Abstract
Background
This study aimed to examine rural–urban differences in the prevalence of cognitive impairment in Japan.
Methods
We targeted 592 residents aged 65 years and older who did not use long-term care insurance services in one rural and two urban areas in Ojiya City, Japan. Of these, 537 (90.7 %) participated in the study. The revised Hasegawa’s dementia scale (HDS-R) was used to assess cognitive function, and cognitive impairment was defined as a HDS-R score ≤20. Lifestyle information was obtained through interviews. The prevalence of cognitive impairment was compared according to the levels of predictor variables by odds ratios (ORs) calculated by a logistic regression analysis.
Results
Mean age of participants was 75.7 years (SD 7.0). The prevalence of cognitive impairment was 20/239 (8.4 %) in the rural area and 6/298 (2.0 %) in the urban areas, for a total of 26/537 (4.8 %) overall. Men tended to have a higher prevalence of cognitive impairment (P = 0.0628), and age was associated with cognitive impairment (P for trend <0.0001). The rural area had a significantly higher prevalence of cognitive impairment (age- and sex-adjusted OR = 4.04, 95 % CI: 1.54–10.62) than urban areas. This difference was significant after adjusting for other lifestyle factors.
Conclusions
The prevalence of cognitive impairment was higher in the rural area relative to urban areas in Ojiya city. This regional difference suggests the existence of potentially modifiable factors other than lifestyle in relation to cognitive impairment.
Keywords: Cross-sectional studies, Mild cognitive impairment, Prevalence, Rural health, Urban health
Introduction
Dementia is becoming a global public health concern. An estimated 36 million people globally had dementia in 2010, and the number is expected to almost double every 20 years [1]. Japan, with a rapidly aging population, is no exception. There were 2.8 million individuals with dementia in 2010, and this is expected to increase to 4.7 million by 2025 [2]. These values would equate to prevalences (in people aged ≥65 years) of 9.5 % in 2010 and 12.8 % in 2025. Given this dire situation, prevention of dementia at any stage would be beneficial.
Cognitive impairment, including mild cognitive impairment (MCI: the early or intermediate stages of dementia) and dementia, poses a public health burden in society, and those with cognitive impairment are prone to age-related frailty [3], which can increase care costs [4]. Therefore, epidemiological profiles of cognitive impairment in communities warrant researching.
The prevalence of cognitive impairment increases steeply with age in elderly populations. There is also a gender difference in the prevalence of cognitive impairment [5]. In addition to demographic factors, environmental factors may also be involved in cognitive impairment or decline [6, 7]. Among them, rural–urban differences in the prevalence of cognitive impairment represent one issue that has attracted much interest. These differences imply potentially modifiable factors, which are interventional targets for dementia prevention. Geographical differences in dementia have been reported [8], and a number of socio-environmental factors, including rural–urban differences, may contribute to such differences. In this context, well-designed studies are needed to assess the contribution of rural–urban differences to cognitive impairment.
There is little evidence concerning this issue in Japan. To address this, we designed a cross-sectional study to investigate the prevalence of cognitive impairment in rural and urban communities within a city by conducting an exhaustive survey. This study aimed to determine rural–urban differences in the prevalence of cognitive impairment in one Japanese community.
Participants and methods
Participants
Ojiya city, with a population of about 38,000, is located in the middle of Niigata Prefecture, Japan. Working populations of the primary, secondary, and tertiary industries in Ojiya in 2010 were 8, 39, and 53 %, respectively [9]. The city government conducted thorough interview investigations in three model areas they had set up, namely, Heisei-cho (an urban area), Matto (a rural, farming area), and central Katakai (an urban area). Regarding Katakai, four business or residential subareas, including Ninocho, Chabatake, Omotesannocho, and Inaba, were investigated. They targeted all residents aged 65 years and older who did not use long-term care insurance. Profiles of the three study areas are shown in Table 1. There were 166 eligible individuals in Heisei-cho, 249 in Matto, and 177 in Katakai, of which 147 (88.6 %), 239 (96.0 %), and 151 (85.3 %), respectively, agreed to participate. In total, 537 of 592 (90.7 %) individuals in these three areas formed the study population. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Niigata University School of Medicine.
Table 1.
Study areas | |||
---|---|---|---|
Heisei-cho | Matto | Katakaia | |
Survey year | 2011 | 2012 | 2013 |
Residence (urban or rural) | Urban | Rural | Urban |
Proportion of farms | 0 % | 64.0 % | 4.8 % |
Proportion of people aged ≥65 years | 32.4 % | 36.4 % | 34.4 % |
Proportion of people using long-term care insurance | 16.2 % | 18.6 % | 21.0 % |
Number of people aged ≥65 yearsb | 166 | 249 | 177 |
Participation rate | 88.6 % | 96.0 % | 85.3 % |
aThe four central subareas, including Ninocho, Chabatake, Omotesannocho, and Inaba, in Katakai were investigated
bPeople using long-term care insurance were excluded
Procedure
Trained nurses in Ojiya interviewed participants in Heisei-cho between October and December 2011, in Matto between August and October 2012, and in Katakai between July and September 2013. Interviewers collected information regarding demographic characteristics, health status (including cognitive function), and lifestyle. Family environment was classified as living with family or alone, and current occupational status was classified as unemployed or employed. Histories of hypertension, cerebrovascular diseases, diabetes, and a family history of dementia were also obtained. Also, we inquired about the extent of property damage caused by the Niigata-Chuetsu earthquake, a large earthquake that hits the Niigata-Chuetsu area of Japan on October 23, 2004 [10]. Property damage was categorized as follows: (1) not damaged, (2) partially damaged (less than half), (3) half or largely (but not completely) destroyed, and (4) completely destroyed according to criteria established by the Japanese government [11].
The revised Hasegawa’s dementia scale (HDS-R) was used to assess cognitive function. The HDS-R is a simple 30-point test of general cognitive function. “Cognitively impaired” was defined as an HDS-R score ≤20 [12]. The HDS-R was originally used to screen for dementia, with a sensitivity of 0.90 and a specificity of 0.82 using a cutoff point of 20/21, and also correlates well with the mini mental state examination (MMSE) [12]. Like the MMSE, the HDS-R has been used in population-based epidemiologic studies, especially in Asia [13, 14].
Information on frequency and average daily consumption of alcohol was obtained. Alcohol consumption (equivalent to Japanese sake) was classified into five categories: (1) non-drinkers, (2) <7 gou (1 gou is equivalent to 180 mL of sake), (3) 7–13 gou, (4) 14–20 gou, and (5) ≥21 gou per week. Brinkman index was calculated based on the number of cigarettes smoked per day multiplied by the number of smoking years to estimate lifetime smoking history. Smoking status was classified as: (1) non-smoker, (2) light smoker (Brinkman index <675), and (3) heavy smoker (Brinkman index ≥675). A score of 675 was the median of current or past smokers. Usual bedtime and sleeping hours were also recorded. Regarding spare time activities, we asked whether participants played any roles in their family or community and whether they were involved in any activities for pleasure.
Statistical methods
The χ 2 test was used to test for independence of categorical data in bivariate analyses. Prevalence of cognitive impairment was calculated and compared according to the levels of predictor variables by odds ratios (ORs) calculated using a logistic regression analysis. The primary predictor variable was area of residence (urban or rural), and other predictors were demographic characteristics, disease history, lifestyle-related factors, and property damage by the earthquake. ORs, adjusted for age (a continuous variable) and sex, were calculated using multiple logistic regression analyses. Linear trends (P for trend) of ORs according to the levels of predictor variables were tested by a logistic regression analysis with discrete variables as predictors and the presence of cognitive impairment as the outcome. Data were analyzed using SAS statistical software (release 9.1.3, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
The mean age of participants was 75.7 years (SD 7.0). Participant characteristics, by area, are summarized in Table 2. Significant area differences in distribution were detected in job status, bedtime, and activities for pleasure. The prevalence of cognitive impairment was 5/147 (3.4 %) in Heisei-cho, 20/239 (8.4 %) in Matto, 1/151 (0.7 %) in Katakai, and 26/537 (4.8 %) overall.
Table 2.
Characteristics | Urban (n = 306) (Heisei-cho and Katakai) | Rural (n = 240) (Matto) | P valuea | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 0.4267 | ||||
65–69 | 73 | (23.9 %) | 51 | (21.3 %) | |
70–79 | 138 | (45.1 %) | 116 | (48.3 %) | |
80–89 | 89 | (29.1 %) | 60 | (25.0 %) | |
90–99 | 6 | (2.0 %) | 13 | (5.4 %) | |
Family environment | 0.8282 | ||||
Living with family | 23 | (7.5 %) | 17 | (7.1 %) | |
Living alone | 283 | (92.5 %) | 223 | (92.9 %) | |
Current job status | 0.0337 | ||||
Unemployed | 215 | (70.3 %) | 151 | (62.9 %) | |
Employed | 89 | (29.1 %) | 89 | (37.1 %) | |
Unknown | 2 | (0.7 %) | 0 | (0.0 %) | |
History of hypertension | 0.9722 | ||||
Absent | 151 | (49.3 %) | 118 | (49.2 %) | |
Present | 155 | (50.7 %) | 122 | (50.8 %) | |
History of cerebrovascular diseases | 0.8820 | ||||
Absent | 287 | (93.8 %) | 226 | (94.2 %) | |
Present | 19 | (6.2 %) | 14 | (5.8 %) | |
History of diabetes | 0.7327 | ||||
Absent | 274 | (89.5 %) | 217 | (90.4 %) | |
Present | 32 | (10.5 %) | 23 | (9.6 %) | |
Family history of dementia | 0.3873 | ||||
Absent | 269 | (87.9 %) | 205 | (85.4 %) | |
Present | 37 | (12.1 %) | 35 | (14.6 %) | |
Alcohol consumptionb(gou/week) | 0.7638 | ||||
Non-drinkers | 153 | (50.5 %) | 114 | (47.5 %) | |
<7 | 87 | (28.7 %) | 76 | (31.7 %) | |
7–14 | 40 | (13.2 %) | 37 | (15.4 %) | |
≥14 | 23 | (7.6 %) | 13 | (5.4 %) | |
Smoking (Brinkman index) | 0.7558 | ||||
Never-smoker | 200 | (65.4 %) | 163 | (67.9 %) | |
<675 | 67 | (21.9 %) | 40 | (16.7 %) | |
≥675 | 39 | (12.7 %) | 37 | (15.4 %) | |
Bedtime | <0.0001 | ||||
–8:59 p.m. | 25 | (8.2 %) | 63 | (26.3 %) | |
9:00–9:59 p.m. | 78 | (25.5 %) | 87 | (36.3 %) | |
10:00–10:59 p.m. | 100 | (32.7 %) | 60 | (25.0 %) | |
11:00 p.m.– | 103 | (33.7 %) | 30 | (12.5 %) | |
Hours of sleep | 0.5683 | ||||
<6 | 44 | (14.4 %) | 48 | (20.0 %) | |
6–6.9 | 77 | (25.2 %) | 57 | (23.8 %) | |
7–7.9 | 111 | (36.3 %) | 58 | (24.2 %) | |
8–8.9 | 59 | (19.3 %) | 48 | (20.0 %) | |
≥9 | 15 | (4.9 %) | 29 | (12.1 %) | |
Having roles in family or community | 0.3288 | ||||
Yes | 290 | (94.8 %) | 223 | (92.9 %) | |
No | 16 | (5.2 %) | 17 | (7.1 %) | |
Having activities for pleasure | 0.0075 | ||||
Yes | 285 | (93.1 %) | 236 | (98.3 %) | |
No | 21 | (6.9 %) | 4 | (1.7 %) | |
Property damage by the 2004 earthquake | 0.8137 | ||||
Not damaged | 30 | (9.8 %) | 30 | (12.5 %) | |
Partially damaged | 218 | (71.2 %) | 160 | (66.7 %) | |
Half or largely destroyed | 34 | (11.1 %) | 39 | (16.3 %) | |
Completely destroyed | 17 | (5.6 %) | 9 | (3.8 %) | |
Not applicable | 7 | (2.3 %) | 2 | (0.8 %) |
aUrban–rural differences tested by multiple logistic regression analyses, adjusting for sex
bEquivalent to Japanese sake (1 gou of sake is equivalent to about 27-g ethanol)
The prevalence of cognitive impairment was compared according to the levels of predictor variables. Sex- and age-adjusted ORs are presented in Table 3. Men tended to have a higher OR, with marginal significance (P = 0.0628). Age was strongly associated with cognitive impairment (P for trend <0.0001). The rural area (Matto) had a significantly higher OR (4.04) than those of the urban areas (Heisei-cho and Katakai). Regarding sleep, those with an earlier bedtime (in particular, those who went to sleep before 9 p.m.) had higher ORs. Family environment, disease history, alcohol drinking, smoking, roles in family or community, pleasure activities, and post-earthquake property damage were not significantly associated with the prevalence of cognitive impairment.
Table 3.
Predictor variables | Prevalence of cognitive impairment | Adjusted ORa (95 % CI) | P or P for trend |
---|---|---|---|
Sex | P = 0.0628 | ||
Men | 16/243 (6.6 %) | 2.25 (0.96–5.27) | |
Women | 10/294 (3.4 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Age (years) | P for trend <0.0001 | ||
65–69 | 1/121 (0.8 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
70–79 | 5/250 (2.0 %) | 2.46 (0.28–21.25) | |
80–89 | 12/147 (8.2 %) | 10.70 (1.37–83.78) | |
90–99 | 8/19 (42.1 %) | 87.77 (10.01–769.42) | |
Family environment | P = 0.3143 | ||
Living with family | 25/498 (5.0 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Living alone | 1/39 (2.6 %) | 0.33 (0.04–2.83) | |
Area of residence | P = 0.0046 | ||
Urban area (Heisei-cho and Katakai) | 6/298 (2.0 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Rural area (Matto) | 20/239 (8.4 %) | 4.04 (1.54–10.62) | |
Current job status | P = 0.4421 | ||
Unemployed | 22/361 (6.1 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Employed | 4/174 (2.3 %) | 0.63 (0.19–2.07) | |
History of hypertension | P = 0.2657 | ||
Absent | 14/265 (5.3 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Present | 12/272 (4.4 %) | 0.62 (0.27–1.44) | |
History of cerebrovascular diseases | P = 0.4350 | ||
Absent | 24/506 (4.7 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Present | 2/31 (6.5 %) | 1.86 (0.39–8.75) | |
History of diabetes | P = 0.4501 | ||
Absent | 23/484 (4.8 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Present | 3/53 (5.7 %) | 1.66 (0.45–6.16) | |
Family history of dementia | P = 0.5046 | ||
Absent | 24/466 (5.2 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Present | 2/71 (2.8 %) | 0.60 (0.13–2.70) | |
Alcohol consumptionb (gou/week) | P for trend = 0.7363 | ||
Non-drinkers | 15/263 (5.7 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
<7 | 5/160 (3.1 %) | 0.49 (0.14–1.71) | |
7–13 | 3/76 (3.9 %) | 0.72 (0.14–3.77) | |
≥14 | 3/35 (8.6 %) | 2.59 (0.44–15.30) | |
Smoking (Brinkman index) | P for trend = 0.5668 | ||
Never-smoker | 15/360 (4.2 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
<675 | 9/105 (8.6 %) | 1.48 (0.48–4.58) | |
≥675 | 2/72 (2.8 %) | 0.45 (0.08–2.49) | |
Bedtime | P for trend = 0.0392 | ||
–8:59 p.m. | 12/87 (13.8 %) | 4.80 (0.93–24.68) | |
9:00–9:59 p.m. | 7/161 (4.3 %) | 1.49 (0.26–8.61) | |
10:00–10:59 p.m. | 5/158 (3.2 %) | 1.44 (0.26-8.11) | |
11:00 p.m.– | 2/131 (1.5 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Hours of sleep | P for trend = 0.1028 | ||
<6 | 2/91 (2.2 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
6–6.9 | 4/130 (3.1 %) | 1.63 (0.27–9.82) | |
7–7.9 | 4/167 (2.4 %) | 1.06 (0.18–6.22) | |
8–8.9 | 8/105 (7.6 %) | 2.48 (0.49–12.69) | |
≥9 | 8/44 (18.2 %) | 5.14 (0.88–30.01) | |
Having roles in family or community | P = 0.1534 | ||
Yes | 20/505 (4.0 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
No | 6/32 (18.8 %) | 2.32 (0.73–7.35) | |
Having activities for pleasure | P = 0.3380 | ||
Yes | 24/513 (4.7 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
No | 2/24 (8.3 %) | 2.34 (0.41–13.32) | |
Property damage by the 2004 earthquake | P for trend = 0.6787 | ||
Not damaged | 4/59 (6.8 %) | 1 (Ref.) | |
Partially damaged | 19/372 (5.1 %) | 1.00 (0.31–3.25) | |
Half or largely destroyed | 2/72 (2.8 %) | 0.97 (0.13–7.35) | |
Completely destroyed | 1/26 (3.8 %) | 0.98 (0.04–21.84) |
aAdjusted for sex and age except for predictor variables of “sex” and “age.” “Sex” and “age” were adjusted for age and sex, respectively
bEquivalent to Japanese sake (1 gou of sake is equivalent to about 27-g ethanol)
Multiple logistic regression analyses were conducted to confirm the independence of variables, including sex, age, residential area, and bedtime (coded as 1, –8:59 p.m. and 2, 9:00 p.m.–). Age (P for trend <0.0001) and residential area (OR = 3.57, 95 % CI: 1.33–9.58, P = 0.0114) were significantly associated with cognitive impairment, but sex (OR = 2.09, 95 % CI: 0.85–5.10, P = 0.1081) and bedtime (OR = 2.06, 95 % CI: 0.83–5.13, P = 0.1185) were not.
Discussion
This study is the first to demonstrate a significant rural–urban difference in the prevalence of cognitive impairment in Japan. Cognitive impairment is associated with falls, fall-related injuries [15], osteoporotic fractures [16], frailty [3], decrease levels of activities of daily living [17], and dementia in the elderly. Thus, the increasing number of elderly individuals with cognitive impairment poses a public health burden in terms of medical and care costs in local governments. One strength of our study is that prevalences were compared between rural and urban communities in a city using an exhaustive survey with a sufficiently high participation rate. Many previous epidemiologic studies compared prevalences between rural and urban populations of different geographical regions [8, 18, 19], which may have reflected regional differences rather than rural–urban differences. The present study was designed to overcome this methodological limitation.
This study used the HDS-R to detect those with cognitive impairment. The HDS-R is frequently used in East Asian populations [13, 14], and is similar to the MMSE in its diagnostic accuracy for dementia [20]. One advantage of using the HDS-R over the MMSE is its diagnostic accuracy regardless of education level [20]. The HDS-R, like the MMSE, is a good screening tool for dementia using a cutoff value of 20/21. It can also be used to screen for MCI, although its diagnostic sensitivity for MCI appears to be lower [21, 22]. Accordingly, HDS-R-assessed cognitive impairment generally reflects dementia and MCI.
We did not include those using long-term care insurance who are at a high risk of developing cognitive impairment. This may have affected the prevalence of cognitive impairment. However, the prevalence of long-term care insurance use is similar in rural (Matto, 18.6 %) and urban (Heisei-cho and Katakai, 18.7 % on average) areas, and thus the prevalence of cognitive impairment in rural areas remained high when long-term care insurance use was considered.
A number of epidemiologic studies have provided evidence regarding rural–urban differences in the occurrence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [8, 18, 19, 23, 24]. For example, Russ et al. [8] conducted a meta-analysis and provided evidence of an association between rurality and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) incidence (OR = 1.64, 90 % CI: 1.08–2.50), although the data were not significant for overall dementia. They also suggested that rural living, especially in one’s early life, may affect the risk of developing AD [8]. In recent years, two epidemiologic studies conducted in East Asian countries reported rural–urban differences in MCI prevalence or incidence. Jia et al. [19] reported a higher prevalence of MCI in rural areas compared with urban areas, with a prevalence ratio of 1.5 (95 % CI: 1.4–1.7) in China. Bae et al. [18] reported a hazard ratio of 1.5 (insignificant) in South Korea.
There are a number of possible reasons for the higher prevalence of cognitive impairment observed in rural areas. First, a low education level is recognized as a risk factor for cognitive impairment [25], and is associated with rurality [19, 24]. Although we did not evaluate education level, it may have contributed to the rural–urban differences in cognitive impairment. Other possible contributors include physical and intellectual activities, low levels of which are considered risk factors for cognitive impairment [25, 26]. The mechanism underlying the influence of rural–urban differences on cognitive impairment should be explored in future studies.
In addition to individual factors, the importance of social environment of older adults in relation to cognition has been suggested, and such factors could be associated with rurality. Clarke et al. [7] reported that residence in a neighborhood with community resources, proximity to public transport, and public spaces in good condition, is inversely associated with cognitive impairment.
“Rurality” is a difficult concept to define. One study characterized rural areas by low population density and a traditional agrarian lifestyle [27]. Russ et al. [8] point out the substantial heterogeneity in studies comparing rural and urban areas, due in part to the difficulty of defining rurality. In this context, caution should be exercised when generalizing the results of the present study.
The prevalence of cognitive impairment tended to be higher in men than in women. This observation is inconsistent with the notion that the prevalence of dementia is higher among women in studies conducted in Japan [28, 29]. Nevertheless, a finding similar to ours, i.e., a higher prevalence of MCI in men than in women, was reported by Petersen et al. [30], who hypothesized that men may experience cognitive decline earlier in life but more gradually, whereas women may transition from normal cognition directly to dementia at a later age but more abruptly. Another potential explanation relates to sampling bias, i.e., long-term care insurance users were not included in this study.
Ojiya city was hit by the 2004 Niigata-Chuetsu earthquake, which may have affected the cognitive status of the elderly to some extent. Indeed, in a study following the 2011 Great East Japan Earthquake, a cognitive function of dementia patients who suffered from the disaster declined more relative to a control group [31]. Although we did not detect an association between property damage and cognitive impairment, its effects on cognitive impairment should be studied further in other settings.
This study has some limitations worth noting. First, the sample size was limited, and thus, potential associations may not have been detected due to insufficient statistical power. Second, we did not evaluate educational level, which is a known risk factor for cognitive impairment. In addition to the educational level, early life factors, such as early life socioeconomic status and adversity, have been pointed out as being associated with an increased risk of dementia-related cognitive impairment [32]. Thus, environmental factors in early life should further be studied. Finally, this study was conducted in a mid-sized city, and thus, our results may not be generalizable to metropolitan areas (e.g., Tokyo).
In conclusion, the prevalence of cognitive impairment was higher in a rural area compared with urban areas in Japan. This regional difference suggests the presence of modifiable factors in relation to cognitive impairment, which should be elucidated in future studies.
Acknowledgments
We are greatly indebted to the staff of Ojiya city office who helped with data collection. This work was supported in part by the Niigata Institute for Traumatic Stress (Mental Health and Welfare Association in Niigata Prefecture), Japan. We used the supercomputer at ACCMS, Kyoto University.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.
References
- 1.Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9:63–75. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Health and Welfare Statistics Association . Kokumin-no-Fukushi-to-Kaigo-no-Doko (Trend of Welfare and Care in Japan) 2014/2015. Tokyo: Health and Welfare Statistics Association; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Robertson DA, Savva GM, Coen RF, Kenny RA. Cognitive function in the prefrailty and frailty syndrome. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62:2118–2124. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Campbell JC, Ikegami N. Long-term care insurance comes to Japan. Health Aff. 2000;19:26–39. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.19.3.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Katz MJ, Lipton RB, Hall CB, Zimmerman ME, Sanders AE, Verghese J, et al. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and Alzheimer dementia in blacks and whites: a report from the Einstein Aging Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2012;26:335–343. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31823dbcfc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wight RG, Aneshensel CS, Miller-Martinez D, Botticello AL, Cummings JR, Karlamangla AS, et al. Urban neighborhood context, educational attainment, and cognitive function among older adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;163:1071–1078. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Clarke PJ, Weuve J, Barnes L, Evans DA, de Leon CFM. Cognitive decline and the neighborhood environment. Ann Epidemiol. 2015;25:849–854. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2015.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Russ TC, Batty GD, Hearnshaw GF, Fenton C, Starr JM. Geographical variation in dementia: systematic review with meta-analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41:1012–1032. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Statistics of Ojiya. 2014. http://www.city.ojiya.niigata.jp/soshiki/somu/tokeisho2014.html. Accessed 20 Jan 2016.
- 10.Nakagawa I, Nakamura K, Oyama M, Yamazaki O, Ishigami K, Tsuchiya Y, et al. Long-term effects of the Niigata-Chuetsu earthquake in Japan on acute myocardial infarction mortality: an analysis of death certificate data. Heart. 2009;95:2009–2013. doi: 10.1136/hrt.2009.174201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Nakamura K, Kitamura K, Kim Y, Someya T. Property damage and long-term psychological distress after the 2004 Niigata-Chuetsu earthquake in Ojiya, Japan: a community-based study. J Public Health. 2015;37:398–405. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdu052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Imai Y, Hasegawa K. The revised Hasegawa’s dementia scale (HDS-R): evaluation of its usefulness as a screening test for dementia. J Hong Kong Coll Psychiatr. 1994;4(Suppl 2):20–24. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jeong JW, Kim KW, Lee DY, Lee SB, Park JH, Choi EA, et al. A normative study of the Revised Hasegawa Dementia Scale: comparison of demographic influences between the Revised Hasegawa Dementia Scale and the Mini-Mental Status Examination. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2007;24:288–293. doi: 10.1159/000107592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rosli R, Tan MP, Gray WK, Subramanian P, Chin AV. Cognitive assessment tools in Asia: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr. 2016;28:189–210. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Muir SW, Gopaul K, Montero Odasso MM. The role of cognitive impairment in fall risk among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Age Ageing. 2012;41:299–308. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Khatib R, Santesso N, Pickard L, Osman O, Giangregorio L, Skidmore C, et al. Fracture risk in long term care: a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective observational studies. BMC Geriatr. 2014;14:130. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-14-130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rajan KB, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, de Leon CFM, Evans DA. Disability in basic and instrumental activities of daily living is associated with faster rate of decline in cognitive function of older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:624–630. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bae JB, Kim YJ, Han JW, Kim TH, Park JH, Lee SB, et al. Incidence of and risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in Korean elderly. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;39:105–115. doi: 10.1159/000366555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Jia J, Zhou A, Wei C, Jia X, Wang F, Li F, et al. The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and its etiological subtypes in elderly Chinese. Alzheimers Dement. 2014;10:439–447. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kim KW, Lee DY, Jhoo JH, Youn JC, Suh YJ, Jun YH, et al. Diagnostic accuracy of mini-mental status examination and revised Hasegawa dementia scale for Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2005;19:324–330. doi: 10.1159/000084558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Fujiwara Y, Suzuki H, Yasunaga M, Sugiyama M, Ijuin M, Sakuma N, et al. Brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment in older Japanese: validation of the Japanese version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2010;10:225–232. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2010.00585.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lin JS, O’Connor E, Rossom RC, Perdue LA, Burda BU, Thompson M, et al. Screening for cognitive impairment in older adults: an evidence update for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Rockville: Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US); 2013. [PubMed]
- 23.Arslantaş D, Ozbabalik D, Metintaş S, Ozkan S, Kalyoncu C, Ozdemir G, et al. Prevalence of dementia and associated risk factors in Middle Anatolia, Turkey. J Clin Neurosci. 2009;16:1455–1459. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2009.03.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nunes B, Silva RD, Cruz VT, Roriz JM, Pais J, Silva MC. Prevalence and pattern of cognitive impairment in rural and urban populations from Northern Portugal. BMC Neurol. 2010;10:42. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-10-42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Gamaldo AA, Teel A, Zonderman AB, Wang Y. Epidemiologic studies of modifiable factors associated with cognition and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:643. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vemuri P, Lesnick TG, Przybelski SA, Machulda M, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, et al. Association of lifetime intellectual enrichment with cognitive decline in the older population. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71:1017–1024. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Prince M, Ferri CP, Acosta D, Albanese E, Arizaga R, Dewey M, et al. The protocols for the 10/66 dementia research group population-based research programme. BMC Public Health. 2007;7:165. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-7-165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sekita A, Ninomiya T, Tanizaki Y, Doi Y, Hata J, Yonemoto K, et al. Trends in prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia in a Japanese community: the Hisayama study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;122:319–25. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 29.Ikejima C, Hisanaga A, Meguro K, Yamada T, Ouma S, Kawamuro Y, et al. Multicentre population-based dementia prevalence survey in Japan: a preliminary report. Psychogeriatrics. 2012;12:120–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8301.2012.00415.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Geda YE, Cha RH, Pankratz VS, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment is higher in men. The Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology. 2010;75:889–897. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f11d85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Furukawa K, Ootsuki M, Kodama M, Arai H. Exacerbation of dementia after the earthquake and tsunami in Japan. J Neurol. 2012;259:1243. doi: 10.1007/s00415-011-6329-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Seifan A, Schelke M, Obeng-Aduasare Y, Isaacson R. Early life epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: a critical review. Neuroepidemiology. 2015;45:237–254. doi: 10.1159/000439568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]