Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Nov 15.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Bull. 2015 Dec 21;142(3):291–313. doi: 10.1037/bul0000031

Assessing Human Mirror Activity With EEG Mu Rhythm: A Meta-Analysis

Nathan A Fox 1, Marian J Bakermans-Kranenburg 2, Kathryn H Yoo 3, Lindsay C Bowman 4, Erin N Cannon 5, Ross E Vanderwert 6, Pier F Ferrari 7, Marinus H van IJzendoorn 8
PMCID: PMC5110123  NIHMSID: NIHMS828034  PMID: 26689088

Abstract

A fundamental issue in cognitive neuroscience is how the brain encodes others’ actions and intentions. In recent years, a potential advance in our knowledge on this issue is the discovery of mirror neurons in the motor cortex of the nonhuman primate. These neurons fire to both execution and observation of specific types of actions. Researchers use this evidence to fuel investigations of a human mirror system, suggesting a common neural code for perceptual and motor processes. Among the methods used for inferring mirror system activity in humans are changes in a particular frequency band in the electroencephalogram (EEG) called the mu rhythm. Mu frequency appears to decrease in amplitude (reflecting cortical activity) during both action execution and action observation. The current meta-analysis reviewed 85 studies (1,707 participants) of mu that infer human mirror system activity. Results demonstrated significant effect sizes for mu during execution (Cohen’s d = 0.46, N = 701) as well as observation of action (Cohen’s d = 0.31, N = 1,508), confirming a mirroring property in the EEG. A number of moderators were examined to determine the specificity of these effects. We frame these meta-analytic findings within the current discussion about the development and functions of a human mirror system, and conclude that changes in EEG mu activity provide a valid means for the study of human neural mirroring. Suggestions for improving the experimental and methodological approaches in using mu to study the human mirror system are offered.

Keywords: mu rhythm, mirror neurons, EEG, action execution, action observation


A fundamental issue in cognitive neuroscience is how the brain is able to encode others’ actions and intentions. In recent years, a potential advance in our knowledge on how these processes take place is the discovery of mirror neurons. This discovery was made using single-cell recordings in the adult Rhesus macaque ventral premotor cortex and inferior parietal lobe while the monkey observed and executed simple actions (di Pellegrino, Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese, & Rizzolatti, 1992; Fogassi et al., 2005). It suggested that perceptual and motor processes share a common neural code. Based on their property of firing to both observed and executed actions, it has been hypothesized that a mirror system may likely be present in the human brain, and may play a role in human understanding of others’ actions and intentions by representing actions, at a cortical level, for both motor execution and observation (Fogassi et al., 2005; Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi, & Rizzolatti, 1996; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Gallese, & Fogassi, 1996; Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 2001). The activation of the motor system during observation of an action has led some researchers to interpret it not only as evidence of a recognition process but also as a means to repeat the observed action and even understand the intention behind it (Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010). Thus, a human mirror system has been suggested to represent a neural mechanism underlying action-perception coupling, creating a self–other matching system that both facilitates recognition of others’ actions and provides a means for imitation.

Investigations of the Human Mirror System With fMRI

There has been much interest in translating the discovery of mirror neurons in monkeys to human neurophysiology and human cognition (Gallese, Gernsbacher, Heyes, Hickok, & Iacoboni, 2011). Single-cell recordings identify mirror neurons in the monkey (di Pellegrino et al., 1992; Fogassi et al., 2005); however, the translation from monkey neurophysiology to human brain activity may not be direct. And absent of a consistent ability to directly assess neuronal activity in the human brain, as is done in monkeys (though see Mukamel, Ekstrom, Kaplan, Iacoboni, & Fried, 2010), direct comparisons between the two species are currently not feasible.

That said, many researchers have attempted to identify a mirror system in humans using noninvasive brain imaging methods. There are human homologues of the regions studied in the monkey—ventral premotor cortex, inferior parietal lobe, and also part of the inferior frontal gyrus—which have been examined for mirroring properties in humans using functional neuroimaging (fMRI). Meta-analyses of fMRI studies reveal that these brain regions appear to demonstrate mirroring properties. Across 139 fMRI and PET studies (Caspers, Zilles, Laird, & Eickhoff, 2010), and another 76 fMRI studies (Molenberghs, Cunnington, & Mattingley, 2012), the inferior frontal gyrus, ventral premotor cortex, and inferior parietal lobe were active to both the execution and observation of body actions (including hands, feet, legs, mouth, and face). Though these patterns cannot be attributed to the activation of mirror neurons in the human brain, researchers interpret these regional activation patterns across action-execution and action-observation as support for neural mirroring or a mirror system more generally.

Meta-analyses of fMRI studies also reveal that regions not classically associated with mirror neurons in the nonhuman primate demonstrate mirroring properties in humans, including the dorsal premotor cortex, superior parietal lobe, temporal gyrus, and cerebellum (Molenberghs et al., 2012). The involvement of the dorsal premotor cortex and superior parietal lobe in both action execution and action observation was reported in an additional meta-analysis examining hand movements (Grèzes & Decety, 2001). Further, activations of specific brain regions for observation of action were shown to differ depending on the instructions given to participants: If participants were told to passively observe, there was consistent activation in regions including the inferior parietal lobe, but when participants were told to observe so that they could later imitate the movement, there was no activation in the inferior parietal lobe (although this null result was based on only eight studies; Caspers et al., 2010). Nonetheless, despite the lack of direct translation from monkey to human neurophysiology, fMRI research reveals brain regions in humans (including the ventral premotor cortex and inferior frontal gyrus) that show consistent mirroring properties, suggesting the possibility of a mirror system in humans.

Functional Significance of the Mirror System

The growing number of investigations that focus on a human mirror system is fueled, in part, by its functional significance for cognitive science. Many scholars argue that the potential impact of such a perception-production neural mechanism extends beyond the action domain. For example, a mirror system has been posited as a fundamental building block for understanding others’ actions (Rizzolatti & Fabbri-Destro, 2008). Such functions include supporting several aspects of social perception, such as the ability to predict others’ movements (Csibra, 2007; Kilner, Friston, & Frith, 2007) and the ability to mentally simulate others’ actions (Gallese & Sinigaglia, 2011; Prinz, 1997). More recently, researchers suggest that neural mirroring may play a role in human infants’ ability to map similarities between self and other, and thus may be involved in providing a foundation for imitation and social–cognitive development (Marshall & Meltzoff, 2014).

Researchers have also emphasized ties between the mirror system and empathy (Carr, Iacoboni, Dubeau, Mazziotta, & Lenzi, 2003; Gallese, 2001, 2005; Iacoboni, 2009), and between the mirror system and language (Rizzolatti & Arbib, 1998; Théoret & Pascual-Leone, 2002; Wolf, Gales, Shane, & Shane, 2001). The general notion behind a role for a mirror system in these complex social and communicative abilities rests on the position that when another’s action is perceived, the human mirror system supports internal representation of this perceived action that is linked via a common neural code to one’s own actions, and through that process of direct matching “the mirror neuron system transforms visual information into knowledge” (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004, p. 172; see also Rizzolatti et al., 2001). Indeed, recent research has shown that transient disruptions to motor cortex (using transcranial magnetic stimulation; TMS) result in impairments in the ability to recognize and anticipate others’ actions (Michael et al., 2014; Stadler et al., 2012).

More recent views of mirror system function differ somewhat from classic presentations. For example, Kilner and colleagues (2007) criticize classic bottom-up, forward connection models (e.g., Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), arguing that the processes by which an observer maps an observed action onto their own motor system and translates the visual information into inferences about intentions and goals are unclear. The notion of inverting such a forward model to infer the cause of an observed action from its elicited internal representation (with matching known cause) is problematic because sensory inputs (visual, proprioceptive, and tactile) are not associated with singular, unique causes. These authors propose a “predictive coding framework,” which outlines alternative processes that draw on Bayesian principles: Backward connections in a hierarchical system give contextual guidance to lower level inputs in a reciprocal process that works to minimize prediction error (Kilner et al., 2007). Csibra (2007) has also criticized the bottom-up, direct matching hypothesis, but unlike Kilner et al. (2007), argues that action mirroring is generated by “emulative action reconstruction” via top-down interpretation processes outside the motor system, and further posits that action understanding may precede rather than follow from action mirroring.

Controversies and Open Questions in Mirror System Research

As already exemplified with the differing views presented in the previous section, there is a good deal of controversy surrounding the proposed origins and functions of a mirror system in both humans and monkeys (e.g., Heyes, 2010; Hickok, 2009; Hickok & Hauser, 2010). Some argue that though neural systems supporting action have been highly investigated, broad inferences about sophisticated social–cognitive functions of these systems reach beyond empirical findings (Dinstein, Thomas, Behrmann, & Heeger, 2008), and strong claims about the function of “action understanding”—an ambiguous term in and of itself—are based on nonfalsifiable logic rather than experimental testing (Steinhorst & Funke, 2014). Indeed, a meta-analysis of fMRI studies investigating action and higher social cognition, such as theory of mind, showed almost no overlap between the regions supporting “mirroring” and those supporting mental-state understanding (Van Overwalle & Baetens, 2009). Even more basic functions such as understanding and recognizing others’ actions have been challenged: Researchers argue that the motor system is unlikely to be responsible for abstract aspects of understanding, including recognizing intentions and goals (Hickok, 2009). And some have argued that the function of a mirror system is to compute (and thereby predict) the motor command for achieving an intention, but not to compute the agent’s intention itself (Jacob, 2008). However, more recent findings challenge these views and seem to support an active role of premotor regions in action understanding (Michael et al., 2014; Urgesi, Candidi, Ionta, & Aglioti, 2007; Urgesi, Moro, Candidi, & Aglioti, 2006).

The fundamental matching properties of a mirror system have also been questioned. Hickok (2009) argues that although most mirror system research shows that neural systems for action execution are indeed correlated with action observation, this work does not clarify the functional significance of this correlation, leaving the strict hypothesis of a single neural code for both action execution and action observation open to alternatives. For example, Heyes (2010, 2014) has argued that mirror neuron activity could reflect associative learning. This associative account suggests that the “self–other matching” properties that are the basis of a mirror system could be formed via contingent execution-observation experiences over time, rather than as the inherent product of an innate system for mirroring. It is also not clear whether a highly flexible mechanism, such as that represented by a mirror system, is sufficiently stable over the course of development to sustain complex cognitive functions. This leaves open the debate about the functional role of a mirror system and its adaptive value.

The role of a mirror system in atypical development and cognition has also been examined. There are proposals that the mirror system may be key to understanding the disordered mind, as in the case of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). Several researchers posit that the motor, communication, and social–cognitive deficits associated with ASD are due, at least in part, to a dysfunctional mirror system (Oberman, McCleery, Ramachandran, & Pineda, 2007; Perkins, Stokes, McGillivray, & Bittar, 2010; Pineda, Carrasco, Datko, Pillen, & Schalles, 2014; Rizzolatti, Fabbri-Destro, & Cattaneo, 2009; Vivanti & Rogers, 2014; J. H. G. Williams, Whiten, Suddendorf, & Perrett, 2001). However, empirical data provide mixed evidence. There have been several investigations of the human mirror system in individuals with ASD using TMS (Enticott, Kennedy, Bradshaw, Rinehart, & Fitzgerald, 2010; Enticott et al., 2012), neuroimaging (Dapretto et al., 2006; Hadjikhani, Joseph, Snyder, & Tager-Flusberg, 2006; Martineau, Andersson, Barthélémy, Cottier, & Destrieux, 2010; J. G. Williams, Higgins, & Brayne, 2006), and electrophysiology (Bernier, Dawson, Webb, & Murias, 2007; Martineau, Cochin, Magne, & Barthélémy, 2008; Oberman et al., 2005) that report structural abnormalities and diminished recruitment during action-processing tasks in ASD samples compared with typical controls. Yet several studies reveal counterevidence showing that neural systems for action execution and action observation are not distinguishable between ASD patients and controls (Enticott et al., 2013; Fan, Decety, Yang, Liu, & Cheng, 2010; Raymaekers, Wiersema, & Roeyers, 2009; Ruysschaert, Warreyn, Wiersema, Oostra, & Roeyers, 2014). And the general view that a deficit in a mirror system plays a fundamental role in canonical ASD impairments has been challenged (Hamilton, Brindley, & Frith, 2007; Southgate & Hamilton, 2008).

Finally, the function and significance of a mirror system across development is still not understood. Some of the most powerful potential effects of a mirror system should be evident during development (Ferrari, Tramacere, Simpson, & Iriki, 2013). Indeed, the fundamental abilities that a mirror system may underlie—the ability to deploy actions strategically in the service of goals, and the ability to understand the goals of social partners in order to produce adaptive social responses—emerge early in infancy and undergo foundational developments in the first years of life (Woodward & Gerson, 2014). There have been several recent explorations of “neural mirroring” in infancy (see Marshall & Meltzoff, 2011), with their number growing. However, there has been little direct investigation of how mirroring changes with development or how supportive it may be in the emergence of action-perception understanding.

A Need for Evaluation

In order to address the debate over the presence and functions of a human mirror system, the efficacy of current tools for identifying the presence of neural mirroring in humans must be evaluated. fMRI research has laid an important foundation for investigation of the mirror system in humans, and several meta-analyses outlined in the Investigations of the Human Mirror System with fMRI section (Caspers et al., 2010; Molenberghs et al., 2012) demonstrate its utility in identifying neural mirroring in humans. But there are also limits to this brain imaging method. Specifically, fMRI investigations with young children, infants, and impaired subjects are extremely difficult because of the noisy testing environment, the need for the participant to lie still for long periods of time, and the separation between caregiver and participant. Data collection from participants is costly, and in pediatric populations, there is high data loss from motion artifact. Research into a mirror system at these early ages, across development, and in impaired populations is key to investigating the plasticity of the proposed mirror system and its role in higher social cognition. Alternative methods are therefore needed for these critical developmental approaches.

In recent years, there has been an increase in the number of studies examining the mu rhythm within the electroencephalogram (EEG) as a potential index of human neural mirroring. This rhythm has been shown to decrease in amplitude—or desynchronize—when humans both execute and observe action, and, as a result, it has been argued that mu desynchronization is linked to mirror system activity (Cuevas, Cannon, Yoo, & Fox, 2014; Muthukumaraswamy & Johnson, 2004a, 2004b; Pineda, 2005). Examinations of the mu rhythm in infancy are now prevalent, with researchers pursuing questions about the development of neural mirroring and its role in social and cognitive development (Marshall & Meltzoff, 2014). There is a prominent view that mu rhythm suppression reflects activity of a human mirror system. As of yet, however, this view has not been systematically evaluated. Such an evaluation is critical to the field of psychology, as it would allow researchers from multiple areas to understand and evaluate the significance of studies that examine action-perception and action-execution links. The present meta-analysis provides a large-scale, systematic evaluation of the extent to which mu rhythm consistently desynchronizes to both action execution and action observation, thus indexing neural mirroring.

Investigations of the Human Mirror System With EEG Mu Rhythm

EEG methods have many advantages over the use of fMRI. EEG is relatively inexpensive and is relatively easy to use with pediatric and special-needs populations. And EEG offers the unique ability to examine the timing of activation to observation or execution of action. However, unlike single-cell recordings in nonhuman primates, and similar to fMRI, EEG cannot pinpoint the activity of specific neurons.

EEG is acquired by placing sensors on a participant’s head and measuring the electricity generated by the brain. These sensors are placed over the scalp in a pattern that roughly corresponds to different areas of the cerebral cortex (left and right frontal, central, temporal, parietal, and occipital areas). First described in 1929 by Berger (Berger, 1929), this electrical activity has two main properties: frequency (oscillations per second) and amplitude (height of the oscillations). Mu rhythm reflects EEG frequency occurring within the standard “alpha” band (i.e., ~8–13 Hz [oscillations per second] in adults; ~6–9 Hz in children) that varies in amplitude as a function of subject action (Lepage & Théoret, 2006; Muthukumaraswamy & Johnson 2004a; Pfurtscheller, Neuper, Andrew, & Edlinger, 1997). For example (see Figure 1), when a subject executes an action (e.g., voluntary hand movement), EEG amplitude in the mu band decreases, often with maximal suppression over central scalp locations (Kuhlman, 1978). Researchers studying mirror activity have examined this decrease in amplitude during observation of action. This decrease in amplitude, which is calculated in reference to a baseline period, is known as desynchronization. The prominence of mu desynchronization recorded from central sites that overlay sensorimotor cortex during action execution suggests it is an index of sensorimotor cortical activation (e.g., Leocani, Toro, Manganotti, Zhuang, & Hallett, 1997; Toro et al., 1994). Though EEG activity at particular electrode locations on the scalp does not necessarily reflect cortical activity directly below these electrodes, there is strong evidence that mu rhythm recorded from central sites is not purely the result of neural activity in occipital regions spreading to central areas (Pineda, 2005). Indeed, a number of studies that estimate cortical source locations underlying mu activity have identified sources primarily clustered around the central sulcus in sensorimotor areas (Hari, Salmelin, Mäkelä, Salenius, & Helle, 1997; Salmelin & Hari, 1994a, 1994b), with some sources observed in parietal areas as well (Salmelin, Hämäläinen, Kajola, & Hari, 1995). Other studies have shown that sources underlying mu rhythm recorded with EEG are also primarily concentrated in central and parietal cortical areas (Manshanden, De Munck, Simon, & Lopes da Silva, 2002; Thorpe, Cannon, & Fox, 2015). Critically, these brain areas are similar to regions that fMRI research has shown activate during both observation and execution of action (Molenberghs et al., 2012). These findings have led researchers to consider mu desynchronization as a candidate for indexing neural mirroring in humans. With the search for a human mirror system, studies have examined the links between mu rhythm desynchronization and observation of others’ actions. Given established links between mu desynchronization and action execution, this rhythm has the potential to demonstrate mirroring properties should a similar pattern of mu desynchronization occur during action observation.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Simulation of mu rhythm desynchronization in the 8- to 13-Hz frequency band. There is a decrease in amplitude in the electroencephalogram from baseline during action observation or execution (action event; highlighted in green). Individual appearing here has consented for her likeness to be published in this article.

Yet the extent to which mu desynchronization may characterize mirror activity in humans is not fully known. Across the now large number of studies that examine mu rhythm activity to action execution and action observation, methods for both eliciting and identifying mu desynchronization vary considerably, especially for action observation (Cuevas et al., 2014). Across individual studies, participants observe a range of different stimuli including, for example, a single hand grasping small objects (e.g., Muthukumaraswamy & Johnson, 2004b), photographs of emotionally expressive eyes (Pineda & Hecht, 2009), whole body movements such as dancing (Orgs, Dombrowski, Heil, & Jansen-Osmann, 2008), and pages of sheet music (Behmer & Jantzen, 2011). There are also variations in how mu desynchronization is calculated across studies in terms of the type of baseline selected as a reference point and the location and number of EEG channels in which activity is examined. These inconsistencies raise questions as to whether mu desynchronization during action observation is consistently present and similar to mu desynchronization during action execution, thus raising the question as to whether mu desynchronization reflects neural mirroring.

Here, we conducted a meta-analysis of 85 EEG studies that all focus on the EEG mu rhythm. We examined the consistency of findings across studies that used EEG to assess responses to action observation and action execution. A clearer understanding of the pattern of results from these studies will help clarify whether mu desynchronization is a valid index of neural mirroring. Given the infant- and child-friendly properties of EEG, meta-analytic results will also provide an important foundation for future research that investigates the role of action and the mirror system in imitation, empathy, and language as these capacities emerge and change across development. Further, investigation of action systems across development provides a platform for testing alternative hypotheses to the origins of a mirror system, such as those that emphasize the role of learned associations through increasing experience (Heyes, 2010). Thus, this meta-analysis marks a first critical step in evaluating the role of mu rhythm in indexing the links between action production and action perception, and more broadly have important implications for the study of a human mirror system.

In addition, beyond fundamental mirroring properties, this meta-analysis can assess the extent to which mu desynchronization may also index properties (i.e., specificity to object-directed or goal-directed action and biological motion) that have been linked to hypothesized functions of the mirror system (e.g., intention-understanding and higher social cognition) and that have been examined in meta-analyses of the human mirror system using fMRI. An examination of these moderators can illuminate the specific contexts to which neural mirroring may be particularly sensitive.

This meta-analysis can also examine the effects of methodological variations in how mu desynchronization is calculated (type of baseline) and how it is elicited during observation (type of stimulus observed). As well, we can examine the extent to which studies demonstrate topographic specificity for central (sensorimotor) scalp locations. Topographic specificity would suggest, though not definitively demonstrate, that mu desynchronization may reflect activation in sensorimotor cortical regions, offering a tighter link to mirroring identified with fMRI in the motor and premotor cortex.

Our meta-analytic approach was as follows. We examined across the now substantial set of human EEG mu rhythm studies: (a) whether there was a significant effect size for mu desynchronization to action execution and to action observation; (b) whether these effects were moderated by key variables that have been shown to moderate neural mirroring activity in the fMRI literature; (c) whether the effect sizes of action execution or action observation were moderated by methodological variation across studies; and (d) whether any such effects exhibit topographic specificity. Critically, the effects and moderators for action execution mu desynchronization are compared with the effects and moderators for action observation mu desynchronization in order to shed light on how well mu rhythm activity demonstrates mirroring properties.

Among the extended set of variables we considered in this study, four deserve brief introduction because of their theoretical importance or their potential effect on mu rhythm activity. These are whether mu rhythm desynchronization is specific to the observation of object-directed action or biological motion, and whether mu desynchronization is influenced by type of baseline and is topographically specific.

Specificity to object-directed action

In monkeys, a classic finding is that neurons that exhibit mirroring properties discharge to object-directed actions but not to non-object-directed actions. Mirror neurons in the macaque were first defined as those that fired when the monkey executed object-directed actions such as grasping, holding, and tearing objects; critically, these neurons also fired when the monkey viewed similar object-directed actions being performed (di Pellegrino et al., 1992). More specifically, Rizzolatti and colleagues (1996) found that mirror neurons in monkeys discharged when the monkey viewed an agent grasping an object, but not when viewing similar hand actions that were not object-directed, or when viewing the object alone. Other studies have supported these early observations (Umiltà et al., 2001).

On the basis of these and other studies, researchers have argued that the selectivity of the monkey mirror neuron system to object-directed action indicates selectivity to goal-directed action more specifically (e.g., Rizzolatti & Fabbri-Destro, 2008; Rizzolatti et al., 1996). Indeed, deliberate and voluntary actions that are directed toward objects are likely to demonstrate the goals behind them: For example, a hand moving toward an object that then pinches the object communicates a clear goal to grasp, whereas it is more difficult to decipher the goal behind a hand moving toward and pinching empty space. Several researchers have attempted to probe the specific function of encoding goals in the monkey mirror neuron system (see Casile, 2013, for recent review), though the extent to which existing research provides evidence for mirror neuron selectivity for goal-directed action (or even object-directed action) per se is currently debated (e.g., Cook, Bird, Catmur, Press, & Heyes, 2014).

Some have argued that although a monkey mirror neuron system may be most responsive to object-directed action, a human mirror system may not be (e.g., Fabbri-Destro & Rizzolatti, 2008). Indeed, if a primary function of the mirror system in monkeys is to encode action goals, then the more cognitively capable human may more easily identify goals in non-object-directed actions and thus demonstrate mirroring activity to viewing actions that are intransitive (e.g., Fadiga, Fogassi, Pavesi, & Rizzolatti, 1995). Individual studies investigating the human mirror system yield inconsistent results: Some studies show activation in purported human mirror regions to object-directed action, whereas others show similar activations to non-object-directed action (see Buccino, Binkofski, & Riggio, 2004, for review). However, considering recent meta-analyses and qualitative reviews of human mirror system investigations with fMRI (Caspers et al., 2010; Morin & Grèzes, 2008; Van Overwalle & Baetens, 2009), there appears to be evidence for regional brain activation during object-directed action. Of those meta-analyses or reviews that systematically examined object- versus non-object-directed actions, there is more consistent activation to object-directed versus non-object-directed actions in brain regions thought to reflect the human mirror system (Brodmann area [BA] 44, lateral premotor cortex BA 6, ventral premotor cortex inferior and superior parietal lobe [Caspers et al., 2010]; ventral premotor and supplementary motor BA 6 [Morin & Grèzes, 2008]; anterior inferior parietal sulcus and premotor cortex [Van Overwalle & Baetens, 2009]). It is possible that studies using human mu rhythm demonstrate similar effects. As such, one goal of this meta-analysis was to examine whether object-directed actions moderate the effect size of mu desynchronization to action observation.

Specificity for biological movement and effectors

A consistent finding in the monkey mirror neuron literature is that activation of these neurons occurs when viewing actions performed by a biological effector (e.g., hand, mouth). Indeed, in the monkey, mirror neurons do not support the encoding of general movement (e.g., a moving car, waving flag)—but rather biological movement specifically. Although there is some evidence demonstrating that mirror neurons fire when a monkey views a tool (nonbiological effector) acting on an object (e.g., Rizzolatti, Fogassi, & Gallese, 2001), these experiments consist of a biological effector (a hand) using a tool and not simply a nonbiological effector acting on its own. Further, only monkeys who have had prior tool use training show mirror neuron activation when viewing experimenters acting on objects with tools (Rochat et al., 2010); monkeys without training do not, even though a prolonged exposure to observing tool actions without having interacted with the tool may activate some mirror neurons within the premotor cortex. Thus, a prominent property of the mirror neuron system in monkeys is its specificity to biological movement and effectors.

It is not clear that a human mirror system is also specific to biological motion. Meta-analyses of fMRI studies yield mixed results. Van Overwalle and Baetens (2009) report that although classic mirror regions (i.e., premotor cortex) were active during observation of biological motion from moving body parts (i.e., the hand), they were not active during observation of whole-body motion, though perhaps this null result was because of the lack of object-directed actions in the whole-body motion cases. Morin and Grèzes (2008) also found lack of specificity to biological motion when actions were not object-directed: The dorsal premotor cortex was activated during observation of both biological and nonbiological non-object-directed actions, suggesting that this region was not specific to biological motion per se but to movement more generally. They also found that the dorsal and ventral premotor cortices were recruited for observing biological actions compared with a static stimulus, but this recruitment was not consistent. Thus, it is not clear that the human mirror system as measured with fMRI shows specificity to biological motion. In the present meta-analysis, we investigate whether such specificity exists for studies using mu desynchronization.

Type of baseline

Mu desynchronization is calculated as the percentage of change in EEG amplitude between an experimental event or executed action and a baseline period. The type of baseline used to compute mu desynchronization will influence the magnitude of the change. If the baseline amplitude in the mu frequency band recorded over central scalp locations is high, then desynchronization of the amplitude in response to a motor action or observation of a motor action will be different than if baseline amplitude is low. In the case of mu desynchronization, certain baseline stimuli may themselves elicit desynchronization of mu amplitude and would decrease the chances of finding significant desynchronization during execution or observation of an action. Tangwiriyasakul, Verhagen, van Putten, and Rutten (2013) investigated the effects of using different types of baseline on mu rhythm desynchronization and found that dynamic (e.g., bouncing balls), rather than static (e.g., picture), baseline stimuli increased the amount of desynchronization for some of their participants. In another study, Puzzo, Cooper, Cantarella, and Russo (2011) examined desynchronization of mu rhythm during observation of actions and found the magnitude of the desynchronization varied based on the type of baseline used (resting EEG or nonbiological movement). Given the variation in type of baseline used across many existing mu rhythm studies, the current meta-analysis investigated type of baseline as a moderator for both action execution and action observation.

Topographic specificity

The mu signal was first identified over central scalp locations. However, there have been few attempts with EEG to localize this signal (but see Thorpe et al., 2015), and fewer still that report the response of amplitude in the mu or alpha frequency band from other scalp locations (Hari et al., 1998; Marshall, Young, & Meltzoff, 2011; van der Helden, Van Schie, & Rombouts, 2010). Research suggests that the mu rhythm is distinct from the occipital alpha rhythm (Formaggio et al., 2008; Hari & Salmelin, 1997; Ritter, Moosmann, & Villringer, 2009; see also Pineda, 2005 for review). Thus, one would expect to find evidence of topographic specificity in central or central-parietal scalp leads during action execution and action observation. On the other hand, lack of topographic specificity might suggest that mu desynchronization may be a result of more general attention processes, or a close coordination of action and attention. Examining topographic specificity across action execution and action observation is a first step in clarifying this important issue and in directing future research.

Summary of Goals of the Meta-Analysis

The current meta-analysis examined the effect sizes for mu desynchronization during execution and observation of action, and contrasted the two to see whether they were significantly different from each other in order to identify neural mirroring. Guided by previous investigations of mirroring with fMRI, two theoretically relevant moderators were identified: (a) object-directed versus non-object-directed action for execution and observation, and (b) biological movement and effector versus nonbiological movement and effector during observation. To better characterize and understand mu rhythm, the current meta-analysis also examined topographic specificity as well as whether the effect sizes of mu desynchronization were moderated by (a) type of baseline, and (b) type of stimulus observed. Significant meta-analytic findings would speak to the consistency across studies of mu desynchronization during action observation and action execution, and would indicate the presence of mirroring should significant effect sizes for desynchronization for both conditions be found. Positive findings on theoretically relevant moderators would highlight the specificity of contexts in which neural mirroring can occur. Findings regarding methodological moderators would highlight the need for tighter experimental control in studies of mu desynchronization.

Method

Literature Search

Studies were identified by searching the electronic databases PubMed, Academic Search Premier, and ProQuest Dissertations and Theses. The keywords “rolandic alpha,” “rolandic mu,” “mu rhythm,” “mu suppression,” “sensorimotor alpha,” “action observation,” “action perception,” “action execution,” “action production,” “action understanding,” and “motor resonance” were used along with each of the secondary keywords “electroencephalography” and “EEG.” Supplementary approaches to identifying relevant studies included searching the references of a review article (Pineda, 2005), word-of-mouth studies, and requesting researchers known for their work on EEG mu rhythm to provide any unpublished data. All studies were cross-referenced to avoid duplicates in the meta-analysis.

Studies were included in the meta-analysis if they met all of the following criteria: (a) examined mu rhythm activity in nonclinical human subjects, (b) used an experimental paradigm that had an observation condition, execution condition or both, and (c) reported sufficient data to estimate an effect size. For studies that included clinical samples effect sizes were calculated separately for the non-clinical or control participants. Studies published in journals and available online between 1990 and June 2014, as well as unpublished data, were included in the meta-analysis. The application of the criteria resulted in a total of 85 studies included in the meta-analysis (see Figure 2 for a flow diagram of study identification).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Flow diagram for identifying articles for the meta-analysis. ++ Two ERP studies were excluded; † Studies that did not provide sufficient statistics such as p values, t values, F values to derive effect size estimates were excluded at this step; †† A majority of dissertations and theses were not relevant to the meta-analysis (e.g., examined speech processing); 91 studies were excluded because of their irrelevance; ** 27 studies excluded based on criteria (six BCI and BMI studies, five imagery only studies, three TMS and induced states studies without control groups, six ERP studies, three studies involving clinical group without control groups, four studies did not have observation and or execution conditions). PQTD = ProQuest Dissertations & Theses; BCI = brain-computer interface; BMI = brain-mind interface; TMS = transcranial magnetic stimulation.

Studies were excluded if they involved brain-computer interface (BCI), mind-machine interface (MMI), or brain-machine interface (BMI). Studies were further excluded if they met any of the following criteria: (a) used an experimental paradigm that only included imagery as the stimulus, (b) involved induced states (e.g., TMS, oxytocin) without control groups, or (c) only measured event-related potentials (ERPs). Review articles and chapters in books were excluded and only empirical studies were considered. Finally, although mu desynchronization has been investigated using other brain imaging methods such as magnetoencephalography (Berchicci et al., 2011; Hari et al., 1998; Honaga et al., 2010; Järveläinen, Schürmann, & Hari, 2004), we did not include these studies. The purpose of excluding non-EEG studies was to enhance homogeneity of the set of studies and avoid the “apples and oranges” issue in meta-analysis.

Coding System

The coding system is outlined in Table 1 (a detailed version can be requested from the authors). Studies were coded for the following: type of condition (observation, execution, or both), whether a baseline was present (no, yes), type of baseline (static, dynamic; biological, nonbiological), and type of stimulus observed. There were three categories for the type of stimulus observed (each used as a separate moderator): (a) biological or nonbiological, (b) dynamic or static, and (c) live or video. A stimulus was considered “biological” if it included a body (or part of a body) of humans or animals. Studies coded as using a biological stimulus were further subdivided to indicate whether they used a social or nonsocial biological stimulus. For a study to be coded as having a social-biological stimulus, it required meeting one of the following criteria: (a) having two or more agents interacting, or one agent interacting with a participant via live or video feed; (b) agent(s) exhibiting a communicative gesture (e.g., waving hello); or (c) agent(s) exhibiting a facial expression of emotion. Type of stimulus observed that may be considered ambiguous (e.g., Rorschach cards) were coded as nonbiological. Studies were also coded for object-directed actions for observation and execution.

Table 1.

Coding System Used for Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis

Variable Coding Description
Design
 Type of condition 1 = Observation only
2 = Execution only
3 = Observation and execution
 Baseline present 0 = No
1 = Yes
 Type of baseline (static vs. dynamic) 1 = Static If a baseline is present, whether the stimulus is moving or still (e.g., picture of hand versus video of hand movement)
2 = Dynamic
5 = No stimulus
 Type of baseline (biological vs. nonbiological) 1 = Biological
2 = Nonbiological
5 = No stimulus
 Type of stimulus observed (biological vs. nonbiological) 1 = Biological (Subcode 1 = of the biological, coded as social; Subcode 2 = coded as nonsocial) Type of stimulus with which the subjects are presented during observation.
2 = Nonbiological
3 = Biological and nonbiological
5 = Auditory
 Type of stimulus observed (dynamic vs. static) 1 = Dynamic Whether the type of stimulus observed is moving or still
2 = Static
 Type of stimulus observed (live vs. video) 1 = Live
2 = Video
 Object-directed 0 = No
1 = Yes
Sample characteristics
 Sample size (N) Numerical value Sample size for which results are reported
 Age 1 = 0–4 years
2 = 5–18 years
3 = >18 years
 Gender 1 = >70% male
2 = >70% female
3 = mixed

Sample characteristics were coded for (a) the final number of subjects reported in the analyses; (b) the mean age of subjects, subsequently grouped into three categories of 0 to 4 years, 5 to 18 years, and older than 18 years of age; and (c) percentage of males and females, subsequently grouped into three categories of more than 70% male, more than 70% female, and mixed.

The effect size was determined based on the reported statistics, including F, p, and t values. In the absence of statistical information, we estimated the effect size based on the narrative report (as in, e.g., Lepage & Théoret, 2006). In such instances, the estimated effect size may underestimate the actual effect size if the effect is not significant but larger than zero.

Sixty percent of the studies were coded by two individuals in order to determine intercoder reliability. Kappas ranged from 0.81 to 1.00 across all moderator variables (the mean kappa was 0.92). Detailed information regarding studies included in the meta-analysis, sample characteristics, and codes assigned to each moderator for each study are provided in the Appendix and in Table 1 of the online supplemental materials.

Meta-Analytic Methods

The effect size index used for all outcome measures was Cohen’s d, the difference between the means of two conditions (execution and baseline, or observation and baseline) divided by their pooled standard deviation, assuming a within-subject correlation of .80. When the effect was reported to be significant without any further statistic, we assumed p = .05; when the effect was reported to be not significant without any further statistic we assumed p = .50 (one-sided). The Comprehensive Meta-Analysis program (version 3.0; Borenstein, Rothstein, & Cohen, 2005) was used to transform the results of the individual studies into this common metric and to combine effect sizes. Heterogeneity across sets of outcomes was assessed using the Qhomogeneity statistic. Because several data sets were heterogeneous in their effect sizes, and because random effects models are more conservative than fixed effects parameters in such cases, combined effect sizes and confidence intervals (CIs) from random effects models are presented.

We used the “trim and fill” method (Duval & Tweedie, 2000a, 2000b) to calculate the effect of potential data censoring or publication bias on the outcome of the meta-analysis. Using this method, a funnel plot was constructed of each study’s effect size against the sample size or the standard error (usually plotted as 1/SE, or precision). It is expected that this plot has the shape of a funnel, because studies with smaller sample sizes and larger standard errors have increasingly large variation in estimates of their effect size as random variation becomes increasingly influential, whereas studies with larger sample sizes have smaller variation in effect sizes (Duval & Tweedie, 2000b; Sutton, Duval, Tweedie, Abrams, & Jones, 2000). The plots should be shaped like a funnel if no data censoring is present. However, given that smaller non-significant studies are less likely to be published (the “file-drawer” problem; Mullen, 1989), studies in the bottom left-hand corner of the plot are often omitted (Sutton et al., 2000). With the trim-and-fill procedure, the k right-most studies considered to be symmetrically unmatched are trimmed and their missing counterparts are imputed or “filled” as mirror images of the trimmed outcomes. This then allows for the computation of an adjusted overall effect size and CI (Gilbody, Song, Eastwood, & Sutton, 2000). Funnel plots for mu rhythm desynchronization during execution and observation are provided in Figures 3 and 4, respectively. Lastly, we tested the influence of moderators on combined effect sizes with the Qcontrast statistic in a random effects model (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). A significant Qcontrast value indicates that the difference in effect size between subsets of studies is significant. Effect sizes of sets of studies with (partly) overlapping samples were compared using the 85% CI around the point estimates. Overlapping 85% CIs indicate the absence of a significant difference between the effect sizes (van IJzendoorn, Juffer, & Poelhuis, 2005). As some of the moderator categories’ effect sizes were not independent (i.e., different categories could contain the same subjects), 85% CIs were computed surrounding the calculated average effect size. These 85% CIs were compared as an exploratory test of whether effect sizes significantly differed from one another. An absence of overlap between 85% CIs is considered a statistically significant difference under a random effects model (Astill, Van der Heijden, van IJzendoorn, & Van Someren, 2012; Goldstein & Healy, 1995; van IJzendoorn et al., 2005). Note that we report the conventional 95% CIs in Tables 2 and 3 to test whether the effect sizes within domains are significantly different from zero, whereas in the text and in Figure 5, we also present the 85% CIs in order to compare combined effect sizes of sets of (overlapping) studies.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Funnel plot with trimmed and filled effect sizes for changes in the mu rhythm during execution. Total k = 39; k = 11 studies were trimmed and filled (red dots), resulting in an adjusted combined effect size of d = 0.38 (red diamond; 95% CI [0.29, 0.46]). The blue dots are the effect sizes of the studies included in the meta-analysis, and the blue diamond is the overall effect size of the studies included in the meta-analysis. The red diamond is the estimated overall effect size based on these studies plus the effects added because of the trim and fill technique to address publication bias: It is the smaller overall effect size. CI = confidence interval; Std diff = standardized difference.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Funnel plot with trimmed and filled effect sizes for changes in the mu rhythm during observation. Total k = 80; k = 27 studies were trimmed and filled (red dots), resulting in an adjusted combined effect size of d = 0.21 (red diamond; 95% CI [0.16, 0.26]). The blue dots are the effect sizes of the studies included in the meta-analysis, and the blue diamond is the overall effect size of the studies included in the meta-analysis. The red diamond is the estimated overall effect size based on these studies plus the effects added because of the trim and fill technique to address publication bias: It is the smaller overall effect size. CI = confidence interval; Std diff = standardized difference.

Table 2.

Combined Effect Sizes for Mu Desynchronization During Execution

Moderators k N d 95% CI Q homogeneity Qa contrast
Total for mu desynchronization over central scalp locations 39 701 .46*** .39, .54 65.88**
Object-directed .13
 No 16 268 .48*** .36, .60 17.75
 Yes 23 433 .45*** .35, .55 47.99***
Type of baseline 1.59
 Static 11 229 .47*** .33, .61 22.19*
 Dynamic 10 200 .39*** .25, .54 7.48
 No stimulus 18 272 .51*** .39, .63 33.28**
Type of baseline .71
 Biological 6 143 .43*** .24, .61 4.46
 Nonbiological 14 253 .46*** .33, .58 23.22*
 No stimulus 18 272 .51*** .39, .63 33.28**
Age .07
 0–4 years 4 85 .43*** .20, .66 3.30
 5–18 years 2 30 .61*** .25, .97 .34
 >18 years 33 586 .46*** .38, .55 60.72**
Gender .19
 >70% male 2 30 .61*** .27, .95 .34
 >70% female 6 118 .46*** .30, .63 19.27**
 Mixed 25 488 .42*** .34, .50 31.50
Effect size 15.85***
 Reported 31 559 .52*** .45, .59 43.21
 Estimated 8 142 .21** .08, .34 3.65
Noncentral scalp locations n.a.
 Frontal 8 119 .19* .03, .35 12.23
 Parietal 6 117 .27** .09, .46 10.23
 Temporal 2 26 .30* .01, .59 .01
 Occipital 9 216 .14* .01, .27 14.83

Note. k = number of study outcomes; N = total sample size; d = effect size (Cohen’s d); 95% CI = 95% confidence interval around the point estimate of the effect size; Qhomogeneity = homogeneity statistic; Qcontrast = moderation statistic; n.a. = not applicable.

a

Subgroups with k < 4 excluded from contrast.

*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Table 3.

Combined Effect Sizes for Mu Desynchronization During Observation

Moderators k N d 95% CI Q homogeneity Qa contrast
Total for mu desynchronization for central scalp locations 80 1508 .31*** .26, .35 161.70***
Object-directed .65
 No 32 668 .29*** .22, .35 82.34***
 Yes 48 840 .32*** .26, .38 73.86**
Type of stimulus observed 5.74
 Biological 69 1320 .30*** .26, .35 141.24*** .49
 Social 6 109 .25*** .10, .41 7.07
 Nonsocial 63 1211 .31*** .26, .36 134.16***
Nonbiological 5 99 .51*** .31, .71 1.64
Biological & nonbiological 4 66 .23 −.00, .46 2.66
Auditory 2 23 .15 −.03, .34 .12
Type of stimulus observed .72
 Dynamic 68 1271 .31*** .26, .36 140.95***
 Static 9 204 .37*** .24, .50 9.00
Type of stimulus observed .32
 Live 16 284 .33*** .23, .44 20.32
 Video 54 1021 .30*** .24, .35 119.00***
Type of baseline 2.00
 Static 27 510 .33*** .26, .40 32.48
 Dynamic 31 590 .31*** .24, .38 49.32*
 No stimulus 21 396 .26*** .18, .33 60.90***
Type of baseline 1.91
 Biological 12 241 .32*** .21, .43 14.80
 Nonbiological 46 859 .32*** .26, .38 67.27*
 No stimulus 21 396 .26*** .18, .33 60.90***
Age 3.41
 0–4 years 17 360 .23*** .14, .32 19.93
 5–18 years 8 112 .33*** .18, .48 6.38
 >18 years 55 1036 .33*** .28, .38 131.43***
Gender 1.65
 >70% male 9 124 .38*** .24, .53 4.76
 >70% female 15 301 .27*** .18, .36 20.94
Mixed 49 950 .31*** .25, .36 109.31***
Effect size 17.64***
 Reported 57 1059 .36*** .31, .42 123.07***
 Estimated 23 449 .18*** .11, .25 18.84
Noncentral scalp locations n.a.
 Frontal 14 263 .21*** .10, .31 22.67*
 Parietal 9 125 .39*** .23, .54 10.86
 Temporal 3 36 .23 −.00, .47 .56
 Occipital 16 394 .28*** .19, .38 31.53**

Note. k = number of study outcomes; N = total sample size; d = effect size (Cohen’s d); 95% CI = 95% confidence interval around the point estimate of the effect size; Qhomogeneity = homogeneity statistic; Qcontrast = moderation statistic; n.a. = not applicable.

a

Subgroups with k < 4 excluded from contrast.

*

p < .05.

*

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Confidence intervals (85%) by scalp locations for effect size of desynchronization during execution (A) and observation (B).

Results

Mu Rhythm Desynchronization During Execution

Overall effect

For mu desynchronization during execution, the combined effect size of the 39 studies was d = 0.46 (N = 701; 95% CI [0.39, 0.54]; p < .01), in a heterogeneous set of outcomes (Q = 65.88, p < .01; see Table 2). This indicates a moderate effect size according to Cohen’s (1988) criteria. The fail-safe number was 2,646, that is, 2,646 studies with null results would be needed to reduce the overall significant result to nonsignificance. This number is larger than Rosenthal’s (1991) criterion of 5k + 10 (k = the number of studies included in the meta-analysis), indicating that the overall effect size is quite robust assuming a zero effect size. With a criterion of d = 0.10 as a baseline, Orwin’s fail-safe number was 129. More than 100 studies with null results would render the overall result not significantly different from d = 0.10. The trim-and-fill approach showed that 11 studies had to be trimmed and filled, with a resulting adjusted combined effect size of d = 0.38 (95% CI [0.29, 0.46]). Thus, results indicate a significant overall effect size for mu desynchronization during action execution.

Theoretically relevant moderators

Whether the execution of an action was object-directed or not did not moderate the effect size. Note that the moderator biological motion was only examined for observation conditions.

Methodological moderators and sample characteristics

Type of baseline did not moderate the effect size, nor did any sample characteristics (age, gender). The only significant moderator of the combined effect size for mu desynchronization during execution was whether the effect size was specified in the study or conservatively estimated on the basis of its being significant or not significant (reported vs. estimated). Studies reporting exact effect sizes showed stronger effects (d = 0.52) than studies reporting that mu desynchronization was significant or not significant without further specification (d = 0.21), Q(1) = 15.85, p < .001.

Topographic specificity

Noncentral scalp locations (frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital) showed overlapping 85% CIs (85% CI for frontal [0.07, 0.31]; parietal [0.13, 0.41]; temporal [0.09, 0.51]; occipital [0.05, 0.23]), indicating no differences in effect size between these four noncentral scalp locations (see Table 2 in the online supplemental material for studies that report statistics on noncentral scalp locations). However, the 85% CIs of the effect sizes for the frontal and occipital scalp locations were not overlapping with the 85% CI of the effect size for the central scalp locations (85% CI [0.41, 0.52]), indicating that the effects for these two noncentral scalp locations were significantly smaller than the effects for the central scalp locations (see Figure 5a). Thus, there is evidence for topographic specificity for mu desynchronization during action execution. Specifically, it was strongest for central scalp locations compared with frontal and occipital scalp locations.

Mu Rhythm Desynchronization During Observation

Overall effect

The combined effect size of the 80 studies on mu desynchronization during observation was d = 0.31 (N = 1,508; 95% CI [0.26, 0.35]; p < .01) in a heterogeneous set of outcomes (Q = 161.70, p < .01; see Table 3). This indicates a small to moderate effect size according to Cohen’s (1988) criteria. The fail-safe number was 7,949, that is, 7,949 studies with null results would be needed to reduce the overall significant result to nonsignificance. Again, this number is larger than Rosenthal’s (1991) criterion, indicating that the overall effect size is quite robust. With a criterion of d = 0.10 as a baseline, Orwin’s fail-safe number was 122, indicating that 122 additional studies with null results would render the overall effect not significantly different from d = 0.10. The trim-and-fill approach showed that 27 studies had to be trimmed and filled, with a resulting adjusted combined effect size of d = 0.21 (95% CI [0.16, 0.26]). Thus, as with mu desynchronization during execution, there was a significant effect size for mu during observation of action.

Theoretically relevant moderators

Neither of the theoretically relevant moderators (i.e., object-directed action or biological motion) was significant. If anything, observing biological stimuli tended to be associated with smaller effect sizes (d = 0.30) compared with observing nonbiological stimuli (d = 0.51).

Methodological moderators and sample characteristics

Type of baseline did not moderate the effect size. Gender was a significant moderator of the effect size. Although the overall Q statistic for the difference between the three groups (>70% male, >70% female, mixed) was not significant (see Table 3), the contrast between samples with a majority of females versus a majority of males indicated that studies with predominantly male samples (d = 0.38) showed significantly more mu desynchronization to observation of action than studies with predominantly female samples (d = 0.27), Q (1) = 4.24, p = .04. Age was not a significant moderator. As with action execution, whether the effect size was specified in the study or estimated (reported vs. estimated) on the basis of it being significant or not significant was a significant moderator. Studies reporting exact effect sizes showed stronger effects (d = 0.36) than studies reporting that mu desynchronization was significant or not significant without further specification (d = 0.20), Q(1) = 17.645, p < .001.

Topographic specificity

Noncentral scalp locations (frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital) showed no differences in effect size; the 85% CIs (85% CI for frontal [0.13, 0.29]; parietal [0.27, 0.50]; temporal [0.01, 0.44]; occipital [0.22, 0.35]) were overlapping for all four noncentral scalp locations, and each of them was also overlapping with the 85% CI of the combined effect size based on the central scalp locations (85% CI [0.27, 0.34]; see Figure 5b). Note that for these analyses, the subsets of studies with assessments for different scalp locations, especially the temporal scalp locations (k = 3), were small (see Table 2 in online supplemental materials for studies that report statistics on noncentral scalp locations). However, it appears that mu desynchronization to action observation does not show topographic specificity to central scalp locations.

Comparing Mu Desynchronization During Action Execution to Mu Desynchronization During Action Observation

A final analysis compared the effect sizes of the two conditions. The 85% CI of the combined effect size for mu desynchronization during execution (85% CI [0.41, 0.52]) did not overlap with that of the combined effect size for mu desynchronization during observation (85% CI [0.27, 0.34]), showing that execution was associated with more mu desynchronization than observation.

Power Analysis and Publication Bias

Mu desynchronization studies using EEG often involve small sample sizes, and studies in this area may be underpowered, which might add to the risk of publication bias against smaller studies with nonsignificant effect sizes. We performed a power analysis with G*Power 3.1 program (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) to compute the minimum sample size required for an individual study to reach the combined effect size that we found in the current meta-analysis (i.e., the assumed population effect size) with a power of 0.80 and a one-sided significance level of 0.05. We assume t test type of statistics for a within-subject or matched-pairs design. The power analysis indicated that a sample size of N = 31 would be required for an individual study to detect the combined effect size of d = 0.46 with a power of 0.80 for execution, and a sample size of N = 66 to detect the combined effect size of d = 0.31 for observation. We also calculated the actual power values of the smallest and largest study within our set of studies to estimate the range of power of the included studies to detect the combined effect size. The power values of the included studies to detect the combined effect size for execution ranged from 0.14 for the study with the smallest sample size (N = 3; Pfurtscheller & Neuper, 1992) to 0.88 for the study with the largest sample size (N = 39; Woodruff, Martin, & Bilyk, 2011). For observation, the power values ranged from 0.16 for the study with the smallest sample size (N = 6; Holmes, Collins, & Calmels, 2006) to 0.61 for the study with the largest sample size (N = 40; Cochin, Barthélémy, Lejeune, Roux, & Martineau, 1998). Certainly for EEG studies on observation, larger sample sizes are needed to detect the expected modest effect sizes and to create a cumulative science (Hulley, Cummings, Browner, Grady, & Newman, 2013). Finally, the risk for finding null effects is rather high in this area because of the small Ns of studies using EEG and the rather modest population effect sizes, and thus the chance of publication bias is increased. Indeed, highly significant Begg and Mazumdar rank correlation, and Egger’s regression intercept, supported the suggestion that considerable publication bias was present (Borenstein, 2005) during both action execution and action observation. Although a publication bias of modest size was found, fail-safe, funnel plot, and trim-and-fill approaches also showed that key findings of this meta-analysis remain significant and theoretically meaningful with the inclusion of all relevant studies. Our findings are therefore in line with the second category of Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, and Rothstein’s (2007, p. 286) publication bias categorization: The impact of publication bias is modest but, critically, the main findings are still valid.

Discussion

At the broadest level, the results of this meta-analysis support the notion that there is consistent EEG mu desynchronization during action execution and action observation. The effect sizes for mu desynchronization during execution (d = 0.46) and observation (d = 0.31) are both significant. Further, these effects occur across diverse experimental conditions that vary in methodology (e.g., type of stimulus observed) and across a variety of actions (e.g., object-directed and non-object-directed). For action execution, results demonstrated topographic specificity for central scalp locations (i.e., greater desynchronization for central compared with frontal and occipital), but this topographic specificity was not found for action observation (although few studies in general reported activity from multiple scalp locations). Additionally, the effect size during execution was significantly greater than the effect size during observation. Note that for both execution and observation, studies that reported the exact effect size (vs. estimated) showed stronger effects; studies that did not report exact effect sizes were assumed to have p values of .05, and thus analyses potentially underestimated the combined effect size for action observation and action execution. Even so, and despite potential differences in topographic specificity, results of the meta-analysis revealed significant overall effects for both execution and observation of action. Thus, data presented here provide robust evidence that EEG mu rhythm is a valid index of neural mirroring.

Examining Neural Mirroring: EEG Mu Versus fMRI

In line with our results, meta-analyses using fMRI to investigate the mirror system also demonstrate mirroring properties in the brain. Specifically, fMRI meta-analyses show that several brain regions are active during both execution and observation of action (Caspers et al., 2010; Molenberghs et al., 2012), suggesting that neural mirroring can be indexed with fMRI. As further comparison with fMRI investigations, two moderators of mu desynchronization were examined in the present meta-analysis, given their theoretical importance and their examination in studies of neural mirroring using fMRI methods: (a) object-directed versus non-object-directed actions, and (b) biological versus nonbiological motions. In both cases, we did not find these to be significant moderators of the effect sizes for mu desynchronization during either execution or observation of action. Lack of power for moderator tests might be an explanation, but the current set of studies is not exceptionally small compared with other meta-analyses (Bakermans-Kranenburg & van IJzendoorn, 2013; van IJzendoorn et al., 2005), and the number of moderators is restricted.

The lack of moderating effects here for both object-directed action and biological motion is somewhat congruent with fMRI studies. For object-directed action, meta-analyses of fMRI data suggest that there is greater activation for object-directed actions compared with non-object-directed actions in human mirror regions. However, across fMRI studies, the particular brain regions activated vary. For example, Van Overwalle and Baetens (2009) report that object-directed action increases the activation of the anterior intraparietal sulcus, whereas for the premotor cortex, it does not. Caspers and colleagues (2010), on the other hand, report activation differences between observation of object-related and non-object-related hand actions in the frontoparietal and temporo-occipital areas. Further, some fMRI studies have shown that non-object-directed communicative gestures activate these regions to a similar degree (Montgomery, Isenberg, & Haxby, 2007).

The lack of moderation of the EEG mu rhythm by whether the type of stimulus observed had biological motion or not also appears to be consistent with human brain imaging studies of neural mirroring. In their review of fMRI studies, Morin and Grèzes (2008) report mixed findings for premotor activity during observation of biological and nonbiological motion—specifically, there is no consistent activation of the same premotor area (whether BA 6 or BA 44) across studies that compare observation of biological and nonbiological motion. In a separate meta-analysis of fMRI studies, observation of biological movement (e.g., whole-body movements such as walking) did not recruit the human mirror areas (Van Overwalle & Baetens, 2009).

The lack of specificity for both object-directed action and biological motion in the current meta-analysis, and the inconsistent findings in meta-analyses of fMRI studies, calls into question exactly what types of stimuli or contexts will elicit mirroring activity in humans. Indeed, the literature on mirror neuron research in nonhuman primates debates the importance of goal-directed actions in eliciting mirror neuron activity (e.g., Cook & Bird, 2013; Cook et al., 2014; Fabbri-Destro & Rizzolatti, 2008). It may be that goal-directed action (vs. simply object-directed) is a more effective moderator of human mirroring activity. Unfortunately, this distinction is rarely examined in the fMRI or EEG mu mirroring literatures.

The lack of a moderating effect for biological motion in both mu rhythm and fMRI data may be a result of the manner in which human subjects perceive or interpret nonbiological motion. For example, what may begin as a “nonbiological” stimulus may change across the duration of the study, as humans have the ability to anthropomorphize even nonbiological objects. Similarly, “non-biological” stimuli may imply the presence of an agent versus presenting as truly inanimate objects.

Two moderators that were not examined in the present meta-analysis but were examined in fMRI meta-analyses are type of instructions given to participants and type of body part observed. Both of these moderators affect the pattern of activation in fMRI studies (Caspers et al., 2010). Within the mu literature, most studies examined observation of hand movements. Very few examined movements of other parts of the body (e.g., foot movement, although, see Saby, Meltzoff, & Marshall, 2013). With regard to type of instructions, Caspers et al. (2010) found that differences in instruction—asking subjects to either passively observe or have the intent to imitate—moderated activity in mirror regions. Information about type of instructions was unavailable across the majority of studies included in the current meta-analysis.

Methodological Issues and Approaches to Move Ahead

The results of this meta-analysis provide an opportunity to identify methodological issues to the study of mirroring and the use of EEG mu desynchronization that could improve the quality of the data and potentially clarify some of the issues raised with respect to the assessment of a human mirror system.

Experimental design

There are several design issues that should be addressed in future research. First, as uncovered in the current meta-analysis, the majority of EEG studies do not include examination of mu desynchronization during both action observation and action execution. Such a within-subject design would allow identification of mu desynchronization to the same stimulus during execution and observation. With this design, one could identify overlap and similarity (i.e., the effects of moderators, topography, and spectral characteristics) between execute and observe conditions in the same set of participants, and thus provide a more precise assessment of mirroring in the EEG.

Control conditions and the confound of attention

In addition to having conditions in which an action is executed and in which an action is observed, studies should have a third condition in which no action is executed or observed but participants are required to engage in the same attention shifts across the scene or view the same stimuli acted upon in the action condition. EEG during these control conditions could be subtracted from the EEG in each of the two action conditions to examine amplitude and suppression during action more precisely. Such controls are critical given that EEG recorded off the scalp is the sum of signals from multiple areas of the cortex. In addition, the mu signal recorded from electrodes over sensorimotor areas is at the same frequency as the alpha signal (8–13 Hz in adults)—a signal exquisitely sensitive to changes in attention state. Control conditions would help account for the effects of and differences in attention to type of stimulus observed.

The fact that the mu signal recorded from electrodes over sensorimotor areas peaks at frequencies in the same band as the posterior alpha rhythm (8–13 Hz in adults) complicates the task of distinguishing between patterns of desynchronization attributable to the two rhythms, especially given mu and alpha can exist simultaneously in overlapping cortical areas (Andrew & Pfurtscheller, 1997). Recent work has established that patterns of alpha activity are highly dependent on various aspects of attentional state. Indeed, it has been well established that deployment of spatial attention to the left or right side of visual space in anticipation of a target results in lateralization of alpha-band EEG amplitude over posterior scalp areas, such that alpha amplitude is enhanced over the hemisphere ipsilateral to the attended hemifield (Kelly, Lalor, Reilly, & Foxe, 2006; Rihs, Michel, & Thut, 2007; Worden, Foxe, Wang, & Simpson, 2000), or else suppressed over the contralateral hemifield (Sauseng et al., 2005; Thut, Nietzel, Brandt, & Pascual-Leone, 2006; Yamagishi, Goda, Callan, Anderson, & Kawato, 2005). These complementary effects have been interpreted two ways. On the one hand, alpha desynchronization has been proposed to enhance the efficacy of the response to the stimulus at the attended location. On the other, alpha synchronization has been proposed to suppress processing of information at the unattended location. In support of the notion that alpha oscillations might serve as a mechanism for active suppression of unwanted information processing in general, Snyder and Foxe (2010) have shown alpha amplitude is also modulated by shifts in featural attention, such that alpha amplitude is enhanced in areas thought to be actively processing the unattended feature. In line with this, work by Romei, Gross, and Thut (2010) has shown that alpha oscillations induced by rhythmic TMS diminish subjects’ ability to detect visual targets in the visual hemifield contralateral to the stimulated hemisphere. Such findings obviate the importance of considering attentional confounds in studies analyzing the modulation of alpha-band EEG, which, critically, includes mu rhythm. To begin to parse the role of attention in investigations of mu desynchronization during action observation, eye-tracking methods—which offer an index of attention by measuring shifts in gaze and gaze duration—may be particularly useful. Additionally, different frequency bands (e.g., beta) should be examined for mirroring properties. Frequencies outside of the alpha range may be less sensitive to attentional confounds, potentially circumventing issues noted in this section. Further research suggests that examination of desynchronization in the beta frequency (e.g., 15–25 Hz) may be more sensitive to action observation (Hari et al., 1998).

Chronometry of mu desynchronization

Few studies have taken advantage of the ability to use EEG to assess the chronometry of mu desynchronization (Avanzini et al., 2012; Brown, Wiersema, Pourtois, & Brüne, 2013; Heimann, Umiltà, & Gallese, 2013; Muthukumaraswamy & Johnson, 2004b; Nyström, 2008; Orgs et al., 2008). There were too few studies to systematically examine timing effects on mu desynchronization in the present meta-analysis. Changes in mu desynchronization may take place before, during, or after observation of an action. Identifying the time course of mu during both action observation and action execution would provide further evidence of mirroring effects in the brain.

Topographic specificity and source localization

Although EEG traditionally has not been seen as a metric that can provide information regarding the source of its signal, there are recent improvements in methods for source localization that would help identify the brain areas that are activated during execution or observation of an action. A recent study by Thorpe and colleagues (2015) conducted a source localization of mu during execution and identified brain areas quite similar to those identified using fMRI methods. Other neuroimaging techniques with good spatial resolution (e.g., fMRI) may also help inform the cortical specificity of the mu rhythm and its relation to human mirror activity. Research that examines connections between mu desynchronization and fMRI Blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signal could help further evaluation of mu as a tool for inferring mirror system activity. As an example, studies of sequential EEG and fMRI (Ball et al., 1999; Braadbaart, Williams, & Waiter, 2013) and simultaneous EEG and fMRI (Arnstein, Cui, Keysers, Maurits, & Gazzola, 2011) demonstrated links between these two brain signals.

Additional moderators

Two additional moderators warrant attention in future research. First, meta-analytic results indicated that the effect size for mu desynchronization was significantly greater for studies with predominately male participants compared with studies with predominately female participants, in particular for observation of action. This result was unexpected and warrants further investigation. Second, the meta-analysis found no significant age differences in the effect sizes for mu desynchronization during observation and execution. When examining mu rhythm desynchronization, studies with infants and young children use different mu frequency bands compared with studies with adults, as the alpha band changes with age (Marshall, Bar-Haim, & Fox, 2002). Future research investigating the development of mu desynchronization during execution and observation of action may identify changes in the mirror system with experience or age.

Conclusion

There has been a great deal of interest and excitement in multiple areas of psychology about the possible existence of a mirror system in the human brain. This system—thought of in some instances as a core neural mechanism underlying links between action and perception—has great intuitive appeal, and the existence of mirror neurons in the nonhuman primate brain spurred on the interest in human neuroscience and psychology for a neural mirroring mechanism. This interest extends to the field of developmental psychology, in which research on the emergence of social cognition, including imitation, empathy, and theory of mind, could potentially benefit from identification of a neural mirroring system.

The present meta-analysis reveals significant effect sizes for mu desynchronization for both execution and observation of action, thus suggesting that measuring the mu rhythm during action execution and action observation is a valid tool for indexing neural mirroring. Although the meta-analysis provides clear evidence for mirroring using EEG, additional findings warrant attention to clarify the specificity of EEG mu as an index for a mirror system. We did not find evidence that mu desynchronization was specific to either object-directed action or biological motion, nor was there evidence for topographic specificity of mu desynchronization for action observation. Future work in this area must attend to experimental details including design, use of appropriate controls for attention confounds, examination of multiple scalp locations, and additional frequency bands. The current results should be placed within the context of the greater body of work from multiple brain imaging methods that have identified a mirror system in the human brain. Critically, results of the current meta-analysis bolster the general pattern of results across other imaging modalities and provide clear evidence that mu desynchronization can index neural mirroring.

Acknowledgments

Nathan A. Fox, Kathryn H. Yoo, Lindsay C. Bowman, Erin N. Cannon, Ross E. Vanderwert and Pier F. Ferrari were supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (P01 HD 064653). Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg and Marinus H. van IJzendoorn were supported by awards from The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (SPINOZA and VICI, respectively) and by the Gravitation Program of the Dutch Ministry of Education, Culture, and Science and the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO grant number 024.001.003). We thank Samuel Thorpe for his assistance with a MATLAB script for the simulated mu desynchronization provided in Figure 1.

Appendix. List of Studies Included in the Meta-Analysis and Their Effect Size Estimates (95% CI)

Study Subsamples Age group Gender N Execution d [95% CI] Observation d [95% CI]
Andelin (2011) >18 years Mixed 30 0.53 [0.27, 0.79] 0.21 [–0.03, 0.44]
Avanzini et al. (2012) >18 years Mixed 11 0.68 [0.21, 1.15]
Babiloni et al. (2008) >18 years Mixed 12 0.52 [0.22, 0.81]
Behmer & Jantzen (2011) Musicians >18 years Mixed 12 0.69 [0.24, 1.14]
Nonmusicians >18 years Mixed 12 0.69 [0.24, 1.14]
Bernier et al. (2007) Nonclinical >18 years >70% male 15 0.95 [0.48, 1.41] 1.01 [0.53, 1.50]
Bernier et al. (2013) 5–18 years >70% male 14 0.37 [0.01, 0.72]
Bilyk (2012) 18–28 years >70% female 37 0.19 [–0.03, 0.40]
Braadbaart et al. (2013) >18 years >70% male 15 0.19 [–0.18, 0.56]
Brown et al. (2013) >18 years >70% female 17 0.33 [0.01, 0.65]
Cannon et al. (2014) Novices >18 years >70% female 12 0.27 [0.01, 0.54]
Observers >18 years >70% male 10 0.62 [0.28, 0.96]
Performers >18 years >70% female 11 0.88 [0.35, 1.40]
Cochin et al. (1998) >18 years Mixed 40 0.26 [0.11, 0.40]
Cochin et al. (1999) >18 years Mixed 20 0.46 [0.15, 0.77] 0.58 [0.25, 0.91]
Cochin et al. (2001) 0–4 years N/Rb 30 0.21 [0.11, 0.32]
Colombi (2008) Nonclinical 5–18 years >70% male 9 0.40 [–0.05, 0.86]
Experiment 5–18 years >70% male 29 0.04 [–0.05, 0.14]
2/nonclinical
Experiment 5–18 years >70% male 19 0.00 [–0.28, 0.28]
3/nonclinical
Cooper et al. (2013) >18 years Mixed 20 0.11 [–0.17, 0.39]
Cuellar et al. (2012) Experiment 1 >18 years >70% female 10 0.17 [0.04, 0.30]
Experiment 2 >18 years >70% female 13 0.14 [0.04, 0.24]
de Klerk et al. (2015)a 0–4 years Mixed 31 0.00 [–0.22, 0.22]
Derambure et al. (1997) Nonclinical >18 years N/Rb 10 0.65 [0.17, 1.14]
Désy & Lepage (2013) >18 years Mixed 18 0.65 [0.29, 1.01] 0.66 [0.30, 1.02]
Fargier et al. (2012) >18 years Mixed 16 0.11 [–0.20, 0.42]
Francuz & Zapała (2011) >18 years Mixed 10 0.96 [0.38, 1.53]
Frenkel-Toledo et al. (2013) >18 years Mixed 27 1.16 [0.23, 2.08] 1.25 [0.31, 2.19]
Giromini et al. (2010) >18 years Mixed 19 0.51 [0.18, 0.84]
Gutsell & Inzlicht (2010) Mixed 30 0.51 [0.25, 0.77] 0.14 [−0.09, 0.37]
Heimann et al. (2013) >18 years Mixed 15 0.68 [0.28, 1.08] 0.68 [0.28, 1.08]
Holmes et al. (2006) >18 years >70% female 6 0.00 [–0.51, 0.51] 0.00 [–0.51, 0.51]
Kranczioch et al. (2008) >18 years >70% female 13 0.71 [0.23, 1.19]
Kumar et al. (2013) N/Rb >70% female 17 0.36 [0.14, 0.59]
Leocani et al. (1997) N/Rb Mixed 9 0.39 [–0.06, 0.84]
Lepage & Théoret (2006) 5–18 years Mixed 15 0.17 [–0.18, 0.52]
Li et al. (2011) >18 years Mixed 14 0.51 [0.13, 0.89]
Marshall et al. (2009) N/Rb Mixed 20 0.39 [0.09, 0.69]
Marshall et al. (2011) 0–4 years Mixed 30 0.31 [0.07, 0.55] 0.31 [0.07, 0.56]
Martineau & Cochin (2003) Nonclinical 0–4 years Mixed 14 0.07 [–0.14, 0.29]
Martineau et al. (2008) 5–18 years Mixed 14 0.02 [–0.31, 0.35]
McCormick et al. (2012) >18 years >70% male 16 0.33 [0.20, 0.47]
McGarry et al. (2012) >18 years >70% female 33 0.22 [0.00, 0.45] 0.22 [0.00, 0.45]
Meyer et al. (2011) 0–4 years >70% male 7 0.83 [0.19, 1.48]
Milston et al. (2013) >18 years >70% female 26 0.46 [0.19, 0.74] 0.46 [0.19, 0.74]
Moore et al. (2008) >18 years Mixed 34 0.29 [0.07, 0.51]
Moore et al. (2012) N/Rb Mixed 11 0.37 [0.19, 0.55]
Müller-Putz et al. (2007) Nonclinical >18 years >70% male 9 0.49 [0.02, 0.96]
Muthukumaraswamy & Johnson (2004a) >18 years Mixed 13 0.38 [0.01, 0.76]
Muthukumaraswamy & Johnson (2004b) >18 years Mixed 8 0.00 [–0.44, 0.44] 0.00 [–0.44, 0.44]
Muthukumaraswamy et al. (2004) >18 years Mixed 12 0.40 [0.01, 0.79] 0.40 [0.01, 0.79]
Nyström (2008) Adults >18 years N/Rb 15 0.35 [0.01, 0.69]
Infants 0–4 years N/Rb 19 0.31 [0.00, 0.61]
Nyström et al. (2011) 0–4 years N/Rb 23 0.27 [0.00, 0.54]
Oberman et al. (2007) Experiment 1 >18 years >70% female 17 0.36 [0.04, 0.69]
Experiment 2 >18 years >70% female 20 0.51 [0.19, 0.83]
Oberman et al. (2008) Nonclinical 5–18 years >70% male 13 0.61 [0.19, 1.02]
Oberman et al. (2005) Nonclinical 5–18 years >70% male 10 0.72 [0.22, 1.23] 0.40 [–0.03, 0.83]
Orgs et al. (2008) Dancers >18 years Mixed 10 0.96 [0.38, 1.53]
Nondancers >18 years Mixed 10 0.23 [–0.18, 0.64]
Perry & Bentin (2009) >18 years Mixed 24 0.23 [–0.04, 0.49]
Perry & Bentin (2010) >18 years Mixed 24 0.13 [–0.13, 0.39]
Perry et al. (2011) >18 years Mixed 28 0.25 [0.00, 0.49] 0.25 [0.00, 0.49]
Perry et al. (2010) >18 years Mixed 24 0.55 [0.26, 0.85]
Pfurtscheller & Neuper (1992) N/Rb N/Rb 3 1.57 [0.13, 3.02]
Pineda & Oberman (2006) >18 years Mixed 18 0.32 [0.01, 0.62] 0.20 [–0.01, 0.41]
Pineda et al. (2011) >18 years Mixed 24 0.56 [0.26, 0.86]
Porcelli et al. (2013) >18 years >70% female 24 0.37 [–0.53, 1.26]
Puzzo et al. (2010) >18 years Mixed 20 0.38 [0.08, 0.68]
Raymaekers et al. (2009) Nonclinical 5–18 years >70% male 20 0.54 [0.22, 0.87] 0.41 [0.11, 0.72]
Reid et al. (2011) 0–4 years Mixed 10 0.67 [0.18, 1.16] 0.11 [−0.29, 0.50]
Ruysschaert et al. (2013) Live setting group 0–4 years Mixed 18 0.30 [–0.01, 0.60]
Video setting group 0–4 years Mixed 16 0.00 [–0.31, 0.31)
Schuch et al. (2010) >18 years >70% female 27 0.99 [0.63, 1.34] 0.57 [0.28, 0.85]
Serrien & Brown (2003) N/Rb N/Rb 6 0.66 [0.03, 1.29]
Silas et al. (2010) >18 years Mixed 33 0.43 [0.19, 0.67] 0.35 [0.12, 0.58]
Southgate & Begus (2013) 0–4 years Mixed 33 0.20 [–0.19, 0.59]
Southgate et al. (2010) 0–4 years Mixed 22 0.29 [0.01, 0.56]
Southgate et al. (2009) 0–4 years Mixed 15 0.71 [0.30, 1.12] 0.42 [0.07, 0.77]
Stancák & Pfurtscheller (1996a) >18 years Mixed 10 0.45 [0.01, 0.89]
Stancák et al. (1997) >18 years Mixed 13 0.38 [0.01, 0.76]
Stancák & Pfurtscheller (1996b) >18 years Mixed 23 0.27 [0.00, 0.54]
Streltsova et al. (2010) >18 years Mixed 11 1.18 [0.56, 1.81] 0.41 [0.20, 0.61]
Ulloa & Pineda (2007) >18 years >70% female 20 0.30 [0.00, 0.59]
Upshaw et al. (2014)a 0–4 years N/Rb 23 0.25 [–0.24, 0.74] 0.06 [–0.12, 0.24]
van Elk et al. (2008) 0–4 years Mixed 12 0.40 [0.01, 0.79]
Virji-Babul et al. (2012)c 0–4 years Mixed 10
Warreyn et al. (2012) 0–4 years Mixed 17 0.48 [0.14, 0.82] 0.40 [0.07, 0.73]
Woodruff et al. (2011) N/Rb Mixed 39 0.28 [0.07, 0.48] 0.28 [0.07, 0.48]
Wriesnegger et al. (2013) >18 years >70% female 17 0.14 [0.02, 0.26]
Yang et al. (2009) >18 years >70% male 16 0.13 [–0.13, 0.39]
Yuan et al. (2010) >18 years Mixed 10 0.96 [0.38, 1.53]

Note. CI - confidence interval.

a

Unpublished at the time of data request.

b

Specific age/gender distribution of participants was not reported (N/R); thus, we could not assign a code.

c

95% CI for observation was calculated for parietal (0.41[–0.02, 0.85]) and temporal (0.11[–0.28, 0.51]) areas.

Footnotes

Contributor Information

Nathan A. Fox, University of Maryland

Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, Leiden University

Kathryn H. Yoo, University of Maryland

Lindsay C. Bowman, University of Maryland

Erin N. Cannon, University of Maryland

Ross E. Vanderwert, Cardiff University

Pier F. Ferrari, University of Parma

Marinus H. van IJzendoorn, Leiden University

References

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis.

RESOURCES