Abstract
Sleep is characterized by a loss of consciousness, which has been attributed to a breakdown of functional connectivity between brain regions. Global field synchronization (GFS) can estimate functional connectivity of brain processes. GFS is a frequency-dependent measure of global synchronicity of multi-channel EEG data. Our aim was to explore and extend the hypothesis of disconnection during sleep by comparing GFS spectra of different vigilance states. The analysis was performed on eight healthy adult male subjects. EEG was recorded during a baseline night, a recovery night after 40 h of sustained wakefulness and at 3 h intervals during the 40 h of wakefulness. Compared to non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, REM sleep showed larger GFS values in all frequencies except in the spindle and theta bands, where NREM sleep showed a peak in GFS. Sleep deprivation did not affect GFS spectra in REM and NREM sleep. Waking GFS values were lower compared with REM and NREM sleep except for the alpha band. Waking alpha GFS decreased following sleep deprivation in the eyes closed condition only. Our surprising finding of higher synchrony during REM sleep challenges the view of REM sleep as a desynchronized brain state and may provide insight into the function of REM sleep.
Keywords: sleep, EEG, global field synchrony, rapid eye movement sleep
1. Introduction
Sleep is a behaviour observed in all organisms studied thus far. Among the behavioural definition of sleep is a significant decline in responsiveness to external stimuli. This reduction in responsiveness, or consciousness, during sleep has been attributed to the breakdown of connectivity between brain regions [1]. According to this idea, called the integrated information theory, consciousness arises when information is integrated across brain regions [1]. Support for this conjecture comes from experimental studies that have used simultaneous high-density EEG and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to examine signal propagation during waking and sleep. These studies have found that when compared with waking, in which the TMS-induced signal propagates across brain regions for up to 300 ms, during early non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep the activity induced by TMS remains localized to the site of stimulation and lasts less than 150 ms [2]. As compared to early NREM sleep, the TMS-evoked response during REM sleep, a state often associated with vivid dreams, was more widespread and longer in duration [3]. However, the TMS response during REM sleep was more localized than that observed during waking, suggesting that the response to REM sleep is in between that of waking and NREM sleep.
Although TMS is a useful tool to probe brain connectivity, it is not a measure of spontaneous cortical activity. One of many ways to measure spontaneous connectivity during different states is through calculating EEG coherence. Simply put, coherence is a measure of the degree of correlation between two EEG derivations in the frequency domain. Large coherence values are believed to indicate functional connectivity between two regions or indicate that a common third region drives both regions [4]. Studies of coherence during sleep have indicated that coherence is higher during NREM when compared with REM sleep in the delta and sigma bands [5,6].
Global field synchronization (GFS) is a method to measure large-scale synchrony introduced by Koenig et al. [7]. This measure uses multi-channel EEG to get a measure of global phase alignment across all derivations as a function of frequency. GFS measures phase synchronization between all derivations and ranges from zero (no predominant phase; minimal phase synchronization among derivations) to one (perfect phase synchronization; all derivations in phase or anti-phase; [7]). Given that the EEG signal is not produced by a single, focal electric source, the observation of a predominant phase across the scalp measurements implies that there is also a preferred phase for the intracranial neuroelectric dynamics, and a spread in phase across scalp measurements must have been generated by intracranial electric sources that differ in phase. This measure has successfully been used to show differences in functional connectivity in schizophrenic and Alzheimer patients as compared to controls [7–9]. Furthermore, applying this method to the anaesthetized brain revealed decreased global synchrony in the gamma range (30.5–80 Hz) [10].
Given that GFS has been shown to be a measure of large-scale synchrony, we apply this method to the sleep EEG of healthy adults during baseline sleep after 16 h of wakefulness in order to compare synchrony in waking, and two sleep states—NREM and REM sleep. Furthermore, we examine GFS following 40 h of sustained wakefulness to determine whether sleep deprivation has an impact on global synchrony. Such sleep deprivation induces stereotypical changes to the sleep EEG power spectrum, most prominently an increase in delta power (0.75–4.5 Hz) [11]. In addition to the effects of sleep deprivation on the sleep EEG, waking EEG theta power (5–8 Hz) increases with time awake [12]. Thus, we also examined GFS of the waking EEG as a function of time awake.
2. Material and methods
2.1. Dataset
The analyses were performed on an existing dataset of eight healthy young male participants of a previous study investigating the effects of sleep deprivation on EEG topography [12,13]. Polysomnographic recordings were obtained during an adaptation night, a subsequent baseline night and a recovery night after 40 h of sustained wakefulness. Waking EEG recordings, consisting of 5 min eyes open, 4–5 min eyes closed and a second 5 min eyes open session, were performed every 3 h during the 40 h sleep deprivation period which started at 07.00 h. During baseline and recovery sleep and for the waking EEG, 27 scalp EEG electrodes (extended 10–20 system; locations shown in the electronic supplementary material, figure S1) were recorded. Bedtime for all three nights was scheduled at 23.00 h and sleep was limited to 8 h for the adaptation and baseline nights and to 12 h for the recovery nights. Participants were instructed to abstain from alcohol and to adhere to regular bedtimes (8 h time in bed) for 3 days prior to the study, verified by ambulatory activity monitoring and sleep–wake diaries. The study protocol and all experimental procedures were approved by the local ethics committees for research on human subjects and participants gave their written informed consent. Sleep EEG data of baseline and recovery sleep after 40 h of sustained wakefulness were analysed. Waking EEG data at 3 h intervals was also analysed. The EEG signals were sampled at 128 Hz during sleep and at 256 Hz during wakefulness (for additional details, see [12,13]).
2.2. Data analysis
Sleep stages were visually scored for 20 s epochs (C3A2 derivation) according to the criteria of Rechtschaffen & Kales [14]. The analysis was restricted to the maximal common length of 7 h 32 min after sleep onset. However, in one subject 4 h 7 min of data were included due to technical problems (see [13] for details). Artefacts were identified as described by Finelli et al. [12,13].
2.3. Global field synchronization
GFS was introduced by Koenig et al. [7] and is briefly summarized here. The EEG was re-referenced to average reference. For consecutive 4 s epochs global functional connectivity was computed in the following way: using the fast Fourier transform with a Tukey window (tapered cosine, ratio of cosine-tapered section length to the entire window length = 0.2), the complex spectrum was determined for each derivation yielding a complex value for each frequency and derivation. At a given frequency, the complex Fourier coefficients of every channel can be mapped onto the complex plane (electronic supplementary material, figure S2). The shape of the resulting cloud of points is indicative of the amount of phase synchronization across derivations: a very elongated cloud indicates that the EEG at the given frequency is dominated by a common phase or anti-phase across all derivations. By contrast, if the cloud is nearly round, no predominant phase is present. To quantify the shape of the cloud in the complex plane, a two-dimensional principal component analysis (PCA) was applied by fitting an elliptical formed bivariate Gaussian distribution where the ratio of the principal axes correspond to the ratio of the PCA eigenvalues (λ1 and λ2). The ratio of the resulting two eigenvalues defines GFS:
2.1 |
; GFS ranges from 0 (no predominant phase; minimal synchronization) to 1 (all derivations in phase or anti-phase; maximal synchronization; [7]) and is independent of the spectral power. We note that it is crucial to apply an appropriate window function (non-rectangular) for determining GFS to avoid spurious synchronization in the beta/gamma range.
GFS makes no assumption about the spatial location of the activity and is independent of the reference [15]. The concept of GFS assumes that most types of measurable brain activity generate signals that are distributed over many electrodes on the scalp. Theoretically, an increase in GFS can have two possible interpretations. On the one hand, it may result from the increased activity of a single source which would be paralleled by an increase in spectral power. On the other hand, increased GFS can result from several active sources that increase their relative amounts of activity operating in a common phase such that they appear as a single meta-generator. The latter scenario refers to the classic binding hypothesis and does not necessarily imply an increase in spectral power [16].
For statistical analysis, GFS was Fisher z-transformed and back transformed for illustration. We analysed the entire GFS spectra during extended waking, baseline sleep and recovery sleep. We performed bootstrap analyses at each frequency bin (0.25 Hz frequency resolution; 0.5–25 Hz range) in order to test whether there were differences in GFS spectra between two states. Although statistical analysis was performed for all bins, we note that a 9-point moving average was used to smooth the spectra for clearer visualization in the figures. The bootstrap test is a non-parametric test and is particularly well suited to spectral analysis as it accounts for multiple comparisons [17]. We made the following comparisons using the bootstrap statistic: (i) in order to confirm that sleep deprivation has an impact on power spectra in the current dataset, we compared NREM and REM spectra before and after sleep deprivation, (ii) in order to examine the impact of sleep deprivation on GFS spectra, GFS spectra before and after sleep deprivation for NREM and REM sleep (separately) were compared, (iii) NREM and REM sleep GFS spectra in the baseline (no sleep deprivation) condition were compared to examine sleep state differences in GFS spectra, and (iv) waking GFS spectra after 15 h awake and NREM and REM sleep GFS spectra separately were compared to examine state differences in GFS spectra. The waking GFS spectrum after 15 h awake was chosen because it was the recording closest to when sleep would occur under normal, non-sleep-deprived circumstances.
In order to test the evolution of GFS in the theta (5–8 Hz) and alpha (9–11 Hz) bands across the sleep deprivation period, we performed a mixed models ANOVA with between-subjects factors time awake (14 time points) and eyes open/closed.
3. Results
3.1. State dependence of global field synchronization
The results of one representative subject for the baseline night of sleep are shown in figure 1. In this figure, sleep stages are shown in figure 1a, the power density and GFS spectrograms across the night as a function of frequency in figure 1b and figure 1c, respectively. In contrast to the power spectrogram, the GFS spectrogram does not show a clear modulation by the NREM-REM sleep cycle. Similar to the power density spectrogram, however, a band is visible in the spindle frequency range (i.e. between 12 and 16 Hz). The increase in GFS in the spindle frequency during NREM sleep is also reflected in the all-night mean of stage 2 (S2), slow wave and NREM sleep depicted in figure 1d. Surprisingly compared to slow wave sleep (SWS), S2 and NREM sleep, GFS was higher in most frequency bands during REM sleep. Two exceptions to this were the two peaks in the GFS spectrum—the spindle band in which GFS was higher in NREM sleep and the theta band in which the NREM and REM spectra did not differ. This is in contrast to the power density spectrum where power in NREM, S2 and SWS is above that of REM sleep up to 16 Hz. This trend was apparent in the single subject (figure 1) data and also in the mean (figure 2).
3.2. Effects of sleep deprivation on global field synchronization and power density spectra during sleep
Consistent with the published literature, we found an increase in spectral power during NREM sleep in the delta band and no change for REM sleep [11]. We note that the range is narrower than previously reported due to the use of the bootstrap statistic, which is more conservative and corrects for multiple comparisons. However, we found no impact of sleep deprivation on GFS during NREM or REM sleep (figure 2). Thus, GFS was highest during baseline/recovery REM sleep when compared with baseline/recovery NREM sleep (figure 2; with the exception of the spindle and theta band).
3.3. Effects of sleep deprivation on global field synchronization during sustained wakefulness
Interestingly, GFS was higher during sleep (NREM and REM sleep) when compared with waking in the eyes open and closed conditions (figure 3). An exception was GFS in the alpha band, which was greater in waking than NREM and REM sleep in the eyes closed condition (9–11 Hz). With regards to the eyes open condition, GFS was not significantly different between the two conditions in this band, with the exception of a few frequency bins. Furthermore, in both eyes open and closed conditions, there were no differences between waking and sleep in the low delta band (less than 2 Hz).
In order to assess the impact of increasing time awake on waking EEG GFS, we chose two bands that appeared to be most impacted by time awake based on visual inspection (electronic supplementary material, figure S3)—the theta (5–8 Hz) and alpha (9–11 Hz) bands. Furthermore, previous studies have shown that these bands are sensitive to prolonged periods of waking [12,18,19]. Subject average and standard deviation of GFS in these bands as a function of time awake and clock time is shown in figure 4 for eyes open (red circles) and closed (blue circles). In the theta band no impact of increasing time awake was found for either eyes open or closed (main effect time, p = 0.26). Furthermore, there was a trend towards a significant effect for eyes open/closed conditions in this band (main effect condition, p = 0.054) and no significant interaction (p = 0.98).
On the other hand, GFS in the alpha band showed a main effect of time (p < 0.001) and condition (p < 0.001), with a trend towards an interaction (p = 0.058). Based on these results, we used a paired t-test to examine the effect of sleep deprivation on GFS in the alpha band. For this analysis, we averaged GFS prior to sleep deprivation to the corresponding time points following a night of sleep deprivation (10.00–22.00). We found a significant decline in GFS following sleep deprivation in the eyes closed condition (p = 0.02) but no change in the eyes open condition (p = 0.15).
4. Discussion
In this study, we examined GFS during three different states—waking, NREM and REM sleep—and found strong state-dependent modulations of GFS. In contrast to what is observed in power spectral measures, we find greatest global synchrony (GFS) during REM sleep when compared with NREM sleep and waking. This disassociation between power and GFS is not surprising given that GFS is independent of power and is a measure of phase synchrony across all derivations. Thus, such synchronous activity is not detectable by visual inspection and is different from EEG coherence as coherence is always between a pair of derivations. Therefore, our analysis of GFS reveals unique information about global synchrony across the brain.
4.1. Effects of sleep deprivation on global field synchronization during sleep
Sleep EEG NREM delta power is a reliable measure of sleep homeostasis—increasing with prolonged wakefulness and dissipating over the course of sleep. Unlike sleep EEG delta power, we see no impact of sleep deprivation on GFS in any frequency band for NREM or REM sleep. Furthermore, we do not find a decrease in GFS across the course of a night (figure 1), suggesting that sleep pressure does not impact brain synchrony. This finding is similar to what is seen in measures of coherence, which also do not change across a night of sleep [5]. This suggests that increased sleep depth does not result in a breakdown or strengthening of global synchrony.
4.2. Effects of sleep deprivation on global field synchronization during sustained wakefulness
Previous analysis of this dataset using power measures revealed a circadian modulation of theta power combined with an increase in power in this band with increasing time awake [12]. On the other hand, in contrast to power, we did not observe a change in GFS with increasing sleep pressure in the theta band, but rather in the alpha band. Others have reported a sensitivity of waking alpha power to increased time awake [18,19]. Meisel et al. [20] observed an increase of synchrony in a broad frequency range during sustained wakefulness (eyes open), whereas we found no change in GFS during the eyes open condition. This observation of increased synchrony was interpreted as increased cortical excitability as a function of time awake. However, the applied synchronization measure was based on phase synchrony calculated for all pairs of derivations and then averaged [20]. Since the measure used by Meisel and colleagues is an average, the increased synchronization may be driven by a subset of derivations and does not reflect global synchronization such as it is captured with GFS. Thus, by calculating GFS we uncover unique information about global synchrony during extended wakefulness.
4.3. Thalamocortical synchrony underlying global field synchronization?
We observed highest synchronization for the spindle band during NREM sleep, the alpha band during waking and REM sleep in general. It has long been established that NREM sleep spindles are generated through thalamocortical loops (e.g. [21]). Furthermore, neuroimaging studies in humans combining EEG with positron emission tomography (PET) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) have also implicated the thalamus in the generation of the waking alpha rhythm, with greater thalamic activity being associated with greater waking alpha power [22–25]. Finally, imaging studies of sleep have revealed high activity of the thalamus during REM sleep [26,27], and one study has reported widespread thalamocortical synchronized activity during phasic REM sleep [28]. Given that the thalamus plays an important role in the rhythms with the highest GFS—namely NREM sleep spindles, waking alpha and REM sleep in general—we hypothesize that the thalamus may be responsible for widespread cortical synchrony. Indeed, the thalamus projects widely to the cortex and is capable of synchronizing activity across cortical networks [29,30]. As such, the thalamus is an ideal candidate for the mechanism underlying our finding of increased GFS for specific oscillations and states that rely on thalamic activity. This conjecture, however, is very preliminary and future studies using simultaneous EEG and fMRI to calculate GFS and correlate it with thalamic activity during different states are necessary. Indeed, a recent study using simultaneous EEG/fMRI found thalamic involvement in the generation of microstates recorded from scalp EEG and proposed that the thalamus is important for the synchrony of oscillations among different cortical regions [31].
Our results highlight a novel aspect of REM sleep, not previously reported—namely that REM sleep exhibits high global synchrony. We propose the involvement of the thalamus in producing this synchrony and hope that this finding will spur research examining synchronous activity and its functional significance during REM sleep.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Helene Bastuji, Marcllo Massimini, Roberto Pascual-Marqui, Simone Sarasso and Vladyslav V. Vyazovskiy for fruitful discussions.
Ethics
This study was approved by the local ethics committee and participants gave their written informed consent.
Data accessibility
Our data are available for download in the supplementary section of the journal and by request from the first authors.
Authors' contributions
P.A. analysed data, designed the study and drafted the manuscript. T.R. analysed data and helped draft the manuscript. R.D. helped with data analysis. T.K. helped with data analysis and drafting of the manuscript. L.T. helped with data analysis and drafted the manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors have no competing interests to declare.
Funding
Financial support came from the Swiss National Science Foundation grant nos. 32003B_146643 (to P.A.) and CRSII3_136249 (to P.A. and T.K.).
References
- 1.Tononi G. 2004. An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neurosci. 5, 42 (doi:10.1186/1471-2202-5-42) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Huber R, Esser SK, Singh H, Tononi G. 2005. Breakdown of cortical effective connectivity during sleep. Science 309, 2228–2232. (doi:10.1126/science.1117256) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Massimini M, Ferrarelli F, Murphy M, Huber R, Riedner B, Casarotto S, Tononi G. 2010. Cortical reactivity and effective connectivity during REM sleep in humans. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 176–183. (doi:10.1080/17588921003731578) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sporns O. 2007. Brain connectivity. Scholarpedia 2, 4695 (doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.4695) [Google Scholar]
- 5.Achermann P, Borbely AA. 1998. Temporal evolution of coherence and power in the human sleep electroencephalogram. J. Sleep Res. 7(Suppl 1), 36–41. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2869.7.s1.6.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kaminski M, Blinowska K, Szclenberger W. 1997. Topographic analysis of coherence and propagation of EEG activity during sleep and wakefulness. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 102, 216–227. (doi:10.1016/S0013-4694(96)95721-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Koenig T, Lehmann D, Saito N, Kuginuki T, Kinoshita T, Koukkou M. 2001. Decreased functional connectivity of EEG theta-frequency activity in first-episode, neuroleptic-naive patients with schizophrenia: preliminary results. Schizophr. Res. 50, 55–60. (doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00154-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Koenig T, van Swam C, Dierks T, Hubl D. 2012. Is gamma band EEG synchronization reduced during auditory driving in schizophrenia patients with auditory verbal hallucinations? Schizophr. Res. 141, 266–270. (doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.07.016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Koenig T, Prichep L, Dierks T, Hubl D, Wahlund LO, John ER, Jelic V. 2005. Decreased EEG synchronization in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 165–171. (doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nicolaou N, Georgiou J. 2014. Global field synchrony during general anaesthesia. Br. J. Anaesth. 112, 529–539. (doi:10.1093/bja/aet350) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Achermann P, Borbely AA. 2011. Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. In Principles and practices of sleep medicine (eds Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC), 5th edn St. Louis, MO: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Finelli LA, Baumann H, Borbely AA, Achermann P. 2000. Dual electroencephalogram markers of human sleep homeostasis: correlation between theta activity in waking and slow-wave activity in sleep. Neuroscience 101, 523–529. (doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00409-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Finelli LA, Borbély AA, Achermann P. 2001. Functional topography of the human nonREM sleep electroencephalogram. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 2282–2290. (doi:10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01597.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. 1986. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Michels L, Luchinger R, Koenig T, Martin E, Brandeis D. 2012. Developmental changes of BOLD signal correlations with global human EEG power and synchronization during working memory. PLoS ONE 7, e39447 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0039447) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kottlow M, Jann K, Dierks T, Koenig T. 2012. Increased phase synchronization during continuous face integration measured simultaneously with EEG and fMRI. Clin. Neurophysiol. 123, 1536–1548. (doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.12.019) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Maris E, Oostenveld R. 2007. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. (doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ferreira C, Deslandes A, Moraes H, Cagy M, Pompeu F, Basile LF, Piedade R, Ribeiro P. 2006. Electroencephalographic changes after one night of sleep deprivation. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 64, 388–393. (doi:10.1590/S0004-282X2006000300007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Strijkstra AM, Beersma DG, Drayer B, Halbesma N, Daan S. 2003. Subjective sleepiness correlates negatively with global alpha (8–12 Hz) and positively with central frontal theta (4–8 Hz) frequencies in the human resting awake electroencephalogram. Neurosci. Lett. 340, 17–20. (doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00033-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Meisel C, Schulze-Bonhage A, Freestone D, Cook MJ, Achermann P, Plenz D. 2015. Intrinsic excitability measures track antiepileptic drug action and uncover increasing/decreasing excitability over the wake/sleep cycle. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 14 694–14 699. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1513716112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Steriade M, Deschenes M, Domich L, Mulle C. 1985. Abolition of spindle oscillations in thalamic neurons disconnected from nucleus reticularis thalami. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 1473–1497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Omata K, Hanakawa T, Morimoto M, Honda M. 2013. Spontaneous slow fluctuation of EEG alpha rhythm reflects activity in deep-brain structures: a simultaneous EEG-fMRI study. PLoS ONE 8, e66869 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0066869) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Goldman RI, Stern JM, Engel J Jr, Cohen MS. 2002. Simultaneous EEG and fMRI of the alpha rhythm. Neuroreport 13, 2487–2492. (doi:10.1097/00001756-200212200-00022) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Larson CL, Davidson RJ, Abercrombie HC, Ward RT, Schaefer SM, Jackson DC, Holden JE, Perlman SB. 1998. Relations between PET-derived measures of thalamic glucose metabolism and EEG alpha power. Psychophysiology 35, 162–169. (doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3520162) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lindgren KA, et al. 1999. Thalamic metabolic rate predicts EEG alpha power in healthy control subjects but not in depressed patients. Biol. Psychiatry 45, 943–952. (doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00350-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Maquet P, Peters J, Aerts J, Delfiore G, Degueldre C, Luxen A, Franck G. 1996. Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature 383, 163–166. (doi:10.1038/383163a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Buchsbaum MS, Hazlett EA, Wu J, Bunney WE Jr. 2001. Positron emission tomography with deoxyglucose-F18 imaging of sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology 25(Suppl 5), S50–S56. (doi:10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00339-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wehrle R, Kaufmann C, Wetter TC, Holsboer F, Auer DP, Pollmacher T, Czisch M. 2007. Functional microstates within human REM sleep: first evidence from fMRI of a thalamocortical network specific for phasic REM periods. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 863–871. (doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05314.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Modha DS, Singh R. 2010. Network architecture of the long-distance pathways in the macaque brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 13 485–13 490. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1008054107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.O'Muircheartaigh J, Keller SS, Barker GJ, Richardson MP. 2015. White matter connectivity of the thalamus delineates the functional architecture of competing thalamocortical systems. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4477–4489. (doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv063) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Schwab S, Koenig T, Morishima Y, Dierks T, Federspiel A, Jann K. 2015. Discovering frequency sensitive thalamic nuclei from EEG microstate informed resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 118, 368–375. (doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Our data are available for download in the supplementary section of the journal and by request from the first authors.