12. Also, there are non-specialized supporting cells around the hair cells that give rise to new hair cells when existing hair cells are damaged (that is, hair cell regeneration). Physiologically, frog, lizard and turtle hair cells have electrical resonance, in which the complex interplay between the voltage-gated channels tunes the receptor potential of each hair cell to a select frequency13,14,15,16. Thus, when a hair cell stereociliary bundle is stimulated at the frequency to which it is tuned, it will best stimulate the auditory nerves that receive synaptic input from it.
In contrast, the mammalian cochlea is a coiled structure in which the sensory hair cells are precisely aligned by highly-specialized supporting cells atop a vibratory BM. Along the length of the cochlea, there are three rows of OHCs and one row of inner hair cells (IHCs). Furthermore, mammalian hair cells do not regenerate. Functionally, a principle feature of the mammalian cochlea is that the passive mechanics of the BM produce a spectral analysis on the sound energy which manifests as travelling waves17. Sound energy is distributed over the length of the cochlea in a tonotopic manner, so that high-frequency sounds cause the BM at the base to vibrate maximally and low-frequency sounds cause the BM at the apex to vibrate maximally. The active force generation properties of OHCs are commonly thought to locally amplify and sharpen the spatial extent of the BM vibrations. This is termed cochlear amplification and it improves auditory sensitivity to quiet sounds and frequency selectivity6,18. An alternative concept is that the active properties of OHCs do not occur on a cycle-by cycle basis, but instead adjust the amount of local damping present within a non-amplifying system19,20. According to both theories, however, functional OHCs are required to achieve the normal sharpness of cochlear vibratory responses. Electrical resonance has not been found within mature mammalian hair cells. Instead, IHCs sense the frequency tuning inherent to the mechanical vibrations of the organ of Corti and convey these complex, nonlinear signals to the afferent auditory nerve21.
The avian basilar papilla has similarities to both the mammalian cochlea as well as the auditory papillae of other non-mammalian species22. Like mammals, there are two types of hair cells, short and tall. Short hair cells (SHCs) sit on a vibratory BM and are only minimally innervated by afferent neurons23. In contrast, tall hair cells (THCs) sit adjacent to them on a stiff fibrocartilage plate (FcP) and are heavily innervated by afferent neurons. The role of the SHCs is unknown, but they have been hypothesized to produce force that amplifies and/or sharpens the mechanical stimulus driving the stereociliary bundles of the THCs, somewhat analogous to the way that mammalian OHCs provide this function for IHCs24,25. Chicken hair cells have both bundle motility and prestin motors that enhance bundle motility, which together can move the tectorial membrane (TM) in a radial direction26. Consistent with this, birds produce otoacoustic emissions in a manner consistent with hair cell force production27. The similarities with the mammalian cochlea are limited, however. Like frogs, turtles and lizards, bird hair cells are arrayed in a plane, are surrounded by a non-specialized supporting cells layer that supports hair cell regeneration, and have electrical tuning28.
Thus, the avian basilar papilla offers a way to study how passive BM mechanics, active properties of hair cells, and hair cell electrical resonance all relate to the evolution of frequency tuning and auditory sensitivity. Furthermore, as an intermediary organ between the auditory papillae of reptiles and amphibians and the cochlea of mammals, these studies may provide clues as to the functional roles of these processes in mammalian hearing. Specifically, we sought to determine whether amplification, defined as increased vibratory amplitude and sharpening of frequency tuning, occurs within the chicken basilar papilla. To accomplish this, we measured sound-induced vibrations within the apical half of the chicken basilar papilla in vivo. We found that, similar to mammals, there was a travelling wave. However, travelling wave vibrations were linear and not amplified. Furthermore, the frequency tuning of the travelling wave was broad and did not match the sharpness found in previously published single-unit chicken auditory nerve recordings. These data demonstrate that the chicken basilar papilla does not have a ‘cochlear amplifier' like in mammals, and suggest that sharp frequency tuning in chickens derives from hair cell electrical resonance.
Results
In vivo imaging of the chicken basilar papilla
We studied the P5-10 chicken basilar papilla in vivo. At this age, auditory thresholds and auditory nerve tuning curves are mature29,30. We used volumetric optical coherence tomography and vibrometry (VOCTV) to measure sound-induced vibrations of the tissues inside the inner ear without opening the surrounding otic capsule bone31. We had to limit our studies to the apical half of the basilar papilla because surgically accessing more basal regions would require resecting some or all of the tympanic membrane, inhibiting sound conduction (Fig. 1a–c and Supplementary Movie 1). We recorded at locations 50, 75 and 85% from the base of the basilar papilla, which is ∼3.9 mm long32.
For localization, we compared the cross-sectional in vivo images obtained with our VOCTV system to ex vivo histologically frozen sections (Fig. 1d–h). While the spatial resolution of our imaging system prevented the identification of single cells, key anatomic regions could be localized. The three fluid-filled scalae (vestibuli, media and tympani) were obvious. The tegmentum vasculosum, analogous to the stria vascularis in the mammalian cochlea because it maintains the electrochemical gradients within the endolymph, was clearly visible as a thick structure separating scala vestibuli from scala media. However smaller structures, such as the SHCs, THCs, the TM, the BM and the FcP under the THCs, could not be discretely identified. Nevertheless, the anatomic image was adequate to localize two important regions: the SHC/BM region and the THC/TM region. We selected the centres of these regions and measured their vibration patterns in response to sound stimuli.
