Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2016 Jan 8;29(3):645–656. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12814

Sex‐specific plasticity and genotype × sex interactions for age and size of maturity in the sheepshead swordtail, Xiphophorus birchmanni

K Boulton 1, G G Rosenthal 2,3, A J Grimmer 4, C A Walling 5, A J Wilson 4,
PMCID: PMC5102681  PMID: 26688295

Abstract

Responses to sexually antagonistic selection are thought to be constrained by the shared genetic architecture of homologous male and female traits. Accordingly, adaptive sexual dimorphism depends on mechanisms such as genotype‐by‐sex interaction (G×S) and sex‐specific plasticity to alleviate this constraint. We tested these mechanisms in a population of Xiphophorus birchmanni (sheepshead swordtail), where the intensity of male competition is expected to mediate intersexual conflict over age and size at maturity. Combining quantitative genetics with density manipulations and analysis of sex ratio variation, we confirm that maturation traits are dimorphic and heritable, but also subject to large G×S. Although cross‐sex genetic correlations are close to zero, suggesting sex‐linked genes with important effects on growth and maturation are likely segregating in this population, we found less evidence of sex‐specific adaptive plasticity. At high density, there was a weak trend towards later and smaller maturation in both sexes. Effects of sex ratio were stronger and putatively adaptive in males but not in females. Males delay maturation in the presence of mature rivals, resulting in larger adult size with subsequent benefit to competitive ability. However, females also delay maturation in male‐biased groups, incurring a loss of reproductive lifespan without apparent benefit. Thus, in highly competitive environments, female fitness may be limited by the lack of sex‐specific plasticity. More generally, assuming that selection does act antagonistically on male and female maturation traits in the wild, our results demonstrate that genetic architecture of homologous traits can ease a major constraint on the evolution of adaptive dimorphism.

Keywords: genetic correlation, life history, plasticity, quantitative genetics, sexual conflict, Xiphophorus birchmanni

Introduction

Sexual dimorphism arises because fitness is limited by different traits in females and males (Bateman, 1948). Whereas fecundity is typically limiting for females, male fitness more often depends on traits that determine mating opportunities within the context of sexual selection imposed by female mate choice and/or male–male competition (Andersson, 1994). An important consequence of this is that homologous traits in males and females can have very different sex‐specific optima (i.e. sexually antagonistic selection). In some cases, sexual antagonism can be fully resolved over evolutionary time by the evolution of sex‐limited traits. However, where this is not the case, the degree of sex‐specific adaptation depends on constraints arising from genetic architecture that is shared between the sexes (Lande, 1980; Fairbairn & Roff, 2006; Poissant et al., 2010). Here, we describe a study of two key life‐history traits – age and size at sexual maturation – in a poeciliid fish and evaluate the extent that shared genetic architecture has the potential to limit sex‐specific adaptation given an expectation of sexually antagonistic selection in the wild. Additionally, we ask whether sex differences in plastic responses to changing levels of conspecific competition offer an alternative route to sex‐specific adaptive phenotypic expression.

Tests of the hypothesis that shared genetic architecture constrains sex‐specific adaptation have focussed largely on estimating the cross‐sex genetic correlation (subsequently denoted r MF) for homologous traits expressed in males and females (Walling et al., 2014; e.g. Pavitt et al., 2014). Strong cross‐sex correlations, whether positive or negative, mean that sex‐specific homologous traits are not free to evolve independently of one another. In the situation where directional selection is antagonistic in the two sexes, the ability of each sex to reach its own optimum will be maximally constrained at r MF = 1 (with no constraint when r MF = 0). Meta‐analysis suggests r MF is usually strongly positive but negatively correlated with the degree of sexual dimorphism and is lower for traits closely linked to fitness (Poissant et al., 2010). These patterns are consistent with the expectation that sex‐specific genetic architecture (and hence reduced r MF) will evolve to at least partially alleviate constraints on adaptive dimorphism (Charlesworth & Charlesworth, 1980). Nonetheless, sexually antagonistic selection persists despite dimorphism (Cox & Calsbeek, 2009), r MF between homologous traits is typically positive (Poissant et al., 2010), whereas negative cross‐sex genetic correlations have been reported for fitness itself (Brommer et al., 2007). These observations suggest that constraints arising from shared genetic architecture between the sexes have not been fully resolved.

Although the patterns described above are consistent with the constraint hypothesis, the restricted focus on r MF has been criticized as inadequate for understanding the potential for, and limitations to, sex‐specific adaption. There are several reasons for this. Firstly, evolutionary responses to sex‐specific selection on homologous traits will depend not just on the cross‐sex genetic correlation but also on the presence and form of genotype‐by‐sex (G×S) interactions more generally. Importantly, these can be manifest not just as r MF < 1, but also as between‐sex differences in the levels of additive genetic variance (Walling et al., 2014; Wyman & Rowe, 2014). Secondly, although studies typically focus on the genetic evolution of (mean) sex‐specific traits, phenotypic plasticity can also contribute to sexual dimorphism (Stillwell et al., 2010). Not all plasticity is adaptive, and we also note the separation of genetic and environmental effects is not always clear‐cut (e.g. in the presence of genotype‐by‐environment interactions). However, where divergence of sex‐specific phenotypic optima is sensitive to local environmental parameters, decoupling of male and female plastic responses could be an important mechanism for allowing adaptive dimorphism (Hallsson & Bjorklund, 2012). For example, where male fitness is limited by intrasexual competition, delayed maturation can be advantageous for males, allowing avoidance of aggression from rivals until their chances of competitive success are improved, for example, at greater age and/or size (Studd & Robertson, 1985; Lyon & Montgomerie, 1986).

Here, we describe a study of sex‐specific maturation traits in Xiphophorus birchmanni, (Lechner and Radda, 1987, sheepshead swordtail) that examines the potential for cross‐sex genetic constraints and sex‐specific plasticity. Although quantitative genetic analyses of cross‐sex genetic architecture have not previously been conducted in this species, several lines of evidence from other Xiphophorus species suggest that sexually antagonistic selection is likely to occur in the wild and that adaptive dimorphism may be facilitated by both cross‐sex decoupling of genetic processes (manifest as G×S) and plastic responses to the social environment. For instance, male‐specific genetic variance for life‐history traits is known to arise from Y‐linked loci with major effects on maturation age and size in populations of X. maculatus (platyfish; Schreibman & Kallman, 1977; Basolo, 1988) and northern swordtails (X. nigrensis, X. montezumae, X. multilineatus; Kallman, 1983; Ryan et al., 1992; Lampert et al., 2010). Nonetheless, behavioural and ecological studies indicate intersexual conflict is often ongoing and likely to be contingent on the social environment (Campton & Gall, 1988; Walling et al., 2007). Growth in male swordtails is almost determinate (i.e. slows down markedly at maturation) whereas females display indeterminate (continuous) growth (Evans et al., 2011). Size is important for both sexes, predicting dominance in males, who are territorial and compete agonistically over access to females (Prenter et al., 2008; Wilson et al., 2013) and fecundity in females. This can result in sex‐specific selection in certain environments, with males delaying maturation in the presence of other males to allow increased size (and thus expected dominance) at maturation, whereas females may even accelerate maturation under such circumstances (Borowsky, 1973; Walling et al., 2007). This illustrates the potential for both ongoing sexual conflict and contrasting sex‐specific plastic responses to contribute to resolution.

