Abstract
According to dual-system theory, instrumental learning and performance depend on the balance between goal-directed and habitual action control. Overreliance on habits has been argued to characterize clinical conditions such as drug addiction or obsessive-compulsive disorder as well as obesity and excessive impulsivity. A tendency toward habitual action control in obesity has already been indicated in the food domain. However, impairments might not be restricted to eating behavior. This has been suggested by domain-general obesity-associated disturbances in executive function as well as alterations in corticostriatal circuits underlying the goal-directed and habitual systems. In this study we examined the balance of goal-directed and habitual action control in a sample of normal-weight, overweight, and obese participants (n = 105) using the slips-of-action test in a non-food context. We tested for continuous or group-based associations between body weight status (BMI) and the devaluation sensitivity index (DSI), a parameter representing the balance of the goal-directed and habitual systems in action control. As personality differences in the domain of impulsivity might affect this relationship, we also examined whether the interaction between BMI and self-reported impulsivity, based on the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale, was related to the DSI. In addition to that, we tested for direct, i.e., weight status independent, relationships between UPPS subdomains of impulsivity and the DSI. We failed to find evidence for a relationship between weight status and sensitivity to devaluation as indexed by the DSI. However, independent of weight status, we observed lower sensitivity to devaluation in sensation seekers, a subtype of impulsivity. To conclude, behavioral flexibility in the sense of disturbances in the balance between the habitual and goal-directed systems seems to be unaffected by weight status in a non-food context. Consequently, stimuli and behavior might not be generally excessively linked in overweight or obesity. However, according to ceiling effects we cannot rule out subtle effects the paradigm was not able to disentangle. Further, future studies are needed to clarify the role of specific subtypes of obesity (e.g., food addiction). The indicated habit propensity in sensation seekers may account for previous reports of weak avoidance behavior and risky decision making.
Keywords: obesity, BMI, impulsivity, sensation seeking, habitual, goal-directed, instrumental discrimination
Introduction
According to dual-system accounts, the flexibility of instrumental action is in part determined by the balance between goal-directed and habitual action control systems (for associative accounts, see e.g., Dickinson, 1985; de Wit and Dickinson, 2009). The goal-directed system encodes associations between actions [responses (R)] and their consequences [outcomes (O)] which are indicated by environmental cues [stimuli (S)]. Habitual action, conversely, is driven by S→R associations only, without consideration of outcome values (de Wit and Dickinson, 2009). The habit system is thought to dominate with excessive training, as it is more efficient than the goal-directed system in a stable environment, though at the expense of flexibility (Dickinson, 1985; de Wit and Dickinson, 2009). Overreliance on habits supposedly characterize clinical conditions that involve impulsive and compulsive behavior, as indicated by instrumental decision-making tasks (i.e., selective outcome devaluation procedures or model-free/model-based reinforcement learning paradigms) that assess the ability to rapidly change behavior to changes in outcome value. Such conditions include obsessive-compulsive disorder (Gillan et al., 2011, 2014), drug addiction (e.g., Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Everitt et al., 2008; Redish et al., 2008; Sjoerds et al., 2013; Voon et al., 2015), and obesity (Volkow and Wise, 2005; Horstmann et al., 2015a). Further, predominance of habitual control has been argued to contribute to individual differences in compulsive tendencies (Snorrason et al., 2016) and impulsivity (Everitt et al., 2008; Hogarth et al., 2012) in non-clinical samples, indicated by outcome devaluation tasks.
Maladaptive habitual behavior is a frequently discussed issue in obesity research (Volkow and Wise, 2005; Corbit, 2016). In a recent study we showed that body weight status was positively related to heightened habit-like behavior in the food domain (Horstmann et al., 2015a). However, behavior might be generally less flexible and goal-directed in obesity. This is indicated by lower performance in tasks assessing domain-general executive function in healthy young participants (reviewed in Vainik et al., 2013), especially inhibitory control (Nederkoorn et al., 2006; Gunstad et al., 2007) and decision making (Pignatti et al., 2006; Weller et al., 2008; Horstmann et al., 2011; Simmank et al., 2015). For instance, there is evidence for obesity-associated impulsive decision making using the delay-discounting task. In this task participants with obesity showed preference of small immediately available monetary rewards over larger delayed ones (Weller et al., 2008; Simmank et al., 2015). Additionally, self-reported impulsivity has been shown to be increased in obesity (e.g., Rydén et al., 2003; Braet et al., 2007; Guerrieri et al., 2008; Mobbs et al., 2010). Impulsivity is a complex trait, including the inability to withhold responses in the face of negative consequences, the preference for small immediate rewards vs. larger delayed ones, acting without forethought, as well as a tendency for sensation seeking and increased disposition to engage in risky behaviors (reviewed in Bari and Robbins, 2013). It is a relevant trait which needs consideration in the investigation of instrumental action control, and may moderate the potential relationship between weight status and the domain-general balance between goal-directed and habitual control. Impulsivity has been proposed to be accompanied by an overreliance on habits at the expense of goal-directed behavioral control (Everitt et al., 2008; Hogarth et al., 2012). Hogarth et al. (2012) assessed individual differences between self-reported impulsivity and goal-directed behavioral control using a food-dependent devaluation procedure. After instrumental training of response-outcome contingencies a chocolate snack was devalued, induced by specific satiety. Testing responding for the devalued outcome (chocolate snack) in extinction revealed impaired goal-directed behavioral control with respect to the aspect of motor impulsivity (i.e., action without thought).
Although previous research suggest behavioral inflexibility already in healthy obese individuals, abnormalities in habit formation might be especially pronounced in certain subtypes. For example, similar to drug addiction the obesity- and impulsivity-associated psychopathology of food addiction (Gearhardt et al., 2009; Meule and Gearhardt, 2014) may be related to impaired goal-directed action control in favor of habitual behavior. Apart from that, behavioral autonomy may also differ between mild and morbid obesity. According to a recently proposed model for dopamine and the severity of obesity (Horstmann et al., 2015b), individuals with overweight and mild obesity may be characterized by a low dopaminergic tone and enhanced behavioral flexibility, whereas morbid obesity might be characterized by a diminished dopaminergic tone and behavioral rigidity.
Both impulsivity and obesity are associated with abnormalities in corticostriatal and corticolimbic pathways [(impulsivity: e.g., Jentsch and Taylor, 1999; Volkow and Fowler, 2000; Cardinal et al., 2001; McClure et al., 2004; Brown et al., 2006; Dalley et al., 2008; Forbes et al., 2009; Davis et al., 2013), (obesity: e.g., Stoeckel et al., 2009; Berridge et al., 2010; Horstmann et al., 2011; Volkow et al., 2011; Dimitropoulos et al., 2012; Nummenmaa et al., 2012; Garcia-Garcia et al., 2013; Kullmann et al., 2014)] underlying goal-directed and habitual behavior (Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010). More specifically, goal-directed control has been associated with function and structure of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate cortices as well as caudate nucleus (Hampton and O’doherty, 2007; Valentin et al., 2007; Tanaka et al., 2008; de Wit et al., 2009, 2012b; Gläscher et al., 2009; Tricomi et al., 2009). The posterior putamen, on the other hand, seems to be involved in habit learning (Tricomi et al., 2009; de Wit et al., 2012b). Links between dysregulated neurotransmitter systems, involved in modulating the balance of goal-directed vs. habitual action control (de Wit et al., 2012a; Worbe et al., 2015), and impulsivity (Dolan et al., 2002; King et al., 2003; Dalley et al., 2008; Forbes et al., 2009; Buckholtz et al., 2010) or obesity (Wang et al., 2001; de Weijer et al., 2011; Eisenstein et al., 2013; Guo et al., 2014; Horstmann et al., 2015b) support the possibility of a general impairment in goal-directed behavior.
We hypothesized body weight status [as measured by the body mass index (BMI)], food addiction as well as impulsivity to be characterized by a disruption in the balance between goal-directed and habitual action control, resulting in overreliance on habits. Further, we expected impulsivity to moderate the relationship between BMI and measures of goal-directed and habitual behavioral control. To investigate these hypotheses, a sample of young and healthy normal-weight, overweight and obese volunteers were assessed on different aspects of self-reported impulsivity (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001) and food addiction (Gearhardt et al., 2009; Meule and Gearhardt, 2014). In addition to these critical analyses we explored the influence of gender on the balance between the goal-directed and the habitual system. This was suggested by previous research indicating gender differences in the sensitivity to reinforcement. Gonadal hormones and their effects on the dopaminergic system supposedly account for these differences (Sofuoglu et al., 1999; Kaasinen et al., 2001; Evans et al., 2002; Lynch et al., 2002; Carroll et al., 2004). Importantly, a previous study using the same instrumental paradigm as applied here, showed an effect of dopamine depletion on instrumental decision-making in women only (de Wit et al., 2012a).
Individual differences in action control were measured using an established instrumental learning task (de Wit et al., 2007, 2012b; Gillan et al., 2011; Worbe et al., 2015; Delorme et al., 2016). In this task, participants were trained to perform specific responses which were indicated by certain associated stimuli, to obtain rewarding outcomes. In the critical ‘slips-of-action’ test phase, some of the outcomes were devalued, allowing evaluation of participants’ ability to adapt responding based on the current goal or outcome value, as opposed to relying on inflexible stimulus-response habits. We restricted the experimental procedure to the standard discrimination type of the original ‘slips-of-action’ test phase, as this discrimination type has been shown to be sufficiently sensitive to evaluate the balance between habitual and goal-directed action control in clinical and pre-clinical pathological conditions (Gillan et al., 2011; Delorme et al., 2016; Snorrason et al., 2016) and after pharmacological manipulations affecting dopamine and serotonin neurotransmission (de Wit et al., 2012a; Delorme et al., 2016).
Materials and Methods
Participants
We investigated 105 (60 females) healthy normal-weight (BMI >= 19 < 25), overweight (BMI >= 25 < 30) and obese volunteers (BMI > 30) recruited from the subject database of the Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences in Leipzig. Volunteers were healthy non-smokers without indication for major depression (Beck’s Depression Inventory, cut-off value 18, Beck et al., 1961). Prior to participation in the study participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the requirements of the local ethics committee of the University of Leipzig. Please see Table 1 (grouping by weight status) and Table 2 (grouping by gender) for details on the samples’ descriptive statistics (Supplementary Table S1 of the Supplementary Material shows correlations between the investigated variables).
Table 1.
Normal-weight (n = 36) | Overweight (n = 35) | Obese (n = 34) | Kruskal–Wallis H | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
BMI (kg/m2) | 20.26 / 21.72 / 23.35 | 21.18 / 26.77 / 27.78 | 31.18 / 33.13 / 36.43 | 92.440 | <0.001 |
YFAS symptoms | 0.0 / 1.0 / 1.0 | 0.0 / 1.0 / 2.0 | 1.0 / 2.0 / 3.0 | 15.727 | <0.001 |
Age | 24.3 / 27.0 / 29.8 | 24.0 / 25.0 / 28.0 | 25.0 / 27.0 / 29.3 | 2.011 | 0.366 |
UPPS Urgency | 23.25 / 26.0 / 30.0 | 22.0 / 27.0 / 32.0 | 23.5 / 27.0 / 31.5 | 0.165 | 0.921 |
UPPS (Lack of) Premeditation | 19.75 / 22.0 / 24.0 | 18.0 / 23.0 / 25.0 | 21.0 / 23.0 / 24.0 | 0.015 | 0.992 |
UPPS (Lack of) Perseverance | 15.25 / 19.0 / 22.75 | 16.0 / 21.0 / 25.0 | 16.0 / 19.5 / 23.75 | 1.584 | 0.453 |
UPPS Sensation Seeking | 25.25 / 32.0 / 37.75 | 28.0 / 33.0 / 35.0 | 25.5 / 31.5 / 36.25 | 0.121 | 0.942 |
IQ | 111.75 / 125.25 / 130.0 | 114.0 / 123.5 / 127.0 | 110.25 / 119.0 / 130.0 | 0.698 | 0.705 |
VPA score | 10.0 / 12.5 / 15.0 | 11.0 / 13.0 / 14.0 | 9.75 / 13.0 / 15.0 | 0.130 | 0.937 |
BDI | 1.25 / 3.0 / 6.0 | 3.0 / 5.0 / 9.25 | 1.0 / 3.5 / 7.25 | 4.312 | 0.116 |
n females / males | Chi2 | p | |||
Gender | 22 / 14 | 19 / 16 | 19 / 15 | χ2: 2.143 | 0.143 |
Including body mass index (BMI), number of symptoms on the Yale Food Addiction Scale (Gearhardt et al., 2009; Meule et al., 2012), age, impulsivity scores based on the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001), IQ (Wiener Matritzen-Test, Formann, 1979), visual short-term memory based on the Visual Paired Associates Test (VPA) of the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1987, 2006), depression symptoms (Beck’s Depression Inventory, Beck et al., 1961), and gender. Indicated are 1st Quartile, Median (bold), and 3rd Quartile as well as group differences based on the Kruskal–Wallis test or χ2-Test (gender).
Table 2.
Females (n = 60) | Males (n = 45) | Mann–Whitney U | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
BMI (kg/m2) | 22.07 / 26.55 / 32.10 | 23.40 / 27.47 / 31.13 | 1265.5 | 0.584 |
YFAS symptoms | 1.0 / 1.0 / 2.0 | 0.0 / 0.0 / 1.0 | 548.0 | <0.001 |
Age | 23.0 / 25.0 / 28.0 | 25.0 / 27.0 / 30.0 | 951.0 | 0.009 |
UPPS Urgency | 23.0 / 28.0 / 33.0 | 22.25 / 26.0 / 29.0 | 1068.5 | 0.097 |
UPPS (Lack of) Premeditation | 18.25 / 22.5 / 24.0 | 21.0 / 23.0 / 24.5 | 1193.0 | 0.307 |
UPPS (Lack of) Perseverance | 16.0 / 20.0 / 22.75 | 16.5 / 20.0 / 24.0 | 1252.0 | 0.525 |
UPPS Sensation Seeking | 24.25 / 30.50 / 35.0 | 30.0 / 34.0 / 37.5 | 973.5 | 0.015 |
IQ | 111.75 /123.5 / 130.0 | 112.5 /123.5 /130.0 | 1248.0 | 0.507 |
VPA score | 11.0 / 13.0 / 15.0 | 10.0 / 12.0 / 15.0 | 1136.5 | 0.165 |
BDI score | 2.0 / 4.0 / 7.0 | 1.0 / 4.0 / 8.0 | 1284.5 | 0.814 |
Including BMI, number of symptoms on the Yale Food Addiction Scale (Gearhardt et al., 2009; Meule et al., 2012), age, impulsivity scores based on the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001), IQ (Wiener Matritzen-Test, Formann, 1979), visual short-term memory based on the VPA of the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1987, 2006), and depression symptoms (Beck’s Depression Inventory, Beck et al., 1961). Indicated are 1st Quartile, Median (bold), and 3rd Quartile as well as gender differences based on the Mann-Whitney U test.
Questionnaires
UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001; German: Schmidt et al., 2008)
The UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale consists of 45 items that are rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree). It contains four subscales corresponding to four facets of impulsivity: Urgency, (Lack of) Premeditation, (Lack of) Perseverance, and Sensation Seeking. The 12-item Urgency scale “refers to the tendency to experience strong impulses, frequently under conditions of negative affect” (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001). (Lack of) Premeditation, an 11-item scale, “refers to the tendency to think and reflect on the consequences of an act before engaging in that act” (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001). (Lack of) Perseverance, a 10-item scale, “refers to an individual’s ability to remain focused on a task that may be boring or difficult” (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001). The 12-item Sensation Seeking scale “incorporates two aspects: (1) a tendency to enjoy and pursue activities that are exciting and (2) an openness to trying new experiences that may or may not be dangerous” (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001). The German adaptation robustly confirms the four-factor dimensionality of the original version with very good internal consistency of the four subscales (Cronbach’s α range: 0.80–0.85; Schmidt et al., 2008). Please see Table 1 (grouping by weight status) and Table 2 (grouping by gender) for sample characteristics regarding the UPPS subdomains.
Yale Food Addiction Scale (Gearhardt et al., 2009; German: Meule et al., 2012)
The Yale Food Addiction Scale is a standardized instrument to identify people with distinctive symptoms indicative of addiction to certain foods. It is a 25-item self-report questionnaire assessing seven food addiction symptoms [based on the seven substance dependence criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV); DSM-IV-TR, 2000] as well as clinically significant impairment or distress. Individuals rate their eating behavior during the last 12 months, referring specifically to high fat and high sugar foods. Ratings comprise a combination of dichotomous and frequency scoring. A symptom is met if at least one question of that criterion is scored as one. A continuous symptom count (range: 0–7) is calculated, adding symptoms which have been met. Additionally, a dichotomous score is calculated to “diagnose” food addiction. Food addiction is diagnosed if at least three symptoms and the criterion of a clinically significant impairment or distress is met. The German version replicates the original one-factorial structure with adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.81–0.83; Meule and Kübler, 2012; Meule et al., 2012). Sample characteristics on the YFAS are shown in Table 1 (grouping by weight status) and Table 2 (grouping by gender).
Higher-Order Cognitive Measures: IQ and Visual Short-Term Memory
The goal-directed system has been indicated to depend on cognitive capacities (Eppinger et al., 2013; Otto et al., 2013; Schad et al., 2014). Therefore measures of higher-order cognitive function (i.e., IQ and visual short-term memory) were considered as control variables. Non-verbal IQ was determined by the Wiener Matritzen-Test (WMT, score range: 0–24 which translates into an IQ range of 60.5–136.5; Formann, 1979). To estimate visual short-term memory, we used a computerized version of the Visual Paired Associates Test (VPA, score range: 0–18), a subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 1987, 2006). Please see Table 1 (grouping by weight status) and Table 2 (grouping by gender) for sample characteristics on IQ and visual short-term memory.
Experimental Paradigm
We applied a simplified version of an established instrumental learning paradigm (e.g., de Wit et al., 2007, 2012b; Gillan et al., 2011). The following description of the paradigm relates to the main task characteristics. Please see Worbe et al. (2015) for further details on the simplified version. The task was programmed in Visual Basic 6.0. In contrast to the original version, participants were presented with animal icons instead of fruit pictures, as we were interested in instrumental performance in a non-food context. The paradigm consisted of four stages: discrimination training phase, outcome-devaluation test, slips-of-action and baseline tests, and questionnaires on contingency knowledge.
Stage 1: Discrimination Training Phase
Twelve animal icons either functioned as stimulus or outcome, resulting in six associative pairs. Participants had to memorize these pairs and the respective associated key press. In detail, on each trial an animal icon [stimulus (S)] outside of a box signaled left or right key press [response (R)]. If the response was correct, an animal icon appeared inside of the box and points were earned [outcome (O)]. If the response was incorrect, the box remained empty and no points were earned. The number of points that could be earned for a correct key press depended on response latency (de Wit et al., 2007). For example, by trial and error participants should learn that an flamingo outside of the box signaled pressing the right key which would be rewarded with a donkey inside and points. Pressing the left key, on the other hand, would result in an empty box and no points (Figure 1A). Participants were informed that they would receive one euro-cent for each collected point. Assignment of the animal pictures to the stimulus vs. outcome sets was counterbalanced. Every stimulus was presented twice in each of eight blocks, resulting in 96 trials altogether. Picture presentation was randomized within each block.
Stage 2: Outcome-Devaluation Test on Response-Outcome Knowledge
This stage was to test if participants learned R→O contingencies. On every trial, two open boxes with animals inside were shown, one that had previously been associated with a left key press and the other one with a right key press. One of these animals (i.e., outcomes) was no longer rewarded, indicated by a cross which was superimposed on this animal (Figure 1B). Participants were instructed to press the key which was previously rewarded by the still-valuable outcome. Feedback was no longer provided, although the participants were told, that correct key presses would still earn them points. Every outcome was devalued twice, resulting in 36 trials altogether.
Stage 3: Slips-of-Action Test
This test was designed to assess the balance between goal-directed (S→O→R) vs. habitual (S→R) behavior. Each of nine blocks started with a 5 s presentation of all six animal outcomes, with two of them being devalued, indicated by a cross superimposed [Figure 1C (1)]. Afterwards, participants were presented a series of closed boxes with animal stimuli outside for 1 s each. They were instructed to no longer open boxes (i.e., withhold response) if the stimulus outside was linked to a devalued outcome inside, but press the appropriate key before a stimulus disappeared which signaled a still-valuable outcome [Figure 1C (2)]. Strong direct S→R associations should trigger responses regardless of the current value of the outcome, resulting in “slips of action” toward devalued outcomes. Good task performance is achieved if participants are able to base responding on the current value of the signaled outcome using S→O→R associations. Altogether the test consisted of 108 trials (72 still-valuable and 36 devalued). Participants did not receive feedback but were told that they would still earn points for correct responses for still-valuable outcomes but lose points for responses for devalued outcomes. To assess the balance between goal-directed and habitual control, a devaluation sensitivity index (DSI) was computed by subtracting the percentage of responses for devalued outcomes from that for still-valuable outcomes (Gillan et al., 2011; de Wit et al., 2012a; Snorrason et al., 2016). This index represents the relative involvement of the habit vs. goal-directed system in action control, i.e., high scores indicate strong goal-directed responding, whereas low scores suggest a habit propensity.
In addition, a baseline test was included. It was identical to the slips-of-action test, except that stimuli were devalued instead of outcomes, i.e., it does not require S→O→R knowledge. Therefore, this test controls for goal-directed behavior but still depends on S→R knowledge. Participants were shown the six stimulus animals with two of them devalued. They were instructed to withhold response for the two devalued ones. A baseline DSI was calculated by subtracting responding for devalued stimuli from responding for still-valuable stimuli. The order of the two tests was counterbalanced.
Stage 4: Questionnaires of Contingency Knowledge
Paper-and-pencil questionnaires were used to measure explicit knowledge of the contingencies. Participants had to indicate: which response was correct for each of the six stimuli (S→R questionnaire), which outcome was associated with which stimulus (S→O questionnaire), and which response was awarded with which of the six outcomes (R→O questionnaire). In addition to that, they indicated on visual analog scales how certain they were about their answers. Questionnaire order was counterbalanced.
Statistical Analysis
Outcome measures were generally non-normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, see Supplementary Table S2; Supplementary Figure S1). Therefore, non-parametric tests were used for statistical analysis. The Friedman test was applied to test for performance differences between instrumental learning blocks. First we tested for continuous relationships between weight status or aspects of impulsivity and instrumental learning (discrimination training), performance during the critical test phase (outcome-devaluation test, slips-of-action test) and explicit knowledge of contingencies (questionnaires). For this, spearman correlations were conducted to inspect associations between BMI or aspects of impulsivity (UPPS scales) and total accuracy during the discrimination training, accuracy on the outcome-devaluation test, the DSI of the slips-of-action and baseline tests and certainty of explicit contingency knowledge. In addition to BMI and impulsivity, control variables (IQ and VPA score) were tested for correlations with the mentioned outcome measures. If control variables correlated with outcome measures, they were controlled for in the corresponding correlation analyses of BMI and the UPPS impulsivity domains using partial rank correlation. Moderation analysis on ranks were performed to examine whether the level of impulsivity moderated the relationship between BMI and accuracy on the outcome-devaluation test or the DSI of the slips-of-action and baseline tests. In addition to these continuous analyses we compared normal-weight, overweight and obese participants groupwise regarding the above-mentioned outcome measures. Differences were evaluated using rank analysis of covariance, including, depending on significant associations with the outcome measures, VPA and/or IQ as covariates. According to the sample’s low variance, food addiction (Gearhardt et al., 2009; Meule et al., 2012) was not explicitly investigated regarding associations with task performance as analyses would not yield meaningful results (see Tables 1 and 2, only seven participants identified as food-addicted). To rule out gender effects in learning of the associations and performance in the critical test phase the following analyses were additionally conducted: Gender differences in learning during the discrimination training phase were evaluated using an 8 (block) × 2 (gender) ANOVA on ranks, including – due to gender differences in these variables (see Table 2) – age and UPPS Sensation Seeking as covariates. Rank analysis of covariance was conducted to test for gender differences in test performance, i.e., accuracy on the outcome-devaluation test and the DSI of the slips-of-action and baseline tests (covariates: age, UPPS Sensation Seeking). SPSS version 20.0 (IBM Corporation, Somers, NY, USA) was used for data analysis.
Results
Discrimination Training Phase
Participants rapidly learned the instrumental discriminations. With progression of the training phase accuracy (percentages of correct responses) increased steadily [first block = Mdn: 67%, interquartile range (IQR): 58–75%; last block = Mdn: 100%, IQR: 91–100%; Figure 2A] and reaction times decreased (first block = Mdn: 937.0 ms, IQR: 808.5–1124.0 ms; last block = Mdn: 560.5 ms, IQR: 523.5–633.0 ms; Figure 2B). Performance during the eight blocks significantly differed from each other (Friedman Test, χ2 = 391.02, p < 0.001), emphasizing the descriptively outlined learning effect. Control measures affected learning performance. Total accuracy during the training phase (averaged over all blocks) positively correlated with estimates of IQ (ρ = 0.304, p = 0.002) and visual short-term memory (ρ = 0.282, p = 0.004). Controlling for these variables, total accuracy during the training phase was not continuously associated with BMI (ρ = 0.075, p = 0.451) nor any assessed measure of impulsivity (UPPS scales: ρ <= 0.116, p >= 0.200), indicated by partial rank correlations. A BMI-based group analysis [8 (block) by 3 (group) ANCOVA on ranks, covariates: VPA, IQ] confirmed the learning effect [main effect of block: F(7,700) = 2.088, p = 0.043], but corresponding to the aforementioned correlation analysis BMI was not indicated to significantly affect performance during training [main effect of group: F(2,100) = 0.50, p = 0.608; block∗group: F(14,700) = 0.803, p = 0.666]. Gender did not affect instrumental learning during the training indicated by repeated measures 8 (block) by 2 (gender) ANCOVA on ranks (covariates: UPPS Sensation Seeking, age) yielding no significant main effects for block [F(7,101) = 0.868, p = 0.531] or gender [F(1,101) = 0.890, p = 0.348] nor a significant block∗gender interaction [F(7,707) = 1.303, p = 0.246].
Outcome-Devaluation Test
Performance was generally very good and in fact near ceiling (Table 3; see Supplementary Figure S1 of the Supplementary Material for a histogram and Supplementary Table S3 for reaction times). We first tested whether control measures were associated with accuracy (percentage of correct responses) in this test. IQ was unrelated, but we did find a significant positive relationship with visual short-term memory: The higher the VPA score was, the more accurately participants responded for the still-valuable outcomes (ρ = 0.277, p = 0.004). Controlling for visual short-term memory, we did not find a continuous relationship between accuracy during the devaluation test and BMI as assessed by partial rank correlation (ρ = -0.09, p = 0.926). Also group analysis (rank analysis of covariance, covariate: VPA) did not suggest differences between normal-weight, overweight and obese participants regarding accuracy of responding for still-valuable outcomes [F(2,104) = 0.201, p = 0.818]. Interestingly, UPPS Sensation Seeking was negatively associated with accuracy during this test: The higher participants scored in this domain of impulsivity the less accurate their choices were (ρ = -0.197, p = 0.045). This correlation remained significant after controlling for the other UPPS scales using partial rank correlation (ρ = -0.217, p = 0.030). Spearman’s correlation analyses did not yield significant relationships between test accuracy and any other UPPS impulsivity domain (ρ <= 0.123, p >= 0.223). A gender difference in accuracy after devaluation, as determined by rank analysis of covariance, was not observed [F(1,103) = 0.733, p = 0.394; covariates: age, UPPS Sensation Seeking].
Table 3.
Slips-of-action test |
Baseline test |
Outcome-devaluation test |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
interquartile range | Devalued (%) | Valuable (%) | DSI | Devalued (%) | Valuable (%) | DSI | Accuracy (%) |
1st Quartile | 5.56 | 74.31 | 54.86 | 4.17 | 84.03 | 70.14 | 88.89 |
Median | 11.11 | 87.50 | 76.39 | 8.33 | 93.06 | 83.33 | 94.44 |
3rd Quartile | 20.83 | 94.44 | 88.19 | 13.89 | 97.22 | 89.58 | 100.00 |
Included are the percentages of responding for devalued and still-valuable outcomes as well as the corresponding devaluation sensitivity index (DSI) in the slips-of-action test; the percentages of responding for devalued and still-valuable stimuli including the corresponding DSI in the baseline test, and accuracy in the outcome-devaluation test.
Slips-of-Action and Baseline Tests
Performance in both tests was very good. There were just few responses for cues indicating devalued outcomes or stimuli and high responding for cues indicating still-valuable outcomes or stimuli (Table 3; see Supplementary Figure S1 of the Supplementary Material for histograms and Supplementary Table S3 for reaction times). Descriptive data on the DSI of the slips-of-action test indicated a general trend toward outcome-based responding (Table 3).
Contrary to our hypothesis, BMI was unrelated to the DSI of the slips-of-action (ρ = -0.029, p = 0.770, Figure 3A) as well as the baseline test (ρ = 0.033, p = 0.738), indicated by correlation analyses. Correspondingly, group analyses did not indicate differences in the sensitivity to devaluation between normal-weight, overweight, and obese participants [slips-of-action test: F(2,104) = 0.577, p = 0.563, Figure 3B; baseline test: F(2,104) = 0.079, p = 0.924]. In line with our hypothesis, impulsivity, i.e., UPPS Sensation Seeking, was negatively associated with the DSI of the slips-of-action (ρ = -0.240, p = 0.014; Figure 4A) and baseline test (ρ = -0.253, p = 0.01; Figure 4B), meaning that the higher participants scored in Sensation Seeking the less they were sensitive to devaluation of both outcomes and stimuli. Correlation between UPPS Sensation Seeking and the DSI remained significant after controlling for the other UPPS domains of impulsivity (slips-of-action test: ρ = -0.270, p = 0.007; baseline test: ρ = -0.326, p = 0.001). No other UPPS scale was related to devaluation sensitivity (ρ <= 0.094, p >= 0.359). We further assessed whether impulsivity (i.e., UPPS Sensation Seeking) moderated the relationship between BMI and task performance in the slips-of-action test, but did not find evidence for a significant interaction with the DSI [F(1,101) = 0.567, p = 0.453]. As control measures were positively correlated with the DSI of the slips-of-action (IQ: ρ = 0.370, p < 0.001; VPA: ρ = 0.418, p < 0.001) and baseline test (IQ: ρ = 0.274, p = 0.005; VPA: ρ = 0.394, p < 0.001), the previously mentioned correlation and group analyses were controlled for IQ and VPA score. Gender did not affect the DSI of the slips-of-action test [F(1,103) = 2.947, p = 0.089, covariates: age, UPPS Sensation Seeking] or baseline test [F(1,103) = 3.195, p = 0.077, covariates: age, UPPS Sensation Seeking], as determined by rank analysis of covariance.
Questionnaires of Contingency Knowledge
Variance of questionnaire measures was extremely low, with a majority of participants accomplishing 100% accuracy and full certainty regarding all kinds of associations (see Table 4; Supplementary Figure S2 of the Supplementary Material for histograms). According to strong ceiling effects, we decided not to perform correlation analyses with respect to the accuracy measures (100% accuracy: S→R = 98%, R→O = 79%, SO = 80% of participants) and certainty of S→R assignments (77% of participants with maximum certainty), as they would not give meaningful results. According to less pronounced ceiling, certainty of R→O (51% of participants reaching maximum certainty) and S→O associations (42% of participants reaching maximum certainty) were inspected for correlations with the variables of interest, excluding participants reaching maximum certainty beforehand. Both control measures were positively associated with R→O (IQ: ρ = 0.389, p = 0.004; VPA: ρ = 0.381, p = 0.004; n = 54) and S→O certainties (IQ: ρ = 0.320, p = 0.011; VPA: ρ = 0.414, p = 0.001; n = 63) and controlled for in the following analyses. BMI and UPPS Sensation Seeking did not correlate with the certainty of R→O (BMI: ρ = 0.066, p = 0.634; UPPS Sensation Seeking: ρ = -0.162, p = 0.242; n = 54) and S→O (BMI: ρ = 0.119, p = 0.352; UPPS Sensation Seeking: ρ = -0.101, p = 0.432; n = 63) knowledge. In addition to that, the comparison of normal-weight, overweight, and obese participants did not indicate group differences in the certainty of R→O [F(2,53) = 1,294, p = 0.283] and S→O [F(2,62) = 0.561, p = 0.573] knowledge.
Table 4.
Interquartile range | S→R accuracy (%) | S→R certainty | R→O accuracy (%) | R→O certainty | S→O accuracy (%) | S→O certainty |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1st Quartile | 100.00 | 9.15 | 100.00 | 8.51 | 100.00 | 7.44 |
Median | 100.00 | 10.00 | 100.00 | 9.90 | 100.00 | 9.77 |
3rd Quartile | 100.00 | 10.00 | 100.00 | 10.00 | 100.00 | 10.00 |
Accuracy (percentages of correct answers) and certainty of S→R, R→O, and S→O assignments (certaintymax = 10, as measured by visual analog scales).
Discussion
The present study addressed the two main questions: (1) Whether there is a domain-general habit propensity in obesity; (2) And whether aspects of impulsivity influence the balance between goal-directed and habitual action control. Regarding excess body weight, we did not find evidence for heightened habitual action control in a non-food context. Consequently, although there is previous indication for obesity-associated domain-general behavioral inflexibility as suggested by paradigms assessing inhibitory control (Nederkoorn et al., 2006; Gunstad et al., 2007) or decision making (Pignatti et al., 2006; Weller et al., 2008; Horstmann et al., 2011; Simmank et al., 2015), these inflexibilities may not extend to abnormal stimulus-response linkage at the expense of outcome-driven action control. On the other hand, obesity-associated impairments in goal-directed behavior and overreliance on habits might not be generalizable, but may be restricted to the food domain. Previous investigations demonstrated food-related behavioral inflexibility in obesity as measured by food-specific versions of the delay-discounting task (Rasmussen et al., 2010) and the go/no-go task (Batterink et al., 2010; He et al., 2014). In the delay-discounting paradigm of Rasmussen et al. (2010) also a monetary condition was included. Interestingly, only food-specific discounting was significantly associated with the percentage of body fat. A recent study also showed a positive relationship between BMI and a behavioral estimate of habitual responding for snack food (Horstmann et al., 2015a). Unfortunately, the majority of these studies focused on the food context without including a comparative neutral condition. Therefore, conclusions regarding specificity of the effects should be drawn with caution. Moreover, abnormalities in habit formation might be a specific issue of certain obese subtypes as opposed to applying generally to obesity. Especially individuals with a diagnosis of binge eating (Voon et al., 2015; Reiter et al., 2016) or food addiction (Gearhardt et al., 2009; Davis et al., 2011; Davis, 2013; Meule and Gearhardt, 2014; Murphy et al., 2014) may be prone to stimulus-guided behavior, which should be clarified in future studies.
Impulsivity, or more specifically the domain of Sensation Seeking, negatively correlated with the DSI of both the slips-of-action and baseline test. In other words, Sensation Seeking was related to the inability to selectively respond for still-valuable outcomes and stimuli while suppressing responses for devalued outcomes and stimuli. This result suggests a general impairment in the ability to inhibit previously learnt responses. The finding of relationships between impulsivity and both balance measures (slips-of-action and baseline test) is consistent with the possibility of enhanced S→R learning and a relative reliance on habits in sensation seekers, but fits less well with the idea of impaired outcome-based learning (as the baseline test controls for outcome-based responding). On the other hand, the negative relationship of impulsivity and accuracy in the outcome-devaluation test does indicate a more specific dysregulation of outcome-based responding. Consequently, the current results are not sufficient to determine if the habit propensity in highly impulsive individuals is due to excessive reliance on habits or weak goal-directed control. An alternative interpretation of our results would be a general impairment in response inhibition in highly impulsive individuals leading to failures to suppress learnt responses across these different tests. Future studies should therefore control for this type of inhibition failure, to cancel out the possibility of inhibition disturbances which are independent of associative learning. Beyond the direct relation between task performance and impulsivity, we failed to find a moderation of the proposed relationship between BMI and the balance measure. Future studies focusing on the abovementioned subtypes of obesity may find interaction effects.
The finding of a habit propensity in impulsivity is in line with previous research showing impaired goal-directed control (Hogarth et al., 2012), cognitive inflexibility (Franken et al., 2008; Crews and Boettiger, 2009; Romer et al., 2009) and heightened cue-reactivity resulting in inhibition failures (reviewed in Bari and Robbins, 2013) in human impulsivity. Interestingly, in this study overreliance on habits was specifically observed in participants scoring high in the subdomain of Sensation Seeking, conceptualized as (1) the tendency to enjoy and pursue activities that are exciting and (2) the openness to trying new experiences that may be risky (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001). The abovementioned study of Hogarth et al. (2012) reported a negative relationship between goal-directedness and Motor Impulsiveness (Patton et al., 1995) in a food context. This conceptualization of impulsivity reflects the tendency to act on the spur of the moment, which probably covers aspects reflecting risk-taking and excitement seeking. Sensation seeking is assumed to go along with strong approach behaviors and weak behavioral avoidance/inhibition (Depue and Collons, 1999; Collins et al., 2012). In addition to that, sensation seeking or the related constructs of novelty or excitement seeking (Zuckerman, 1979; Cloninger et al., 1993) have been associated with the inability to delay responding in the face of a larger reward (Cyders and Coskunpinar, 2011), with risky and unpredictable decision making (Finn, 2002; Suhr and Tsanadis, 2007; Bornovalova et al., 2009; Noël et al., 2011), as well as with impairments in response inhibition (Finn, 2002; Noël et al., 2011). Another study indicates a diminished ability of sensation seekers to evaluate the negative outcomes in their decisions (Cservenka et al., 2013). According to our findings, these alterations may be due to heightened reliance on S→R guided action control with inappropriate consideration of outcome values. Individual differences in corticostriatal loops, previously associated with trait impulsivity, might constitute the underlying mechanism (Jentsch and Taylor, 1999; Volkow and Fowler, 2000; Cardinal et al., 2001; McClure et al., 2004; Brown et al., 2006; Dalley et al., 2008; Forbes et al., 2009; Davis et al., 2013), which should be clarified in future studies. Striatal dopaminergic disturbances in sensation seekers may contribute to these alterations (Golimbet et al., 2007; Munafò et al., 2008; Gjedde et al., 2010).
Apart from impulsivity cognitive capacities, as assessed by estimates of non-verbal IQ and short-term memory, were related to the balance between outcome- and stimulus-based responding. This is in line with previous research showing goal-directed behavior to depend on higher-order cognitive resources (Eppinger et al., 2013; Otto et al., 2013; Schad et al., 2014). According to their relationships with instrumental learning and performance, we recommend including estimates of IQ and working or short-term memory as standard control variables into studies investigating instrumental performance.
This study is accompanied by some limitations. Most substantial may be the issue of low task difficulty and corresponding ceiling effects. Therefore, the paradigm may not be sensitive enough to detect subtle differences in goal-directed vs. habitual behavior potentially associated with weight status. Future studies might adapt the task to make it more challenging, e.g., by consideration of the originally introduced congruent and incongruent discrimination conditions (de Wit et al., 2007). Thereby variance may be enahnced in order to disentangle potential subtle relationships. Furthermore, the instrumental learning phase may be adapted. To strengthen habit formation overtraining might be a fruitful approach (Tricomi et al., 2009). On the other hand, alternative tasks assessing behavioral flexibility, e.g., reversal learning tasks (e.g., Cools et al., 2006; Waltz et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2014; Culbreth et al., 2015), might be more sensitive to detect subtle differences with respect to weight status. Moreover, domain-general impairments in goal-directed behavior may play a role in gaining weight and in the end obesity if food motivation is also strong (Nederkoorn et al., 2010; Rollins et al., 2010; Appelhans et al., 2011). Unfortunately, with the current paradigm we cannot evaluate the aspect of food motivation, which should be considered in future investigations. Apart from limitations of the paradigm, sample characteristics might have limited the ability to detect differences. Unfortunately, we were not able to do analyses based on the aspect of food addiction, as only few participants showed symptoms and just seven participants were diagnosed as food addicted (Gearhardt et al., 2009). In addition, here we focused on young and healthy individuals. Impairments in goal-directed behavior might be specifically apparent in morbidly obese (Horstmann et al., 2015b) or older obese individuals (de Wit et al., 2014). Future studies therefore may broaden age and BMI range. Moreover, with the current paradigm we cannot draw conclusions on the generalizability of the effect. Future comparative studies, using paradigms that differentiate between neutral and specific (e.g., food) contexts, are needed to clarify this aspect. Finally, according to low task difficulty one may argue that differences in participants’ motivation might have accounted for the effect of Sensation Seeking. Although we cannot rule out this possibility, providing participants with monetary reward – a universally effective reinforcer – is expected to maintain high motivation in all participants, especially as there is no previous indication for differences in the sensitivity to reward depending on Sensation Seeking. Also the excellent - near ceiling - performance among all participants indicate continuous engagement in the task.
Conclusion
This study is indicative of an impaired balance between goal-directed and habitual action control in the impulsivity domain of Sensation Seeking leading to a habit propensity. However, we failed to find an imbalance between goal-directed and habitual behavior in overweight or obesity in a non-food context. We recommend that future studies should (1) adapt the applied task to make it more challenging, thereby enhancing variance in task performance, and (2) focus on specific subtypes of obesity which might be especially prone to an overreliance on habits. Moreover, neuroimaging may contribute to a more detailed specification of proposed habit propensities by identifying brain structures mediating differences in action control.
Author Contributions
AD, AH, and SdW are responsible for study conception and design. AD acquired the data and did the analysis. AD, AH, and SdW interpreted the results. AD did the drafting of the work. AD, AH, and SdeW revised the draft critically, approved the final version for publication and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work, ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer MP and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Maria Pössel for her help during data acquisition. Moreover, we thank Zsuzsika Sjoerds, Lorenz Deserno, and Florian Schlagenhauf for their valuable feedback on the results.
Footnotes
Funding. The work of AH is supported by the IFB Adiposity Diseases, Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF), Germany, FKZ: 01E01001 (http://www.bmbf.de) and by the German Research Foundation (DFG) (http://www.dfg.de), within the framework of the CRC 1052 ‘Obesity Mechanisms. AD is funded by a research grant from the FAZIT Foundation (http://www.fazit-stiftung.de). SdeW is supported by a Vidi grant 452-13-006 of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO).
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00213/full#supplementary-material
References
- Appelhans B. M., Woolf K., Pagoto S. L., Schneider K. L., Whited M. C., Liebman R. (2011). Inhibiting food reward: delay discounting, food reward sensitivity, and palatable food intake in overweight and obese women. Obesity (Silver Spring) 19 2175–2182. 10.1038/oby.2011.57 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine B. W., O’Doherty J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35 48–69. 10.1038/npp.2009.131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bari A., Robbins T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 108 44–79. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batterink L., Yokum S., Stice E. (2010). Body mass correlates inversely with inhibitory control in response to food among adolescent girls: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 52 1696–1703. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A. T., Ward C. H., Mendelson M., Mock J., Erbaugh J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4 561–571. 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge K. C., Ho C.-Y., Richard J. M., DiFeliceantonio A. G. (2010). The tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res. 1350 43–64. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova M. A., Cashman-Rolls A., O’Donnell J. M., Ettinger K., Richards J. B., DeWit H., et al. (2009). Risk taking differences on a behavioral task as a function of potential reward/loss magnitude and individual differences in impulsivity and sensation seeking. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93 258–262. 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.10.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braet C., Claus L., Verbeken S., Van Vlierberghe L. (2007). Impulsivity in overweight children. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 16 473–483. 10.1007/s00787-007-0623-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown S. M., Manuck S. B., Flory J. D., Hariri A. R. (2006). Neural basis of individual differences in impulsivity: contributions of corticolimbic circuits for behavioral arousal and control. Emotion 6 239–245. 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckholtz J. W., Treadway M. T., Cowan R. L., Woodward N. D., Li R., Ansari M. S., et al. (2010). Dopaminergic network differences in human impulsivity. Science 329:532 10.1126/science.1185778 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinal R. N., Pennicott D. R., Lakmali C., Robbins T. W., Everitt B. J. (2001). Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science 292 2499–2501. 10.1126/science.1060818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll M. E., Lynch W. J., Roth M. E., Morgan A. D., Cosgrove K. P. (2004). Sex and estrogen influence drug abuse. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 25 273–279. 10.1016/j.tips.2004.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloninger C. R., Svrakic D. M., Przybeck T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50 975–990. 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins H. R., Corbly C. R., Liu X., Kelly T. H., Lynam D., Joseph J. E. (2012). Too little, too late or too much, too early? Differential hemodynamics of response inhibition in high and low sensation seekers. Brain Res. 1481 1–12. 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cools R., Altamirano L., D’Esposito M. (2006). Reversal learning in Parkinson’s disease depends on medication status and outcome valence. Neuropsychologia 44 1663–1673. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit L. H. (2016). Effects of obesogenic diets on learning and habitual responding. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 9 84–90. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crews F. T., Boettiger C. A. (2009). Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 93 237–247. 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cservenka A., Herting M. M., Seghete K. L. M., Hudson K. A., Nagel B. J. (2013). High and low sensation seeking adolescents show distinct patterns of brain activity during reward processing. Neuroimage 66 184–193. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Culbreth A. J., Gold J. M., Cools R., Barch D. M. (2015). Impaired activation in cognitive control regions predicts reversal learning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 42 1–10. 10.1093/schbul/sbv075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cyders M., Coskunpinar A. (2011). Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: Is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31 965–982. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley J. W., Mar A. C., Economidou D., Robbins T. W. (2008). Neurobehavioral mechanisms of impulsivity: fronto-striatal systems and functional neurochemistry. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 90 250–260. 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.12.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis C. (2013). Compulsive overeating as an addictive behavior: overlap between food addiction and binge eating disorder. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2 171–178. 10.1007/s13679-013-0049-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davis C., Curtis C., Levitan R. D., Carter J. C., Kaplan A. S., Kennedy J. L. (2011). Evidence that ‘food addiction’ is a valid phenotype of obesity. Appetite 57 711–717. 10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis F. C., Knodt A. R., Sporns O., Lahey B. B., Zald D. H., Brigidi B. D., et al. (2013). Impulsivity and the modular organization of resting-state neural networks. Cereb. Cortex 23 1444–1452. 10.1093/cercor/bhs126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Weijer B. A., van de Giessen E., van Amelsvoort T. A., Boot E., Braak B., Janssen I. M., et al. (2011). Lower striatal dopamine D2/3 receptor availability in obese compared with non-obese subjects. EJNMMI Res. 1:37 10.1186/2191-219X-1-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit S., Corlett P. R., Aitken M. R., Dickinson A., Fletcher P. C. (2009). Differential engagement of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex by goal-directed and habitual behavior toward food pictures in humans. J. Neurosci. 29 11330–11338. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1639-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit S., Dickinson A. (2009). Associative theories of goal-directed behaviour: a case for animal-human translational models. Psychol. Res. 73 463–476. 10.1007/s00426-009-0230-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit S., Niry D., Wariyar R., Aitken M. R. F., Dickinson A. (2007). Stimulus-outcome interactions during instrumental discrimination learning by rats and humans. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process 33 1–11. 10.1037/0097-7403.33.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit S., Standing H. R., Devito E. E., Robinson O. J., Ridderinkhof K. R., Robbins T. W., et al. (2012a). Reliance on habits at the expense of goal-directed control following dopamine precursor depletion. Psychopharmacology 219 621–631. 10.1007/s00213-011-2563-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit S., van de Vijver I., Ridderinkhof K. R. (2014). Impaired acquisition of goal-directed action in healthy aging. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 14 647–658. 10.3758/s13415-014-0288-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit S., Watson P., Harsay H., Cohen M., van de Vijver I., Ridderinkhof K. R. (2012b). Corticostriatal connectivity underlies individual differences in the balance between habitual and goal-directed action control. J. Neurosci. 32 12066–12075. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1088-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme C., Salvador A., Valabrègue R., Roze E., Palminteri S., Vidailhet M., et al. (2016). Enhanced habit formation in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain 139 605–615. 10.1093/brain/awv307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Depue R. A., Collons P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behav. Brain Sci. 22 491–569. 10.1017/S0140525X99002046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A. (1985). Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 308 67–78. 10.1098/rstb.1985.0010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dimitropoulos A., Tkach J., Ho A., Kennedy J. (2012). Greater corticolimbic activation to high-calorie food cues after eating in obese vs. normal-weight adults. Appetite 58 303–312. 10.1016/j.appet.2011.10.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolan M., Deakin W. J. F., Roberts N., Anderson I. (2002). Serotonergic and cognitive impairment in impulsive aggressive personality disordered offenders: are there implications for treatment? Psychol. Med. 32 105–117. 10.1017/S0033291701004688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DSM-IV-TR (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th Edn Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenstein S. A., Antenor-Dorsey J. A. V., Gredysa D. M., Koller J. M., Bihun E. C., Ranck S. A., et al. (2013). A comparison of D2 receptor specific binding in obese and normal-weight individuals using PET with (N-[(11)C]methyl)benperidol. Synapse 67 748–756. 10.1002/syn.21680 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eppinger B., Walter M., Heekeren H. R., Li S. C. (2013). Of goals and habits: age-related and individual differences in goal-directed decision-making. Front. Neurosci. 7:253 10.3389/fnins.2013.00253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans S. M., Haney M., Foltin R. W. (2002). The effects of smoked cocaine during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle in women. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 159 397–406. 10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt B. J., Belin D., Economidou D., Pelloux Y., Dalley J. W., Robbins T. W. (2008). Review. Neural mechanisms underlying the vulnerability to develop compulsive drug-seeking habits and addiction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363 3125–3135. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt B. J., Robbins T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8 1481–1489. 10.1038/nn1579 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finn P. R. (2002). Motivation, working memory, and decision making: a cognitive-motivational theory of personality vulnerability to alcoholism. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 1 183–205. 10.1177/1534582302001003001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes E. E., Brown S. M., Kimak M., Ferrell R. E., Manuck S. B., Hariri A. R. (2009). Genetic variation in components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity. Mol. Psychiatry 14 60–70. 10.1038/sj.mp.4002086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Formann A. K. (1979). Wiener Matritzen-Test eds Formann A. K., Piswanger K. Weinheim: Beltz Test Gesellschaft. [Google Scholar]
- Franken I. H. A., van Strien J. W., Nijs I., Muris P. (2008). Impulsivity is associated with behavioral decision-making deficits. Psychiatry Res. 158 155–163. 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Garcia I., Jurado M. A., Garolera M., Segura B., Sala-Llonch R., Marques-Iturria I., et al. (2013). Alterations of the salience network in obesity: a resting-state fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34 2786–2797. 10.1002/hbm.22104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gearhardt A. N., Corbin W. R., Brownell K. D. (2009). Preliminary validation of the Yale Food Addiction Scale. Appetite 52 430–436. 10.1016/j.appet.2008.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillan C. M., Morein-Zamir S., Urcelay G. P., Sule A., Voon V., Apergis-Schoute A. M., et al. (2014). Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 75 631–638. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillan C. M., Papmeyer M., Morein-Zamir S., Sahakian B. J., Fineberg N., Robbins T. W., et al. (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 168 718–726. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gjedde A., Kumakura Y., Cumming P., Linnet J., Møller A. (2010). Inverted-U-shaped correlation between dopamine receptor availability in striatum and sensation seeking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107 3870–3875. 10.1073/pnas.0912319107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gläscher J., Hampton A. N., O’Doherty J. P. (2009). Determining a role for ventromedial prefrontal cortex in encoding action-based value signals during reward-related decision making. Cereb. Cortex 19 483–495. 10.1093/cercor/bhn098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golimbet V. E., Alfimova M. V., Gritsenko I. K., Ebstein R. P. (2007). Relationship between dopamine system genes and extraversion and novelty seeking. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 37 601–606. 10.1007/s11055-007-0058-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guerrieri R., Nederkoorn C., Jansen A. (2008). The effect of an impulsive personality on overeating and obesity: current state of affairs. Psychol. Top. 17 265–286. [Google Scholar]
- Gunstad J., Paul R. H., Cohen R. A., Tate D. F., Spitznagel M. B., Gordon E. (2007). Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr. Psychiatry 48 57–61. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo J., Simmons W. K., Herscovitch P., Martin A., Hall K. D. (2014). Striatal dopamine D2-like receptor correlation patterns with human obesity and opportunistic eating behavior. Mol. Psychiatry 19 1078–1084. 10.1038/mp.2014.102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampton A. N., O’doherty J. P. (2007). Decoding the neural substrates of reward-related decision making with functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104 1377–1382. 10.1073/pnas.0606297104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He Q., Xiao L., Xue G., Wong S., Ames S. L., Schembre S. M., et al. (2014). Poor ability to resist tempting calorie rich food is linked to altered balance between neural systems involved in urge and self-control. Nutr. J. 13:92 10.1186/1475-2891-13-92 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogarth L., Chase H. W., Baess K. (2012). Impaired goal-directed behavioural control in human impulsivity. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 65 305–316. 10.1080/17470218.2010.518242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horstmann A., Busse F. P., Mathar D., Müller K., Lepsien J., Schlögl H., et al. (2011). Obesity-related differences between women and men in brain structure and goal-directed behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:58 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horstmann A., Dietrich A., Mathar D., Pössel M., Villringer A., Neumann J. (2015a). Slave to habit? Obesity is associated with decreased behavioural sensitivity to reward devaluation. Appetite 87 175–183. 10.1016/j.appet.2014.12.212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horstmann A., Fenske W. K., Hankir M. K. (2015b). Argument for a non-linear relationship between severity of human obesity and dopaminergic tone. Obes. Rev. 16 821–830. 10.1111/obr.12303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jentsch J. D., Taylor J. R. (1999). Impulsivity resulting from frontostiatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 146 373–390. 10.1007/PL00005483 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaasinen V., Någren K., Hietala J., Farde L., Rinne J. O. (2001). Sex differences in extrastriatal dopamine d(2)-like receptors in the human brain. Am. J. Psychiatry 158 308–311. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King J. A., Tenney J., Rossi V., Colamussi L., Burdick S. (2003). Neural substrates underlying impulsivity. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1008 160–169. 10.1196/annals.1301.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kullmann S., Heni M., Linder K., Zipfel S., Häring H.-U., Veit R., et al. (2014). Resting-state functional connectivity of the human hypothalamus. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35 6088–6096. 10.1002/hbm.22607 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch W. J., Roth M. E., Carroll M. E. (2002). Biological basis of sex differences in drug abuse: preclinical and clinical studies. Psychopharmacology 164 121–137. 10.1007/s00213-002-1183-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure S. M., Laibson D. I., Loewenstein G., Cohen J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306 503–507. 10.1126/science.1100907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meule A., Gearhardt A. N. (2014). Five years of the Yale Food Addiction Scale: taking stock and moving forward. Curr. Addict. Rep. 1 193–205. 10.1007/s40429-014-0021-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Meule A., Kübler A. (2012). Food cravings in food addiction: the distinct role of positive reinforcement. Eat. Behav. 13 252–255. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meule A., Vögele C., Kübler A. (2012). Deutsche übersetzung und validierung der Yale Food Addiction Scale. Diagnostica 58 115–126. 10.1026/0012-1924/a000047 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mobbs O., Crépin C., Thiéry C., Golay A., Van der Linden M. (2010). Obesity and the four facets of impulsivity. Patient Educ. Couns. 79 372–377. 10.1016/j.pec.2010.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munafò M. R., Yalcin B., Willis-Owen S. A., Flint J. (2008). Association of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene and approach-related personality traits: meta-analysis and new data. Biol. Psychiatry 63 197–206. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy C. M., Stojek M. K., MacKillop J. (2014). Interrelationships among impulsive personality traits, food addiction, and Body Mass Index. Appetite 73 45–50. 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nederkoorn C., Houben K., Hofmann W., Roefs A., Jansen A. (2010). Control yourself or just eat what you like? Weight gain over a year is predicted by an interactive effect of response inhibition and implicit preference for snack foods. Health Psychol. 29 389–393. 10.1037/a0019921 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nederkoorn C., Smulders F. T. Y., Havermans R. C., Roefs A., Jansen A. (2006). Impulsivity in obese women. Appetite 47 253–256. 10.1016/j.appet.2006.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noël X., Brevers D., Bechara A., Hanak C., Kornreich C., Verbanck P., et al. (2011). Neurocognitive determinants of novelty and sensation-seeking in individuals with alcoholismdempster. Alcohol. Alcohol. 46 407–415. 10.1093/alcalc/agr048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nummenmaa L., Hirvonen J., Hannukainen J. C., Immonen H., Lindroos M. M., Salminen P., et al. (2012). Dorsal striatum and its limbic connectivity mediate abnormal anticipatory reward processing in obesity. PLoS ONE 7:e31089 10.1371/journal.pone.0031089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otto A. R., Raio C. M., Chiang A., Phelps E. A., Daw N. D. (2013). Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110 20941–20946. 10.1073/pnas.1312011110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patton J. H., Stanford M. S., Barratt E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol 51 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pignatti R., Bertella L., Albani G., Mauro A., Molinari E., Semenza C. (2006). Decision-making in obesity: a study using the Gambling Task. Eat. Weight Disord. 11 126–132. 10.1007/BF03327557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rasmussen E. B., Lawyer S. R., Reilly W. (2010). Percent body fat is related to delay and probability discounting for food in humans. Behav. Processes 83 23–30. 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redish A. D., Jensen S., Johnson A. (2008). A unified framework for addiction: vulnerabilities in the decision process. Behav. Brain Sci. 31 415–437; discussion 437–87 10.1017/S0140525X0800472X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiter A. M., Heinze H.-J., Schlagenhauf F., Deserno L. (2016). Impaired flexible reward-based decision-making in binge eating disorder: evidence from computational modeling and functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 10.1038/npp.2016.95 [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rollins B. Y., Dearing K. K., Epstein L. H. (2010). Delay discounting moderates the effect of food reinforcement on energy intake among non-obese women. Appetite 55 420–425. 10.1016/j.appet.2010.07.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romer D., Betancourt L., Giannetta J. M., Brodsky N. L., Farah M., Hurt H. (2009). Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia 47 2916–2926. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rydén A., Sullivan M., Torgerson J. S., Karlsson J., Lindroos A.-K., Taft C. (2003). Severe obesity and personality: a comparative controlled study of personality traits. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 27 1534–1540. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schad D. J., Jünger E., Sebold M., Garbusow M., Bernhardt N., Javadi A. H., et al. (2014). Processing speed enhances model-based over model-free reinforcement learning in the presence of high working memory functioning. Front. Psychol. 5:1450 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01450 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt R. E., Gay P., D’Acremont M., Van Der Linden M. (2008). A German adaptation of the upps impulsive behavior scale: psychometric properties and factor structure. Swiss J. Psychol. 67 107–112. 10.1024/1421-0185.67.2.107 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Simmank J., Murawski C., Bode S., Horstmann A. (2015). Incidental rewarding cues influence economic decisions in people with obesity. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:278 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00278 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sjoerds Z., de Wit S., van den Brink W., Robbins T. W., Beekman A. T., Penninx B. W., et al. (2013). Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Transl. Psychiatry 3:e337 10.1038/tp.2013.107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snorrason I., Lee H. J., de Wit S., Woods D. W. (2016). Are nonclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms associated with bias toward habits? Psychiatry Res. 241 221–223. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sofuoglu M., Dudish-Poulsen S., Nelson D., Pentel P. R., Hatsukami D. K. (1999). Sex and menstrual cycle differences in the subjective effects from smoked cocaine in humans. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 7 274–283. 10.1037/1064-1297.7.3.274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoeckel L. E., Kim J., Weller R. E., Cox J. E., Cook E. W., Horwitz B. (2009). Effective connectivity of a reward network in obese women. Brain Res. Bull. 79 388–395. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.05.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suhr J. A., Tsanadis J. (2007). Affect and personality correlates of the Iowa Gambling Task. Pers. Individ. Dif. 43 27–36. 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka S. C., Balleine B. W., O’Doherty J. P. (2008). Calculating consequences: brain systems that encode the causal effects of actions. J. Neurosci. 28 6750–6755. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1808-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tricomi E., Balleine B. W., O’Doherty J. P. (2009). A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29 2225–2232. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06796.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vainik U., Dagher A., Dubé L., Fellows L. K. (2013). Neurobehavioural correlates of body mass index and eating behaviours in adults: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37 279–299. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valentin V. V., Dickinson A., O’Doherty J. P. (2007). Determining the neural substrates of goal-directed learning in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 27 4019–4026. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0564-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow N. D., Fowler J. S. (2000). Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 10 318–325. 10.1093/cercor/10.3.318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow N. D., Wang G.-J., Baler R. D. (2011). Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: implications for obesity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15 37–46. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow N. D., Wise R. A. (2005). How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat. Neurosci 8 555–560. 10.1038/nn1452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon V., Derbyshire K., Rück C., Irvine M. A., Worbe Y., Enander J., et al. (2015). Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol. Psychiatry 20 345–352. 10.1038/mp.2014.44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waltz J. A., Kasanova Z., Ross T. J., Salmeron B. J., McMahon R. P., Gold J. M., et al. (2013). The roles of reward, default, and executive control networks in set-shifting impairments in schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 8:e57257 10.1371/journal.pone.0057257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang G. J., Volkow N. D., Logan J., Pappas N. R., Wong C. T., Zhu W., et al. (2001). Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet 357 354–357. 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03643-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale - Revised. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (2006). Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene: WIE; Übersetzung und Adaption der WAIS-III ed. von Aster M. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services. [Google Scholar]
- Weller R. E., Cook E. W., Avsar K. B., Cox J. E. (2008). Obese women show greater delay discounting than healthy-weight women. Appetite 51 563–569. 10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiteside S. P., Lynam D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Pers. Individ. Dif. 30 669–689. 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Worbe Y., Savulich G., de Wit S., Fernandez-Egea E., Robbins T. W. (2015). Tryptophan depletion promotes habitual over goal-directed control of appetitive responding in humans. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18: yv013 10.1093/ijnp/pyv013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Z., Manson K. F., Schiller D., Levy I. (2014). Impaired associative learning with food rewards in obese women. Curr. Biol. 24 1731–1736. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M. (1979). Sensation Seeking: Beyond the Optimal Level of Arousal. Hillsday, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.