Physiological assessment of auditory function
Even though the vibratory measurements we made were performed without creating an opening into the inside of the basilar papilla, it is still possible that auditory function was in some way compromised in the experimental preparation. Therefore, we assessed the health of the peripheral auditory system by making electrophysiological recordings using auditory brainstem responses (ABRs) and cochlear microphonics (CMs). ABRs provide a comprehensive assessment of the function of the middle ear, basilar papilla and auditory neurons, whereas CMs provide an assessment of the summed receptor potential that stem from hair cell transduction33. ABRs were measured using click stimuli, similar to previously published recordings34,35,36. CMs were measured using 2.75 kHz sine wave stimuli. In a subgroup of chickens, we recorded ABRs and CMs at three time points during the experiment. The first time was just after induction of anaesthesia (that is, before any of the surgical procedures were started), the second time was after the vibratory measurements were finished in the living chicken, and the third time was after euthanasia but before the vibratory measurements were repeated in the dead chicken.
ABR waveforms showed no change in morphology after the vibratory measurements were completed in the living chickens compared with the baseline recordings (Fig. 1i,j). Similarly, there was difference in the peak-to-peak amplitudes between the two measurements (Fig. 1k) (two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), P=0.30; n=15). Auditory thresholds, defined as the stimulus intensity at which the peak-to-peak ABR amplitude rose to 5x above the average noise floor measured during the last 20 ms of the recordings, were not different after completion of the vibrometry compared with baseline (41.3±1.9 versus 42.2±2.0 dB SPL, mean±s.e.m.; paired t-test, P=0.59; n=15). These thresholds are slightly lower than has been previously published (∼50 dB SPL)36, but importantly, they remained stable throughout the experiment. Post-mortem, our data confirmed that there were no ABR waveforms and that the peak-to-peak responses dropped to within the noise floor.
Similarly, the CM magnitude demonstrated no changes between the baseline recordings and the recordings made after performing vibrometry in the living chickens (Fig. 1l) (two-way ANOVA, P=0.84; n=13). The CM also disappeared post-mortem. Together these data demonstrate that our experimental preparation was viable and argue that peripheral auditory function during vibrometry studies in living chickens was intact. Furthermore, these data confirm that euthanasia produces the expected loss of auditory function. In particular, the drop in the CM to the point where they were undetectable supports the concept that there was a large drop in hair cell transduction currents in our experimental preparation after killing.
Mechanics of the SHC/BM region
With the laser of our VOCTV system oriented perpendicular to the BM, we measured the transverse motion of the SHC/BM region in response to pure tone stimuli. The magnitude responses demonstrated low-pass filter characteristics (Fig. 2, top row). The corner frequency shifted to lower frequencies as the measurement location was moved apically. Increasing the stimulus intensity led to proportionally larger vibratory displacements, a linear response. The phase responses (Fig. 2, middle row) followed the magnitude responses, demonstrating a phase lag that increased as the stimulus frequency was increased. The lack of nonlinearity in the magnitude responses was highlighted by normalizing the vibration of the SHC region to the vibration of the columella (Fig. 2, bottom row). The response curves all overlapped. Interestingly, these normalized data demonstrated that the basilar papilla, independent of the middle ear, had tuned responses. This was because the transfer function of the chick middle ear had low-pass filter characteristics with a very low corner frequency (<150 Hz; discussed further below).
However, the important point is that the net mechanical response of the chick peripheral auditory system, reflecting the mechanical stimulus seen by the hair cells, is low-pass in nature. Therefore, we fit each data set with a low-pass filter model to permit quantitative comparisons (Fig. 3a). From the vibratory responses measured in each chicken to a 60 dB SPL stimulus over the frequency spectrum, we calculated the F3dB, the stop-band attenuation, and the slope of the roll-off. The F3dB of the mechanical responses we measured was similar to tonotopic maps of the chicken basilar papilla derived from single-unit auditory nerve studies29,37,38, patch-clamped hair cells recordings of electrical resonance39,40, and patterns of hair cell loss after acoustic trauma32,41,42 (Fig. 3b,e). On the basis of our data, the formula for the chicken tonotopic map using a logarithmic fit was:
where D is the distance from the basal end of the basilar papilla (in %) and F3dB is the corner frequency of the mechanical vibratory response (in Hz). The stop band attenuation was similar at the three different locations (one-way ANOVA, P=0.13) whereas the roll-off slope was lower at more apical locations (one-way ANOVA followed by non-paired t-tests, P50%-75%=0.017, P50%-85% <0.001 and P75%-85%<0.01)(Fig. 3c,d).
While the mechanical vibratory pattern of chicken basilar papilla demonstrated a tonotopic distribution, the low-pass filter characteristics of the mechanical responses did not match the band-pass filter characteristics found within auditory neurons or hair cells. We calculated vibratory threshold plots at the location 75% from the base by determining the sound intensity required to evoke a 30 nm magnitude displacement (Fig. 3f). When compared against representative auditory nerve single-unit tuning curves37,43 or patch-clamped hair cell recordings39, it is clear that the mechanical response of the chicken BM was not tuned in a similar fashion. Thus, the transverse vibration of the BM cannot be solely responsible for the sharp frequency tuning found within the auditory nerve. This contrasts with the mammalian cochlea, where the tuning in the BM vibratory response is nearly identical to the tuning of the auditory nerve21.
The basilar papilla supports travelling wave propagation
The existence of travelling waves within the avian inner ear remains unresolved. On one hand, point measurements of basilar papilla vibration measured in pigeons demonstrate frequency-dependent phase lags44 and recordings from brainstem neurons in the chicken brainstem demonstrate phase/frequency relations45 that are both consistent with travelling waves. On the other hand, auditory neuronal recordings in the barn owl indicate that the derived group delays do not match the tonotopic spatial frequency representation of the basilar papilla46,47,48.
A critical feature of a travelling wave is that it takes time for the vibration created by a given sound stimulus to propagate from the base to the apex, thus the group delay should be larger at more apical locations17,49,50,51. Because of the tonotopic layout of the basilar papilla, high-frequency stimuli should also have larger group delays than low-frequency stimuli. Therefore, we used the vibratory data measured at the 50, 75 and 85% distances from the base in eight chickens to derive the group delay. This was done by calculating the instantaneous slope of the phase versus frequency plots by fitting them with quadratic functions and taking the derivatives. We found larger group delays at the apical locations, and for all locations, higher-frequency stimuli had larger group delays (Fig. 4a), consistent with travelling wave propagation.
To confirm this conclusion, we then performed a different experiment where 300 Hz tone pip stimuli were presented and the vibratory motion was measured at multiple locations along the length of the basilar papilla. We measured the time delay between the first vibratory peak at eight different locations that ranged between 70 and 80% of the distance from the base. As would be expected with travelling wave propagation, the group delay was longer for more apical measurement locations (Fig. 4b). Together, these data demonstrate that the chicken basilar papilla supports travelling wave propagation, consistent with the notion that it functions like a series of longitudinally coupled filter-banks as does the mammalian cochlea17.
Radial motion does not demonstrate linear or nonlinear gain
As shown in Fig. 2, the transverse vibratory response of the chicken BM is different from that of the mammalian BM is that it does not demonstrate nonlinear gain. In the mammalian cochlea, this means that the ratio of BM vibration to sound intensity is greater for quiet stimuli than for loud stimuli. One reason why the mammalian cochlea may achieve this is because the force of prestin-based somatic electromotility is directed transversely7. In contrast, the prestin-based force production that has been found in chicken SHCs and THCs is directed in the radial direction through tilting of the cuticular plate26. Thus, it is possible that nonlinear gain in the chicken could be missed if only transverse motion is measured.
We thus rotated the preparations to achieve an angle of 45–55° to the optical axis. This provided a measurement that substantially reflects the amount of motion in the radial direction, as determined by the vector projection on the optical axis, that is, 70.7–81.9% radial vibration (Fig. 5a). We recorded from five chickens at a location 75% from the base with this approach. Similar to the previous transverse vibration measurements, there was no evidence of nonlinear gain (Fig. 5b–d). We averaged the normalized responses at both 40 and 70 dB SPL stimuli, and these data confirmed that there were no differences in the response patterns (two-way ANOVA, P=0.197; Fig. 5e).
Although the mechanical response of the chicken basilar papilla does not have nonlinear gain, it is possible that linear gain might exist. In this case, there would be a proportional increase in vibrational magnitude at all stimulus intensities. To test for this possibility, we measured vibratory responses in both live and dead chickens. Care was taken to measure from the same location, either the columella or the SHC/BM region, before and 15 min after animal euthanasia. As discussed previously, the columella response demonstrated low-pass filter characteristics with a corner frequency that was below our measurement range (<150 Hz; Fig. 5f). Importantly, there were no significant differences in the vibratory magnitudes of either the columella or the SHC/BM region over the frequency spectrum or intensity range (two-way ANOVA, P=0.616, 0.33, and 0.88, respectively; Fig. 5f–h). Together, these data demonstrate that there was no measureable linear or nonlinear gain to pure tone stimuli in the chicken basilar papilla that were vulnerable to death. The vibratory responses appeared passive and linear.
Mechanics of the THC/TM region
The THCs are responsible for delivering the afferent signal to the auditory neurons, but they do not sit on the vibratory BM. This raises the question of how the stereociliary bundles of the THCs are stimulated so that the chicken can hear. To address this question, we measured transverse vibratory responses at the 75% location at both the SHC/BM region and THC/TM regions in nine chickens.
We found that, even though it sits on the FcP, the THC/TM region vibrates substantially. It had linear characteristics, similar to the SHC/BM region but with lower magnitudes (Fig. 6a,b). For statistical comparison, the magnitude of the vibratory response at the F3db was higher in the SHC/BM region compared with the THC/TM region (two-way ANOVA, P=0.018; Fig. 6c). Nevertheless, the vibratory response of the THC region was consistent with levels known to evoke mechanoelectrical transduction responses in chicken hair cells ex vivo and mammalian hair cells in vivo31,39. The stop band attenuation and the slope of the roll-off were different between the two regions (paired t-test, P<0.01 and P<0.05, respectively; Fig. 6d,e).
We then performed vibrational measurements over a cross-section of the basilar papilla at a location 75% of the way from the base (Fig. 7). We presented a constant acoustic stimulus at 200, 500 or 800 Hz at 60 dB while the laser was stepped across the specimen. The magnitudes and phases of the vibratory responses were displayed using pseudocolour-gradient scales. Data are presented for every voxel in which the vibratory response was above the noise floor of 1 nm. The magnitude responses were normalized to the maximum vibratory response in the image. The phase of the region of the BM at 200 Hz was zeroed, and all other phase data were referenced to this. The largest magnitude of vibration was centred on the BM. Importantly, all structures within the sensory epithelium, including the BM, TM, SHCs, THCs and FcP vibrated together in phase at all frequencies.
To quantify the data contained within these images, we averaged the magnitude and phase of vibration within 10 pixel boxes at three different regions (SHC/BM, THC/TM and the FcP) from three chickens. For all frequencies, the largest vibration magnitudes occurred at the SHC/BM region. Away from this area, the magnitudes were reduced (ANOVA followed by non-paired t-tests, P<0.01 for all comparisons). Nevertheless, the FcP and THC/TM regions demonstrated vibratory responses typically considered adequate to support sound transduction (that is, magnitude >1 nm). Furthermore, this proves that the FCP is not a rigid structure at acoustic frequencies.
For consistency, the phase data measured with this approach were incremented by one cycle at 500 Hz and was incremented by two cycles at 800 Hz, in order to match the progressive phase lag we found when phase was measured with smaller frequency steps (for example, Figs 2b2, 4c and 5a2). At every frequency, all three regions vibrated in phase (two-way ANOVA, P>0.05). Therefore, there is a single vibratory mode of the chicken basilar papilla which clearly contrasts with the frequency-dependent, multi-modal vibratory patterns found within the mammalian organ of Corti31,52,53.
Labile distortion products are present
One aspect of bird auditory physiology that requires reconciliation is the generation of otoacoustic emissions, which supports the concept of hair cell force generation within the intact organ. However, unlike in the mammalian cochlea, we could not detect any evidence of travelling wave amplification in the chicken basilar papilla when a single frequency stimulus was presented. To look for evidence of active properties within the chicken basilar papilla that might be too subtle to detect with single-tone stimuli, we studied the creation of distortion products. When tones of two different frequencies are presented, nonlinearities of the mechanoelectrical transduction process produce distortion products. Active properties associated with hair cells can amplify these distortion products so that they can be measured both in the vibratory response of the BM and as distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs). Since there is no stimulus tone at the same frequency, distortion products are relatively easy to detect with Fourier analysis.
We measured DPOAEs over the frequency spectrum of 100 Hz to 5 kHz by presenting two simultaneous pure tones F2=1.2 × F1 at equal intensities, and measuring the acoustic pressure at the 2F1-F2 frequency in the ear canal with a microphone. Simultaneously, we measured the vibratory response of the SHC/BM region at the 75% location. We found DPOAEs only within frequencies ranging from 2 to 5 kHz (Fig. 8a,b). These responses were labile, and disappeared within 15 min of euthanasia, demonstrating that they stem from the animal and not from within our stimulus or measurement setup.
The magnitude of the DPOAEs we measured were ∼65–70 dB below the stimulus intensity of the primary tones. While one publication found that this intensity difference between the DP and the primary tones was less than what we found (∼50 dB) (ref. 54), a large body of previously published DPOAE data from chickens demonstrate that this difference is similar to what we found, ranging from 60–75 dB (refs 55, 56, 57, 58, 59). Furthermore, it has been well-established that chickens generate DPOAEs that are substantially lower in magnitude than those commonly measured in many laboratory animals, even though the phase characteristics of the DPOAEs among the different species are similar55. Therefore, together with the ABR and CM recordings (as shown in Fig. 1), these data support the concept that our experimental preparation was healthy and that the function of the basilar papilla was not compromised.
The distortion products we measured as otoacoustic emissions were not detectable as vibrations of the SHC/BM region. However, this is not surprising because the frequencies were much higher than the corner frequency of our measurement location (∼400 Hz). Nevertheless, we found robust distortion product vibrations in the BM at lower frequencies (range 400–1,200 Hz) (Fig. 8c,d). These too were labile and disappeared after animal euthanasia. Interestingly, we could never detect these low-frequency distortion products as DPOAEs with the microphone.
Since distortion products are present in living but not dead chickens, we interpret these data to mean that an active process is present within the basilar papilla of the live chicken. This active process is strong enough to amplify distortions generated during the transduction of two simultaneously applied tones. These distortions can emanate out of the ear canal when generated in the basal end of the basilar papilla near the columella footplate, but not when generated in the apical end of the basilar papilla. However, the effect of the active process on the vibration of the basilar papilla at the sound stimulus frequency is negligible compared with the magnitude of the passive travelling wave.
Discussion
Herein, we show that mechanical properties of the chicken peripheral auditory system do not match previously published measures of frequency tuning in chicken hair cells and auditory nerves. Rather than demonstrating sharp frequency tuning, the chicken basilar papilla vibrates with low-pass filter characteristics. Thus, low-frequency sounds produce vibrations along most of the length of the basilar papilla that are similar in amplitude, before the response rolls off near the apex (Supplementary Movies 2–4). This means that many more hair cells will be stimulated mechanically than will convey an electrical response to the auditory nerve. Our finding that mechanical tuning is much broader than previously measured electrical and neural tuning is consistent with the concept that frequency tuning in birds derives from the electrical characteristics of their hair cells and not the underlying mechanical response of the BM.
Furthermore, while active hair cell properties are present and measurable within the apical half of the chicken basilar papilla as labile distortion products, there is no evidence that there is amplification or sharpening of the travelling wave. Thus, there is no ‘cochlear amplifier', regardless of whether the mammalian cochlear amplifier involves the popular concept of the injection of power by hair cells18 or the alternative concept of active hair cell properties modulating local damping to control a fluid waveguide19. In either case, the mechanism of frequency tuning in the chicken basilar papilla is significantly different from that of the mammalian cochlea, where auditory nerve tuning matches the mechanical vibrations of the organ of Corti21. However, we could only study the apical half of the chicken basilar papilla, in which the F3dB was <650 Hz. Regardless, the frequency limit of hair cell electrical resonance has been predicted to reach 4.3 kHz in the chicken, which is sufficient is provide sharp tuning over the entire frequency range of hearing60. Thus, sharp mechanical tuning may not be necessary in birds. Alternatively, the tuning properties and force production by stereociliary bundle mechanics can function at acoustic frequencies2,61 which may be the primary source of hair cell tuning at higher frequencies. In addition, the possibility does exist that cochlear amplification may occur within the basal region of the chicken basilar papilla, which we could not access. Consistent with this idea, the level of prestin in chicken hair cells is higher in the base than in the apex26. There are also significant gradients along the length of the basilar papilla with regards to the size, orientation, and numbers of stereocilia per hair cell62, which may affect the ability of the bundle to produce and transmit force.
There has been only one prior report, to our knowledge, in which BM vibration was measured in a living bird44, in which pigeons were studied using the Mossbauer technique. This work was done before modern optical interferometric techniques were used for studying cochlear mechanics, and because of the trauma associated with opening the bone of the basilar papilla and the mass of the radioactive source that had to be placed on the BM, questions have persisted regarding their finding of purely passive mechanical responses. Our measurements were made using a less invasive approach and performed in an avian species that permits comparison with the significant amount of hair cell and auditory nerve data that are available. Our data demonstrate that the travelling wave is a primarily a passive phenomenon in birds.
Furthermore, our data argue that the theories of force production by SHCs in order to transfer mechanical energy to the THCs (which convey afferent signals to the auditory nerve) are incorrect. While OHCs perform this role and transfer energy to IHCs in the mammalian cochlea through the TM63,64, this does not happen to a significant level in the apical half of the chicken basilar papilla. Consistent with this, the lack of collagen in the avian TM suggests that it would be difficult to transfer force through it65,66. In addition, the avian TM is anchored to the apical surface of the basilar papilla at the perimeter of each hair cell67, making it difficult to envision how forces could be directed radially through it. Instead of needing SHCs to provide the force to stimulate their bundles, our data indicate that the FcP is not a rigid structure and that it vibrates with adequate magnitude to permit the THCs to be stimulated, independent of the SHCs. Thus the role of SHCs, if any, remains unclear. One possibility is SHCs provide a way for efferent activity to modulate the stiffness of the epithelium, which is perhaps mediated via prestin. Further work in other species that have a similar inner ear anatomy (other birds or perhaps some reptiles), but in which the basal end is more accessible, may be necessary to answer this question.
Methods
Animal preparation
The study protocol was approved by the Stanford IACUC and all experiments conform to the relevant regulatory standards. Both male and female chicks (Gallus domesticus) were studied between days P5-10. Chicks were anaesthetized with ketamine and xylazine and the temperature was maintained at 40–41 °C with a heating pad. The head was fixed with hard clay. A tracheotomy was performed to secure the airway. The musculature was removed to expose the apical half of the basilar papilla without disturbing the otic capsule bone. The artery crossing the basilar papilla was carefully protected. The VOCTV measurements started from base to apex and the hearing was monitored with ABR and CMs during the experiment. After performing all the desired experiments in the living chicken, the animal was killed by anaesthesia overdose so as not to move the head. Further measurements were made post-mortem. Finally, vibration measurements were made from the columella to determine the input to the inner ear.
Volumetric optical coherence tomography and vibrometry (VOCTV)
The VOCTV system was custom-built and has been fully described previously31. It comprised a broadband swept-source with a centre wavelength of 1,310 nm and 50 kHz sweep rate (SSOCT-1,310, Axsun Technologies, Billerica, MA), dual-balanced photodetector (WL-BPD600MA, Wieserlabs, Munich, Germany), and digitizer (NI-5761, National Instruments, Austin, TX). An adaptor attached to the bottom of a dissecting microscope (Stemi-2000, Zeiss, Jena, Germany) scanned the beam in both the x and y directions. For all experiments, the power on the sample was 16 mW. The spatial resolution of our system was measured to be 9.8 μm laterally, assessed by visually discriminating separate lines on an Air Force target, and 15 μm axially in air, assessed as the full-width, half-maximum reflection from a mirror. When imaging through the otic capsule bone in vivo, the spatial resolution is degraded, and is estimated to be 20–25 μm in each dimension68.
Sound stimuli were synthesized in software and output by speakers (MDR EX37B, Sony, Tokyo, Japan) connected to an ear bar that were inserted into ear canal. We calibrated the stimuli and measured DPOAEs using a probe-tip microphone in the ear bar as previously described69. At any given x-y location of the scan mirror, the vibratory data from all pixels along the optical path of the laser were collected and analysed simultaneously. Our most common stimulus protocol was to present tones between 0.1 and 1 kHz in frequency steps of 0.05 kHz at intensities between 30 and 70 dB SPL in 10 dB steps, and between 1 and 5 kHz in frequency steps of 0.25 kHz at intensities between 50 and 90 dB SPL in 10 dB steps. This stimulus protocol was selected so that we did not waste time collecting vibratory data that was likely to be below the noise floor (0.1–0.2 nm under typical experimental conditions) or above the saturation point (500–2,000 nm depending upon the software settings) of our system.
The duration of the sound stimulus ranged from 50 to 200 ms. When possible, this was adjusted to achieve a noise floor that was at least 20 dB below the vibration magnitude or 0.1 nm, whichever was greater. Thus, lower sound intensities required longer measurement times. In addition, vibration data from any given voxel were not analysed if the anatomic image intensity of that voxel was <3 s.d. of the noise floor of the background intensity or if the vibration magnitude at the stimulus frequency was below a threshold set at the mean+3 s.d. of the noise floor measured at nearby frequencies.
Auditory brainstem responses and cochlear microphonics
ABRs and CMs were measured using routine techniques70. Briefly, the ABR potentials were measured from subcutanteous needle electrodes positioned near the left side neck muscles and at the vertex of the head, with a ground electrode placed in the left rear leg. A bioamplifier (DP-311, Warner Instruments, Hamden, CT, USA) was used to amplify the signal 10,000 times. Clicks were created by creating a step pulse of 3 ms duration. To calibrate the stimulus intensity, we measured the click emitted from the speaker with the microphone and determined the root-mean-square value of the sound during the time of the stimulus. During the experiments, the click intensity level was raised in 10 dB steps from 10 to 80 dB SPL. At each sound level, 260 responses were sampled and averaged. The peak-to-peak value of the ABR was measured. Thresholds were defined to be when the peak-to-peak value was 5x higher than the mean noise floor, which was measured during the last 20 ms of the repetition time, after the ABR response was completed.
The CM was measured using the same recording electrodes and bioamplifier. The stimuli were 20 ms 2.75 kHz tones with intensities ranging from 30 to 80 dB SPL. The CM magnitude was determined by Fourier transform (FFT).
Histology
After euthanasia, the chicken basilar papillae were dissected out from the temporal bone and fixed in fixative mixture (25% glutaraldehyde and 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer) overnight at 4°. The tissue was then decalcified in 0.12 M EDTA in 0.1 M PB for 3 days. To view the gross anatomy of the inner ear organs, microdissection was performed to remove the surrounding otic capsule bone and images were taken through a Leica dissection microscope. To perform frozen section analysis, the basilar papilla was embedded in optimal cutting temperature (OCT) compound, frozen and cut in 10 μm sections. Unstained images were acquired using light microscopy with a Zeiss LSM5 Pascal system 227 and a 5X/0.5 EC Plan-NEOFLUAR ∞/0.17 objective.
Data availability
All relevant data are available from the authors upon request.
Additional information
How to cite this article: Xia, A. et al. Hair cell force generation does not amplify or tune vibrations within the chicken basilar papilla. Nat. Commun. 7, 13133 doi: 10.1038/ncomms13133 (2016).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Drs Kuni Iwasa, Charles Steele, Sunil Puria, Tony Ricci and Mirko Scheibinger for helpful discussions and Yanling Du, Jeffrey Chen and Maya Ben-Efraim for technical assistance. Artwork by Chris Gralapp. This project was funded by NIH-NIDCD DC014450, DC013774 and DC010363.
Footnotes
Author contributions A.X., P.D.R., B.E.A. and J.S.O. designed the experiments. A.X., P.D.R. and X.L. performed the experiments. A.X., P.D.R. and J.S.O. analysed the data. A.X., B.E.A. and J.S.O. wrote the manuscript. J.S.O. supervised the project.
References
- Howard J. & Hudspeth A. J. Compliance of the hair bundle associated with gating of mechanoelectrical transduction channels in the bullfrog's saccular hair cell. Neuron 1, 189–199 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudspeth A. J. Integrating the active process of hair cells with cochlear function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 600–614 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ricci A. J., Crawford A. C. & Fettiplace R. Mechanisms of active hair bundle motion in auditory hair cells. J. Neurosci. 22, 44–52 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fettiplace R. & Kim K. X. The physiology of mechanoelectrical transduction channels in hearing. Physiol. Rev. 94, 951–986 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bormuth V., Barral J., Joanny J.-F., Jülicher F. & Martin P. Transduction channels' gating can control friction on vibrating hair-cell bundles in the ear. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 7185–7190 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng J. et al. Prestin is the motor protein of cochlear outer hair cells. Nature 405, 149–155 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brownell W. E., Bader C. R., Bertrand D. & de Ribaupierre Y. Evoked mechanical responses of isolated cochlear outer hair cells. Science 227, 194–196 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergevin C., Manley G. A. & Köppl C. Salient features of otoacoustic emissions are common across tetrapod groups and suggest shared properties of generation mechanisms. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci 112, 3362–3367 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manley G. A. Cochlear mechanisms from a phylogenetic viewpoint. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 11736–11743 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Dijk P., Mason M. J., Schoffelen R. L. M., Narins P. M. & Meenderink S. W. F. Mechanics of the frog ear. Hear. Res. 273, 46–58 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Neill M. P. & Bearden A. Laser-feedback measurements of turtle basilar membrane motion using direct reflection. Hear. Res. 84, 125–138 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peake W. T. & Ling A. Basilar-membrane motion in the alligator lizard: its relation to tonotopic organization and frequency selectivity. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 67, 1736–1745 (1980). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawford A. C. & Fettiplace R. An electrical tuning mechanism in turtle cochlear hair cells. J. Physiol. 312, 317–338 (1981). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs P. A. & Evans M. G. Voltage oscillations and ionic conductances in hair cells isolated from the alligator cochlea. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 164, 151–163 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashmore J. F. Frequency tuning in a frog vestibular organ. Nature 304, 536–538 (1983). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis R. S. & Hudspeth A. J. Voltage- and ion-dependent conductances in solitary vertebrate hair cells. Nature 304, 538–541 (1983). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Bekesy G. Experiments in Hearing McGraw-Hill (1960). [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y., Steele C. R. & Puria S. Cochlear outer-hair-cell power generation and viscous fluid loss. Sci. Rep. 6, 19475 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Heijden M. Frequency selectivity without resonance in a fluid waveguide. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 14548–14552 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen J. Cochlear micromechanics—a physical model of transduction. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 68, 1660–1670 (1980). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narayan S. S., Temchin A. N., Recio A. & Ruggero M. A. Frequency tuning of basilar membrane and auditory nerve fibers in the same cochleae. Science 282, 1882–1884 (1998). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köppl C. Birds-same thing, but different? Convergent evolution in the avian and mammalian auditory systems provides informative comparative models. Hear. Res. 273, 65–71 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirokawa N. The ultrastructure of the basilar papilla of the chick. J. Comp. Neurol. 181, 361–374 (1978). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele C. R. in Diversity in Auditory Mechanics eds Lewis E. R.et al. 455–461World Scientific Publishing Co. (1996). [Google Scholar]
- Iwasa K. H. & Ricci A. J. in Mechanics of Hearing eds Corey D. P., Karavitak K. D. American Institute of Physics (2015). [Google Scholar]
- Beurg M., Tan X. & Fettiplace R. A prestin motor in chicken auditory hair cells: active force generation in a nonmammalian species. Neuron 79, 69–81 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen L., Sun W. & Salvi R. J. Electrically evoked otoacoustic emissions from the chicken ear. Hear. Res. 161, 54–64 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs P. A., Nagai T. & Evans M. G. Electrical tuning in hair cells isolated from the chick cochlea. J. Neurosci. 8, 2460–2467 (1988). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manley G. A., Brix J. & Kaiser A. Developmental stability of the tonotopic organization of the chick's basilar papilla. Science 237, 655–656 (1987). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones T. A., Jones S. M., Paggett K. C., Timothy A. & Emer- K. C. P. Emergence of hearing in the chicken embryo. 96, 128–141 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed]
- Lee H. Y. et al. Noninvasive in vivo imaging reveals differences between tectorial membrane and basilar membrane traveling waves in the mouse cochlea. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 3128–3133 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryals B. M. & Rubel E. W. Patterns of hair cell loss in chick basilar papilla after intense auditory stimulation. Frequency organization. Acta Otolaryngol. 93, 205–210 (1982). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dallos P. Electrical correlates of mechanical events in the cochlea. Audiology 14, 408–418 (1975). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katayama A. Postnatal development of auditory function in the chicken revealed by auditory brain-stem responses (ABRs). Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 62, 388–398 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girod D. A., Park R. H., Park D. L. & Durham D. Changes in the avian cochlea after single high-dose gentamicin. Am. J. Otolaryngol. 21, 379–388 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sánchez Fernández J. M., Martínez Ibargüen A., Avalos Cuica N., Sánchez del Rey A. & Santaolalla Montoya F. Auditory function recovery following acoustic overstimulation. Acta Otolaryngol. 124, 427–430 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen L., Salvi R. & Shero M. Cochlear frequency-place map in adult chickens: intracellular biocytin labeling. Hear. Res. 81, 130–136 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones S. M. & Jones T. A. The tonotopic map in the embryonic chicken cochlea. Hear. Res. 82, 149–157 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan X., Beurg M., Hackney C., Mahendrasingam S. & Fettiplace R. Electrical tuning and transduction in short hair cells of the chicken auditory papilla. J. Neurophysiol. 109, 2007–2020 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu Y. C. & Fettiplace R. A developmental model for generating frequency maps in the reptilian and avian cochleas. Biophys. J. 70, 2557–2570 (1996). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubel E. W. & Ryals B. M. Development of the place principle: acoustic trauma. Science 219, 512–514 (1983). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lifshitz J., Furman A. C., Altman K. W. & Saunders J. C. Spatial tuning curves along the chick basilar papilla in normal and sound-exposed ears. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 5, 171–184 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones S. M. & Jones T. A. Neural tuning characteristics of auditory primary afferents in the chicken embryo. Hear. Res. 82, 139–148 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smolders J. W., Gummer A. W. & Klinke R. Travelling wave motion along the pigeon basilar membrane. ORL J. Otorhinolaryngol. Relat. Spec. 48, 93–97 (1986). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warchol M. E. & Dallos P. Neural response to very low-frequency sound in the avian cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 166, 83–95 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontaine B., Köppl C. & Peña J. L. Reverse correlation analysis of auditory-nerve fiber responses to broadband noise in a bird, the barn owl. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 16, 101–119 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köppl C. Phase locking to high frequencies in the auditory nerve and cochlear nucleus magnocellularis of the barn owl, Tyto alba. J. Neurosci. 17, 3312–3321 (1997). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köppl C. Frequency tuning and spontaneous activity in the auditory nerve and cochlear nucleus magnocellularis of the barn owl Tyto alba. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 364–377 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meenderink S. W. F. & van der Heijden M. Reverse cochlear propagation in the intact cochlea of the gerbil: evidence for slow traveling waves. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 1448–1455 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggero M. A. Systematic errors in indirect estimates of basilar membrane travel times. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 67, 707–710 (1980). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ren T., He W. & Porsov E. Localization of the cochlear amplifier in living sensitive ears. PLoS ONE 6, e20149 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao S. S. et al. Vibration of the organ of Corti within the cochlear apex in mice. J. Neurophysiol. 112, 1192–1204 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramamoorthy S. et al. Filtering of acoustic signals within the hearing organ. J. Neurosci. 34, 9051–9058 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Froymovich O., Rebala V., Salvi R. J. & Rassael H. Long-term effect of acoustic trauma on distortion product otoacoustic emissions in chickens. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 97, 3021–3029 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergevin C., Freeman D. M., Saunders J. C. & Shera C. A. Otoacoustic emissions in humans, birds, lizards, and frogs: evidence for multiple generation mechanisms. J. Comp. Physiol. A. Neuroethol. Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol. 194, 665–683 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burkard R., Salvi R. & Chen L. 2f1-f2 distortion product otoacoustic emissions in White Leghorn chickens (Gallus domesticus): effects of frequency ratio and relative level. Audiol. Neurootol. 1, 197–213 (1996). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kettembeil S., Manley G. A. & Siegl E. Distortion-product otoacoustic emissions and their anaesthesia sensitivity in the European starling and the chicken. Hear. Res. 86, 47–62 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ipakchi R., Kyin T. & Saunders J. C. Loss and recovery of sound-evoked otoacoustic emissions in young chicks following acoustic trauma. Audiol. Neurootol. 10, 209–219 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lichtenhan J. T., Chertoff M. E., Smittkamp S. E., Durham D. & Girod D. A. Predicting severity of cochlear hair cell damage in adult chickens using DPOAE input-output functions. Hear. Res. 201, 109–120 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu Y. C., Art J. J., Goodman M. B. & Fettiplace R. A kinetic description of the calcium-activated potassium channel and its application to electrical tuning of hair cells. Prog. Biophys. Mol. Biol. 63, 131–158 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fettiplace R. Active hair bundle movements in auditory hair cells. J. Physiol. 576, 29–36 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tilney L. G. & Tilney M. S. The actin filament content of hair cells of the bird cochlea is nearly constant even though the length, width, and number of stereocilia vary depending on the hair cell location. J. Cell Biol. 107, 2563–2574 (1988). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legan P. K. et al. A targeted deletion in alpha-tectorin reveals that the tectorial membrane is required for the gain and timing of cochlear feedback. Neuron 28, 273–285 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia A. et al. Deficient forward transduction and enhanced reverse transduction in the alpha tectorin C1509G human hearing loss mutation. Dis. Model. Mech. 3, 209–223 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killick R., Malenczak C. & Richardson G. P. The protein composition of the avian tectorial membrane. Hear. Res. 64, 21–38 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thalmann I. et al. Composition and supramolecular organization of the tectorial membrane. Laryngoscope 97, 357–367 (1987). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka K. & Smith C. A. Structure of the avian tectorial membrane. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 84, 287–296 (1975). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao S. S. et al. Quantitative imaging of cochlear soft tissues in wild-type and hearing-impaired transgenic mice by spectral domain optical coherence tomography. Opt. Express 19, 15415–15428 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia A. et al. Prestin regulation and function in residual outer hair cells after noise-induced hearing loss. PLoS ONE 8, e82602 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xia A. et al. Altered traveling wave propagation and reduced endocochlear potential associated with cochlear dysplasia in the BETA2/NeuroD1 null mouse. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 8, 447–463 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are available from the authors upon request.