Abstract
Objective:
We investigated differences in regional cortical thickness between previously identified empirically derived mild cognitive impairment (MCI) subtypes (amnestic MCI, dysnomic MCI, dysexecutive/mixed MCI, and cluster-derived normal) in order to determine whether these cognitive subtypes would show different patterns of cortical atrophy.
Methods:
Participants were 485 individuals diagnosed with MCI and 178 cognitively normal individuals from the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Cortical thickness estimates were computed for 32 regions of interest per hemisphere. Statistical group maps compared each MCI subtype to cognitively normal participants and to one another.
Results:
The pattern of cortical thinning observed in each MCI subtype corresponded to their cognitive profile. No differences in cortical thickness were found between the cluster-derived normal MCI subtype and the cognitively normal group. Direct comparison between MCI subtypes suggested that the cortical thickness patterns reflect increasing disease severity.
Conclusions:
There is an ordered pattern of cortical atrophy among patients with MCI that coincides with their profiles of increasing cognitive dysfunction. This heterogeneity is not captured when patients are grouped by conventional diagnostic criteria. Results in the cluster-derived normal group further support the premise that the conventional MCI diagnostic criteria are highly susceptible to false-positive diagnostic errors. Findings suggest a need to (1) improve the diagnostic criteria by reducing reliance on conventional screening measures, rating scales, and a single memory measure in order to avoid false-positive errors; and (2) divide MCI samples into meaningful subgroups based on cognitive and biomarkers profiles—a method that may provide better staging of MCI and inform prognosis.
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI), a prodromal state between normal aging and dementia,1 is typically divided into amnestic or nonamnestic subtypes, with single-domain or multi-domain distinctions.2,3 A limitation of this conventional classification system is that it combines patients with very different cognitive profiles. Previous research has identified considerable heterogeneity beyond the amnestic/nonamnestic distinction with respect to neuropsychological performance in MCI samples.4–7 In a recent study,7 cluster analysis was performed on 825 individuals with MCI from the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI). Although nearly all participants (99.8%) were originally classified simply as amnestic MCI by conventional criteria,2,8 results showed 4 cognitive phenotypes. This included 3 impaired subtypes: amnestic MCI (34.9%), dysnomic MCI (18.5%), and dysexecutive/mixed MCI (12.5%). A large fourth cluster (34.2%) was characterized by intact neuropsychological performance. This cluster-derived normal (CDN) group performed within normal limits on all neuropsychological measures examined, despite other performances on screening measures that led to their MCI diagnosis. The existence of this group, which comprises over one-third of the ADNI MCI cohort, indicates that the conventional diagnostic criteria for MCI may be highly susceptible to false-positive errors, especially in light of this group's normal CSF Alzheimer disease (AD) biomarker profiles7 and normal amyloid imaging scans using florbetapir PET.9 In addition, longitudinal data over an average of roughly 2 years showed that the CDN group demonstrated a lower rate of progression to AD (10.7%) and a higher rate of reversion to cognitively normal (9.2%) relative to the impaired MCI groups (progression rate 35%–56%; reversion rate 1%–2%).7
It has been proposed that individuals with nonamnestic subtypes of MCI will progress to non-AD forms of dementia, such as vascular dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies, or frontotemporal dementia.2 In the MCI subtypes identified in the ADNI sample,7 all 3 impaired subtypes demonstrated memory impairment (i.e., there was no nonamnestic group). Of those who progressed to dementia, nearly all received a diagnosis of AD as opposed to another form of dementia. Thus, the amnestic MCI, dysnomic MCI, and dysexecutive/mixed MCI subtypes may represent different stages rather than distinct disease trajectories. Although interest in characterizing MCI subtypes continues to increase, whether ordered patterns of cortical atrophy exist across these cognitive subtypes has not been established. The current study aimed to investigate regional cortical thickness patterns among the empirically derived MCI subtypes.7
METHODS
Data were obtained from the ADNI database (adni.loni.usc.edu). ADNI is the result of efforts of many coinvestigators from a range of academic institutions and private corporations. Further information about ADNI is available in the e-Methods at Neurology.org or at adni-info.org.
Standard protocol approvals, registrations, and patient consents.
The ADNI study was approved by an ethical standards committee on human experimentation at each institution. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants or authorized representatives.
Participants.
Participants included all ADNI MCI (n = 825) and normal controls (NC; n = 284) from our original sample.7 The NC group contained participants who remained classified as cognitively normal for the duration of their participation in the ADNI study (range of 1–7 years of follow-up). Local quality inspection was performed on all 1,109 baseline scans, and high-quality scans (e.g., those without significant motion artifact or poor segmentation; see e-Methods) were identified and included in the sample. The final sample of 663 participants represents 60.1% of the original amnestic MCI group, 60.8% of the original dysnomic MCI group, 60.8% of the original dysexecutive/mixed MCI group, 55.7% of the original CDN group, and 62.7% of the original NC group.
MRI processing and analysis.
All image processing and analyses were performed at the Multimodal Imaging Laboratory, University of California, San Diego. Images were downloaded from the ADNI database and processed using FreeSurfer software (v 5.3.0; surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu).10 Cortical thickness measurements were obtained using well-known and validated procedures, as described previously11 (also see e-Methods).
Cortical thickness estimates for each individual were computed at each vertex (∼1 mm spacing) across the cortical mantle and within 32 gyral-based regions of interest (ROIs) per hemisphere, as described by Desikan et al.12 Mean thickness for each ROI was calculated by averaging the cortical thickness measurements based on the unsmoothed data within a given ROI.
Statistical analyses.
The cluster groups were originally derived using 2 language measures, 2 attention/executive function measures, and 2 memory measures from participants' baseline neuropsychological evaluation7 (see e-Methods). For the subsample (n = 663) in the current study, differences in clinical and biomarker characteristics were examined using χ2 analysis, analysis of variance, and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) adjusted for age and education. Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests were conducted for significant omnibus tests (corrected for 9 pairwise comparisons; α = 0.05/9 = 0.006).
To create statistical group maps for cortical thickness, individual surfaces were resampled into a common spherical coordinate system that aligned cortical folding patterns across participants.13 The surface maps were compared between NCs and each cluster-derived group using a general linear model. A series of one-way multivariate ANCOVAs (MANCOVAs) was conducted to compare each cluster-derived group separately to the NCs on mean thickness values in the ROIs. We also compared the MCI subtypes to one another using a series of one-way MANCOVAs. Covariates included age, education, sex, and scanner field strength (n = 311 with a 1.5T scan; n = 352 with a 3T scan). Bonferroni correction was applied to each analysis to account for multiple ROI comparisons (corrected for 64 ROI comparisons; α = 0.05/64 = 0.0008).
RESULTS
Demographics and neuropsychological performance.
Demographic characteristics are presented in table 1. Mean performance for each group on the 6 neuropsychological measures is shown in table 1 (raw scores) and figure 1 (age- and education-adjusted z scores). Group performance in this subsample (n = 663) is nearly identical to the full original sample (n = 1,109)7; see also e-Results.
Table 1.
Performance on diagnostic measures.
Three measures are considered in ADNI's diagnosis of MCI: Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (WMS-R) Logical Memory–II Story A, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR). There were no differences on these measures among amnestic, dysnomic, and dysexecutive/mixed MCI. However, all 3 groups scored worse than the CDNs and the NCs (p < 0.001). The CDN group also scored worse than the NCs on ADNI's diagnostic measures (p < 0.001), which accounts for their original MCI classification.7
Biomarker characteristics and longitudinal clinical outcomes.
Table 1 shows the prevalence of abnormal CSF biomarkers (based on established cutpoint concentrations14), APOE genotype, and progression/regression rates for the subsample. There were no differences among the amnestic, dysnomic, and dysexecutive/mixed MCI groups in CSF biomarkers or APOE status; there were also no significant differences between the CDNs and NCs (see also e-Results). The CDN group showed the lowest rate of progression to AD and highest rate of reversion to normal. These biomarker and progression results are similar to findings in the original full sample.7
Regional cortical thickness patterns relative to NCs.
Significant differences in regional cortical thickness between each cluster-derived MCI subtype relative to the NCs are displayed at the vertex-wise level on the lateral and medial surface maps in figure 2, and at the ROI level in table 2. (See table e-1 for cortical thickness values for all 32 ROIs per hemisphere.) For amnestic MCI, thinning was observed primarily in medial and lateral temporal lobe regions bilaterally (p < 0.0001). Additional atrophy was seen in some parietal and frontal regions (p < 0.0008); these findings reached significance in the ROI analyses but were subthreshold on the surface maps. The dysnomic MCI group demonstrated thinning primarily in medial and lateral temporal lobe regions (p < 0.0001), with greater atrophy in the left lateral temporal cortex relative to the right (comparison of left vs right lateral temporal ROIs: t[92] = 6.18, p < 0.0001, d = 0.64), as well as atrophy in frontal lobe regions (p < 0.0001). For dysexecutive/mixed MCI, a widespread pattern of cortical thinning was observed including atrophy in frontal, temporal, parietal, and cingulate cortices bilaterally (p < 0.0001), with relative sparing of occipital and paracentral regions. There were no significant differences between the CDNs and the NCs.
Table 2.
Regional cortical thickness comparisons among MCI subtypes.
Significant differences in regional cortical thickness between the amnestic MCI and dysexecutive/mixed MCI groups are displayed at the vertex-wise level in figure 3. ROI analyses showed that the dysexecutive/mixed MCI group had greater cortical thinning relative to the amnestic MCI group in frontal (left caudal middle frontal gyrus), lateral temporal (left superior temporal gyrus, left middle temporal gyrus, left inferior temporal gyrus), and parietal regions (left supramarginal gyrus, left and right inferior parietal cortex, and right precuneus); all ps < 0.0008. No significant differences were found between amnestic vs dysnomic MCI, or between dysnomic vs dysexecutive/mixed MCI, once Bonferroni correction was applied. In comparison to the CDN group, the amnestic MCI and dysnomic MCI groups demonstrated thinning in medial and lateral temporal lobe regions; see table 2. The dysexecutive/mixed MCI group showed atrophy primarily in frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices relative to the CDN group; see table 2.
DISCUSSION
This study examined patterns of cortical atrophy in empirically derived MCI subtypes. Findings revealed heterogeneous patterns of cortical thinning in MCI participants that are not captured by conventional diagnostic criteria. Importantly, the pattern of cortical thinning observed for each subtype corresponded to their cognitive profile, suggesting that cognitive impairments accumulate in a systematic manner that is commensurate with the accumulation of region-specific atrophy. Consistent with their isolated deficits in episodic memory, the amnestic MCI group showed significant medial temporal lobe (MTL) thinning. These findings provide an important anchoring of our results to previous studies,15,16 and validation of cluster analytic techniques for identifying reliable cognitive phenotypes. In the dysnomic MCI group, the finding of lateral temporal lobe atrophy with greater left hemisphere involvement is consistent with the observed naming deficit. Although language was their primary impairment, the dysnomic MCI group also demonstrated poor memory, which fits with their thinning in MTL. A previous study examining patterns of gray matter atrophy in MCI17 demonstrated similar results, with atrophic changes predominantly affecting the left MTL in a language-impaired subgroup of nonamnestic MCI. Findings also dovetail with another study18 that found gray matter loss in bilateral temporal regions as well as frontal and subcortical regions in a language variant of nonamnestic MCI. The dysnomic MCI subtype may represent a more intermediate stage of cognitive decline consistent with the Braak staging scheme in which stages III and IV begin to demonstrate neurofibrillary tangles spreading from entorhinal/medial temporal cortex to adjacent inferolateral temporal and frontal cortices.
The dysexecutive/mixed MCI group exhibited a fairly widespread pattern of atrophy. This reflects their neuropsychological profile, which is characterized by poor performance across all cognitive domains tested. These finding are consistent with those of a previous study19 that found greater cortical thinning in superior and lateral frontoparietal regions in a dysexecutive-predominant MCI subgroup relative to memory impairment-predominant participants. Our findings in the dysexecutive/mixed MCI group also corroborate previous studies of multidomain amnestic MCI. In addition to bilateral MTL atrophy, multidomain amnestic MCI typically involves widespread thinning within parietal, temporal, and frontal regions.17,18,20,21 However, the pattern of thinning in the dysexecutive/mixed MCI group was less consistent with previous studies of the conventional nonamnestic MCI subtype, in which heterogeneous patterns of gray matter loss and less MTL focus have been noted.17,18 The current study did not include a purely dysexecutive group, as all subtypes demonstrated memory impairment, limiting our ability to examine nonamnestic forms of MCI. The dysexecutive/mixed MCI group would be expected to have even more Braak staging-related pathology compared to the amnestic MCI and dysnomic MCI subtypes, given the severity of their cognitive impairments and the widespread distribution of cortical thinning.
Perhaps our most striking and novel finding was the lack of difference in cortical thickness between the CDNs and the NCs in any ROI examined. In addition, there were significant differences between the CDNs and the 3 impaired MCI subtypes, paralleling the neuropsychological differences found between CDNs and other MCI groups. The normal cortical thickness profile in the CDN group is consistent with their intact cognitive performance coupled with our previous findings that these individuals evidenced CSF AD biomarker profiles that did not differ from the normative reference group, had low rates of progression to AD relative to the other empirically derived groups, and were equally as likely to revert to cognitively normal as they were to progress to dementia.7 Taken together, these findings offer additional support for the premise that conventional diagnostic criteria are susceptible to a high rate of false-positive diagnostic errors. Given that the CDN group comprises roughly one-third of ADNI's total MCI sample, including these individuals in research studies of MCI will undoubtedly weaken or obscure meaningful findings. Indeed, we have shown that removing these false-positive diagnoses by applying a novel actuarial diagnostic method22 to ADNI's MCI cohort results in stronger relationships between cognition, biomarkers, and rates of progression to AD.23
Another notable finding from the current study was the lack of difference between the impaired MCI subtypes on the 3 measures used by ADNI to arrive at an MCI diagnosis, despite significant variability in cortical atrophy profiles. All groups performed comparably on the MMSE and WMS-R Logical Memory. On the CDR, participants in all 3 impaired MCI groups were given a global CDR score of 0.5, and their scores on the CDR sum of boxes also did not differ from one another. Based on these measures alone, it appears that these individuals represent a homogenous sample of MCI participants. However, it is clear from the cortical thickness maps and performance on the neuropsychological battery (e.g., naming; executive function) that there is substantial cognitive and neuropathologic heterogeneity within the sample. The dysexecutive/mixed MCI group, in particular, demonstrated greater severity in neuropsychological deficits and more widespread cortical thinning. One might expect that individuals in this more impaired group would also show poorer scores on the diagnostic measures, or perhaps even be classified as having early AD. Instead, their scores are comparable to the less impaired MCI groups and, even more concerning, fall into the same general range as the nonimpaired CDN group. Limitations of the CDR have been described previously, including one study24 which found that global CDR scores of 0.5 masked variability within an MCI sample in terms of functional abilities, cognitive test performance, cortical thinning in frontal and parietal lobe regions, and rates of progression to dementia. ADNI has expanded its diagnostic scheme by classifying participants as either early MCI or late MCI (determined by the WMS-R Logical Memory). However, the data suggest that these labels do not improve clarity or accuracy of the diagnosis, as 42% of the CDN group was classified as late MCI, and 20% of the dysexecutive/mixed MCI group was considered early MCI. Our data suggest that these coarse diagnostic measures are not capturing the variability in biomarkers or cognitive profiles that exists within MCI. Nonetheless, the conventional diagnostic criteria for MCI are routinely used in large-scale research studies, clinical trials, and clinical practice.
Our findings of substantial variability in MCI are consistent with those from a study25 that used cluster analysis to identify biomarker profiles in the ADNI MCI cohort. Results of that study revealed 4 unique clusters based on 11 biological variables (e.g., cortical thinning, CSF values, white matter hyperintensities). One cluster showed the poorest biomarker profile, with means similar to the AD group on some measures, and significant cognitive decline longitudinally. A healthy cluster group was also identified which had biomarkers similar to the NC group and showed stable or improved cognitive performance over time. These 2 clusters from this previous study25 appear remarkably similar to our dysexecutive/mixed MCI group and CDN group, respectively. These complementary methodologies provide compelling evidence for the enormous diversity of cognitive and biomarker profiles in samples classified as MCI based on conventional criteria. Considerable heterogeneity exists not only at the level of MCI, but even within autopsy-confirmed AD, as one study26 found that 2 atypical variants—coined limbic-predominant and hippocampal-sparing subtypes—accounted for 25% of AD cases. Thus, better characterization of MCI subtypes may be critical not only for staging MCI severity, but perhaps also for identifying individuals with different underlying variants of AD.
In the current study, the comparison of each MCI subgroup to the NCs suggested a differential pattern of cortical thinning for each MCI subgroup. However, direct comparison between the MCI subtypes only partially confirmed this impression and more strongly suggested an ordered pattern of cortical atrophy. Specifically, cortical thickness patterns across the 3 impaired MCI groups appear to reflect increasing disease severity within the MCI cohort—from mild (amnestic) to severe (dysexecutive/mixed), with a possible intermediate moderate stage (dysnomic). This is consistent with the behavioral data showing increasing neuropsychological impairment across groups. While results suggest our group differences are likely more quantitative than qualitative in nature, the word “subtype” is used to remain consistent with existing MCI literature that makes similar distinctions (e.g., single-domain vs multi-domain amnestic MCI subtypes) for patients along a disease continuum.
The presence of heterogeneous cortical atrophy patterns in our empirically derived MCI subtypes suggests a strong need to (1) improve the diagnostic criteria by reducing reliance on conventional screening measures, rating scales, and a single memory measure in order to avoid false-positive errors; and (2) divide MCI samples into meaningful subgroups based on cognitive and biomarkers profiles—a method that may provide better staging of MCI and inform prognosis. A limitation of this study is the lack of assessment of visuospatial functioning, as early visuospatial deficits may be indicative of prodromal AD. The possibility of type II error is also a consideration, given our rigorous threshold for statistical significance. Notable strengths of the study include the large number of MRI scans that were analyzed, and careful manual inspection locally with rigorous quality control procedures in order to ensure optimal quality of all imaging data. A future direction of this line of research will be to examine changes in cortical thickness longitudinally in our CDN group and each cognitive subtype in order to determine how changes are related to risk of progression to dementia. Information gleaned from such studies could have important clinical utility, as knowledge of regional cortical thickness and cognitive profiles could be useful for establishing the diagnosis or stage of MCI, or for predicting prognosis at an individual level.
Supplementary Material
GLOSSARY
- AD
Alzheimer disease
- ADNI
Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative
- ANCOVA
analysis of covariance
- CDN
cluster-derived normal
- CDR
Clinical Dementia Rating
- MANCOVA
multivariate analysis of covariance
- MCI
mild cognitive impairment
- MMSE
Mini-Mental State Examination
- MTL
medial temporal lobe
- NC
normal controls
- ROI
region of interest
- WMS-R
Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised
Footnotes
Supplemental data at Neurology.org
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
E.C.E.: study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting/revising the manuscript for content, acquisition of data, statistical analysis. J.E.: analysis and interpretation of data, drafting/revising the manuscript for intellectual content. M.W.B.: study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting/revising the manuscript for intellectual content, obtained funding. K.M.L.: analysis and interpretation of data, drafting/revising the manuscript for intellectual content. B.G.: analysis and interpretation of data. L.D.-W.: study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting/revising the manuscript for intellectual content. C.R.M.: study concept and design, analysis and interpretation of data, drafting/revising the manuscript for intellectual content, study supervision and coordination, obtained funding.
STUDY FUNDING
This study was supported by Alzheimer's Association grant NIRG-13-281806 (C.R.M.) and National Institutes of Health grants R01 AG049810 (M.W.B.) and K24 AG026431 (M.W.B.). Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award number W81XWH-12-2-0012). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through contributions from the following: Alzheimer's Association; Alzheimer's Drug Discovery Foundation; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen Idec Inc.; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd. and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; GE Healthcare; Innogenetics, N.V.; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development LLC; Medpace, Inc.; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC; NeuroRx Research; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Synarc Inc.; and Takeda Pharmaceutical Company. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study at the University of California, San Diego. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory of Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California.
DISCLOSURE
The authors report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures.
REFERENCES
- 1.Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol 1999;56:303–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med 2004;256:183–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med 2004;256:240–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Delano-Wood L, Bondi MW, Sacco J, et al. Heterogeneity in mild cognitive impairment: differences in neuropsychological profile and associated white matter lesion pathology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009;15:906–914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Libon DJ, Xie SX, Eppig J, et al. The heterogeneity of mild cognitive impairment: a neuropsychological analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010;16:84–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Clark LR, Delano-Wood L, Libon DJ, et al. Are empirically derived subtypes of mild cognitive impairment consistent with conventional subtypes? J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2013;19:635–645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Edmonds EC, Delano-Wood L, Clark LR, et al. Susceptibility of the conventional criteria for mild cognitive impairment to false-positive diagnostic errors. Alzheimers Dement 2015;11:415–424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Petersen RC, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, et al. Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI): clinical characterization. Neurology 2010;74:201–209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bangen KJ, Clark AL, Werhane M, et al. Cortical amyloid burden differences across empirically-derived mild cognitive impairment subtypes and interaction with APOE ε4 genotype. J Alzheimers Dis 2016;52:849–861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis: I: segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 1999;9:179–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:11050–11055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 2006;31:968–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM. High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp 1999;8:272–284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Shaw LM, Vanderstichele H, Knapik-Czajka M, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarker signature in Alzheimer's disease neuroimaging initiative subjects. Ann Neurol 2009;65:403–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Singh V, Chertkow H, Lerch JP, Evans AC, Dorr AE, Kabani NJ. Spatial patterns of cortical thinning in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Brain 2006;129:2885–2893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wang L, Goldstein FC, Veledar E, et al. Alterations in cortical thickness and white matter integrity in mild cognitive impairment measured by whole-brain cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol 2009;30:893–899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Whitwell JL, Petersen RC, Negash S, et al. Patterns of atrophy differ among specific subtypes of mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2007;64:1130–1138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zhang H, Sachdev PS, Wen W, et al. Gray matter atrophy patterns of mild cognitive impairment subtypes. J Neurol Sci 2012;315:26–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dickerson BC, Wolk DA. Dysexecutive versus amnesic phenotypes of very mild Alzheimer's disease are associated with distinct clinical, genetic and cortical thinning characteristics. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82:45–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Raamana PR, Wen W, Kochan NA, et al. The sub-classification of amnestic mild cognitive impairment using MRI-based cortical thickness measures. Front Neurol 2014;5:76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Seo SW, Im K, Lee JM, et al. Cortical thickness in single-versus multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage 2007;36:289–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Jak AJ, Bondi MW, Delano-Wood L, et al. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;17:368–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bondi MW, Edmonds EC, Jak AJ, et al. Neuropsychological criteria for mild cognitive impairment improves diagnostic precision, biomarker associations, and prediction of progression. J Alzheimers Dis 2014;42:275–289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Chang YL, Bondi MW, McEvoy LK, et al. Global clinical dementia rating of 0.5 in MCI masks variability related to level of function. Neurology 2011;76:652–659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nettiksimmons J, Decarli C, Landau S, Beckett L. Biological heterogeneity in ADNI amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement 2014;10:511–521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Murray ME, Graff-Radford NR, Ross OA, Petersen RC, Duara R, Dickson DW. Neuropathologically defined subtypes of Alzheimer's disease with distinct clinical characteristics: a retrospective study. Lancet Neurol 2011;10:785–796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.