Abstract
Recent studies show differences in individual motion and shoaling tendency between strains of the same species. Here, we analyse collective motion and response to visual stimuli in two morphologically different strains (TL and AB) of zebrafish. For both strains, we observed 10 groups of 5 and 10 zebrafish swimming freely in a large experimental tank with two identical landmarks (cylinders or discs) for 1 h. We tracked the positions of the fish by an automated tracking method and compute several metrics at the group level. First, the probability of the presence shows that both strains avoid free space and are more likely to swim in the vicinity of the walls of the tank and the landmarks. Second, the analysis of landmarks occupancy shows that AB zebrafish are more present in their vicinity than TL ones and that both strains regularly transit from one to the other one with no preference on the long duration. Finally, TL zebrafish show a higher cohesion than AB zebrafish. Thus, environmental heterogeneity and duration of the trials allow to reveal individual and collective behavioural variabilities among different strains of zebrafish. These results provide a new insight into the need to take into account individual variability of zebrafish strains for studying collective behaviour.
Keywords: collective behaviour, decision-making, zebrafish, phenotypes
1. Introduction
Collective decision-making has been evidenced in many animal species and contexts [1] including food collection [2], problem-solving [3,4], collective movement [5–11] or nest-site selection [12,13]. In this later case, social animals have to select a resting site among several potential options in a complex environment. This selection can be made either through individual decisions or complex decision-making processes involving the participation of all individuals [14] and can be temporary or permanent according to the needs and living style of the considered species.
This process of collective decision has been studied for a long time in social animals that select a permanent home: social insects, fish and birds. In particular, experiments on fish have been generally designed to observe preferences for particular environmental features or landmarks for a relatively short experimental time (few seconds [15], 5 min [16], 10 min [17], up to 30 min per trial [18]). These studies have shown, for example, that landmarks in a bare tank arouse interest and attract the fish [18,19] and that variations of the shape of these landmarks can change territory features [20,21].
While these studies provide numerous insights on the individual and collective preferences in fish groups, they generally rely on the comparison between two or more qualitatively different alternatives. Thus, the selection of one option is often based on an intrinsic preference for a particular feature in comparison with the others. Such asymmetric choices may hide the collective decision that results from the internal processes of decision-making of the group.
Furthermore, it has been evidenced with fish (Oreochromis niloticus, Gambusia holbrooki and Notemigonus crysoleucas) that the group sizes impact motion [22,23] (speed, turning speed and nearest-neighbour distance) and some moving behaviours [24], such as the milling and the alignment. Depending on the species, studies show opposite results: Becco et al. [22] work with Oreochromis niloticus with two group sizes (330 and 905 fish); Tunstrom et al. [24] work with Notemigonus crysoleucas with four group sizes (30, 70, 150 and 300 fish). Increasing the group size of Oreochromis niloticus makes stronger alignments, while for Notemigonus crysoleucas alignments decrease. In this study, we test whether and how collective behaviours and decision-making are affected by the strain and the group sizes.
Here, our aim is to characterize the collective behaviour of groups of zebrafish swimming in an environment with identical landmarks. We observe the collective motion of small shoals of different group sizes (5 and 10 fish) and of two different zebrafish strains (laboratory wild-type AB or TL). Zebrafish are a gregarious vertebrate model organism that can be used to study the cohesion of the group and its decision-making [25,26]. Originating from India, the zebrafish is a diurnal species that prefers staying in groups both in nature and in the laboratory [9,27,28]. There is a wide variability in shoal sizes: zebrafish live in small groups in shallow freshwaters [29,30] and can aggregate into larger groups of 300 individuals [31]. Zebrafish living in a variety of habitats with varying structural complexities [31,32] (from river channels, irrigation canals to beels), we set our experimental method relying on the observations of fish freely swimming in an open environment along with heterogeneous landmarks rather than in a constraining set-up (i.e. mazes as used in [15–17]). We observe for 1 h per replicate each group of fish swimming in a large experimental tank (1×1 m and 1.20×1.20 m) with two spots of interest (landmarks).
The landmarks consist of two striped yellow-green opaque plastic cylinders placed in the water column or two blue transparent floating Perspex discs providing shadow. We choose these colours in the visible spectrum of the zebrafish according to the results of [33]. We expect that these landmarks placed in a homogeneous environment could induce a choice of one preferred option by the zebrafish as evidenced for other species faced with identical resources [13,34,35]. On the one hand, we test with cylinders the effect of visual and physical cues in the water column on collective choices. Since zebrafish are known to swim along the walls of the experimental tank [36], cylinders could act as such walls in the water column. On the other hand, we test with floating discs the impact of visual and physical cues above water, on collective choices. We placed discs and cylinders as landmarks to see whether and how the two strains of zebrafish will adapt their group behaviour and their preferences for landmarks.
2. Results
2.1. Differences of group features between AB and TL strains of zebrafish
We test two types of landmarks: striped yellow and green cylinders and floating Perspex transparent blue discs (see Material and methods), on two laboratory strains of zebrafish (TL and AB) for 10 replicates. We computed the interindividual distances between all fish to characterize the cohesion of the group for both strains and group sizes in the presence of the cylinders. The distribution of all interindividual distances (figure 1) shows that the groups of TL zebrafish have a stronger cohesion (5 TL: median=0.12 m and 10 TL: median=0.14 m) than the groups of AB zebrafish (5 AB: median=0.27 m and 10 AB: median=0.23 m). The intrastrain comparison for the two group sizes shows that the distributions significantly differ from each other (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, 5 AB versus 10 AB, D=0.102, p<0.001; 5 TL versus 10 TL, D=0.080, p<0.001). The interstrain comparison for similar group sizes also reveals a statistical difference between the distributions (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, 10 AB versus 10 TL, D=0.184, p<0.001; 5 AB versus 5 TL, D=0.161, p<0.001).
The distributions of the average interindividual distances measured at each time step confirm these results (figure 2, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, 5 AB versus 10 AB, D=0.433, p<0.001; 5 TL versus 10 TL, D=0.051, p<0.001; 10 AB versus 10 TL, D=0.333, p<0.001; 5 AB versus 5 TL, D=0.464, p<0.001). The medians of the distributions of the average interindividual distances for 10 AB is 0.33 m, for 10 TL is 0.29 m, for 5 AB is 0.39 m and for 5 TL is 0.30 m. Moreover, the time series of the average interindividual distance reveals that the cohesion of the fish decreases for both strains and both group sizes through the time (electronic supplementary materials, figure S11).
In the presence of the floating discs, TL zebrafish were also significantly more cohesive than the AB zebrafish, as shown by the distribution of the interindividual distances (medians for 10 AB: 0.35 m, for 10 TL: 0.23 m, figure 3, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, D=0.135 and p<0.001). The previous analysis matches with the one obtained for the measurements of average interindividual distances (medians for 10 AB: 0.45 m, for 10 TL: 0.41 m, figure 4, Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, D=0.230 and p<0.001).
2.2. Group sizes affect strain preferences for landmarks
In the presence of the two cylinders, the groups of 10 AB zebrafish are mainly present along the wall of the tank and around the cylinders, as shown by their average probability of the presence computed for the 10 replicates, each of 1 h observation time (figure 5). On the contrary, groups of 5 TL, 10 TL and 5 AB zebrafish are less observed near the cylinders but are still present along the walls of the tank (figure 5). With the two discs we observed similar behaviours than in the previous experiments with cylinders. The maximum probability of the presence under the discs with AB zebrafish reaches 4.5×10−3 while for TL zebrafish it reaches only 1×10−3 (figure 6). Again, TL zebrafish spent the majority of their time near the borders of the tank. The probabilities of the presence computed for each experiment are shown in the electronic supplementary materials, figures S1–S6.
Then, we studied in more detail the dynamics of the presence of groups of 10 zebrafish near the cylinders. The fish of the AB strain form a cohesive group that regularly transits from one landmark to the other one at the beginning of the trial. Then the group starts to split into multiple subgroups and the periodicity of the visits becomes less regular (electronic supplementary material, figure S7). These oscillations are also observed for groups of the TL strain but contrarily to AB zebrafish, this phenomenon is observed for the whole experimental time (electronic supplementary material, figure S8). To quantify the dynamics of these transitions, we computed the number of majority events detected near one of the two landmarks or outside of them. A majority event was counted when 7 or more individuals were simultaneously present in the same zone independently of the duration of this majority event. Figure 7a shows that the median and mean number of majority events are always smaller with the TL strain, but this difference is only significant for the majorities detected near one of the cylinders (cylinder 1, Mann–Whitney U-test, U=26, p=0.038 ; cylinder 2, Mann–Whitney U test, U=48, p=0.455 ; outside, Mann–Whitney U-test, U=38, p=0.192). We also characterized the transitions of the fish from one landmark to the other one by analysing the succession of majority events. In particular, we counted the number of ‘Collective transitions’ (i.e. two majority events nearby different cylinders separated by a majority outside), the number of ‘One-by-one transitions’ (i.e. succession of a majority event in one cylinder and a majority event in the other cylinder) and finally the number of ‘Collective U-turns’ (i.e. two majority events in the same cylinder separated by a majority outside). It reveals that the main transitions occurring for AB zebrafish are the collective ones while some collective U-turns and almost no individual transitions were detected (figure 7b, red). Similarly, almost no individual transition was observed for the TL zebrafish that perform mainly collective transitions (figure 7b, green). TL zebrafish also performed numerous collective U-turns. The absence of individual transitions reveals that both strains are mostly swimming in groups but with different collective dynamics. We compared the results with Mann–Whitney U-tests: ‘One-by-one transitions’ (U=3.0, p<0.001) and ‘Collective U-turns’ (U=27.5, p<0.050) between AB and TL are significantly different while ‘Collective transitions’ between AB and TL are not (U=40, p=0.236).
To highlight the differential effects of the strains and the group sizes on space fidelity around cylinders, we measured the proportion of positions that were detected at 25 cm from the centre of the cylinders (figure 8). This measurement confirms that groups of 10 AB zebrafish were more present near the cylinders than groups of 5 AB. By contrast, while groups of 5 TL responded similarly to groups of 5 AB, groups of 10 TL zebrafish were less detected near the cylinders. A two-way ANOVA (group size, strain, n=10 for each experimental condition) indicated a non-significative effect of the group size (p=0.07, F=3.59, d.f.=1) but a significant effect of the strain (p<0.001, F=43.17, d.f.=1) and a significant strain/group size interaction (p<0.001, F=35.38, d.f.=1) on the attractivity of the cylinders. The interaction effect indicates here a strain-specific effect of the group size on the time spent near the landmarks: groups of 10 AB are more attracted by the cylinders than groups of 5 AB, but on the contrary, 10 TL are less detected near the cylinders than groups of 5 TL. To confirm this observation, we compared the proportions for all groups (5 or 10 zebrafish, AB or TL strain) with a one-way ANOVA. The test confirmed that the size of the group and the type of strain have a significant influence on the attractivity of the cylinders (p<0.001, F=27.38, d.f.=3). Finally, a Tukey’s honest significant difference criterion post hoc test shows that attractivity of the cylinders is always different except for the couple 5 AB versus 5 TL. This series of tests confirmed that groups of 10 AB are more attracted by the cylinders than groups of 5 AB and 5 TL which are more attracted by cylinders than groups of 10 TL.
Finally, we compared the probability to be near the landmarks (cylinders or discs) for 10 AB and 10 TL zebrafish with a two-way ANOVA (figure 9). It revealed that the type of landmarks (p<0.001, F=11.37, d.f.=1) and the strain of zebrafish affect the attraction (p<0.001, F=102.95, d.f.=1), while there is also evidence of an interaction effect between type of landmarks and strains (p=0.02, F=5.67, d.f.=1). To confirm this observation, we compared the proportions for all groups (10 zebrafish, AB or TL strain in the presence of cylinders or discs) with a one-way ANOVA. The test confirmed that the type of the strain and the type of the landmark have a significant influence on the attractivity of the cylinders (p<0.001, F=39.99, d.f.=3). Finally, a Tukey’s honest significant difference criterion post hoc test shows that all paired comparisons are significantly different except for the 10 AB discs and 10 AB cylinders. This series of tests confirmed that groups of 10 AB zebrafish are more attracted by the landmarks than groups of 10 TL which show more attraction for cylinder type than disc type.
3. Discussion
We investigated whether collective motion and collective choice can differ in groups of 5 and 10 individuals of the same species (zebrafish Danio rerio) but of different strains (AB versus TL) in the presence of two identical landmarks. One hour observation trials show that the zebrafish groups do not select one of the two landmarks. The fish were mainly swimming together and oscillating from one landmark to the other with short resting times. Thus, while all individuals can be present at the same landmark for a few seconds (electronic supplementary material, S7 and S8), the probability of the presence computed for the entire experimental time shows that the fish were equally present at both stimuli. Therefore, no collective choice emerged on the long duration for both strains of zebrafish and group sizes. Hence, long or short study durations can reveal opposite conclusions on collective motions. In other species, site fidelity has been studied for longer time period. It has been reported that Hemigrapsus sanguineus exhibited low site fidelity after 24 h: 17.8% and 2.3% of the released crabs were recovered within 0.1 m from the two studied sites [37]; for Helograpsus haswellianus, the authors have shown that the recapture rates of marked crabs were very low across their four week lunar study (5.8% and 5.3% on two different sites at a distance of 70 km), while recaptures often occurred in the same trap (31.3 and 25%) [38]. The authors suggest that this low rate of site fidelity could be due to foraging issues. For Genyonemus lineatus, researchers report evidence of site fidelity during the first 7 days of their study (over 240 days) with low and variable residency times [39]. For Cynoscion regalis, it has been found that there is a strong link between the birth location of the parents and their spawning location: from 60 to 81% [40].
Our methodology is complementary to typical Y-maze experiments. We extend and compare their conclusions to our observations with repeated interactions between the fish and their environment. During an hour, the collective behaviour of zebrafish contrasts with other collective species in which spatial fidelity emerges from the interactions between the individuals that take place in the resting sites (in cockroaches [41], in hymenoptera [12]). These oscillations from one site to the other one could originate from individual differences among group members: bold and shy behavioural profiles have been evidenced in zebrafish according to the intrinsic propensity of each fish to explore new environments [42,43]. It has been shown that zebrafish with high boldness lost their shoal cohesion when making decisions, unlike other species (Gasterosteus aculeatus) where boldness as attractor has been identified [44]. In this context, the presence of bolder fish in the group could favour the transition from one spot to the other one while groups composed only by shy individuals could show less frequent departures [45]. Boldness and shyness as modifiers of group behaviour occur also in mammalians, for example, where bold sheep tend to split big groups into smaller subgroups of equal size [46]. We did not identify significant differences between both strains for collective transitions but we revealed significant differences for the ‘one-by-one’ transitions and the collective U-turns. AB zebrafish show more ‘one-by-one’ transitions than TL and TL zebrafrish show more collective U-turns than AB. We could categorize the ‘one-by-one’ transitions as more dangerous because they occur when the group transit from a landmark to the other one, one fish after another. The fish are thus exposed in the tank and we could link these transitions with bold personalities. Following the same reasoning, collective U-turns can be assimilated as shy events because fish go back to their previous known location. Hence, there could be a difference of boldness between both strains that might be highlighted by a deeper analysis of the transitions.
A more detailed analysis shows quantitative differences among the two studied strains and group sizes. Concerning the response of the fish to the landmarks, we highlight that groups of 5 AB and TL zebrafish show the same attraction to the cylinders by computing the probability for the fish to be observed near these landmarks. This attraction increases for groups of 10 AB zebrafish but decreases for groups of 10 TL zebrafish. This strain difference is also observed in the experiments with floating discs. In addition, the type of the landmarks seems to be determinant for TL zebrafish as they prefer objects immersed in the water column to objects lying on the surface of the water. Hence, differences of collective behaviour between the two tested strains of zebrafish exist.
These different responses to heterogeneities may be based on the intrinsic preference of the fish of a particular strain for congeners or for landmarks. Such difference has already been shown in shoaling tendency between several strains of guppies [47] and zebrafish [48,49]. In their natural environment, fish have to balance the costs of risks and benefits of moving in groups or staying near landmarks [50]. Our results suggest that there are more collective transitions than collective U-turns and ‘one-by-one’ transitions. It means that zebrafish (whatever the strain) prefer to transit in a group and thus avoid group fission when crossing an open area. Moving in groups first of all prevents the fish from being static prey, and second allow spatial recognition and easier food and predator detection. The drawback is that the takeover of the fish on the territory is punctual and they have less chance to find areas where they can hide. Staying around landmarks gives the fish a feeling of control of the territory and the possibility to hide from predators. In that case the drawback is that the prey will rarely cross the territory of the fish.
Regarding the structure of the group, we notice that whatever the group size of TL zebrafish, the cohesion of the group does not change and is always stronger than those of groups of AB zebrafish. Also, the bigger the group of AB zebrafish, the stronger the cohesion. These differences of group cohesion may be based on differences of physical features between AB and TL zebrafish. Cohesion differences could be explained by phenotype differences between the two strains. Some studies demonstrated that a large variability exists in the individual motion and shoaling tendency of the zebrafish according to their age or strain. For example, adults AB and casper zebrafish swim longer distance than ABstrg, EK, TU or WIK zebrafish [51]. Likewise, the interindividual distance between shoal members decreases from 16 body length to 3.5 body length between day 7 and five months after fertilization [52]. Also, it has been demonstrated that the fin size has an impact on the swimming performance and the behaviour of the zebrafish [53]. AB and TL zebrafish show different fin lengths and different patterns on the skin: TL zebrafish are homozygous for leot1 and lofdt1, where leot1 is a recessive mutation causing spotting in adult zebrafish and lofdt1 is a dominant homozygous viable mutation causing long fins [54,55]. Thus, AB zebrafish have short fins and TL zebrafish show long fins. One may suggest that TL zebrafish move a higher quantity of water with their long fins when swimming and thus emit a stronger signal of presence (hydrodynamical signal). Thus, it may be easier for conspecifics in the moving shoal to perceive the signal through their lateral line and realign themselves according to their conspecifics. If the realignment becomes easier, it is simpler for TL zebrafish to keep their position in the shoal, which increases its cohesion. Following a similar hypothesis, the signal of presence is weaker for AB zebrafish due to their shorter fins. Thus, realignment in the moving shoal is less performant and their cohesion decreases. Hemmings [56] and Partridge & Pitcher [57] showed that fish use vision for attraction and the lateral line for repulsion. Recently, it has been revealed that the visual field (binocular and lateral) modify the group cohesion [58]. The zebrafish strain showing the bigger visual field should be less cohesive. However, none of the previous mutations (leo and lof) could explain these differences of visual fields in both strains of zebrafish. The lateral line could also explain the differences of cohesion between AB and TL zebrafish: the mutation of the leo gene (TL zebrafish) may have an effect on it, lowering its performance. Hence, unlike the AB zebrafish, the repulsion may occur when TL zebrafish are closer to each other, forcing them to be more cohesive. Each of these hypotheses could explain the collective behaviours observed during the experiments and nothing prevents merging all of them.
Finally, we did not quantify aggressive behaviour during the experiments. There are several studies trying to find a link between aggression and shoal size. Morgan [59] shows that aggressive interactions of Pimephales notatus decrease significantly as shoal size increases, while Rehnberg & Smith [60] show that there is no clear evidence between levels of aggression and shoal size with Danio rerio. The issue of aggressive behaviour could be left open for another study.
In conclusion, this study demonstrates that behavioural differences exist at the individual and collective levels in the same species of animal. The analysis of the dynamics reveals that AB and TL zebrafish mainly oscillate in groups between landmarks. In addition, increasing the size of the group leads to opposite results for the two strains: groups of 10 AB zebrafish are proportionally more detected near the landmarks than groups of 5 AB while groups of 10 TL zebrafish are less attracted by the landmarks than groups of 5 TL. Finally, the two tested zebrafish strains show differences at the structural level: (1) groups of TL zebrafish are more cohesive than groups of AB zebrafish and (2) AB zebrafish collective responses to landmarks show that they are generally more present near the cylinders and floating discs than TL zebrafish. Thus, this study provides evidence that zebrafish do not select resting site on the mid-term and highlights behavioural differences at the individual and collective levels among the two tested strains of zebrafish. Future studies of collective behaviour should consider the tested strains, the intra-strain composition of the shoals and the duration of each trial.
4. Material and methods
4.1. Fish and housing
We acquired 500 adult common laboratory wild-type zebrafish (200 AB strain and 300 TL strain) from Institut Curie (Paris) and raised them under the same conditions in tanks of 60 l by groups of 50. The zebrafish AB line show a zebra skin, short tail and fin. The zebrafish TL line show a spotted skin, long tail and fin and barbel. Both strains are 3.5 cm long. The zebrafish used for the experiments are adult fish between 5 and 18 months of age. During this period, zebrafish show a shoaling tendency allowing study of their collective behaviours. We kept fish under laboratory condition, 27°C, 500 μS salinity with a 9 D : 15 L cycle. The fish were fed two times per day (Special Diets Services SDS-400 Scientific Fish Food). Water pH is maintained at 7.5 and nitrites (NO2−) are below 0.3 mg l−1. We measured the size of the caudal fins of 10 AB (about 0.4 cm) and 10 TL (about 1.1 cm) zebrafish.
4.2. Experimental set-up
The experimental tank consists of a 1.2×1.2 m tank confined in a 2×2×2.35 m experimental area surrounded by a white sheet, in order to isolate the experiments and homogenize luminosity. The wall of the experimental tank was covered with white tape and the water column is 6 cm. Water pH is maintained at 7.5 and nitrites (NO2−) are below 0.3 mg l−1. The experiments with discs were performed in the experimental tank, while those with cylinders were performed in a white square arena (1×1×0.15 m) placed in the experimental tank. Groups of zebrafish were randomly formed at the beginning of the experiments.
The experiments were recorded by a high-resolution camera (2048×2048 px, Basler Scout acA2040-25gm) placed above the experimental tank and recording at 15 fps (frame per second). Luminosity is ensured by 4 fluorescents lamps of 80 W placed on each side of the tank, on the floor and directed towards the walls to provide indirect lighting.
To trigger interest of fish, we placed symmetrically in the set-up either two floating discs (ϕ=20 cm) or two cylinders (ϕ=10 cm, height=15 cm) surrounded by yellow- and green-striped tape [61–63]. To avoid the presence of a blind zone, the cylinders were slightly tilted towards the centre of the tank. The centre of both discs and cylinders are at from two opposite corners along the diagonal of the tank (figure 10).
4.3. Experimental procedure
We recorded the behaviour of zebrafish swimming in the experimental tank for 1 h. Before the trials, the attractive landmarks are put in the set-up and fish are placed with a hand net in a cylindrical arena (20 cm diameter) made of Plexiglas placed in the centre of our tank. Following a 5 min acclimatization period, this arena is removed and the fish are able to freely swim in the experimental arena. We performed 10 trials for each strain with the floating discs and 10 trials for each combination of parameters (number of fish×strain) with the cylinders for a total of 60 experiments. Each fish was never tested twice in the same experimental condition.
4.4. Tracking and data analysis
The methodology based on massive data gathering [64] has now become standard in studies on animal collective behaviour with flies, Drosophila melanogaster [65,66], birds, Sturnus vulgaris [67–69] and fish, Notemigonus crysoleucas [70]. The experiments with cylinders were recorded at 15 fps and tracked online by a custom-made tracking system based on blob detection. We call a batch a group of 10 experiments. For these batchs, each experiment consists of 540 000 positions (10 zebrafish×54 000 frames) and 270 000 positions (5 zebrafish×54 000 frames). For experiments with discs, we faced tracking troubles. Since the fish below the floating discs were difficult to distinguish by the program due to a lack of sufficient contrast, experiments with floating disc were tracked offline by two custom Matlab scripts. A first script automatically identifies the positions of the fish swimming outside of the floating discs by blob detection. Since this method did not allow a perfect detection of all the individuals, we developed a second script that was run after the first one and that plotted the frame where a fish (or more) was undetected for the user to manually identify the missing individual(s). It allowed us to identify the fish that were partially hidden during a collision/superposition with another fish or the fish that were situated under the floating discs. Since this analysis tool is time-costly, we only analysed 1 fps for all experiments with discs. For these batchs, each experiment consists of 36 000 positions (10 zebrafish×3600 positions).
Since our tracking system did not solve collision with accuracy, we did not calculate individual measurements but characterized the aggregation level of the group. The probability of the presence of the fish was calculated by the cumulated positions of all individuals along the entire experiment. We also calculated the distance between each individual fish and the attractive landmarks (and averaged it) as well as the interindividual distances between the fish and the average interindividual distance. Finally, we computed the time of shelter occupancy as the time that is spent by the fish at less than 25 cm from the attractive landmarks. These time sequences were calculated according to the number of fish present near the landmarks. All scripts were coded in Python using scientific and statistic libraries (numpy, pylab, scilab and matplotlib).
To compute the number of majority events, the number of fish was averaged over the 15 frames of every second. This operation guarantees that a majority event is ended by the departure of a fish and not by an error of detection during one frame by the tracking system. Electronic supplementary material, figures S9 and S10 show the proportions of the durations of the majority events before and after this interpolation.
4.5. Statistics
For figures 8 and 9, ten measurements of means of the probability for different groups of zebrafish to be near the landmarks are plotted. They have been tested using a two-way ANOVA. We then compared the data between each group using a one-way ANOVA and finally used a Tukey’s honest significant difference criterion post hoc test. We did these tests on Matlab and chose 0.001 as significance level. In the electronic supplementary material, table S1, we show the number of majority events after interpolation of the data at 1 fps. This table is related to figure 7. We used Mann–Whitney U tests to compare the number of events between strains, areas and transition types. These tests are performed on 10 values of majority events for each strain, area and transition type. These tests were made with the Python package scipy. We chose 0.001 (***), 0.01 (**) and 0.05 (*) as significance levels. For figure 1 as well as figures 2 and 3, we compared the distribution with Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests. These tests were made with the Python package scipy. We chose 0.001 as significance level.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Filippo Del Bene (Institut Curie, Paris, France), who provided the fish observed in the experiments reported in this paper.
Ethics
The experiments performed in this study were conducted under the authorization of the Buffon Ethical Committee (registered to the French National Ethical Committee for Animal Experiments No. 40) after submission to the state ethical board for animal experiments.
Data accessibility
Supporting data will be available from the Dryad Digital Repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.01j61 [71].
Authors' contributions
A.S. carried out the laboratory work, the data analysis and the design of the set-up and drafted the manuscript; B.C. carried out the statistical analyses and participated in the data analyses and in the writing of the manuscript; J.H. conceived of the study, designed the study, coordinated the study and helped draft the manuscript. All authors gave final approval for publication.
Competing interests
We have no competing interests.
Funding
This work was supported by European Union Information and Communication Technologies project ASSISIbf, FP7-ICT-FET-601074.
Disclaimer
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Sumpter DJT, Zabzina N, Nicolis SC. 2012. Six predictions about the decision making of animal and human groups. Manag. Decis. Econ. 33, 295–309. (doi:10.1002/mde.2553) [Google Scholar]
- 2.Detrain C, Deneubourg JL. 2008. Collective decision-making of foraging patterns in ants and honeybees. Adv. Insect Physiol. 35, 123–173. (doi:10.1016/S0065-2806(08)00002-7) [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dussutour A, Deneubourg J-L, Beshers S, Fourcassié V. 2009. Individual and collective problem-solving in a foraging context in the leaf-cutting ant Atta colombica. Anim. Cogn. 12, 21–30. (doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0165-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jeanson R, Dussutour A, Fourcassié V. 2010. Key factors for the emergence of collective decision in invertebrates. Front. Decis. Neurosci. 6, 121 (doi:10.3389/fnins.2012.00121) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Petit O, Bon R. 2010. Decision-making processes: the case of collective movements. Behav. Process. 84, 635–647. (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.04.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Sueur C, Deneubourg JL, Petit O. 2011. From the first intention movement to the last joiner: macaques combine mimetic rules to optimize their collective decisions. Proc. R. Soc. B 278, 1697–1704. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.2084) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Radakov DV. 1973. Schooling in the ecology of fish. New York, NY: John Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Parrish JK, Viscido SV, Grünbaum D. 2002. Self-organized fish schools: an examination of emergent properties. Biol. Bull. 202, 296–305. (doi:10.2307/1543482) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Engeszer RE, Da Barbiano LA, Ryan MJ, Parichy DM. 2007. Timing and plasticity of shoaling behaviour in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Anim. Behav. 74, 1269–1275. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.032) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Herbert-Read JE, Perna A, Mann RP, Schaerf TM, Sumpter DJT, Ward AJW. 2010. Inferring the rules of interaction of shoaling fish. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 18 726–18 731. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1109355108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hemelrijk CK, Hildenbrandt H. 2012. Schools of fish and flocks of birds: their shape and internal structure by self-organization. Interface Focus 2, 726–737. (doi:10.1098/rsfs.2012.0025) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Franks NR, Pratt SC, Mallon EB, Britton NF, Sumpter DJT. 2002. Information flow, opinion polling and collective intelligence in house-hunting social insects. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 357, 1567–1583. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1066) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Amé J-M, Halloy J, Rivault C, Detrain C, Deneubourg J-L. 2006. Collegial decision making based on social amplification leads to optimal group formation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 5835–5840. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0507877103) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Conradt L, Roper TJ. 2005. Consensus decision making in animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 449–456. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2005.05.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ward AJW, Herbert-Read JE, Sumpter DJT, Krause J. 2011. Fast and accurate decisions through collective vigilance in fish shoals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 2312–2315. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1007102108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sison M, Gerlai R. 2010. Associative learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio) in the plus maze. Behav. Brain Res. 207, 99–104. (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.043) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Miller N, Garnier S, Hartnett AT, Couzin ID. 2013. Both information and social cohesion determine collective decisions in animal groups. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 5263–5268. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1217513110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kistler C, Hegglin D, Würbel H, König B. 2011. Preference for structured environment in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and checker barbs (Puntius oligolepis). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 135, 318–327. (doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.014) [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sullivan M, Lawrence C, Blache D. 2015. Why did the fish cross the tank? Objectively measuring the value of enrichment for captive fish. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 174, 181–188. (doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2015.10.011) [Google Scholar]
- 20.Suriyampola PS, Eason PK. 2014. A field study investigating effects of landmarks on territory size and shape. Biol. Lett. 10, 20140009 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2014.0009) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Suriyampola PS, Eason PK. 2015. The effects of landmarks on territorial behavior in a convict cichlid, Amatitlania siquia. Ethology 121, 785–792. (doi:10.1111/eth.12393) [Google Scholar]
- 22.Becco Ch, Vandewalle N, Delcourt J, Poncin P. 2006. Experimental evidences of a structural and dynamical transition in fish school. Physica A 367, 487–493. (doi:10.1016/j.physa.2005.11.041) [Google Scholar]
- 23.Herbert-Read JE, Krause S, Morrell LJ, Schaerf TM, Krause J, Ward AJW. 2012. The role of individuality in collective group movement. Proc. R. Soc. B 280, 20122564 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.2564) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tunstrom K, Katz Y, Ioannou CC, Huepe C, Lutz MJ, Couzin ID. 2013. Collective states, multistability and transitional behavior in schooling fish. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1002915 (doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002915) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Norton W, Bally-Cuif L. 2010. Adult zebrafish as a model organism for behavioural genetics. BMC Neurosci. 11, 90 (doi:10.1186/1471-2202-11-90) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Oliveira RF. 2013. Mind the fish: zebrafish as a model in cognitive social neuroscience. Front. Neural Circuits 7, 131 (doi:10.3389/fncir.2013.00131) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Spence R, Gerlach G, Lawrence C, Smith C. 2008. The behaviour and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol. Rev. Camb. Phil. Soc. 83, 13–34. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00030.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.McClure MM, McIntyre PB, McCune AR. 2006. Notes on the natural diet and habitat of eight danionin fishes, including the zebrafish (Danio rerio). J. Fish Biol. 69, 553–570. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2006.01125.x) [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pritchard VL, Lawrence J, Butlin RK, Krause J. 2001. Shoal choice in zebrafish, Danio rerio: the influence of shoal size and activity. Anim. Behav. 62, 1085–1088. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1858) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Parichy DM. 2015. Advancing biology through a deeper understanding of zebrafish ecology and evolution. eLife 4, e05635 (doi:10.7554/eLife.05635) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Suriyampola PS, Shelton DS, Shukla R, Roy T, Bhat A, Martins EP. 2016. Zebrafish social behavior in the wild. Zebrafish 13, 1–8. (doi:10.1089/zeb.2015.1159) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Arunachalam M, Raja M, Vijayakumar C, Malaiammal P, Mayden RL. 2013. Natural history of zebrafish (Danio rerio) in India. Zebrafish 10, 1–14. (doi:10.1089/zeb.2012.0803) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Risner ML, Lemerise E, Vukmanic EV, Moore A. 2006. Behavioral spectral sensitivity of the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Vis. Res. 46, 2625–2635. (doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.12.014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Beckers R, Deneubourg JL, Goss S, Pasteels JM. 1990. Collective decision making through food recruitment. Insectes Soc. 37, 258–267. (doi:10.1007/BF02224053) [Google Scholar]
- 35.Beckers R, Deneubourg JL, Goss S. 1992. Trails and U-turns in the selection of a path by the ant Lasius niger. J. Theoret. Biol. 159, 397–415. (doi:10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80686-1) [Google Scholar]
- 36.Collignon B, Séguret A, Halloy J. 2016. A stochastic vision-based model inspired by zebrafish collective behaviour in heterogeneous environments. R. Soc. open sci. 3, 150473 (doi:10.1098/rsos.150473) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Brousseau DJ, Baglivo JA, Filipowicz A, Sego L, Alt C. 2002. An experimental field study of site fidelity and mobility in the Asian Shore Crab, Hemigrapsus sanguineus. Northeastern Nat. 9, 381–390. (doi:10.1656/1092-6194(2002)009[0381:AEFSOS]2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 38.Katrak G, Dittmann S. 2011. Site specific distribution of the mud shore crab Helograpsus haswellianus in temperate wetlands. Wetlands Ecol. Manage. 19, 433–448. (doi:10.1007/s11273-011-9227-1) [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wolfe BW, Lowe CG. 2015. Movement patterns, habitat use and site fidelity of the white croaker (Genyonemus lineatus) in the Palos Verdes Superfund Site, Los Angeles, California. Mar. Environ. Res. 109, 69–80. (doi:10.1016/j.marenvres.2015.06.002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Thorrold SR, Latkoczy C, Swart PK, Jones CM. 2001. Natal homing in a marine fish metapopulation. Science 291, 297–299. (doi:10.1126/science.291.5502.297) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Amé JM, Rivault C, Deneubourg JL. 2004. Cockroach aggregation based on strain odour recognition. Anim. Behav. 68, 793–801. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.01.009) [Google Scholar]
- 42.Dahlbom SJ, Lagman D, Lundstedt-Enkel K, Sundström LF, Winberg S. 2011. Boldness predicts social status in zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS ONE 6, e23565 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023565) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Guayasamin O, Couzin I. 2013. Personality matters: a quantitative comparison of asocial and social behaviors in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Princeton University Senior Theses.
- 44.Harcourt JL, Sweetman G, Johnstone RA, Manica A. 2009. Personality counts: the effect of boldness on shoal choice in three-spined sticklebacks. Anim. Behav. 77, 1501–1505. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.03.004) [Google Scholar]
- 45.Leblond C, Reebs SG. 2006. Individual leadership and boldness in shoals of golden shiners (Notemigonus crysoleucas). Behaviour 143, 1263–1280. (doi:10.1163/156853906778691603) [Google Scholar]
- 46.Michelena P, Sibbald AM, Erhard HW, McLeod JE. 2008. Effects of group size and personality on social foraging: the distribution of sheep across patches. Behav. Ecol. 20, 145–152. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arn126) [Google Scholar]
- 47.Magurran AE, Seghers BH, Shaw PW, Carvalho GR. 1995. The behavioral diversity and evolution of guppy, Poecilia reticulata, populations in Trinidad. In Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 24 (eds PJB Slater, JS Rosenblatt, CT Snowdon, M Milinski), pp. 155–202. London, UK: Academic Press.
- 48.Wright D, Rimmer LB, Pritchard VL, Krause J, Butlin RK. 2003. Inter- and intra-population variation in shoaling and boldness in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Naturwiss 90, 374–377. (doi:10.1007/s00114-003-0443-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wright D, Ward AJW, Croft DP, Krause J. 2006. Social organization, grouping, and domestication in fish. Zebrafish 3, 141–155. (doi:10.1089/zeb.2006.3.141) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Millot S, Bégout M-L, Chatain B. 2009. Risk-taking behaviour variation over time in sea bass Dicentrarchus labrax: effects of day–night alternation, fish phenotypic characteristics and selection for growth. J. Fish Biol. 75, 1733–1749. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.2009.02425.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Lange M, Neuzeret F, Fabreges B, Froc C, Bedu S, Bally-Cuif L, Norton WHJ. 2013. Inter-individual and inter-strain variations in zebrafish locomotor ontogeny. PLoS ONE 8, e70172 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0070172) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Buske C, Gerlai R. 2011. Shoaling develops with age in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Progress Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1409–1415. (doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.09.003) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Plaut I. 2000. Effects of fin size on swimming performance, swimming behaviour and routine activity of zebrafish Danio rerio. J. Exp. Biol. 203, 813–820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Iovine MK, Johnson SL. 2000. Genetic analysis of isometric growth control mechanisms in the zebrafish caudal fin. Genetics 155, 1321–1329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Watanabe M, Iwashita M, Ishii M, Kurachi Y, Kawakami A, Kondo S, Okada N. 2006. Spot pattern of leopard Danio is caused by mutation in the zebrafish connexin41.8 gene. EMBO Rep. 7, 893–897. (doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400757) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hemmings CC. 1966. Olfaction and vision in fish schooling. J. Exp. Biol. 45, 449–464. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Partridge BL, Pitcher TJ. 1980. The sensory basis of fish schools: relative roles of lateral line and vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A 135, 315–325. (doi:10.1007/BF00657647) [Google Scholar]
- 58.Pita D, Moore BA, Tyrrell LP, Fernández-Juricic E. 2006. Vision in two cyprinid fish: implications for collective behavior. EMBO Rep. 7, 893–897. (doi:10.1038/sj.embor.7400757) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Morgan MJ. 1988. The effect of hunger, shoal size and the presence of a predator on shoal cohesiveness in bluntnose minnows, Pimephales notatus Rafinesque. J. Fish Biol. 32, 963–971. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1988.tb05439.x) [Google Scholar]
- 60.Rehnberg BG, Smith RJF. 1988. The influence of alarm substance and shoal size on the behaviour of zebra danios, Brachydanio rerio (Cyprinidae). J. Fish Biol. 33, 155–163. (doi:10.1111/j.1095-8649.1988.tb05457.x) [Google Scholar]
- 61.Gerlai R. 2014. Social behavior of zebrafish: from synthetic images to biological mechanisms of shoaling. J. Neurosci. Methods 234, 59–65. (doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.04.028) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Lau BYB, Mathur P, Gould GG, Guo S. 2011. Identification of a brain center whose activity discriminates a choice behavior in zebrafish. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 2581–2586. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1018275108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Saverino C, Gerlai R. 2008. The social zebrafish: behavioural responses to conspecific, heterospecific and computer animated fish. Behav. Brain Res. 191, 77–87. (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.013) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Reiser M. 2002. The ethomics era? Nat. Methods 6, 413–414. (doi:10.1038/nmeth0609-413) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Branson K, Robie AA, Bender B, Perona P, Dickinson HM. 2009. High-throughput ethomics in large groups of drosophila. Nature 6, 451–457. (doi:10.1038/nmeth.1328) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Dankert H, Wang L, Hoopfer ED, Anderson DJ, Perona P. 2009. Automated monitoring and analysis of social behavior in drosophila. Nat. Methods 6, 297–303. (doi:10.1038/nmeth.1310) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Ballerini M et al. 2007. Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: evidence from a field study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 1232–1237. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0711437105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Miller N, Gerlai R. 2007. Quantification of shoaling behaviour in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 184, 157–166. (doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Miller N, Gerlai R. 2011. Shoaling in zebrafish: what we don’t know. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 17–25. (doi:10.1515/rns.2011.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Strandburg-Peshkin A et al. 2013. Visual sensory networks and effective information transfer in animal groups. Curr. Biol. 23, R709 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.059) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Séguret A, Collignon B, Halloy J. 2016. Data from: Strain differences in the collective behaviour of zebrafish (Danio rerio) in heterogeneous environment. Dryad Digital Repository. (doi:10.5061/dryad.01j61) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Supporting data will be available from the Dryad Digital Repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.01j61 [71].