We use quantitative genetic analyses and experimental manipulation of the competitive environment (housing density) to provide the first assessment of the cross‐sex genetic architecture for maturation traits in X. birchmanni and to examine sex‐specific plasticity. To investigate cross‐sex genetic architecture, we apply pedigree‐based animal models to characterize the genetic covariance structure within and across sexes for age and size at maturation. We first estimate the magnitude of genetic variation in sex‐specific life‐history traits before formally testing for G×S interactions. If present, G×S interactions generate sex‐specific genetic variance that would facilitate evolution towards divergent phenotypic optima in male and females if sexually antagonistic selection does indeed operate on these traits as thought. If absent, shared genetic architecture will constrain further evolution of sexual dimorphism. To examine sex‐specific plasticity, we combine a density treatment (low vs. high) with analysis of variation in sex ratio among mixed family groups of juveniles raised to maturation. In general, we expect high rearing density to increase social stress arising from competitive interactions, leading to increased age and/or decreased size at maturity in both sexes. However, we anticipate that maturation traits will respond to male–male competition by being elevated in male‐biased groups housed at high density. We therefore predict that, if sex‐specific adaptive plastic responses are possible, then (conditional on main treatment effect of density) males should mature later and at larger size where male–male competition is high. Conversely, females should not delay maturation and may accelerate it (Walling et al., 2007).

Materials and methods

Fish husbandry and phenotyping

In the spring of 2010, one hundred adult Xiphophorus birchmanni (40 male, 60 female) were sampled by minnow trap from the Arroyo Coacuilco river (near Coacuilco, municipality of San Felipe Orizatlán, Hidalgo, Mexico) and imported to the UK (April 2011). They were housed in breeding groups comprising one male: three females, in 30‐L glass aquaria enriched with 3–5 mm diameter gravel and live plants. Water was maintained at 21–23 °C and a 12:12 hour light: dark cycle provided. Fish were fed twice daily on proprietary flake food (ZM foods, http://www.zmsystems.co.uk/) and previously frozen bloodworm and daphnia. Between August 2010 and May 2011, a captive bred generation was produced (n = 384) comprising 61 full‐sibling broods. Mean brood size born was 8.72 with a range from 1 to 24. Note that in some cases, multiple broods were collected from the same parental pair such that full‐sibship sizes represented in the data set are larger (mean = 16.18, range 1–51). Given the group housing regime, full‐sib families are nested within half‐sibships, with a total of 32 female and 19 male parents contributing to the offspring generation.

To collect broods, breeding groups were inspected daily and obviously gravid females were removed to isolation tanks enriched with stones and artificial plants to provide refuge for newborn offspring. Isolated females were also checked daily and returned to their breeding group tanks after giving birth. Broods were initially raised in 30‐L tanks, partitioned into two equal volumes (using an acrylic frame covered with fine‐gauge black nylon net) such that two families were raised in each tank. Tanks were grouped in ‘stacks’, each comprising six 30‐L tanks on a common recirculating water supply. This reduces the potential for between‐tank variation in water quality to introduce bias in genetic parameter estimation. Large broods were divided across tank partitions (setting a maximum of eight offspring per 15 L volume).

At an average age of 16 weeks (range 12–27 weeks), offspring were tagged with visible implant elastomer (http://www.nmt.us/products/vie/vie.shtml) and assigned to mixed family groups (n = 8 fish per group) subject to one of two density treatments. Low‐density groups (L) were housed in a full 30‐L tank, whereas high‐density groups (H) were housed in a half tank (i.e. 15 L volume partition of a 30‐L tank as described above). Six stacks, each comprising four low‐ and four high‐density groups on a recirculating water supply, were sequentially established (Fig. S1). Variation in age of fish entering the experiment was thus unavoidable as a stack could only be set up when 64 fish (eight groups × eight fish per group) reached a size suitable for tagging. Juveniles of this species cannot be sexed from external characters and the sex ratio of mixed family groups was therefore uncontrolled. Fish were fed twice daily with a mixed diet of fresh brine shrimp nauplii and crushed flake from birth until mixing of families and subsequently on the same diet as the wild caught breeding groups (with L and H groups receiving equal ration).

As part of a wider study involving long‐term behavioural and growth phenotyping (see Boulton et al., 2014), all fish in the experiment were measured for standard length (SL) using digital callipers and live weight (WT) using a digital balance at 4 week intervals, for a period of 28 weeks in total. Age of maturation (AM) was recorded for each individual as age at the first sample date where sexing from external morphology was possible. For males, this was when the first thickening of the anal fin rays associated with gonopodium formation became apparent (i.e. following Snelson, 1989). Typically, this is sooner than the development of other secondary male characters such as the nuchal hump, vertical stripes and enlarged and pigmented dorsal fin seen in this species. Female AM was determined from a suite of characters that differentiate juveniles from mature females (abdomen shape, darkening of the ‘gravid spot’ and lateral line). Given the lack of a single objective criterion to discriminate mature females from (unsexable) juveniles, measurement error is likely to be higher for female relative to male traits. However, all designations were made by a single investigator and were blind with respect to pedigree (and previously assigned sex) thus no expectation of bias arises with respect to genetic hypotheses (but see later Discussion). Weight and standard length at maturity (WTM, SLM) were simply defined as the corresponding size measurements at AM. Sex could not be determined for a total of ten fish still alive at the end of the 28‐week density treatment (n = 368 of the starting 384). However, continued monitoring of individuals for purposes out‐with this study (Boulton et al., 2014) meant that maturation trait data were subsequently obtained (one female and nine males).

Analysis and quantitative genetic modelling

Exploratory data analysis was first conducted in R. We used simple linear models (i.e. without random effects) to estimate the relationships between maturation traits and to test for differences in phenotypic means across sex and density treatment classes. We then used a series of animal models fitted using asreml (Version 3; VSN International Ltd, Hemel Hempstead, UK) to formally test hypothesized plastic and genetic influences simultaneously as follows. First, for each sex‐specific maturation trait, we fitted a univariate model with the phenotype (y) of each individual (i) specified as follows:

yi=μ+Stack+Density+GSi+SRi+Density:GS+Density:SR+ai+εi (1)

where μ is the mean, Stack is a seven‐level factor included to account for effects of any variation in water chemistry, and Density is a factor denoting treatment (Low (L) = 8 fish in 30 L, High(H) = 8 fish in 15 L). Group size (GS) and sex ratio (SR) experienced were defined as individual, rather than group‐level covariates. GSi is the geometric mean number of fish in i's group, averaged across the monthly assay points up to and including age of maturity (AMi). Group size (and its interaction with Density) was included to control for any effects of mortality on phenotypes of surviving group mates (although, in practice, mortality levels were low; see Results). SRi was similarly defined as the geometric mean (over assay points up to and including AMi) of the proportion of that individual's tank mates that are mature males [i.e. number of males in group excluding self/(number in group −1)]. Geometric means across assay points were used to define GSi and SRi to better capture cumulative effects of social environment, but both variables were then centred to an (arithmetic) mean of zero across all individuals to aid interpretation of model estimates.

Additive genetic merit (ai) was included as a random effect, assumed to be normally distributed with a mean of zero, and variance (V A, the additive genetic variance) to be estimated using the pedigree structure (Wilson et al., 2010). Residuals (Ɛi) are assumed to be uncorrelated across observations and normally distributed with a mean of zero and variance (V R) to be estimated. Inference on fixed effects was based on conditional Wald F‐tests implemented by ASReml. Significance of V A was determined by likelihood ratio tests (LRT) comparing model fit with and without the additive genetic effect. For testing V A in univariate models, we assume the LRT test statistic is distributed as a 50:50 mix of χ12 and χ02 following (Visscher, 2006). Heritability was estimated as V A/V P with the phenotypic variance (V P) determined as V A+V R (i.e. conditional on fixed effects).

The univariate model was then extended to the multivariate case to estimate the genetic variance–covariance matrix (G) between all six sex‐specific traits (AMF, WTMF, SLMF, AMM, WTMM and SLMM), with additive covariance estimates also rescaled to give the corresponding genetic correlations (r G). Fixed effects on each trait were as specified above. The full estimate of G was used to qualitatively assess the presence of G×S interactions. For more formal inference, we tested these conditions using a series of bivariate model comparisons applied to each homologous trait pair using likelihood ratio tests (Table 1). These comparisons tested for (A) heterogeneity of total phenotypic variance (V P) across sexes and (B) G×S interactions (manifest as r MF < 1 and/or V A(F) ≠ V A(M)). Note that in the absence of G×S, aiF = aiM for any pair of sex‐specific homologous traits (e.g. AMF, AMM), thus it follows that V A(F) = V A(M) and r MF = 1 (the ‘No G×S’ scenario in Table 1). To further explore whether patterns of G×S detected were driven by cross‐sex genetic correlations or heterogeneity of V A we compared (C) the full G×S model to one with freely estimated r MF but with V A(F) constrained to equal V A(M) and (D) the full G×S model to one where V A(F) and V A(M) were free to differ but rMF was constrained to equal +1. Note that for comparisons (B)–(D), all models included heterogeneous residual variance (i.e. V R(F) and V R(M) were free to differ) to prevent differences in environmental variance (or measurement error) generating spurious support for differences in sex‐specific additive variance estimates.

Table 1.

Cross‐sex tests for (A) heterogeneity of phenotypic variance (V P) and (B) G×S interactions. Also presented are comparisons of the full G×S model to restricted scenarios where (C) mf is freely estimated but V A assumed homogeneous and (D) V A(F) are allowed to differ V A(M) but mf is constrained to unity. For each comparison, null (H0) and alternate (H1) hypotheses are shown with statistical inference from likelihood ratio tests

Trait Comparison H0 H1 χ2 d.f. P
AM (A) Homogeneous V P Heterogeneous V P 3.80 1 0.051
(B) No G×S G×S 5.90 2 0.052
(C) G×S r MF = +1, V A assumed homogeneous 0.90 1 0.343
(D) G×S V A heterogeneous, r MF = +1 assumed 1.70 1 0.427
WTM (A) Homogeneous V P Heterogeneous V P 11.9 1 <0.001
(B) No G×S G×S 12.5 2 0.002
(C) G×S r MF = +1, V A assumed homogeneous 0.04 1 0.842
(D) G×S V A heterogeneous, r MF = +1 assumed 7.10 1 0.029
SLM (A) Homogeneous V P Heterogeneous V P 0.222 1 0.638
(B) No G×S G×S 14.9 2 <0.001
(C) G×S r MF = +1, V A assumed homogeneous 0.122 1 0.727
(D) G×S V A heterogeneous, r MF = +1 assumed 9.07 1 0.011

Results

Exploratory data analysis

Size at maturity increases with age at maturity as expected (Fig. 1). Regressions of size at maturity (SLM) on age at maturity (AM) are significantly positive in females (β (SE) = 0.034 (0.004) mm day−1, P < 0.001) and males (β (SE) = 0.019 (0.004) mm day−1, P < 0.001). Pooling data and including SEX (male relative to female) and SEX: AM effects (as well as a main effect of AM) in the linear model confirms that the relationship in females is significantly steeper (SEX: AM coefficient (SE) = −0.0153 (0.006), t = −2.705339, P = 0.007). The two measures of size at maturity are strongly correlated in both sexes (female, r WTM.SLM = 0.953, P < 0.001; male, r WTM.SLM = 0.919, P < 0.001); therefore, regressions of weight at maturity (WTM) on AM yield very similar patterns (results not shown). In addition to having steeper regressions of size on AM (Fig. 1), estimated correlations are stronger in females [r AM.WTM = 0.554 (0.057), r AM.SLM = 0.570 (0.056)] than in males [r AM.WTM = 0.223 (0.068), r AM.SLM = 0.350 (0.063)]. Testing against a null model of equal sex‐specific correlations indicates this difference is statistically significant for r AM.WTM (χ12 = 13.1, P < 0.001) and rAM.SLM (χ12 = 6.62, P = 0.010). While suggesting a degree of decoupling of size and age of maturity in males relative to females, we note that this result could also be driven by measurement bias (e.g. if body size unintentionally influences scoring of maturity status in females).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Observed size (standard length) at maturation as a function of age by sex. Circles denote phenotypic observations (n F = 148, n M = 195) whereas solid lines illustrate predictions from simple linear regressions with shaded areas denoting predicted mean ± SE.

Comparison of trait means across sexes and treatment classes shows all traits to be sexually dimorphic but provides little evidence for plastic responses to the density treatment (Fig. 2). Note that as maturity status is assessed from external morphology using sex‐specific criteria here, we cannot be certain whether similar patterns would be found using physiological assays of maturation. However, based on criteria used, males mature on average 23.7 (5.60) days later than females (t = 4.22341, P < 0.001), at 0.248 (0.035) g heavier (t = 7.031341, P < 0.001) and at 2.99 (0.314) mm longer (t = 9.545341, P < 0.001; results from linear models with sex as categorical predictor). Fish tended to mature later and at smaller size at high density in both sexes but effects were largely nonsignificant. Overall mean AM is significantly higher at high density (linear model with Density as categorical predictor; difference of +12.4 (5.65) days, t = 2.196341, P = 0.029). Statistical support for this effect is not robust to the addition of the sex effect into the linear model although the effect size is similar (effect of high density = +10.4 (5.55) days, t = 1.878340, P = 0.061).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Mean observed (a) maturation age (AM), (b) weight (MWT) and (c) standard length (MSL) by sex for low‐ and high‐density treatments. White columns denote low (L)‐density treatment and shaded columns indicate high (H)‐density treatment for male (M) and female (F) values with bars showing mean ± SE (n FL = 83, n ML = 92, and n FH = 65, n MH = 103).

Genetic variation and G×S interactions

Univariate animal models also provided evidence of genetic variation for maturation traits (Table 2). Heritability estimates from univariate models range from 0.113 to 0.462 and are significant at α = 0.05 except for male AM (h2 = 0.113 (0.112), χ0,12 = 2.11, P = 0.073). The corresponding estimates from the full (six trait) multivariate model are similar, although slightly higher (ranging from 0.166 to 0.477; Table 3). In general, genetic correlation estimates are characterized by high uncertainty, although the strong positive estimates between WTMF and SLMF, and WTMM and SLMM are nominally significant based on |r G| > 1.96*SE. Of particular note are the cross‐sex (within‐trait) genetic correlation estimates (r MF) of 0.066, −0.291 and −0.108 for AM, WTM and SLM, respectively (Table 3). Thus, not only are cross‐sex genetic correlations not close to +1 (the expected value in the absence of G×S), but for size at maturity traits, they are actually negative (albeit not significantly less than zero).

Table 2.

Estimated variance components and heritabilities (h2) from univariate animal models for age (AM), weight (WTM) and size (SLM) at maturity where V P, V A and V R are the phenotypic, additive genetic and residual variances, respectively

Sex Trait V P (SE) V A (SE) V R (SE) h2 χ2 P
Female AM 1313 (185) 543 (311) 770 (229) 0.413 (0.202) 9.77 0.001
WTM 0.067 (0.009) 0.024 (0.015) 0.043 (0.012) 0.360 (0.198) 6.20 0.006
SLM 7.28 (1.05) 3.36 (1.83) 3.92 (1.30) 0.462 (0.208) 9.59 0.001
Male AM 1764 (189) 200 (204) 1564 (227) 0.113 (0.112) 2.11 0.073
WTM 0.115 (0.013) 0.028 (0.018) 0.087 (0.016) 0.242 (0.145) 7.40 0.003
SLM 7.74 (0.931) 2.43 (1.45) 5.31 (1.11) 0.314 (0.166) 8.56 0.002

Also presented are likelihood ratio tests of V A with the test statistics assumed to be distributed as a 50:50 mix of χ2 on 1 and 0 degrees of freedom are indicated. Standard errors are indicated in parentheses.

Table 3.

Estimated heritabilities (shaded column) and genetic variance–covariance–correlation (G matrix) containing additive genetic variances (V A, shaded diagonal), covariances (covA, below diagonal) and correlations (r G, above diagonal), all with standard errors indicated in parentheses. All parameter estimates are from a multivariate (six trait) model and are conditional on fixed effects fitted as described in main text

Heritability G matrix
AMF WTMF SLMF AMM WTMM SLMM
AMF 0.477 (0.207) 643 (341) 0.410 (0.330) 0.361 (0.318) 0.066 (0.488) −0.137 (0.406) −0.378 (0.347)
WTMF 0.368 (0.202) 1.63 (1.79) 0.020 (0.020) 0.987 (0.025) −0.410 (0.497) −0.291 (0.436) −0.084 (0.411)
SLMF 0.460 (0.208) 16.7 (19.3) 0.282 (0.165) 3.33 (1.82) −0.526 (0.438) −0.304 (0.394) −0.108 (0.381)
AMM 0.166 (0.132) 28.8 (212) −1.10 (1.43) −16.5 (15.5) 294 (247) −0.027 (0.518) −0.029 (0.499)
WTMM 0.274 (0.154) −0.619 (1.86) −0.008 (0.012) −0.099 (0.134) 0.083 (1.58) 0.032 (0.020) 0.892 (0.087)
SLMM 0.336 (0.165) −15.5 (16.0) −0.021 (0.104) −0.319 (1.13) −0.796 (13.7) 0.262 (0.147) 2.62 (1.48)

More formal comparison of bivariate (cross‐sex models) indicated that the null hypothesis of homogeneity in total phenotypic variance could be rejected for WTM (comparison (A) in Table 1). Statistical support for heterogeneous V P in AM was marginally nonsignificant. In both cases, phenotypic variance conditional on fixed effects is higher in males (as is also qualitatively the case for SLM). The full G×S model (allowing r MF < +1 and V A(F) ≠ V A(M)) was significantly better than the null model for WTM and SLM (comparison (B) in Table 1) although marginally nonsignificant for AM (χ22 = 5.90, P = 0.052). The full G×S model was not significantly better than the more restricted formulation where rMF was free but homogeneity of V A imposed in any case (comparison (C) in Table 1) for any trait. However, it was preferred in comparison (D) for WTM and SLM. We therefore conclude that, for WTM and SLM, there is evidence for significant genotype‐by‐sex interactions driven primarily by cross‐sex genetic correlations of <1 (rather than heterogeneity of sex‐specific genetic variances). For AM [where r MF = 0.066 (0.488)], statistical support for G×S is slightly more equivocal since, as noted above, the overall test for G×S was marginally nonsignificant. However, post hoc comparison between a model with no G×S (such that V A(F) = V A(M) and r MF = ±1) and one where rMF was allowed to depart from unity (with homogeneity of V A imposed) suggests the latter is a significantly better fit to the data +1 (χ12 = 5.00, P = 0.025; comparison not shown in Table 1).

A graphical representation of these G×S interactions is illustrated in Fig. 3, with the red line denoting the 95% confidence interval for the null distribution of bivariate breeding values (estimated assuming aF = aM such that V A(F) = V A(M) and r MF = +1). In all cases, this line is a very poor fit to the distribution of bivariate breeding values estimated under the unconstrained model allowing G×S, represented by the grey ellipse.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Cross‐sex genetic covariance structures for (a) maturation age (AM), (b) size at maturity (SLM) and (c) weight at maturity (WTM). Shaded ellipses denote the 95% confidence interval for the distribution of (bivariate) genetic merits in the population, and solid points indicate BLUP for individuals in the data (both from the full multivariate model). For comparison, red lines indicate the distribution of breeding values estimated under the assumption of no G×S.

Animal model‐based estimates of phenotypic plasticity

Univariate animal models of sex‐specific traits confirmed the finding from our exploratory analysis that plastic responses to density were limited (Table 4). In males, a significant density by group size (GS) interaction was found on age of maturation (AM). The positive sign of this coefficient implies that the effect of higher GS (a significant reduction in male maturation age) is less strong at high density than at low (Table 4). For females, higher GS was associated with later maturation. No other effects of the density treatment were detected while GS did not significantly influence maturation size traits in either sex. Plastic responses to sex ratio variation were detected in both sexes (Table 4). For focal males, the presence of mature male group mates results in later maturity at larger size. For focal females, AM also increases with sex ratio (SR), but no significant effects on size at maturity were detected. Significant (or marginally nonsignificant) stack effects were found on all traits except male SLM (Table 4). These likely reflect average plasticity in response to between‐stack variation in water conditions and/or uncontrolled temporal patterns in the laboratory environment (as stacks were set up sequentially; see Materials and methods). As they are not relevant to hypotheses being tested here, we do not discuss these further.

Table 4.

Fixed effect results from univariate animal models for age (AM), weight (WTM) and size at maturity (SLM), where GS is group size and SR is sex ratio

Trait Effect (Level) Female Male
Coefficient (SE) d.f. F P Coefficient (SE) d.f. F P
AM μ 236 (8.89) 1,14.2 1511 <0.001 267 (9.92) 1,11 3191 <0.001
Stack (B) −33.0 (10.3) 5,124.6 8.10 <0.001 −31.3 (12.3) 5,120 4.65 <0.001
(D) −57.4 (10.0) −40.2 (11.8)
(E) −59.3 (11.4) −34.9 (11.9)
(F) −51.8 (13.3) −57.0 (12.4)
(G) −34.2 (11.5) −37.4 (11.5)
Density (High) 8.87 (6.74) 1,126.2 0.77 0.384 −8.89 (6.14) 1,175.1 2.19 0.144
GS 21.5 (57.3) 1,132.1 5.15 0.026 −159 (29.1) 1,183.6 40.9 <0.001
SR 70.0 (21.9) 1,135.9 18.5 <0.001 99.7 (21.6) 1,182.4 44.7 <0.001
Density:GS −84.7 (61.0) 1,120.3 1.93 0.170 87.7 (32.5) 1,176.8 7.30 0.008
Density:SR 39.9 (37.0) 1,131.4 1.16 0.285 11.2 (27.3) 1,181.7 0.17 0.676
WTM μ 1.08 (0.063) 1,13.3 846 <0.001 1.46 (0.082) 1,13.8 1149 <0.001
Stack (B) 0.093 (0.074) 5,121.8 2.25 0.054 0.020 (0.101) 5,134.2 3.95 0.002
(D) −0.067 (0.072) 0.002 (0.097)
(E) −0.099 (0.082) −0.042 (0.098)
(F) 0.147 (0.095) −0.218 (0.102)
(G) 0.048 (0.083) −0.316 (0.094)
Density (High) −0.029 (0.045) 1,126.6 0.39 0.533 −0.087 (0.048) 1,173.3 3.18 0.078
GS −0.186 (0.416) 1,132.6 0.78 0.380 0.285 (0.230) 1,183 1.10 0.298
SR 0.134 (0.158) 1,136.1 0.93 0.338 0.636 (0.170) 1,179.6 22.3 <0.001
Density:GS 0.048 (0.443) 1,120.8 0.01 0.908 −0.214 (0.255) 1,176 0.70 0.403
Density:SR −0.005 (0.268) 1,132.1 0.00 0.983 −0.088 (0.216) 1,178.6 0.17 0.678
SLM μ 34.3 (0.664) 1,14.6 7039 <0.001 37.7 (0.678) 1,13.8 11733 <0.001
Stack (B) 0.691 (0.765) 5,126.5 2.23 0.056 0.566 (0.838) 5,137.5 1.29 0.272
(D) −1.04 (0.746) 0.109 (0.802)
(E) −0.949 (0.842) −0.151 (0.806)
(F) 1.24 (0.981) −1.10 (0.844)
(G) 0.883 (0.852) −1.14 (0.770)
Density (High) −0.336 (0.495) 1,125.4 0.75 0.388 −0.668 (0.387) 1,171.6 2.94 0.091
GS −0.593 (4.21) 1,131.4 0.65 0.421 2.23 (1.85) 1,181.3 0.11 0.731
SR 2.20 (1.61) 1,135.5 2.87 0.095 5.41 (1.37) 1,177.6 26.0 <0.001
Density:GS −0.875 (4.48) 1,119.4 0.04 0.839 −2.45 (2.05) 1,174.9 1.42 0.237
Density:SR 0.594 (2.72) 1,130.5 0.05 0.821 −0.531 (1.73) 1,176.4 0.09 0.753

Discussion

Our results demonstrate that different genetic mechanisms underlie variation in age and size at maturity in males and females. The genetic architecture therefore acts to mitigate intersexual conflict over these traits. This finding is consistent with previous studies in swordtails that show a small number of loci on the Y chromosome can be responsible for much of the among‐male variation in these traits. Sexual dimorphism is evident in all traits, with males tending to mature later and at larger size (based on our assessment of maturity status), and in the relationship between age and size at maturity. Although positively correlated in both sexes (as expected from studies of other fish including poeciliids (Snelson, 1984; Rowe & Thorpe, 1990; Morita & Fukuwaka, 2006), AM explains more variation in maturation size traits for females than males. Regressions of size traits on AM also show that absolute juvenile growth is faster in females. However, despite this sexual dimorphism in mean phenotype, we found only limited support for putatively adaptive sex‐specific plastic responses in maturation traits to competition (i.e. density, sex ratio). In what follows, we first highlight the evolutionary implications of the G×S effects found before considering the results pertaining to plasticity in more detail.

Genetic (co)variance structure and G×S interactions

Quantitative genetic models revealed the presence of both genetic (co)variation and significant genotype‐by‐sex interactions. Thus, not only are life histories free to evolve, but also to the extent that natural selection acts antagonistically in the wild, there is also potential for adaptive evolution of increased sexual dimorphism. Estimates of heritability were lower in males than in homologous female traits, a pattern driven by higher levels of residual variation rather than differences in additive genetic variance (V A; discussed further below). Competition in general, and contest competition in particular, is expected to increase variance in resource dependent traits, as winners gain resource at the expense of losers (Wilson, 2014). Thus, higher residual variance in male traits is consistent with the well‐documented importance of male–male competition in swordtails (Earley, 2006), including X. birchmanni (Wilson et al., 2013). Within‐sex genetic correlations (r G) between SLM and WTM were close to +1 but interestingly we did not find strong genetic correlations between these traits and age at maturity. In females, moderate positive (but nonsignificant) estimates of r G were found between traits, whereas in males, these estimates were close to zero. Within both sexes, estimates of r G between traits were characterized by high uncertainty and therefore should be interpreted cautiously. Nonetheless, while there is perhaps some suggestion that the tighter (positive) phenotypic correlation between age and size of maturity in females relative to males is mirrored at the genetic level, there is no strong evidence for a genetic basis to the widely assumed fitness trade‐off between age and size of maturity in either sex (Stearns, 1992; Roff, 2002; Kruuk et al., 2008).

Formal testing for G×S interactions provided evidence that the genetic basis of life‐history variation differs between males and females. While V A for homologous traits did not differ significantly between the sexes as has been reported elsewhere (Wyman & Rowe, 2014), estimated cross‐sex genetic correlations were close to zero, in contrast to the vast majority of empirical studies of r MF. These low genetic correlations between homologous traits in males and females imply a considerable degree of genetic decoupling. Consequently, additive variance can be considered largely sex‐specific and shared genetic architecture is not expected to be an important constraint on adaptive dimorphism if males and females are subject to antagonistic selection. While our experiment does not provide any information on the detail of this genetic architecture, low estimates of r MF will arise from sex‐linkage and/or sex‐limited expression of autosomal genes. Both phenomena represent evolutionary solutions to the problem of sexual antagonism (Charlesworth & Charlesworth, 1980) that are known to affect expression of size, growth, colouration and behavioural traits in poeciliids (Lindholm & Breden, 2002; Postma et al., 2011). Y‐linked variation with allelic effect sizes sufficiently large to induce phenotypically distinct male morphs are known in some Xiphophorus species (Schreibman & Kallman, 1977; Ryan et al., 1992; Cummings & Gelineau‐Kattner, 2009). For instance, in X. nigrensis and X. multilineatus membership of one of three adult size morphs is strongly predicted by copy number variation of the melanocortin 4 receptor (mc4r) gene on the Y chromosome (Lampert et al., 2010). Although there is no evidence of distinct male size morphs, in X. birchmanni, we note that higher allelic copy numbers (and or variation in copy number) could easily rise to unimodal phenotypic distributions. Thus, we consider mc4r, a good candidate for contributing to the male‐specific genetic variance found here, although this remains to be tested.

Our conclusions with respect to the quantitative genetics of male and female life‐history traits are contingent on several potentially important caveats. Firstly, parameters are estimated under an additive model and assume absence of maternal and/or other early life common environmental effects (as offspring were raised in families until large enough to tag and mix). Maternal effects on offspring traits are known to occur in poeciliid fishes, including X. birchmanni (Reznick et al., 1996; Kindsvater et al., 2012), although the extent of their persistence to impact adult traits is variable (Lindholm et al., 2006). Here, the failure of some wild caught adults to reproduce under laboratory conditions meant the size and structure (i.e. limited half‐sib structuring) of our progeny data set is not sufficient to effectively disentangle any maternal effects. While upward bias of additive genetic parameters is certainly possible (Falconer & Mackay, 1996), no systematic bias towards finding reduced rMF is expected. Secondly, although we have considered both age and size of maturation in this study, genetic covariance structure may well exist with other components of life history (e.g. fecundity, adult growth and longevity) within and between sexes. Multivariate analysis can identify constraints not apparent from pairwise genetic correlations alone (Walsh & Blows, 2009), although we note the converse is also true. Specifically, it has recently been argued that multivariate treatments of sexual dimorphism may actually reveal greater evolutionary potential for dimorphism than previously thought based on r MF estimates (Wyman et al., 2013); see also (Gosden & Chenoweth, 2014; Walling et al., 2014) for related discussion. Thirdly, we have implicitly assumed an absence of GxE such that genetic covariance is modelled as being constant with social environment (i.e. density and/or sex ratio). Available data were insufficient to support modelling of life‐history traits disaggregated by both sex and environment, and thus, our genetic estimates should be viewed as averaged across any genotype‐by‐environment (GxE) effects present. GxE can be of considerable importance for sexually selected traits (Hunt & Hosken, 2014), although explicit studies of GxExS are currently lacking. Despite the formidable empirical challenges, we suggest that experiments to address this gap in our knowledge could offer great insights into the evolution of sexual dimorphism. This is because GxExS implies the presence of sex‐limited genetic variance, and thus potential for independent evolution, not just of male and female traits but also of male and female plasticity in those traits.

Sex specificity of social plasticity

There was evidence of some social plasticity in both sexes, although life‐history traits were influenced more by sex ratio variation (SR) than by the experimentally applied density treatment. Broadly, responses are consistent with predictions made under the presumption that high density increases competition and that for males, this is exacerbated by a high sex ratio (i.e. the presence of more mature rivals). However, while both sexes show a similar trend towards later maturation at smaller size under high density, effects were small and not statistically supported in the mixed models. Nevertheless, to the extent that plastic responses to the treatment are occurring, the trend is consistent with negative density dependence on life history with respect to expected fitness consequences. Although we also found a significant positive effect of group size (GS) on female maturation age (consistent with density dependence), in males, the corresponding effect was actually negative (though less so at high density). These latter results are difficult to interpret as GS effects were modelled to control for within‐group mortality rather than to test a priori hypotheses. It is possible that agonistic interactions between males that have already matured within‐group increase their mortality risk, and thus, lower GS may indicate that competition has been intense for males. However, as only 16 of 384 fish died before the end of the experiment variation in GS is very low (and nonrandom with respect to groups). We therefore consider it quite possible that this result is an artefact arising from data structure.

The direct effects of density on life history were thus limited and also similar in males and females. However, we also predicted plasticity in response to sex ratio variation and sex differences in this response, with males responding to a greater extent at high density. Perhaps unsurprisingly given the lack of main effects, we found no significant interactions between density and sex ratio on either male or female life history to support the second of these predictions. Nevertheless, both male and female traits did respond to sex ratio, albeit in similar directions. Maturation occurs later and at larger size in the presence of more adult males in both sexes. This is consistent with our predictions of adaptive plasticity for males. Increased male–male competition results in sexual selection that favours larger maturing males in swordtails, even if this comes at the cost of a delayed maturation time (Basolo, 1988; Beaugrand et al., 1996; Benson & Basolo, 2006).

Delayed female maturation in male‐biased groups is counter to our expectations. With increasing numbers of mature males, we predicted females should not delay maturation and may even advance it. This prediction was based on an assumption that more available males would increase the fitness benefits of early maturation. It is possible that mature males were socially dominant to females and thus able to monopolize resources (e.g. food) in the experimental conditions. If so, delayed female maturation may be a consequence of resource limitation. Harassment by males could also be a factor as it is energetically costly for both sexes and can disrupt female social structures (Darden et al., 2009; Darden & Watts, 2012). Given the lack of fitness data and the presence of artificial conditions, we cannot completely exclude the possibility that this response confers some potential benefits under natural conditions. Nonetheless, while SR effects on female WTM and SLM are positive, they are modest and not significant. Consequently, it seems unlikely that delayed maturation in females can be compensated for by size‐related increases in fecundity later. Whatever the explanation, our results suggest that males and females tend towards later and larger maturity in the presence of mature male group mates, the response being larger in males, where we predicted it to be adaptive.

Conclusions

In summary, our study sought to test for sex‐specific genetic and social environment effects on age and size of maturity in the sheepshead swordtail. We found that these traits are sexually dimorphic and responsive to social factors expected to determine the intensity of competition. At high density, there was a general tendency towards maturing later and at smaller size in both sexes. Although generally consistent with expected non‐sex ‐specific density dependence, these effects were modest and nonsignificant. Moreover, males were also found to delay maturation in the presence of mature rivals, a putatively adaptive response given that this results in larger adult size (and thus higher success in male–male competition). Interestingly, females showed a similar pattern (albeit with smaller phenotypic changes) and delayed maturation in response to increased sex ratio. This is contrary to adaptive predictions, suggesting that a lack of sex‐specific plasticity could limit the expression of (adaptive) sexual dimorphism in social environments where male–male competition is high. Conversely, our quantitative genetic analyses illustrate that life‐history traits are subject to G×S interactions – age and size at maturity are heritable in both sexes but the cross‐sex genetic correlations between homologous traits are close to zero (and significantly less than +1). Thus, to the extent that natural selection on maturation traits does act antagonistically in the wild, our results show that the genetic architecture of homologous traits can ease a major constraint on the evolution of adaptive dimorphism.

Supporting information

Figure S1 Schematic of recirculating stack system used to house fish.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by an EPSRC Studentship to KB and a BBSRC David Phillips Fellowship to AJW. CAW was funded by a NERC Junior Research Fellowship. The federal government of Mexico provided permission to collect fish. GGR was funded by the U.S. National Science Foundation. We are grateful to Dan Nussey, Mark Briffa and John Hunt for useful discussions that contributed to the development of the manuscript.

References

  1. Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton. [Google Scholar]
  2. Basolo, A.L. 1988. Female preference for body size, sword length and sword colour in the swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri . Am. Zool. 28: A152–A152. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bateman, A.J. 1948. Intra‐sexual selection in Drosophila . Heredity 2: 349–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beaugrand, J.P. , Payette, D. & Goulet, C. 1996. Conflict outcome in male green swordtail fish dyads (Xiphophorus helleri): interaction of body size, prior dominance/subordination experience, and prior residency. Behaviour 133: 303–319. [Google Scholar]
  5. Benson, K.E. & Basolo, A.L. 2006. Male‐male competition and the sword in male swordtails, Xiphophorus helleri . Anim. Behav. 71: 129–134. [Google Scholar]
  6. Borowsky, R.L. 1973. Social control of adult size in males of Xiphophorus variatus . Nature 245: 332–335. [Google Scholar]
  7. Boulton, K. , Grimmer, A.J. , Rosenthal, G.G. , Walling, C.A. & Wilson, A.J. 2014. How stable are personalities? A multivariate view of behavioural variation over long and short timescales in the sheepshead swordtail, Xiphophorus birchmanni . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 68: 791–803. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brommer, J.E. , Kirkpatrick, M. , Qvarnstrom, A. & Gustafsson, L. 2007. The intersexual genetic correlation for lifetime fitness in the wild and its implications for sexual selection. PLoS ONE 2: e744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Campton, D.E. & Gall, G.A.E. 1988. Effect of individual and group rearing on age and size at maturity of male Mosquitofish. Gambusia affinis . J. Fish Biol. 33: 203–212. [Google Scholar]
  10. Charlesworth, D. & Charlesworth, B. 1980. Sex differences in fitness and selesction for centric fusions between sex‐chromosomes and autosomes. Genet. Res. 35: 205–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cox, R.M. & Calsbeek, R. 2009. Sexually antagonistic selection, sexual dimorphism, and the resolution of intralocus sexual conflict. Am. Nat. 173: 176–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cummings, M.E. & Gelineau‐Kattner, R. 2009. The energetic costs of alternative male reproductive strategies in Xiphophorus nigrensis . J. Comp. Physiol. A. 195: 935–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Darden, S.K. & Watts, L. 2012. Male sexual harassment alters female social behaviour towards other females. Biol. Lett. 8: 186–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Darden, S.K. , James, R. , Ramnarine, I.W. & Croft, D.P. 2009. Social implications of the battle of the sexes: sexual harassment disrupts female sociality and social recognition. P. R. Soc. B. 276: 2651–2656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Earley, R. 2006. Xiphophorus: carving a niche towards a broader understanding of aggression and dominance. Zebrafish 3: 283–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Evans, J.P. , Pialstro, A. & Schlupp, I. 2011. Ecology and Evolution of Poecillid Fishes. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fairbairn, D.J. & Roff, D.A. 2006. The quantitative genetics of sexual dimorphism: assessing the importance of sex‐linkage. Heredity 97: 319–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Falconer, D.S. & Mackay, T.F.C. 1996. Introduction to Quantitative Genetics, 4th edn Pearson Education Ltd, Harlow, Essex, UK. [Google Scholar]
  19. Gosden, T.P. & Chenoweth, S.F. 2014. The evolutionary stability of cross‐sex, cross‐trait genetic covariances. Evolution 68: 1687–1697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hallsson, L.R. & Bjorklund, M. 2012. Selection in a fluctuating environment and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in the seed beetle Callosobruchus maculatus . J. Evol. Biol. 25: 1564–1575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. (Hunt J. & Hosken D.J., eds) 2014. Genotype‐by‐Environment Interactions and Sexual Selection. Wiley Blackwell, Chichester. [Google Scholar]
  22. Kallman, K.D. 1983. The sex determining mechanism of the poeciliid fish, Xiphophorus montezumae, and the genetic control of the sexual maturation process and adult size. Copeia 1983: 755–769. [Google Scholar]
  23. Kindsvater, H.K. , Rosenthal, G.G. & Alonzo, S.H. 2012. Maternal size and age shape offspring size in a live‐bearing fish, Xiphophorus birchmanni . PLoS ONE 7: e48473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kruuk, L.E.B. , Slate, J. & Wilson, A.J. 2008. New answers for old questions: the evolutionary quantitative genetics of wild animal populations. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 39: 525–548. [Google Scholar]
  25. Lampert, K.P. , Schmidt, C. , Fischer, P. , Volff, J.N. , Hoffmann, C. , Muck, J. et al 2010. Determination of onset of sexual maturation and mating behavior by melanocortin receptor 4 polymorphisms. Curr. Biol. 20: 1729–1734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lande, R. 1980. Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection, and adaptation in polygenic characters. Evolution 34: 292–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lechner, P. & Radda, A.C. 1987. Revision des Xiphophorus montezumae/cortezi ‐ komplexes und neubeschreibung einer subspezies. Aquaria: Vivaristische Fachzeitschrift für die Schweiz und Österreich 34 : 189–196.
  28. Lindholm, A. & Breden, F. 2002. Sex chromosomes and sexual selection in poeciliid fishes. Am. Nat. 160: S214–S224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lindholm, A.K. , Hunt, J. & Brooks, R. 2006. Where do all the maternal effects go? Variation in offspring body size through ontogeny in the live‐bearing fish Poecilia parae . Biol. Lett. 2: 586–589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lyon, B.E. & Montgomerie, R.D. 1986. Delayed plumage maturation in passerine birds: reliable signalling by subordinate males? Evolution 40: 605–615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Morita, K. & Fukuwaka, M. 2006. Does size matter most? The effect of growth history on probabilistic reaction norm for salmon maturation. Evolution 60: 1516–1521. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Pavitt, A.T. , Walling, C.A. , Pemberton, J.M. & Kruuk, L.E.B. 2014. Heritability and cross‐sex genetic correlations of early‐life circulating testosterone levels in a wild mammal. Biol. Lett. 10: doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0685. [Epub ahead of print]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Poissant, J. , Wilson, A.J. & Coltman, D.W. 2010. Sex‐specific genetic variance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism: a systematic review of cross‐sex genetic correlations. Evolution 64: 97–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Postma, E. , Spyrou, N. , Rollins, L.A. & Brooks, R.C. 2011. Sex‐dependent selection differentially shapes genetic variation on and off the guppy Y chromosome. Evolution 65: 2145–2156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Prenter, J. , Taylor, P.W. & Elwood, R.W. 2008. Large body size for winning and large swords for winning quickly in swordtail males, Xiphophorus helleri . Anim. Behav. 75: 1981–1987. [Google Scholar]
  36. Reznick, D. , Callahan, H. & Llauredo, R. 1996. Maternal effects on offspring quality in poeciliid fishes. Am. Zool. 36: 147–156. [Google Scholar]
  37. Roff, D.A. 2002. Life History Evolution. Sinauer Associates, Ltd, Sunderland. [Google Scholar]
  38. Rowe, D.K. & Thorpe, J.E. 1990. Suppression of maturation in male Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L) parr by reduction in feeding and growth during spring months. Aquaculture 86: 291–313. [Google Scholar]
  39. Ryan, M.J. , Pease, C.M. & Morris, M.R. 1992. A genetic polymorphism in the swordtail Xiphophorous nigrensis – testing the prediction of equal fitnesses. Am. Nat. 139: 21–31. [Google Scholar]
  40. Schreibman, M.P. & Kallman, K.D. 1977. The genetic control of the pituitary‐gonadal axis in platyfish, Xiphophorous maculatus . J. Exp. Zool. 200: 277–293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Snelson, F.F. 1984. Seasonal maturation and growth of males in a natural population of Poecilia latipinna . Copeia 1984: 252–255. [Google Scholar]
  42. Snelson, F.F.J. 1989. Social and environmental control of life‐history traits in poeciliid fishes In: Ecology and Evolution of Livebearing Fishes (Poecillidae) (Meffe G.K. & Snelson F.F., eds), pp. 149–161. Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey. [Google Scholar]
  43. Stearns, S.C. 1992. The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford University Press, Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  44. Stillwell, R.C. , Blanckenhorn, W.U. , Teder, T. , Davidowitz, G. & Fox, C.W. 2010. Sex differences in phenotypic plasticity affect variation in sexual size dimorphism in insects: from physiology to evolution. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 55: 227–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Studd, M.V. & Robertson, R.J. 1985. Life span, competition, and delayed plumage maturation in malse passerines: the breeding threshold hypothesis. Am. Nat. 126: 101–115. [Google Scholar]
  46. Visscher, P.M. 2006. A note on the asymptotic distribution of likelihood ratio tests to test variance components. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 9: 490–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Walling, C.A. , Royle, N.J. , Metcalfe, N.B. & Lindstrom, J. 2007. Green swordtails alter their age at maturation in response to the population level of male ornamentation. Biol. Let. 3: 144–146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Walling, C.A. , Morrisey, M. , Foerster, K. , Clutton‐brock, T. , Pemberton, J.M. & Kruuk, L.E.B. 2014. A multivariate analysis of genetic constraints to life history evolution in a wild population of red deer. Genetics 198: 1735–1749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Walsh, B. & Blows, M.W. 2009. Abundant genetic variation plus strong selection = multivariate genetic constraints: a geometric view of adaptation. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 40: 41–59. [Google Scholar]
  50. Wilson, A.J. 2014. Competition as a source of constraint on life history evolution in natural populations. Heredity 112: 70–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wilson, A.J. , Réale, D. , Clements, M.N. , Morrissey, M.M. , Postma, E. , Walling, C.A. et al 2010. An ecologist's guide to the animal model. J. Anim. Ecol. 79: 13–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wilson, A.J. , Grimmer, A. & Rosenthal, G.G. 2013. Causes and consequences of contest outcome: aggressiveness, dominance and growth in the sheepshead swordtail, Xiphophorus birchmanni . Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67: 1151–1161. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wyman, M.J. & Rowe, L. 2014. Male bias in distributions of additive genetic, residual and phenotypic variances of shared traits. Am. Nat. 184: 326–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wyman, M.J. , Stinchcombe, J.R. & Rowe, L. 2013. A multivariate view of the evolution of sexual dimorphism. J. Evol. Biol. 26: 2070–2080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure S1 Schematic of recirculating stack system used to house fish.


Articles from Journal of Evolutionary Biology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES