Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Epilepsy Behav. 2016 Oct 26;64(Pt A):283–288. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.09.013

Cognitive Functioning One Month and One Year following Febrile Status Epilepticus

Erica F Weiss 1, David Masur 1, Shlomo Shinnar 1, Dale C Hesdorffer 1, Veronica J Hinton 1, Melanie Bonner 1, Julie Rinaldi 1, Virginia Van de Water 1, James Culbert 1, Ruth C Shinnar 1, Syndi Seinfeld 1, William Gallentine 1, Douglas R Nordli Jr 1, L Mathew Frank 1, Leon Epstein 1, Solomon L Moshé 1, Shumei Sun 1; the FEBSTAT study team1
PMCID: PMC5115943  NIHMSID: NIHMS816104  PMID: 27794237

Abstract

Objective

To determine early developmental and cognitive outcomes of children with Febrile Status Epilepticus (FSE) one month and one year after FSE.

Methods

One hundred ninety four children with FSE were evaluated on measures of cognition, receptive language, and memory as part of the FEBSTAT study and compared to 100 controls with simple febrile seizures (FS).

Results

Children with FSE did not differ dramatically on tasks compared to FS controls at one month after FSE, but demonstrated slightly weaker motor development (p=0.035) and receptive language (p=0.034) at one year after FSE. Performances was generally within the low average to average range. Within the FSE cohort non-white children performed weaker on many of the tasks compared to Caucasian children. At the one year visit, acute hippocampal T2 findings on MRI were associated with weaker receptive language skills (p= 0.0009) and human herpes virus 6 or 7 (hhv6/7) viremia was associated with better memory performances (p=0.047).

Conclusion

FSE does not appear to be associated with significant cognitive impairment on early developmental measures although there is a trend for possible receptive language and motor delay one year after FSE. Further follow-up, which is in progress, is necessary to track long term cognitive functioning.

Keywords: Cognition, Febrile Status Epilepticus, Febrile seizure, Children

INTRODUCTION

Febrile seizures (FS) are relatively common seizures that occur, in association with a febrile illness (temperature greater than 38.4 °C) without concurrent central nervous system infection or acute electrolyte imbalance, in 2-4% of children [1-5]. Simple FS are isolated, brief, generalized seizures and complex FS are focal, prolonged (greater than 10 or 15 minutes depending on definition) or multiple (more than one seizure during a febrile illness) [1-5]. Febrile status epilepticus (FSE) are very prolonged FS or those lasting longer than 30 minutes [2-6]. Brief and/or simple FS are thought to be benign [3-5]. FSE, however, can cause acute hippocampal injury leading to hippocampal sclerosis [6-9] and has also been causally linked to subsequent mesial temporal lobe epilepsy [6, 9-11]. There is good evidence from epidemiological studies that complex FS and FSE are not associated with impairment of global intellectual function [12-14]. However, these studies did not specifically examine memory, a cognitive function subserved by the hippocampus, which is known to be impaired in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy [15, 16].

The FEBSTAT study (Consequences of Prolonged Febrile Seizures in Childhood) is a prospective multicenter study following children from the time of their FSE episode with serial MRIs, EEGs and neuropsychological testing [17, 18]. It is designed to study the relationship between FSE and subsequent acute hippocampal injury, development of hippocampal sclerosis (HS) and later development of TLE. Memory and other cognitive functions are assessed and a determination is made as to whether or not children are impaired even prior to the development of epilepsy [18]. In this report we present the results of the cognitive testing at baseline and one year after the baseline FSE.

METHODS

Participants

Two hundred and fourteen children who met criteria for FSE and were enrolled in the FEBSTAT (N=199), and the Columbia Study of Febrile Seizures (N=15) studies. Characteristics of the overall samples can be found in Table 1. Comparison children with a first simple febrile seizure (SFS: N=102) were drawn from the Columbia Study.

Table 1.

Description of the FSE cohort at baseline

FSE cases (N=214) Simple FS controls (N = 102)
Mean age (months) (SD) 20.2 ± 12.7 22.0 ± 10.6
Median age (months) (IQR) 15.8 (12.0 – 23.2) 19.7(15.8 – 25.03)
Sex (%)
Male 111 (51.9) 59 (57.84)
Female 103 (48.1) 43 (42.16)
Mean duration of febrile seizure (min) 87.7 (70.1) 22.0 ± 10.6
Median duration of febrile 70.0 3.00
seizure (IQR) (min) (45.0-105.0) (2.00 - 5.00)
Type of seizure (%)
Continuous 121 (56.5) 102 (100.00)
Intermittent without full recovery 69 (32.2) 0 (0.00)
Intermittent with drug therapy 24 (11.2) 0 (0.00)
Focal seizure (%)
Definitely 148 (69.2) 0 (0.00)
No (Generalized) 66 (30.8) 102 (100.00)
Definite cerebral lateralization (%)
Left 38 (17.8) 0 (0.00)
Right 29 (13.6) 0 (0.00)
No definite cerebral lateralization 147 (68.7) 102 (100.00)
Seizure classification (%)
Generalized tonic–clonic 66 (30.8) 102 (100.00)
Complex partial only 4 (1.9) 0 (0.00)

Partial with secondary generalization 144 (67.3) 0 (0.00)
Prior development (%)
Normal 183 (85.5) 100 (98.04)
Suspect 13 (6.1) 2 (1.96)
Abnormal 18 (8.4) 0 (0.00)
Baseline MRI findings (%)
Any definite abnormality 140 (73.7) 79 (89.77)
Increased T2 signal in the hippocampus 50 (26.3) 9 (10.23)
HIMAL (%)
No 140 (89.2) 79 (97.53)
Yes 17 (10.8) 2 (2.47)
HHV6 and HHV7 (%)
No 111 (65.7) NA
Yes 58 (34.3) NA
Acute T2 Signal
No 140 (88.6) 102 (100.00)
Yes 18 (11.4) 0 (0.00)
Baseline EEG findings (%)
Any abnormality 108(54.3) NA
Epileptiform abnormality 91(45.7) NA

NA- Not applicable

Of those enrolled, 187 FSE and 100 SFS children completed cognitive evaluations at one month and 155 FSE and 89 SFS children completed evaluations at one year. Some children within the FEBSTAT cohort who did not complete one month cognitive evaluation completed the one year evaluation (12 children) while a percentage of the group completed one month but not one year evaluation (35 children). A small portion of the children were administered incorrect or inappropriate measures and those were exclude from the analysis. As a result, a total of 182 one month and 151 one year FSE visits were used in this analysis. As some children were only able to complete one of the measures due to age or other limitations, individuals numbers analyzed for each measure are included in each table as appropriate. We could not find any demographic or neurological differences between those children who had testing at one time point versus those who were seen for both one month and one year visits.

Briefly, the FEBSTAT study prospectively recruited 199 children with FSE from June 2003 through March 2010 from five academic medical centers across the United States. Eligible subjects were children age 1 month to 5 years who presented with a first episode of FSE. FSE was defined as a FS [3, 19] lasting 30 minutes or longer, or repetitive FSs lasting at least 30 minutes without regaining alertness [20]. Children with prior afebrile seizures, acute CNS infection/insult and/or known severe neurologic disability were excluded. The detailed methodology of assembling the cohort is described in elsewhere [17, 18].

The Columbia Study of Febrile Seizures [21] recruited 159 children with a first febrile seizure who received almost the same work-up as the FEBSTAT cohort at baseline and at one-year. The seizure semiology and classification were all reviewed by the FEBSTAT phenomenology core and classified [18, 21]. The majority of the Columbia cohort (90.6%) did not meet criteria for FSE but rather presented with a first simple FS (SFS) (64.2%) or a first complex FS that was not FSE (26.4%). The children with first SFS from the Columbia cohort have been used as controls for the FEBSTAT study for phenomenology and imaging [8, 18] and are the controls in this analysis.

There was no significant difference in age or gender between the FSE and SFS groups, but the SFS group was significantly more Hispanic (89%) than the FSE population (29%; p <0.001).

Procedures

Once identified, the children and their parents were recruited within 72 hours. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents in all cases. Clinical data was collected and the child received a neurological evaluation, EEG, MRI, and blood work (including serum to assay HHV6/HHV7 centrally for viremia or reactivation). One month later, the child was scheduled for a neuropsychological assessment and was once again evaluated by a neurologist. At that time additional blood work, and parental behavioral rating scales were completed. Neuropsychological assessment, EEG, MRI, neurological evaluation, blood work and behavioral rating scales were repeated one year after initial evaluation. The children in the Columbia cohort also received imaging at baseline and one year using a similar imaging protocol as well as cognitive testing at baseline and one year but did not have EEGs or virology testing [18,21].

All procedures were approved by institutional review boards for the protection of human subjects at all the participating institutions.

Measures

Given the young age of the children at their one month visit (median age 17 months) and one year visit (median age 29 months), the test battery was designed to assess general development and aspects of verbal and non-verbal memory to the extent possible in this age group. Children who were over the age 5 at their one year visit were assessed with more comprehensive batteries including formal estimates of IQ, memory, and attention/concentration.

The following measures were included in this analysis:

  • Bayley Scales of Infant Development- 2nd Edition (Bayley-II) [22]

    The Bayley-II assesses mental and motor development in infants and young children, aged 1month to 36 months. Individually administered age appropriate items assess sensory acuity and response, early memory and problem solving, beginning language skills, basic abstract thinking and concept formation; as well as gross and fine motor skills to create composite scores. The Bayley-II is organized into two indices: mental development index (Bayley MDI) and psychomotor development index (Bayley PDI) which have a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 [22]. When appropriate the Bayley-II index scores were corrected for prematurity. Children with FSE and controls with simple febrile seizures received these tests.

  • Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-3rd Edition (PPVT-3) [23]

    The PPVT-3 is a well-established measure of receptive vocabulary for children and adults aged 30 months and up. It shows excellent internal consistency and test-retest reliability, and its validity is supported by strong correlations with other measures of language [23, 24]. The PPVT has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Children with FSE and controls with simple febrile seizures received this test.

  • McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities- Memory Index [25]

    Children aged 30 months to 8 years received the McCarthy – Memory Index (MI) at baseline. At the inception of this study, the McCarthy was the only standardized measure that included an assessment of broad memory functioning, including visual, auditory, verbal and numerical memory, within this age range [25-27]. The Memory Index has a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Only children with FSE from the FEBSTAT cohort age 30 months or older received the McCarthy Scales.

Neuropsychological assessments were completed at each site by a licensed neuropsychologist or a trained psychometrician/ psychology technician under the supervision of a licensed psychologist. The test administration order was the same at each site. All paper tests were initially scored at the individual sites, and then sent to a central location for double scoring. Double scoring was performed by the first author (EFW) and trained psychology technicians under the supervision of a licensed clinical neuropsychologist (DM). Any methodological or scoring issues arising at each site were resolved at the central location. Results in this paper use the central scoring.

Statistical analysis

Frequencies, percentages, means, and standard deviations were used to summarize demographic characteristics, and seizure phenomenology. Chi-square tests were used for categorical variables and T-tests or ANOVA were used for continuous variables when scores on the Bayley mental, Bayley motor and Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test were compared by gender and race/ethnicity in children with FSE. T-tests were used to compare children with FSE to SFS controls on the Bayley and PPVT at one month and at one year in all ages and with restriction to those children aged 12 months or less. Linear regression compared FSE to simple febrile seizure controls on the Bayley and the PPVT with adjustment for maternal education.

All analyses were conducted via SAS 9.4

RESULTS

One hundred eighty two children with FSE completed the neuropsychological evaluation at one month and 151 children completed the neuropsychological evaluation at one year.

Cognitive and motor development and memory in FSE

At one month, mental and motor development index scores of children with FSE were within the low end of the average range, without significant differences between mental and motor development (Table 2). Duration of FSE, focality, prior febrile seizure, type of seizure (continuous versus intermittent), hippocampal volume, hippocampal malrotation (HIMAL), acute T2 signal in the hippocampus on MRI, and human herpes virus 6/ human herpes virus 7 infection (HHV6/7) did not predict Bayley scores at one month after FSE. Compared to Caucasian children, race/ethnicity was significant for African-American children on the MDI and for Hispanic children on the PDI. As would be expected, 10 children who were noted to have abnormal development (as assessed by medical report and neurological evaluation) at enrollment performed worse than the remaining 141 children on the Bayley MDI [Mean (SD) 67.4 (18.6) versus 93.7 (14.1), p<0.0001].

Table 2.

Cognitive Performance at One Month and One Year in FSE by Demographic Factors

1-month Demographics Bayley MDI Bayley PDI PPVT-3 McCarthy MI

N Mean(Std) p N Mean(Std) p N Mean(Std) p N Mean(Std) p

Overall 151 91.97(15.79) NS** 158 91.18(17.91) NS 24 77.5(22.16) NS 22 38.95(10.83) NS

Gender Female 71 93.34(16.5) NS 75 91.84(17.33) NS 9 73(21.56) NS 9 37.33(6.98) NS
Male 80 90.75(15.13) 83 90.58(18.49) 15 80.2(22.81) 13 40.08(13.02)

Race Black, Non-Hispanic 54 88.89(16.09)* 0.044 55 91.09(20.62) NS 7 66.43(21.79)* 0.019 8 42.5(11.77) NS
White, Non-Hispanic 53 95.00(14.92)* 56 94.32(14.3) 9 91.44(15.94)* 9 40.22(9.65)

Hispanic 36 91.44(14.99) NS 39 87.69(16.71)* 0.041 8 71.5(22.42)* 0.049 5 31.00(8.92) NS
White, Non-Hispanic 53 95.00(14.92) 56 94.32(14.3)* 9 91.44(15.94)* 9 40.22(9.65)

Not White 90 89.91(15.63) NS 94 89.68(19.07) NS 15 69.13(21.49)* 0.013 13 38.08(11.88) NS
White 53 95.00(14.92) 56 94.32(14.3) 9 91.44(15.94)* 9 40.22(9.65)

HHV67 No HHV6/7 Infection 74 90.12(15.49) NS 80 87.94(17.96)* 0.048 11 78.27(16.30) NS 10 38.80(8.63) NS
HHV6/7 Infection 45 93.29(15.74) 46 94.59(18.08)* 6 74.00(23.68) 6 38.50(10.97)

Acute T2 Signal No Acute T2 Signal 121 91.50(15.40) NS 128 91.77(17.66) NS 18 82.06(20.00) NS 16 40.81(11.67) NS
Acute T2 Signal 12 93.08(16.95) 12 82.33(15.07) 4 68.25(27.78) 4 35.75(6.34)

HIMAL No HIMAL 125 92.13(15.28) NS 132 91.51(17.79) NS 20 80.25(22.49) NS 19 39.79(11.17) NS
HIMAL 11 88.27(16.70) 11 89.55(17.81) 2 72.50(4.95) 2 38.00(2.83)
1-year Overall 95 87.40(19.38) NS 95 89.33(19.50) NS 56 85.54(21.45) NS 40 42.45(8.24) NS

Gender female 45 89.76(21.24) NS 44 90.77(19.88) NS 22 85.64(23.62) NS 16 42.38(6.93) NS
male 50 85.28(17.48) 51 88.08(17.32) 34 85.47(20.30) 24 42.50(9.15)

Race Black, Non-Hispanic 29 86.59(17.17) NS 27 93.11(17.73) NS 18 79.11(19.43) NS 12 43.42(8.53) NS
White, Non-Hispanic 32 93.47(13.88) 31 86.68(13.72) 22 91.59(23.13) 17 43.06(8.33)

Hispanic 30 81.00(23.71)* 0.016 33 88.39(21.88) NS 14 83.29(20.71) NS 9 39.22(8.11) NS
White, Non-Hispanic 32 93.47(13.88)* 31 86.68(13.72) 22 91.59(23.13) 17 43.06(8.33)

Not White 59 83.75(20.77)* 0.009 60 90.52(20.1) NS 32 80.94(19.78) NS 21 41.62(8.42) NS
White 32 93.47(13.88)* 31 86.68(13.72) 22 91.59(23.13) 17 43.06(8.33)

HHV67 No HHV6/7 Infection 47 88.30(21.77) NS 48 88.13(21.37) NS 29 87.76(19.31) NS 25 40.52(8.01)* 0.047
HHV6/7 Infection 26 86.42(16.12) 25 92.44(14.03) 19 81.32(23.92) 10 46.70(7.94)*

Acute T2 Signal No Acute T2 Signal 77 88.52(19.15) NS 77 89.97(18.97) NS 47 89.87(15.39)* 0.009 36 42.31(8.12) NS
Acute T2 Signal 4 82.75(24.16) 4 77.75(15.33) 5 68.00(30.19)* 1 34.00(.)

HIMAL No HIMAL 79 88.73(19.18) NS 79 89.89(18.66) NS 46 87.07(20.96) NS 30 43.20(7.77) NS
HIMAL 4 76.00(21.53) 4 77.50(20.87) 8 85.13(20.29) 9 40.44(10.13)
*

T-Test Significance Level: <.05; MDI: Mental Development Index; PDI: Psychomotor Development Index; PPVT: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; MI: Memory Index

**

NS: Not Significant

Twenty-four children with FSE were old enough to complete the McCarthy and PPVT-3 at their one month visit. Overall performances were within the borderline to low average range (6th to 14th percentile when compared to their age peers) with significant differences between race/ethnicity groups. Compared to non-Hispanic Caucasian children, African-American and Hispanic children performed significantly worse on the PPVT-3 (p=0.02 and 0.049, respectively). There were no significant differences in performances based on FS phenomenology or MRI findings.

At one year, Bayley-II, McCarthy Memory Index and PPVT-3 performances were all on the cusp of the low average to average range (Table 2). There were no significant differences due to gender, FSE duration, prior FS, or FS type. HHV6/7 infection appeared to be protective for memory scores on the McCarthy, and children who had acute T2 hippocampal findings on MRI demonstrated weaker receptive language abilities (Table 2). Hispanic children performed worse on the MDI than non-Hispanic children. Non-white children continued to perform worse on the PPVT-3, but the differences were no longer statistically significant (p=0.075).

Cognitive and motor development in FSE compared to simple febrile seizure controls

When FSE was compared to SFS in children of all ages, there was no difference at one month for the Bayley MDI, Bayley PDI and PPVT-3 (Table 3). Children with FSE, however, performed about 5 standard points worse on Bayley PDI (p=0.04) and about 8 standard points worse on the PPVT-3 (p=0.03) when compared to the SFS cohort at one year. There was no difference in Bayley MDI. In a linear regression adjusted for maternal education, there was a 5-point increase in MDl for FSE (p=0.03) at baseline, but no statistically significant findings for the PDI and PPVT-3 at baseline and one year. There were no differences between FSE and simple febrile seizures when children 12 months or younger were examined with separate exclusions for abnormal development and abnormal MRI. There was also no age effect within our cohort, meaning that children who were younger than one year at time of FSE were not different than those who were older than one year at time of FSE when compared with SFS controls.

Table 3.

Comparison of Cognition Scores among FSE and SFS and in those 12 months or younger

FSE SFS

All ages at study entry
Baseline N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 151 91.97 15.79 91 88.18 13.72 0.058
Bayley PDI 158 91.18 17.91 91 93.18 14.17 0.332
PPVT-3 24 77.5 22.16 9 83.56 24.46 0.501
One year N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 95 87.4 19.38 62 83.69 11.75 0.138
Bayley PDI 95 89.33 18.5 62 94.52 12.14 0.035
PPVT-3 56 85.54 21.45 29 93.76 13.69 0.034
≤12 months of age at study entry
Baseline N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 44 93.32 13.11 11 92.55 13.94 0.863
Bayley PDI 45 91.91 15.32 11 98.73 13.52 0.182
One Year1 N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 32 86.75 20.93 10 78.00 13.33 0.222
Bayley PDI 33 89.97 20.12 10 93.30 9.02 0.465
≤12 months at study entry, excluding abnormal development
Baseline N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 43 93.49 13.22 11 92.55 13.94 0.835
Bayley PDI 44 91.89 15.5 11 98.73 13.52 0.185
One Year1 N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 31 87.97 20.09 10 78.00 13.32 0.151
Bayley PDI 32 91.25 19.02 10 93.30 9.02 0.645
≤12 months at study entry, excluding abnormal development and abnormal MRI
Baseline N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 37 93.05 13.67 8 89.25 14.85 0.485
Bayley PDI 38 92.55 15.31 8 95.88 14.62 0.577
One Year1 N Mean Std N Mean Std p-value
Bayley MDI 30 88.43 20.26 8 76.38 14.3 0.124
Bayley PDI 31 90.71 19.09 8 90.38 6.97 0.937

MDI: Mental Development Index; PDI: Psychomotor Development Index; PPVT: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; MI: Memory Index

1

only 2 with PPVT-3 in FSE and none in simple febrile seizure

DISCUSSION

This evaluation of children at one month and one year following first FSE demonstrated generally low average to average performance on measures of development, early memory, and receptive language ability. There were no significant differences based on gender, MRI, EEG, or phenomenology at baseline or one year but being white versus non-white was related to early developmental attainment and early language abilities. At one year, FSE was associated with weaker performance on a task of receptive language and mildly weaker psychomotor performance on the Bayley2. Acute hippocampal T2 findings on MRI correlated with weaker word recognition while HHV6/7 infection was associated with improved memory performances on the McCarthy.

Overall, while the current performances of our cohort are slightly below normative expectations, these data are consistent with the results of large epidemiological studies evaluating cognition in children with FS and for FSE, which have for the most part established generally intact cognitive functioning following FS [12-14, 28, 29, 30] and good prognosis following FSE [31].

Although we demonstrate statistically weaker motor development in FSE than SFS at one year consistent with Dr. Hesdorffer's findings that children who had longer seizures tend to demonstrate motor delays [32], it should be noted that the difference in scores in not clinically significant- ie both cohorts of children perform on the cusp of the low average to average range. The weaker receptive performances on the PPVT in children with FSE compared to SFS does demonstrate an interesting trend toward possible temporal lobe involvement, but given the limited sample size in the SFS cohort we are hesitant to make further claims. Cognitively, the PPVT requires the child to bridge orally produced words with mental representations of those words activating the brains’ processing of auditory information/language and recall.

Consistent with our one year data, Roy and colleagues reported that while children with FSE performed within the average range, their performances were below the control group and SFS peers [33]. Furthermore, they found that younger children (<11 months) with FSE were more likely to evidence motor delay than older children [33]. Although we also found a difference in motor development at one year between children with FSE and SFS, when we looked at children primarily under 12 months of age at study enrollment, the difference disappeared. Verity and colleagues reported that a greater proportion of children who had FS prior to 12 months were more likely to be considered for special education in early childhood, but when reassessed at 10 years of age, those children performed as well as those who had FS when older than 1 year of age [1]. Within our cohort, children with FSE who were less than 12 month old at the baseline visit did not differ from the rest of the cohort.

The North London study also reported poorer performances in children with FSE compared to controls [34]. Their findings on the Bayley-III revealed cognitive index scores of 90-95 (average) at baseline and 92-93 (average) at one year, were is consistent with our cohort's performances on the Bayley-II [34]. Interestingly, we found that those children who had human herpes virus infection (HHV6/7) performed half a standard deviation better on McCarthy memory task at one year after FSE than those who had FSE without HHV6/7 infection. This is consistent with our finding that when adjusting for age, HHV6/7 infection was associated with better growth of the right and left hippocampus [8]. Our sample size of children old enough to perform the McCarthy at one year post FSE is small due to the young median age of 17 months at FSE making it difficult to expand upon these findings. However, children with HHV6/7 infections and FSE performed similarly to those without HHV6/7 infection on the Bayley and PPVT. Therefore, although HHV6 and HHV7 infections has been linked to encephalitis and subsequent cognitive decline, evaluations of individuals who do not have encephalitis but present with FSE do not show higher levels of cognitive impairment in those where the FSE is associated with HHV6 or HHV7 infection [35].

The small sample size of older children at one year post FSE also limits our ability to extrapolate the cognitive correlates of MRI findings of T2 hyperintensities in the hippocampus. Children who had acute hippocampal T2 hyperintensity performed worse on the receptive vocabulary task (PPVT) than those who did not have those imaging markers. A recent study from Brazil demonstrated impaired PPVT performances in children with TLE and rolandic epilepsy [36]. Logically, children with acute hippocampal findings should have also demonstrated weaker performance on memory tasks, but most of these children were unable to complete the McCarthy memory scales. The five year post FSE testing is in progress and should provide an answer to this key question in the future.

Our findings demonstrated significantly weaker performance on receptive language (PPVT-3) measures in non-white children when compared with non-Hispanic Caucasian children. We believe that this effect is moderated by the location of our medical centers in inner city areas where portions of the population have low SES and do not have access to early childhood education or children's books. The concept of the “black-white score gap” and its environmental associations is well known in the literature [37, 38]. There has been evidence of 18-21 point differential between black and white children on the PPVT-R at age 3, that is moderated by family income, structure and home environment [39], as well as, disproportianate bias towards minorities on the PPVT-3 [40] which would be complicated in households were English is not the primary language [41]. Similar concerns of normative bias against low- income children have also been raised with regard to the Bayley-II [42].

Memory performances on the McCarthy Scales Memory Index were within the borderline to average range (12th-27th percentile). While limited by the small sample size, this does appear to indicate that memory performances may be mildly reduced even early after FSE, raising the possibility of hippocampal involvement. Unfortunately, the Columbia SFS and FSE subjects did not complete these memory tasks and normal controls were unavailable. Additional limitations include the fact that the SFS Columbia cohort was demographically different (89% Hispanic) than the FSE cohort (29% Hispanic) and that this may have impacted upon our between group comparisons particularly on the mental and language tasks. Finally, we have a limitation shared across all cognitive research in young children secondary to the limited number of cognitive measures available for this age group. Although the field is slowly growing, there are relatively few aspects of cognitive performance that can be accurately assessed in an 18-month old child. As these children age, we will be able to investigate specific cognitive abilities with greater reliability.

In summary we found generally low average to average abilities in children one month and one year following FSE with a trend towards slightly delayed motor and receptive language development at one year following FSE. Further follow-up of the FEBSTAT cohort will expand on these findings and provide much needed additional information on the cognitive functioning of children with FSE as they age.

HIGHLIGHTS.

  • Early cognitive outcomes of children with Febrile Status Epilepticus (FSE) are reported.

  • Development, early memory, and receptive language were generally within the low average to average range.

  • Children with FSE performed similarly compared to simple febrile seizure (SFS) controls but there was a trend for weaker receptive language and motor development one year after FSE

  • HHV6/7 infection in those with FSE was not associated with worse cognitive outcomes

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Supported by NINDS grant 2R37 NS43209 (PI: S. Shinnar, MD, PhD), NICHD grant 36867 (PI: D.C. Hesdorffer PhD) and Center for Clinical and Translational Research grant UL1TR000058.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

DISCLOSURES

Dr. Shinnar is funded by NIH grants and serves on a DSMB for UCB Pharma. He has received personal compensation for serving on Scientific Advisory Boards for Accorda, AstraZenica, and Upsher Smith, for consulting for Accorda, AstraZenica, Neurelis, Upsher-Smith and Xeris. He has received royalties from Elsevier for co-editing the book “Febrile Seizures”. He has also given expert testimony in medico-legal cases. Dr. Hesdorffer is funded by grants from NINDS, PCORI, CDC, PACE, Epilepsy Foundation, and The Epilepsy Study Consortium. She is a consultant to the Injury Prevention Center at Mount Sinai Medical Center and on the Advisory Boards for Acorda, UpsherSmith, and Consulting Cyberonics. She has had travel and hotel reimbursed by the International League Against Epilepsy meetings in 2014 and 2015. Dr. Moshé is funded by grants from NIH, CURE, US Department of Defense, the Heffer Family and the Segal Family Foundations and the Abbe Goldstein/Joshua Lurie and Laurie Marsh/ Dan Levitz families. He is serving as Associate Editor of Neurobiology of Disease, and is on the editorial board of Epileptic Disorders, Brain and Development, Pediatric Neurology and Physiological Research. He receives from Elsevier an annual compensation for his work as Associate Editor in Neurobiology of Disease and royalties from 2 books he co-edited. He received a consultant's fee from Eisai and UCB.

The other authors have nothing to disclose.

FEBSTAT Study Team:

Montefiore and Jacobi Medical Centers, Bronx, NY: Shlomo Shinnar MD PhD (PI), Jacqueline Bello MD, William Gomes MD PhD, James Hannigan RT, Sharyn Katz R-EEGT FASET, David Masur PhD, Solomon L. Moshé MD, Ruth C. Shinnar RN MSN, Erica F. Weiss PhD.

Ann & Robert Lurie Children's Hospital, Chicago, IL: Leon G Epstein MD (site PI), John Curran MD, , Andrew Kim MD, Diana Miazga, Julie Rinaldi PhD.

Columbia University, New York, NY: Dale C. Hesdorffer PhD (site PI), Stephen Chan MD, Veronica J. Hinton PhD, Binyi Liu MS, Yokasta Tineo BA.

Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC: Darrell Lewis MD (site PI), Melanie Bonner PhD, Karen Cornett BS, MT, William Gallentine DO, James MacFall PhD , James Provenzale MD, Allen Song PhD, James Voyvodic PhD, Yuan Xu BS

Eastern Virginia Medical School, Norfolk, VA: L. Matthew Frank MD (site PI), Joanne Andy RT, Terrie Conklin RN, Susan Grasso MD, Connie S. Powers REEGT, David Kushner MD, Susan Landers RT, Virginia Van de Water PhD.

International Epilepsy Consortium/ Department of Biostatistics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond Virginia: Shumei Sun PhD (site PI), John M Pellock MD, Brian J Bush MSMIT, Lori L Davis BA, Xiaoyan Deng MS, Christiane Rogers, Cynthia Shier Sabo MS

Mount Sinai Medical Center, New York NY: Emilia Bagiella PhD

Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA: John M Pellock MD (site PI), Tanya Brazemore R-EEGT, James Culbert PhD, Kathryn O’Hara RN, Syndi Seinfeld DO, Jean Snow RT-R

Additional Collaborators: Joan Conry MD Childrens National Medical Center, Safety Monitor; Tracy Glauser MD Cincinnati Childrens Medical Center, Genomics; Jeffrey L Noebels MD PhD, Baylor College of Medicine, Genetics; Douglas R Nordli, MD, Children's Hospital of Los Angeles

References

  • 1.Verity C, Butler N, Golding J. Febrile convulsions in a national cohort followed up from birth. I--Prevalence and recurrence in the first five years of life. BMJ. 1985;290:1307–1310. doi: 10.1136/bmj.290.6478.1307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Stafstrom C. The incidence and prevalence of febrile seizures. In: Baram T, Shinnar S, editors. Febrile seizures. Academic Press; San Diego: 2002. pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.National Institutes of Health . Febrile Seizures: Consensus Development Conference Summary. National Institute of Health; Bathesda, MD: 1980. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Steering Committee on Quality Improvement Management-Subcommittee on Febrile Seizures Febrile Seizures: Clinical Practice Guideline for the Long-term Management of the Child With Simple Febrile Seizures. 2008;121:1281–1286. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-0939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Shinnar S. Febrile seizures. In: Swaiman K, Ashwal S, Ferriero D, Schor N, editors. Swaiman's Pediatric Neurology. Fifth ed Elsevier Saunders; 2012. pp. 790–797. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Shinnar S. Febrile seizures and mesial temporal sclerosis. Epilepsy Currents. 2003;3:115–118. doi: 10.1046/j.1535-7597.2003.03401.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Shinnar S, Bello JA, Chan S, Hesdorffer DC, Lewis DV, MacFall J, et al. MRI abnormalities following febrile status epilepticus in children The FEBSTAT study. Neurology. 2012;79:871–877. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318266fcc5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lewis DV, Shinnar S, Hesdorffer DC, Bagiella E, Bello JA, Chan S, et al. Hippocampal sclerosis after febrile status epilepticus: The FEBSTAT study. Annals of Neurology. 2014;75:178–185. doi: 10.1002/ana.24081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Provenzale JM, Barboriak DP, VanLandingham K, MacFall J, Delong D, Lewis DV. Hippocampal MRI signal hyperintensity after febrile status epilepticus is predictive of subsequent mesial temporal sclerosis. American Journal of Roentgenology. 2008;190:976–983. doi: 10.2214/AJR.07.2407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.French J, Williamson P, Thadani V, Darcey T, Mattson R, Spencer S, et al. Characteristics of medial temporal lobe epilepsy: I. Results of history and physical examination. Annals of neurology. 1993;34:774–780. doi: 10.1002/ana.410340604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mathern GW, Pretorius JK, Babb TL. Influence of the type of initial precipitating injury and at what age it occurs on course and outcome in patients with temporal lobe seizures. Journal of neurosurgery. 1995;82:220–227. doi: 10.3171/jns.1995.82.2.0220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ellenberg JH, Nelson KB. Febrile seizures and later intellectual performance. Archives of neurology. 1978;35:17–21. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500250021004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Verity CM, Greenwood R, Golding J. Long-term intellectual and behavioral outcomes of children with febrile convulsions. New England Journal of Medicine. 1998;338:1723–1728. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199806113382403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hirtz DG. Cognitive outcome of febrile seizures. In: Baram T, Shinnar S, editors. Febrile seizures. Academic Press; San Diego: 2002. pp. 53–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bell BD, Giovagnoli AR. Recent innovative studies of memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychology review. 2007;17:455–476. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9049-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hermann B, Seidenberg M, Bell B, Rutecki P, Sheth R, Ruggles K, et al. The Neurodevelopmental Impact of Childhood-onset Temporal Lobe Epilepsy on Brain Structure and Function. Epilepsia. 2002;43:1062–1071. doi: 10.1046/j.1528-1157.2002.49901.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Shinnar S, Hesdorffer D, Nordli D, Pellock J, O'Dell C, Lewis D, et al. Phenomenology of prolonged febrile seizures Results of the FEBSTAT study. Neurology. 2008;71:170–176. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000310774.01185.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hesdorffer DC, Shinnar S, Lewis DV, Moshé SL, Nordli DR, Pellock JM, et al. Design and phenomenology of the FEBSTAT study. Epilepsia. 2012;53:1471–1480. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03567.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sidenvall R, Forsgren L, Blomquist HK, Heijbel J. A community-based prospective incidence study of epileptic seizures in children. Acta Paediatrica. 1993;82:60–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1993.tb12518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Maytal J, Shinnar S. Febrile Status Epilepticus. Pediatrics. 1990;86:611. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hesdorffer DC, Benn EKT, Bagiella E, Nordli D, Pellock J, Hinton V, Shinnar S, for the FEBSTAT Study Team Distribution of febrile seizure duration and associations with development. Ann Neurol. 2011;70:93–100. doi: 10.1002/ana.22368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bayley N. Bayley scales of infant development: Manual. Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Dunn LM, Dunn LM. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. 3rd ed. American Guidance Service; Circle Pines, MN: 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Strauss E, Sherman EM, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McCarthy D. McCarthy Scales of Children's Abilities. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, Tx: 1972. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Forns-Santacana M, Göamez-Benito J. Factor structure of the McCarthy scales. Psychology in the Schools. 1990;27:111–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mitchell WG, Chavez JM, Lee H, Guzman BL. Academic underachievement in children with epilepsy. Journal of Child Neurology. 1991;6:65–72. doi: 10.1177/088307389100600114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Knudsen FU. Febrile seizures: treatment and prognosis. Epilepsia. 2000;41:2–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.2000.tb01497.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Chang YC, Guo NW, Huang CC, Wang ST, Tsai JJ. Neurocognitive Attention and Behavior Outcome of School-Age Children with a History of Febrile Convulsions: A Population Study. Epilepsia. 2000;41:412–420. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.2000.tb00182.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Leaffer EB, Hinton VJ, Hesdorffer DC. Longitudinal assessment of skill development in children with first febrile seizure. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2013;28:83–87. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2013.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Neville BG, Chin RFM, Scott RC. Childhood convulsive status epilepticus: epidemiology, management and outcome. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica. 2007;115:21–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2007.00805.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hesdorffer DC, Benn EKT, Bagiella E, et al. Distribution Of Febrile Seizure Duration And Associations With Development. Annals of neurology. 2011;70(1):93–100. doi: 10.1002/ana.22368. doi:10.1002/ana.22368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Roy H, Lippé S, Lussier F, Sauerwein HC, Lortie A, Lacroix J, et al. Developmental outcome after a single episode of status epilepticus. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2011;21:430–436. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Martinos MM, Yoong M, Patil S, Chong WK, Mardari R, Chin RFM, et al. Early developmental outcomes in children following convulsive status epilepticus: A longitudinal study. Epilepsia. 2013;54:1012–1019. doi: 10.1111/epi.12136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zerr D, Komaroff A. Cognitive Dysfunction from HHV-6A and HHV-6B. In: Flamand L, Lautenschlager I, Krueger G, Ablashi D, editors. Human Herpesviruses HHV-6A, HHV-6B & HHV-7: Diagnosis and Clinical Management. Elsevier Science; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Boscariol M, Casali RL, Amaral MIR, Lunardi LL, Matas CG, Collela-Santos MF, et al. Language and central temporal auditory processing in childhood epilepsies. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2015;53:180–183. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jencks C. Racial Bias in Testing. In: Jencks C, Phillips M, editors. The Black-White test score gap. Brookings Institution Press; 2011. pp. 55–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sohn K. The dynamics of the evolution of the Black–White test score gap. Education Economics. 2012;20:175–188. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK, Smith J, Duncan GJ, Lee K. The Black-White Test Score Gap in Young Children:Contributions of Test and Family Characteristics. Applied Developmental Science. 2003;7:239–252. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Champion TB, Hyter YD, McCabe A, Bland-Stewart LM. “A Matter of Vocabulary”: Performances of Low-Income African American Head Start Children on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III. Communication Disorders Quarterly. 2003;24:121–127. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Laosa LM. Ethnic, socioeconomic, and home language influences upon early performance on measures of abilities. ETS Research Report Series. 1984;1984:i–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Black MM, Hess CR, Berenson-Howard J. Toddlers from low-income families have below normal mental, motor, and behavior scores on the revised Bayley scales. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2000;21:655–666. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES