Skip to main content
Journal of Neurophysiology logoLink to Journal of Neurophysiology
. 2016 Aug 17;116(5):2405–2419. doi: 10.1152/jn.00438.2016

Model of the songbird nucleus HVC as a network of central pattern generators

Eve Armstrong 1,, Henry D I Abarbanel 2,3
PMCID: PMC5116487  PMID: 27535375

Building on the observations by Hahnloser et al. of a remarkable sparse command center for sculpting avian song production, and accounting for significant input from the New York University laboratory of M. Long, we have developed a model of nucleus HVC in the songbird system. It accounts for many observed properties of song production and provides a biophysical foundation for the sparse firing of projection neurons as well as the occasional “anomalous neuron” observed by Hahnloser et al.

Keywords: central pattern generator, dynamical systems, dynamics, HVC, winnerless competition

Abstract

We propose a functional architecture of the adult songbird nucleus HVC in which the core element is a “functional syllable unit” (FSU). In this model, HVC is organized into FSUs, each of which provides the basis for the production of one syllable in vocalization. Within each FSU, the inhibitory neuron population takes one of two operational states: 1) simultaneous firing wherein all inhibitory neurons fire simultaneously, and 2) competitive firing of the inhibitory neurons. Switching between these basic modes of activity is accomplished via changes in the synaptic strengths among the inhibitory neurons. The inhibitory neurons connect to excitatory projection neurons such that during state 1 the activity of projection neurons is suppressed, while during state 2 patterns of sequential firing of projection neurons can occur. The latter state is stabilized by feedback from the projection to the inhibitory neurons. Song composition for specific species is distinguished by the manner in which different FSUs are functionally connected to each other. Ours is a computational model built with biophysically based neurons. We illustrate that many observations of HVC activity are explained by the dynamics of the proposed population of FSUs, and we identify aspects of the model that are currently testable experimentally. In addition, and standing apart from the core features of an FSU, we propose that the transition between modes may be governed by the biophysical mechanism of neuromodulation.

NEW & NOTEWORTHY

Building on the observations by Hahnloser et al. of a remarkable sparse command center for sculpting avian song production, and accounting for significant input from the New York University laboratory of M. Long, we have developed a model of nucleus HVC in the songbird system. It accounts for many observed properties of song production and provides a biophysical foundation for the sparse firing of projection neurons as well as the occasional “anomalous neuron” observed by Hahnloser et al.

in the song system of the avian brain, nucleus HVC plays a central role at the junction of the auditory and song production pathways. The sparse firing of excitatory projection neurons within HVC into the song production pathway via nucleus RA (HVCRA cells) has been the subject of both experiments and model development. A critical role of the inhibitory interneurons (HVCI cells) was suggested by Gibb et al. (2009a) (hereafter GGA1) and supported by experiments by Kosche et al. (2015) (hereafter KVL15). In light of those experiments and earlier observations, we suggest an architecture for HVC that is consistent with a critical role for both structured inhibition and excitation. This paper lays out and explores the consequences of this architecture, which is built on functional units in HVC that underlie the formation of syllables in vocalization.

We model the nucleus HVC as a pattern-generating network capable of qualitatively reproducing many observations at the whole cell, population, and behavioral levels. In doing so, we offer a biophysical explanation for the sparse firings of HVC projection neurons established by Hahnloser et al. (2002) and the experiments that subsequently uncovered details of the underlying neuronal processes and neural circuit relationships, particularly the work of KVL15.

We depart from previous modeling efforts by shifting focus from the sparse firings alone to a broader picture including other salient features of HVC that complementary lines of research have illuminated. In particular, we are interested in a cell's effect on a circuit at times when it is active and equally at times when it is not active.

Our model consists of two key components: 1) a subcircuit that can assume one of two modes of behavior, depending on inhibitory synaptic coupling strengths, and 2) a mechanism capable of effecting a rapid transition between these modes. We find that these aims can be accomplished by invoking Lotka-Volterra-like dynamics subject to possible neuromodulatory mechanisms that have been proposed to explain neuronal activity in a variety of species but that have been studied minimally within the context of the avian song generation system.

Hahnloser et al. (2002) established the pattern-generating capability of HVC by demonstrating that some HVCRA neurons reliably participate in one sparse pattern during song. Lesioning studies had previously identified HVC as residing high on the control pathway for song production, thereby poised to pass its instructions downstream where the song is ultimately generated by the syrinx and lungs. The cellular dynamics in HVC have been significantly illuminated by KVL15, which focused on the interplay between HVCRA and HVCI neuronal activity. KVL15 demonstrated via a series of GABA antagonist (gabazine)-induced responses that the role of inhibition is central in modulating activity of the HVCRA cells. They established the importance of both a structured inhibition and structured excitation for song generation, where all activity is set within an ambient background of excitation. In addition, KVL15 reported high in vitro rates of reciprocal connectivity between cell pairs and disynaptic connectivity between HVCRA cells, which Mooney and Prather (2005) noted are reminiscent of such rates in pattern-generating networks throughout the central nervous systems in other species. Pattern-generating activity has also been induced via electrical stimulations in slice preparations of HVC (Solis and Perkel 2005).

Previous HVC models have focused on producing the observed series of HVCRA neuron activations, by invoking a feedforward “synfire” chain of excitation (Li and Greenside 2006; Long et al. 2010; GGA1). GGA1 suggested in addition that inhibition plays a mediating role upon such a chain. One of the artificial constructs of GGA1 was the introduction of a neuronal oscillator loop that could transition between an “on” and “off” state. This functional loop was arrayed in a chain stimulated in an unspecified manner to excite a signal moving down the chain, as interneuron activity confined the excitation to a short temporal window. In this way, GGA1 suggested that inhibition is integral to the series propagation, yet their proposed mechanism was carefully engineered without biophysical motivations. Cannon et al. (2015) also proposed a chain modulated by inhibition, without offering a biophysical motivation for the form of that modulation. Moreover, while chain models can capture the propagation of series activations, the chain model is troublesome in that, by its very definition, it does not represent the picture of a strongly interconnected web, the picture that emerges from KVL15, where both structured excitation and structured inhibition play integral roles.

An alternative to the chain model has been offered in terms of a competition among neurons. Two basic types of competition have been applied to the modeling of neuronal networks: winner-take-all (e.g., Verduzco-Flores et al. 2012) and winnerless (e.g., Yildiz and Kiebel 2011). While the latter authors proposed no particular connectivity for effecting the functionality, we found their invocation of winnerless competition (WLC) appealing for the relative simplicity with which it can, in principle, generate activity reminiscent of HVC activity. More importantly, the WLC formalism describes an identified biophysical process: the observed phenomenon of mutual inhibition. Indeed, the framers of WLC themselves have suggested WLC as a likely underlier of series activity in HVC (Afraimovich et al. 2004; Rabinovich et al. 2006). We have taken this idea as a critical component of the model that we propose in this paper.

We propose a model of HVC in which there exists a basic architectural element capable of transitioning between two modes of behavior. We call this element a “functional syllable unit” (FSU). The modes are “quiescence,” in which the excitatory cells are silent above threshold, and “active,” in which the excitatory cells are permitted to activate in a series and where the activity is sustained via WLC. Each mode can occur over a distinct range of values of the synaptic coupling strengths among the interneurons in the FSU. We attribute a transition between modes to a neuromodulatory mechanism that is capable of altering those coupling strengths. If a full song is taken to be comprised of a population of these FSUs, then we can illustrate how the observed population activity of both excitatory and inhibitory neurons during song, and during quiescence, in HVC can be reproduced. That is: our model reproduces not only series activity of excitatory cells but, more broadly, the behavior of both excitatory and inhibitory cells both during quiescence and during song, at the population level.

In the next section we shall describe this proposed core HVC element, using Hodgkin-Huxley neurons with calcium dynamics. The individual HVCI neurons have an experimental biophysical basis from the dissertation work of Daou et al. (2013) and subsequent experiments by Daou in the Margoliash laboratory at the University of Chicago (private communication) (Breen et al. 2016). The model HVCRA neuron is based on experimentally observed currents (Daou et al. 2013; Long et al. 2010) and is being tested further using protocols of Kadakia et al. (2016).

The remainder of the paper is structured as follows.

1) constituents of a functional syllable unit lays out the model neurons and synapses used in the functional architecture.

2) In results, numerical results from the Hodgkin-Huxley based circuit show how the connectivity can give rise to two distinct modes of dynamics. Furthermore, there exist narrow “transition regions” between these modes, within which lie additional modes that may occur on rare occasion; these modes should be identifiable in the laboratory.

3) In building complete songs we offer an example of creating a species-specific song using these FSUs as building blocks, thereby demonstrating how the basic qualitative population activity can be reproduced by our model.

4) In discussion, we examine winnerless competition as a theoretical framework for the competitive inhibitory neuron dynamics and our suggestion that neuromodulation is the biophysical switch sculpting the HVC interneuron activity. Then we turn to ways in which the model predictions can be tested experimentally.

A FUNCTIONAL SYLLABLE UNIT

The central constituent of our model is a functional syllable unit (FSU). We consider HVC to be comprised of numerous FSUs, each of which is comprised of an “inner” inhibitory loop that can assume one of two operational modes, depending on the synaptic coupling strengths among the inhibitory cells: 1) a state in which all inhibitory neurons fire continually, and 2) a state in which the inhibitory neurons are forced, via mutual inhibition, to fire in alternation. We call these modes “quiescent” and “active,” respectively.

These two modes are captured in a Lotka-Volterra-like system, which expresses the quiescent mode when the inhibitory coupling strengths are weak; above some threshold value of coupling strengths, the circuit makes a bifurcation to the active mode. This phenomenon of mode switching was formalized in another neural context as “winnerless competition” by Rabinovich et al. (2001).

The inhibitory loop of HVCI neurons is connected to an “outer” loop of excitatory HVCRA projection neurons as shown in Fig. 1. Each triangle represents one inhibitory neuron and each circle represents an ensemble of HVCRA neurons. The filled-circle- and arrow-headed lines represent inhibitory and excitatory functional synaptic connections, respectively.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

A functional syllable unit (FSU), comprised of 3 inhibitory interneurons (HVCI) and 3 ensembles of nucleus RA (HVCRA) neurons. Triangles and circles represent the former and latter populations, respectively. Circle- and arrow-headed lines represent inhibitory and excitatory functional connections, respectively.

With this connectivity, we can make the following associations between behavior and FSU activity. When the bird is not singing, all FSUs in HVC sit in a background of excitation sufficient for quiescence: all HVCI cells fire continually, thereby, via the wiring of Fig. 1, silencing all HVCRA neurons. The coupling strengths in this FSU then are increased beyond the threshold required for a bifurcation to active mode. This switching might be effected via an injection of an inhibitory neurotransmitter such as GABA, the primary type of inhibitory neurotransmitter in HVC (Dutar et al. 1998; Schmidt and Perkel 1998), in the vicinity of a particular FSU. The HVCI neurons within that particular FSU now are forced into a winnerless competition, and they fire sequentially. The wiring of Fig. 1 then effects sequential firings of the HVCRA neurons. To be clear: during its active state, an FSU represents the playing of one syllable, where, in the example architecture of Fig. 1, three HVCRA ensembles fire in sequence. The syllable can play until the elevated inhibition drops below some threshold, at which point the FSU rapidly returns to quiescence.

To model the full song of a specific species, we consider HVC to be a collection of such pattern-generating structures. While “Syllable A” is presumed to be initiated by an incentive to vocalize, we attribute subsequent syllables to a feedback loop, most likely from the motor area (e.g., Vallentin and Long 2015), in which a series of FSUs in HVC is targeted by a succession of inhibitory neurotransmitter injections. Such a loop was proposed by Gibb et al. (2009b), who suggested the nucleus Uva as a possible source of the feedback.

CONSTITUENTS OF A FUNCTIONAL SYLLABLE UNIT

Neurons

To build our model, we consider the HVCI and HVCRA populations. HVC neurons projecting to Area X and the AFP are not considered. We focus on the HVCRA and HVCI populations because they have been clearly identified as playing fundamental roles in song generation in the adult.

We consider the neurons to be Hodgkin-Huxley-type neurons all with sodium, potassium, and leak currents. The specific inhibitory model we take from Breen et al. (2016), and we base our excitatory model on Kadakia et al. (2016). Both of these models were constructed in light of recent electrophysiological data on calcium channels in these cells (Daou et al. 2013). The HVCI neurons have T-type calcium and hyperpolarization-activated currents; the HVCRA neurons have l-type calcium channels and a potassium channel activated by increased intracellular calcium concentrations (hereafter the K/Ca channel). In addition, the HVCRA neuron is treated as a two-compartment structure consisting of soma and dendrite. The time evolution of the membrane potential V(t) of the neurons is expressed as follows.

Inhibitory interneuron.

CdVi(t)dt=IL,i(t)+INa,i(t)+IK,i(t)+ICaT,i(t)+IH(t)+jiIsyn,ij(t)+Ibackground+noise(t) 1)

Excitatory projection neuron: somatic compartment.

CdVs,i(t)dt=IL,i(t)+INa,i(t)+IK,i(t) +gSD(Vd(t),Vs(t))+Ibackground+noise(t)

Excitatory projection neuron: dendritic compartment.

CdVd,i(t)dt=ICaL,i(t)+IKCa,i(t)+gSD(Vs(t),Vd(t))

where C is the membrane capacitance and noise(t) is a low-amplitude background noise term. The Isyn terms represent the synaptic input currents, Ibackground is a DC current representing ambient background excitation, and the gSD terms couple the compartments. The ion channel currents for the ith neuron are:

IL,i(t)=gL(ELVi(t))
INa,i(t)=gNa,im(t)3h(t)(ENaVi(t))
IK,i(t)=gK,in(t)4(EKVi(t))
ICaT,i(t)=gCaT,ia(t)3b(t)3GHK(Vi(t),[Ca]i(t))
ICaL,i(t)=gCaL,iq(t)2GHK(Vi(t),[Ca]i(t))
IKCa,i=gKCa[Ca]i(t)2[Ca]i(t)2+ks2(EKVd,i(t))
IH,i(t)=gHH(t)2(EHVi(t))

where “GHK(Vi(t),[Ca]i(t))” is defined as:

GHK(Vi(t),[Ca]i(t))=Vi(t)[Ca]i(t)Caexte2FVi(t)/RTe2FVi(t)/RT1.

The parameters denoted “g” are the maximum conductances of each current; the parameters denoted “E” are the respective reversal potentials. [Ca]i(t) is the intracellular Ca2+ concentration as a function of time. Caext is the extracellular concentration of Ca2+ ions. In the GHK current, F is the Faraday constant, R is the gas constant, and T is temperature, which we take as 37°C. The gating variables Ui(t) = [m(t), h(t), n(t), a(t), b(t), q(t), H(t)] satisfy:

dui(t)dt=(u(Vi(t))ui(t))/τui(Vi(t));
u(Vi)=0.5[1+tanh((Viθu,i)/σu,i)]
τui(Vi)=tu0+tu1[1tanh2((Viθu,i)/σu,i)].

There is one exception for H(t): H and τH take different values of σH (see Table 1). The calcium dynamics evolve

d[Cai](t)dt=ϕICaX+Ca0[Cai](t)τCa
Table 1.

Parameter values for neuronal Na, K, L, and H currents

Quantity Value Quantity Value
gL, gH, EL
gL,0 0.00303 μS gL,1 0.00302 μS
gL,2 0.00299 μS gL,3 0.00301 μS
gL,4 0.00298 μS gL,5 0.00297 μS
EL,0 −60.0 mV EL,1 −59.96 mV
EL,2 −59.94 mV EL,3 −80.0 mV
EL,4 −80.05 mV EL,5 −79.95 mV
gH,1 2.0−3 μS gH,2 1.99−3 μS
gH,3 2.01−3 μS
Na, K, and L Current Kinetics and Other Cellular Properties
gNa 1.2 μS gK 0.2 μS
ENa 50.0 mV EK −77.0 mV
θm −40.0 mV θh −60.0 mV
σm 16.0 mV σh −16.0 mV
t0,m 0.1 ms t0,h 1.0 ms
t1,m 0.4 ms t1,h 7.0 ms
C 0.01 μF θn −55.0 mV
gSD 0.05 nS σn 25.0 mV
t0,n 1.0 ms
t1,n 5.0 ms
H Current Kinetics
EH −40.0 mV θH −60.0 mV
σH for H −11.0 mV σH for τH 21.0 mV
t0,H 0.1 ms t1,H 193.5 ms

Top: neurons are distinguished via slightly different values of gL and EL, where gL,0 denotes the maximum leak conductance of cell zero (out of 5). HVCI neurons are further distinguished by different values of gH. Middle: values for Na, K, and L current kinetics and other cellular properties. Bottom: values for H current kinetics. Units: mV, millivolts; ms, milliseconds; μF, micro-Farads; μS, micro-Siemens.

where the “X” of the subscript “CaX” represents “T” (for T-type) and “L” (for L-type), for the inhibitory neuron and excitatory dendrite, respectively. Ca0 is the equilibrium concentration of calcium inside the cell, and ks is a Michaelis-Menten constant.

We further distinguish the excitatory and inhibitory neurons via their respective resting potential EL and synaptic reversal potential Esyn,rev. To preclude possible effects due to symmetries, we render the neurons distinguishable via slightly different values of their leak maximum conductances gL and resting potentials EL (for all neurons), CaT, and H current maximum conductances gCaT and gH (for the interneurons), and CaL and KCa current maximum conductances gCaL and gKCa (for the excitatory projection neurons). Values for the Na, K, L, and H currents and basic cellular properties are listed in Table 1; values for the calcium dynamics are in Table 2.

Table 2.

Parameter values for calcium dynamics

Quantity Value Quantity Value
gCaT, gCaL, and gKCa
gCaT,0 1.0−4 μS gCaT,1 1.01−4 μS
gCaT,2 1.01−4 μS gCaL,3 0.00301 μS
gCaL,4 0.00298 μS gCaL,5 0.00297 μS
gKCa,3 −60.0 mV gKCa,4 −59.96 mV
gKCa,5 −59.94 mV
CaT and CaL Current Kinetics and Basic Cellular Properties
θa −70.0 mV θb −65.0 mV
σa 10.0 mV σb −10.0 mV
t0,a 0.1 ms t0,b 1.0 ms
t1,a 0.2 ms t1,b 5.0 ms
C 0.01 μF θq −40.0 mV
Caext 2500 μM σq 10.0 mV
ks 2.5 μM t0,q 1.0 ms
ϕ 0.06 μM·ms·−1 nA−1 t1,q 0.0 ms
τCA 10.0 ms Ca0 0.2 μM

Top: neurons are distinguished via slightly different values of gCaT (for HVCI neurons) and gCaL and gKCa (for HVCRA neurons). Bottom: values for CaT and CaL current kinetics and basic cellular properties. Values were chosen based on electrophysiology of HVC neurons in vitro (Daou et al. 2013).

Synapses

For the synapse dynamics, we adopt the formalism of Destexhe and Sejnowski (2001) and Destexhe et al. (1994) for electrically delivered neurotransmitter pulses, with one alteration: we define the inhibitory synapse coupling strengths gij not as fixed numbers but rather as functions of the maximum neurotransmitter concentration Tmax presented to a postsynaptic neuron. Within this framework, Tmax itself is a function of some modulatory process that may be external to HVC. Details of this formulation are presented in Behavior of an FSU in terms of Tmax and gij.

Isyn,ij=gij(Tmax(t))sij(t)(Esyn,iVi(t)) 2)
dsij(t)dt=α(Tmax(t),Vj(t))[1sij(t)]βsij(t) 3)
α(Tmax(t),Vj(t))=Tmax/T01+exp(Vj(t)VP)/KP 4)

Isyn,ij is the current seen by postsynaptic cell i as a result of input from presynaptic cell j. Esyn,i is the synaptic reversal potential of cell i, Vi(t) is the instantaneous membrane voltage of cell i, and sij(t) is the gating variable of the synapse entering (postsynaptic) cell i from (presynaptic) cell j. T0 has units of ms-mM so that α(Tmax,V), the rate of gate opening, has units of 1/time; β, the rate of gate closing, also has units of 1/time. Vj(t) is the presynaptic membrane voltage, and VP and KP are parameters governing the shape of the distribution of neurotransmitter rise and fall as it drives gating variables sij. Parameter values for the synapse equations are given in Table 3.

Table 3.

Parameter values for synapses

Quantity Value Quantity Value
Esyn, inh −80.0 mV βinh 0.18 ms−1
Esyn, exc 0.0 mV βexc 0.38 ms−1
Vp 2.0 mV T0 1.0 ms-mM
Kp 5.0 mV

We consider two broad classes of neurotransmitter: excitatory and inhibitory. The maximum concentration of excitatory neurotransmitter we take to have a constant value of 1.5 mM;1 the maximum concentration of inhibitory neurotransmitter (Tmax,inh) we permit to vary. We define the inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths (gij,inh-to-inh) so that they increase with Tmax,inh. Tmax,inh is some function of the activity of a cell that may be external to HVC. We will discuss the selection of Tmax,inh, and the dependence of gij on that value, in Behavior of an FSU in terms of Tmax and gij.

RESULTS

In this section we illustrate, via the time course of membrane voltages of an FSU's constituent neurons, how an FSU functions dynamically. The steps are as follows.

1) We will demonstrate that the excitatory neurons of an FSU will fire when given a low background excitation.

2) We will show that imposing sufficient inhibition upon these excitatory neurons leads to a quiescent FSU, for a low range of inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths.

3) We will show that a higher range of these coupling strengths, combined with a higher value of Tmax, can effect a regime in which the inhibitory neurons alternate their firing patterns; this scenario represents an active FSU.

4) By exploring the behavior of an FSU over ranges of both Tmax and the synapse coupling strengths gij, we will demonstrate: 1) the quiescent and active modes are robust to small variations in these parameter values; and 2) there exist additional transient modes of behavior, which one might expect to occasionally encounter in the laboratory.

5) We will note that connections from the excitatory to inhibitory cells are critical for the stable propagation of a series in active mode.

For all voltage time series shown in this paper, the dynamical model was written in Python, and the equations of motion were integrated using Python's adaptive fourth-order Runge-Kutta “odeINT” with a step of 0.1 ms.

Essential Architecture of an FSU

Our FSU is constructed as depicted in Fig. 1. The essential features of the connectivity are these:

1) There is all-to-all connectivity among the HVCI neurons.

2) Each HVCI neuron synapses to neurons in two out of three of the HVCRA ensembles as shown, where no two HVCI neurons “omit” the same HVCRA ensemble.

3) Neurons within each HVCRA ensemble synapse to all three HVCI neurons as shown.

With this connectivity, each HVCRA ensemble connects disynaptically, via an interneuron, to both of the other HVCRA ensembles. There are no monosynaptic connections between HVCRA ensembles in an FSU.

We leave unspecified 1) the rate of excitatory-to-excitatory connections within an ensemble, 2) the number of neurons within each ensemble to which one HVCI neuron projects, and 3) the number of neurons within an ensemble that project to a particular HVCI neuron. The number of HVCRA neurons per ensemble may be taken to be eight, for agreement with the observed ∼8:1 ratio of HVCRA to HVCI neurons in the nucleus; however, our model does not require any particular value.

HVCRA Neurons Spike in Absence of Inhibition

We first sought to simulate an environment in which HVCRA neurons will, in the absence of inhibition, fire above the threshold required to generate an action potential, the environment that is indicated by the results of KVL15. To this end, we gave the three HVCRA ensembles of Fig. 1 an injected current of 0.3 nA with a random variation of 3%. Figure 2 shows the voltage traces of three HVCRA cells, one in each ensemble. All projection cells spike.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Voltage traces of three HVCRA projection neurons, one in each of the three ensembles. They fire when given a low background current, in the absence of inhibition. The background current is a constant 0.3 nA with a random 3% variation.

A Quiescent FSU

Now we involve the interneurons. First, we connect the interneurons all-to-all and set these synapse strengths at values near 0.01 mS (see Table 4) and with the value of Tmax at an initial low value of 0.5 mM. With these choices, the HVCI cells fire continually.

Table 4.

Synapse strengths gij for the voltage traces in Fig. 3 for a quiescent FSU

Cell 0 1 2 3 4 5
0 0.0 0.011 0.011 1.11 1.1 1.11
1 0.011 0.0 0.01 1.11 1.1 1.1
2 0.011 0.011 0.0 1.11 1.1 1.1
3 1.1 1.11 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0
4 0.0 1.11 1.1 0.0 0.0 0.0
5 1.11 0.0 1.11 0.0 0.0 0.0

Units are micro-Siemens (μS). The value of 0.011 μS in (row 0, column 1) corresponds to g01: the synapse entering cell 0 from cell 1. Here, the inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths are of order 0.01 μS (they must be below ∼0.5 μS for continual firing of the HVCI neurons to occur); the inhibitory-to-excitatory coupling strengths must be above ∼1 μS to overpower the background excitation and silence the HVCRA neurons; the excitatory-to-inhibitory connections here are of order 1 μS, but they need not be any particular value for continual activity of the HVCI neurons in the FSU to occur.

Next, we permit each HVCI neuron to synapse directly to two of the three HVCRA ensembles, where no two HVCI neurons “omit” the same HVCRA ensemble. We set these inhibitory-to-excitatory connection strengths to be of order 1.0 μS. With these choices, the inhibition is sufficiently strong to overpower the background stimulation of 0.3 nA and silence all the HVCRA ensembles. That is: each HVCI neuron actively suppresses two of the HVCRA ensembles.

Figure 3 illustrates this result. The voltage traces of the HVCI cells (Fig. 3, top) shows that they are firing continually. As each HVCI neuron projects to two HVCRA ensembles, the three previously uninhibited HVCRA ensembles are suppressed. The corresponding schematic is shown in Fig. 3, bottom, where black- and white-filled symbols represent neural activity above and below threshold, respectively. This regime we call quiescence.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

A quiescent FSU. Voltage traces of the three HVCI neurons (top) and three HVCRA neurons each representing 1 ensemble (middle), all within a quiescent FSU, where the inhibitory-upon-inhibitory coupling strengths (see Table 4) are sufficiently low to permit the HVCI neurons to fire continually, and Tmax is 0.5 mM. Bottom: corresponding schematic, where triangles (cells 0, 1, and 2) and circles (cells 3, 4, and 5) represent HVCI and HVCRA neurons, respectively. The cell numbers on the schematic correspond to the numbering of the voltage traces. Black and white shapes indicate activity above and below threshold only, respectively. Each HVCI projects to two HVCRA neurons. When all three HVCI neurons are active simultaneously, the three HVCRA ensembles are suppressed.

An Active FSU

To transition from quiescent to active mode, our FSU requires two modifications:

1) The HVCI-to-HVCI coupling strengths must increase by roughly two orders of magnitude: from ∼0.01 to 2.0 mS (see Table 5).

Table 5.

Synapse strengths gij for the voltage traces in Fig. 4 of an active FSU

Cell 0 1 2 3 4 5
0 0.0 2.1 2.0 1.11 1.1 1.11
1 2.1 0.0 2.1 1.11 1.1 1.1
2 2.0 2.1 0.0 1.11 1.1 1.1
3 1.1 1.11 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0
4 0.0 1.11 1.1 0.0 0.0 0.0
5 1.11 0.0 1.11 0.0 0.0 0.0

Units are micro-Siemens (μS). The value of 0.011 μS in (row 0, column 1) corresponds to g01: the synapse entering cell 0 from cell 1. Here, the inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths must reach a threshold ∼2 μS for a stable series of bursting activity of HVCRA neurons to occur. The inhibitory-to-excitatory strengths may remain of order 1 μS; the excitatory-to-inhibitory strengths must reach a value of order 1 μS for the series of HVCI to occur.

2) Tmax must increase by a factor of roughly three: from ∼0.5 to 1.8 mM.

This second requirement implies the direct correlation between synapse coupling strengths and neurotransmitter concentration that we have embodied in Eqs. 7 and 8.

With these adjustments, and with the same background current, the HVCI cells now alternate their spiking activity. Given the wiring, the activity of the HVCRA ensembles also alternates. The alternations are a particular repeating series. Figure 4 illustrates this activity in a three-frame “movie.”

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

An active FSU, represented in a 3-frame “movie.” Here, the inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths are roughly 2 μS (Table 5) and Tmax = 1.8 mM, so that the inhibitory neurons may engage in competitive dynamics. Left: three “pairs” of voltage traces, each representing one HVCI neuron (top in each pair) and the one HVCRA ensemble to which that particular HVCI neuron does not directly project (bottom in each pair). Cells designated as “pairs” may fire simultaneously. Thus, series activity of the HVCI neurons effects a series of activity of the HVCRA ensembles. Right: the corresponding schematic of each pair, where a currently active pair is highlighted by a specific color. The activity proceeds clockwise, beginning with the “green pair” (top), followed by the “blue pair” second (middle), and “red pair” third (bottom). The numbering of cells on the schematic corresponds to the numbering of the voltage traces.

Figure 4, left, shows three pairs of voltage traces. Each pair represents the activity of one HVCI neuron and one HVCRA ensemble, where the HVCRA ensemble in each pair is the one HVCRA ensemble in the FSU to which that particular HVCI neuron does not project. Figure 4, right, shows the corresponding schematics, where each pair is sequentially highlighted by a specific color. Cells in each pair may fire simultaneously. By this wiring, series activity of the HVCI neurons effects a series of activity of the HVCRA ensembles.

First, the “green pair” fires (Fig. 4, top row). Here, the green HVCI neuron has activated and is able to suppress the other two HVCI neurons, which are colored in white in the schematic to indicate that they are currently inactive above threshold. The active (green) HVCI neuron projects to two of the three HVCRA ensembles; these two are colored in white in the schematic to indicate that they are not active above threshold. The third HVCRA ensemble, to which the active HVCI neuron does not project, is the only uninhibited ensemble. In the presence of the background excitation, this HVCRA ensemble bursts (starting around t = 250 ms); hence, it is also colored green.

Next, activity in the “inner loop” of inhibitory neurons proceeds clockwise, to the “blue” HVCI neuron. An analogous situation to the above is now established, wherein the active (blue) HVCI neuron suppresses the other two HVCI neurons and two of the HVCRA ensembles, and the one unsuppressed HVCRA ensemble (also blue) may burst (at t ∼315 ms). Finally, in the third row, the third “red pair” bursts at t ∼375 ms.

This active FSU represents one syllable of song during which three ensembles of HVCRA neurons fire in a sequence. Note again that to effect this series, along with the increase in the inhibitory coupling strengths, an increase in Tmax from 0.5 to 1.8 mM was required.

Figure 5 demonstrates that this particular series repeats, as long as there are elevated values of gij and Tmax. The six voltage traces are shown, now over three times the duration of Fig. 4. In Fig. 5, the panels are arranged so that the voltage traces of the three inhibitory and excitatory neurons are shown at top and bottom, respectively. This organization is intended to facilitate recognition that the inhibitory (excitatory) cells all possess roughly identical waveforms with phase offsets.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Three rotations of the series activity represented in Fig. 4, where the three inhibitory cells and excitatory ensembles are each grouped together at top and bottom, respectively. Color coding is as defined in Fig. 4.

Table 5 lists the synapse strengths for this active FSU, showing that the strengths of the inhibitory-to-inhibitory connections are roughly two orders of magnitude greater than their quiescence values. We note one concern regarding the relative weights of these strengths in active mode: they had to be close, to one part in 20, for the series WLC to occur. This point is addressed in discussion.

Behavior of an FSU in Terms of Tmax and gij

We have noted that the activity of an FSU can be altered by effecting changes in the couplings among HVCI neurons and the maximum neurotransmitter concentration Tmax in the synaptic gating variable driving function α(Tmax,V), where we use “Tmax” to refer to the concentration of neurotransmitter governing inhibitory activity.2

In this framework, Tmax itself is a function of some unspecified mechanism that may be external to HVC (see discussion). To characterize the range and stability of an FSU's behavioral modes, we examined the activity of the FSU's cells over a broad set of values for Tmax and the inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths gij. Figure 6 shows these results. It depicts FSU behavior over the ranges Tmax: [0.5,10.5] mM and gij: [0.01,10.5] nS, where we characterize “behavior” in terms of the HVCRA ensemble activity. The symbols that represent this activity are defined in Fig. 6, right.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

FSU behavior as a function of Tmax and gij, where “behavior” is defined in terms of the HVCRA activity. The symbols defining behavior are listed at right. The overlaid black path represents the trajectory for one neurotransmitter injection, as dictated by Eqs. 5–8. The two chief modes [quiescence and active bursting series winnerless competition (WLC)] dominate the space and are robust to small changes in these parameter values. In fact, the top row, at Tmax = 10.5 mM, looks the same through a Tmax value of 50 mM. In addition, there exist (at least) five modes of behavior within the transition regions between the two dominant modes, which one might expect to occasionally encounter in the laboratory. The few locations on the grid that contain no symbol showed some combination of rarer modes and quiescence, and were difficult to characterize.

We note three items regarding Fig. 6. First, the quiescent (solid blue circles) and active WLC bursting (green plus signs) modes are the most common behaviors, and they are robust to small variations in Tmax and gij: both span wide ranges of these parameter values. In fact, Fig. 6, top, at Tmax = 10.5, looks the same up to a Tmax value of 50 mM. Second, while the transition regions between quiescent and active mode are narrow compared with the permitted parameter ranges for these two dominant modes, the regions do possess a finite area, within which lie additional modes of behavior. We will return to these additional modes later. Third, we have drawn a black line on the plot. This line represents a “path” through (Tmax,gij) space, parameterized by time, that is capable of representing an FSU's transition between quiescent and active mode. We shall now discuss the significance of such a path and the method used to construct the particular path depicted in Fig. 6.

Using the known characteristics of a single song syllable as our guide, we sought to construct an equation for Tmax(t) and a relation between Tmax(t) and gij(t) that might permit an FSU to transition seamlessly between the quiescent and active states described earlier in this paper. Any such path in (Tmax,gij) space will produce a particular waveform of voltage behavior in both the HVCI and HVCRA neurons in an FSU, depending on which modes (the symbols in Fig. 6) are visited over the course of that path, and the rate at which they are traversed.

A detailed exploration of the classes of paths and their manifestations as voltage activity in HVC is beyond the scope of this paper; however, it will constitute a major topic of future investigations of FSUs. In this paper we explore a single path in this space, which, again, we selected by requiring that it be capable of effecting the quiescent and active behavior described earlier.

We began our path design by first asking that Tmax(t) sit at some minimum value until the FSU receives a neurotransmitter injection at time t = 0. At t = 0, Tmax should then rise rapidly with time. Tmax reaches a maximum value at some time t1, and then it instantly begins to decay more gradually. This process is captured by the following three steps:

Tmax(t)=Tmaxlowerbound(t<0) 5)
Tmax(t)=Tmaxlowerboundet/τr(0<t<t1) 6)
Tmax(t)=ϑTmaxlowerboundet/τf(t>t1), 7)

where τr and τf are constants dictating the rates of rise and fall, respectively, t1 is the time of transition from rise to fall:

t1=τrlog(TmaxupperboundTmaxlowerbound),

and ϑ is a constant chosen for continuity at t1:

ϑ=TmaxlowerboundTmaxupperbounde(t1/τr+t1/τf).

Tmaxlowerbound and Tmaxupperbound are the lower and upper bounds of Tmax, respectively. We chose Tmaxlowerbound to be 0.5 mM so that when the stimulating neurotransmitter signal arrives, the FSU will be in quiescent mode. We chose Tmaxupperbound to be 5.0 mM, a value that falls well within the permitted range for active behavior (Fig. 6). The rise and fall constants were chosen, in light of the relation between Tmax and gij to be discussed below, to mimic the time course of a typical syllable of song. The rise constant τr was taken to be 1 ms for a rapid transition from quiescent to active mode. The fall constant τf was taken to be 4 ms so that active mode would be sustained for ∼200 ms, the typical duration of a song syllable.

To model the response of the synapse strengths gij to Tmax(t), we have taken the form:

gij(Tmax)=μTmaxγ, 8)

and we require that gij rise no farther than a preselected value, which occurs when Tmax rises above a critical value of Tsaturation. We have imposed this requirement to achieve the rapid rise in the strengths gij with Tmax while avoiding the (otherwise inevitable) exponential increase of gij to values that are too strong for active mode to occur; such is the case for gij greater than ∼6. We selected the parameter values in Eq. 8 by imposing the Tmax-gij relations for generating the quiescent- and active-mode time series that were presented earlier in this section. Those relations were as follows: gij(Tmax = 0.5 mM) = 0.01 nS, and gij(Tmax = 1.8 mM) = 2.0 nS, for quiescent and active mode, respectively. The selected parameter values are Tsaturation = 2.0 mM, m = 0.18, and g = 4.2.

The time courses of Tmax and gij that correspond to the path of Fig. 6 are displayed in Fig. 7. The values of all parameters governing the Tmax-gij relation are listed in Table 6.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

The time course of neurotransmitter injection, in terms of Tmax(t) (top) and gij(t) (bottom) according to Eqs. 5–8, corresponding to the black path of Fig. 6.

Table 6.

Parameter values for the Tmax-gij relation

Quantity Value Quantity Value
Tmaxlowerbound 0.5 mM μ 0.18
Tmaxupperbound 5.0 mM γ 4.2
Tmaxsaturation 2.0 mM τr 1.0 ms
τf 4.0 ms

The lower bound on Tmax of 0.5 mM yields quiescent, but not active, FSU behavior. The upper bound on Tmax was chosen to lie within the permitted range for generating active mode (see Fig. 6). Tsaturation was chosen so that the initial fast rise in gij as a function of Tmax, which was observed in simulations, would be obeyed. Similarly, μ and γ were chosen so that gij assumes values of 0.01 and 2.0 nS for Tmax values of 0.5 and 1.8 mM, respectively. The rise and decay constants τr and τf were chosen so that the time course mimics the time course during song: a rapid transition from quiescence to active state and a sustaining of the active state for a typical syllable duration of ∼200 ms.

While our choice has some desirable features and allows us to probe the consequences of following a path in (Tmax,gij) space, it is clear that the degrees of freedom here are numerous. As noted, we plan to return to examine the possibilities.

We now return to our examination of the modes present in Fig. 6. As noted, in addition to the two dominant modes pictured in the diagram, we have identified five additional modes that inhabit a small but finite area. Each of these “rarer modes” occurs reliably given the respective values of Tmax and gij indicated in the diagram. These modes are 1) alternating (winnerless competition) bursts, where the alternations occur in no particular repeating series (magenta plus signs), 2) alternating bursts of two out of the three HVCRA neurons, while the third is suppressed (yellow plus signs), 3) alternating single spikes of two out of the three HVCRA neurons, while the third is suppressed (yellow stars), 4) transition from purely spiking to purely bursting activity (cyan left-pointing triangle), and 5) transition from series winnerless competition bursting to quiescence (blue rightpointing triangle).

One might expect, then, to occasionally encounter in the laboratory an HVCRA neuron exhibiting one of these alternative modes of behavior, rather than simply quiescence or simply a one-time burst. In light of this discovery of additional modes, in building complete songs, we will examine an unusual and unexplained HVCRA voltage trace of Hahnloser et al. (2002).

Importance of the Excitatory-to-Inhibitory Connections

In our simulations of an active FSU, we found an unexpected result regarding the excitatory-to-inhibitory connections (as pictured in Fig. 1): the series of HVCI neuron firings cannot occur without them. We discovered this requirement when, while maintaining the minimum value of 1.8 mM for Tmax and 2 mS for the inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths that are required for active mode, we lowered the excitatory-to-inhibitory connections from 1.0 to 0.01 nS. The series of HVCI neuron firings then failed to occur. Rather, one HVCI neuron continuously suppressed the other two. We noted a similar failing of the series when we removed any one of the excitatory connections independently. An increase in the inhibitory-to-inhibitory coupling strengths did not remedy this effect: similar results occurred for all values of inhibitory-upon-inhibitory connections from 1 to 20 μS. We conclude the following: in our model circuit, mutual inhibition alone is insufficient to achieve winnerless competition; feedback from the excitatory cells is required. We find this result to be extremely interesting, with the possible implication that variable concentrations of excitatory neurotransmitters are also contributing to HVC dynamics (see discussion).

Model Scalability

Our FSU model is, in principle, generalizable to an arbitrary number of HVCI neurons and HVCRA ensembles. For small networks (with neuronal populations on the order of tens to hundreds), the following rules work to effect the bimodal behavior that we have described:

1) The all-to-all inhibitory-to-inhibitory connectivity must be preserved.

2) Each HVCI neuron projects to all but one HVCRA ensemble.

3) Each HVCI neuron “chooses” a distinct HVCRA ensemble to deny a projection; that is: no two HVCI neurons omit the same HVCRA ensemble.

To examine how the model might scale for networks of biological size is beyond the scope of this paper. We also have yet to examine how the excitatory-to-inhibitory projections scale.3

BUILDING COMPLETE SONGS

We now demonstrate how multiple FSUs can be used to build a complete song. We use as our example the highly stereotyped song “motif” of the zebra finch. In doing so, we will show how the observed qualitative behavior of both excitatory and inhibitory populations in HVC during song are reproduced.

A trained male zebra finch sings a motif consisting of an invariant number of syllables separated by gaps corresponding to inhalation. Each recorded HVCRA neuron is observed to fire once during each motif, and reliably at a particular temporal location. Each HVCI neuron is observed to fire relatively continually throughout the song, with intermittent silences. During normal, uninterrupted singing, the syllable order is invariant and both syllables and the silent inter-syllable gaps are precisely timed upon repeated renditions of the song. This information is represented in the experimental raster plot by Hahnloser et al. (2002), which we have reproduced in Fig. 8.

Fig. 8.

Fig. 8.

A raster plot of spike times of HVCRA and HVCI neurons during repeated renditions of the zebra finch motif, reproduced from Hahnloser et al. (2002). The reader may find it of interest to compare this figure to the simulated raster plot of Fig. 9. [Reprinted from Nature by permission from Macmillan Publishers (Hahnloser et al. 2002)].

Within the “FSU framework”, the full motif could be attributable to a chain-like propagation linking FSU to FSU within HVC. In light of various lines of evidence that the syllables represent relatively independent structures in HVC, we seek another explanation. Studies of learning in juveniles have shown that syllables become stabilized by inhibition independently as they are learned (Vallentin et al. 2016). Attempts to interrupt song have indicated that individual syllables are relatively robust compared with the full motif: the motif can be interrupted by noninvasive techniques (Cynx 1990), but syllable interruption requires direct electrical interference (Ashmore et al. 2005; Vu et al. 2005). Finally, KVL15 found that global GABA antagonist infusions to HVC degraded song while local GABA infusions affected the activity of certain cells in the region of infusion but the overall song was preserved. These three lines of evidence suggest that, within our model framework, the FSUs function relatively independently. In our example model of a zebra finch four-syllable motif, then, we take the inter-FSU connectivity to be essentially nonexistent and instead invoke a neural feedback loop as a mechanism for activations of a succession of four FSUs.

To simulate a full song consisting of four syllables, we sequentially exposed a set of four FSUs to an identical injection of neurotransmitter according to our formulation of the path described by Eqs. 5–8 (the black path of Fig. 6), with the corresponding shapes of rise and fall of Tmax and gij with time depicted in Fig. 7. The decay constant for Tmaxf in Eq. 7) was chosen so that the permitted duration of series WLC bursting activity is ∼200 ms, a typical syllable duration. This value permits time for all three HVCRA ensembles in one FSU to burst exactly once.

Figure 9 shows the simulated raster plot that results when four unconnected FSUs are sequentially given the injection pictured in Fig. 7. In Fig. 9, top, a sequence of four neurotransmitter injections is represented. We assume that the first injection is initiated by an external stimulus, while the subsequent three are sustained by a neural feedback loop. Just below the Tmax(t) illustration in Fig. 9, top, the four corresponding FSUs are depicted, where electrodes labeled 1-10 have been attached by an experimenter to neurons whose identities are not known to the experimenter. The first eight electrodes attach to HVCRA neurons; electrodes 9 and 10 attach to HVCI neurons.

Fig. 9.

Fig. 9.

A simulated raster plot of bursting HVCRA and spiking HVCI neurons during song. Top: 4 neurotransmitter injections sequentially target 4 FSUs. Ten electrodes (arrowheads) have each been inserted into one neuron, by an experimenter who is blind to the neurons' identities. The resulting action potential timings are shown at bottom. See text for important notes.

Below the FSU diagrams is the raster plot of firings of the ten neurons over the course of the song, where the vertical numberings correspond to the numberings of the electrodes at Fig. 9, top. neuron 1, whose spike timings occur first, corresponds to one HVCRA neuron in the “top” ensemble of FSU 1. Neurons 2 and 3 correspond to different HVCRA neurons in the same “left” ensemble of FSU 1.

We make three specific notes regarding the simulated raster plot. First, the spike timings of neurons 2 and 3 line up in nearly the same temporal window but are offset slightly by different noise terms in the voltage time course of the somatic compartment for each neuron.4 The reader might find it of interest to compare these simulated plots for neurons 2 and 3 to the observed raster plots of neurons 2 and 3 in the plot of Hahnloser et al. (2002) (Fig. 8).

Second, note the atypically long simulated spike train of neuron 7, which belongs to FSU 2. The rotation of activity around FSU 2 was timed to reach this neuron during the decay stage of Tmax, such that FSU 2 entered one of the “rarer modes” that may occur within a transition region: the mode in which two HVCRA ensembles are spiking in alternation while the third is suppressed. This mode is represented by a yellow star in Fig. 6; note that the black path passes directly through it. The reader may find it of interest to compare these simulated timings of neuron 7 to the observed timings of neuron 5 in Hahnloser et al. (2002) (Fig. 8). Those authors found neuron 5 to reliably exhibit activity that was not describable as a single burst, upon repeated song renditions. To date, a satisfying explanation of that behavior has not been offered. Our model predicts that activity reminiscent of that behavior, that is, neither strictly quiescence nor strictly active WLC, should be observed in HVCRA neurons upon occasion.

Finally, note the behavior of the two HVCI neurons (9 and 10) in Fig. 9, bottom. For the majority of the song, when the FSU to which each HVCI belongs is quiescent, the HVCI neuron spikes continually. This is also the case for population activity of HVCI cells when the awake bird is not singing: the HVCI neurons are active continually and HVCRA neurons are essentially silent (Kozhevnikov and Fee 2007). Then, when the FSU to which a particular HVCI neuron belongs becomes active, there appear occasional lapses in the simulated activity of that HVCI neuron, consistent with observations.

The songs of other species might be constructed via similar considerations of the relationships among syllables. The starling sings a more complicated pattern that reflects a richer interplay among syllables. A model of the starling song thus might embody more complicated connectivities among FSUs. We look forward to investigating this possibility. In general, for the song of any species, we expect that inter-FSU connectivity is significantly less extensive than within-FSU connectivity.

DISCUSSION

Central-Pattern-Generator-Like Activity as a “Winnerless Competition”

The observed phenomenon of patterned activity among inhibitory neurons has been explained in terms of an interplay between competition and background excitation. The phenomenon was formalized in a framework called “winnerless competition” (WLC) by Rabinovich et al. (2001). WLC has been used to model some biological circuits that display central-pattern-generator-like (CPG) behavior, including a pyloric circuit (Huerta et al. 2001), molluscan hunting behavior (Varona et al. 2002), and olfactory processes in locusts (Laurent et al. 2001).

Rabinovich et al. (2001) describe a “node” as a cooperative ensemble of neural clusters in state space; they showed that one can alter the stabilities of nodes by altering the synapse strengths. For low values of coupling strengths all nodes are stable attractors, corresponding to neurons (or ensembles) that may all be active simultaneously. For higher coupling values, each node can correspond to a saddle fixed point, where a trajectory in state space is a closed heteroclinic orbit that sequentially traverses stable limit cycles in the vicinity of each saddle fixed point. This configuration represents sequential switching of activity among the three nodes. (See Rabinovich et al. 2013 for pictorial illustrations of these two modes of activity.) The neuronal interactions are defined in terms of competitive Lotka Volterra dynamics. (See Zeeman 1993 for a bifurcation analysis of a competitive three-dimensional Lotka Volterra system, and Afraimovich et al. 2004 for a phase space analysis of a “winnerless” Lotka Volterra system).

The dynamics of our model are consistent with this framework, save one feature: the reliance of stable series activations on feedback from the excitatory HVCRA neurons. In our simulations, this feedback was a necessary condition for series propagation in an active FSU, in addition to threshold values of Tmax and inhibitory-to-inhibitory couplings. Such dynamics are not considered within the context of WLC theory. We are uncertain of the biophysical implications of this feature; we suspect that the excitatory feedback serves to stabilize the competitive mode of behavior so that patterns of alternating activity can occur.

Plausibility of a Chemically Delivered Signal to Alter Synaptic Coupling Strengths

We have suggested that a bird's need to vocalize leads to an injection of inhibitory neurotransmitter in the vicinity of an FSU, which in turn initiates the first syllable of song. We have further suggested that a feedback loop triggers sequential releases of neurotransmitter, to play a complete song. Here we examine the plausibility of such a neuromodulatory mechanism and its timescale of action.

Sources of neuromodulation.

Sources of transient changes in synaptic action (commonly called “synaptic agonist transients” or “transients”) have been identified throughout the central nervous systems of many species. In some mammalian brains, the ventral tegmental area (VTA) has been identified as a reservoir of dopaminergic cells (Phillipson 1979) that, in the avian brain, project to and impose neuromodulatory effects upon regions throughout the central nervous system and are a common source of transients. The VTA mediates a variety of tasks in the brain, including inhibitory action in some locations (e.g., Stamatakis et al. 2013). While direct projections from VTA to HVC in the avian brain have not been identified, there does exist some evidence for a role of VTA in mediating birdsong: multineuron activity in the VTA of Bengalese finches has been found to consistently increase before the initiation and termination of song bouts (Kapur 2008). We consider it reasonable to postulate that VTA affects HVC, even indirectly, as the VTA serves numerous brain areas in a neuromodulatory role. We do not make any claim that VTA is involved in HVC modulation, but rather we suggest that it merits an examination.

Mechanisms of neuromodulation and effects on synapses.

Both the mechanisms of neuromodulatory action and their effects on synapses are difficult to study experimentally, due to the inaccessibility of narrow synaptic clefts, uncertainties in the geometry of the synapse structure, and the observation that synapses vary widely in geometry (Scimemi and Beato 2009). Rise times are coupled to receptor dynamics; decay times can be mediated by diffusion, reuptake, binding to receptors, and enzymic breakdown.

The magnitude over which a neurotransmitter concentration can change in a synaptic cleft varies widely. Most sources cite a saturating value on the order of 1 mM before dropping over two to three orders of magnitude (Scimemi and Beato 2009; Barberis et al. 2011). The effects of such changes on synaptic coupling are generally measured via current responses (e.g., Mozrzymas 2004), but no direct methods are available to measure the transient at synapses.

For a coarse idea, we look to behavioral studies. Here, we implicitly assume that a change in macroscopic behavior reflects a change, on the cellular level, in circuit modality. Much work in this area has focused on dopamine as a modulator of behavior. Dopamine levels in Area X of zebra finches have been found to be significantly lower during undirected compared with sexually motivated song (Heimovics and Riters 2008), which suggests that dopaminergic neurotransmission may differentially modulate vocal behavior depending on context.

We distill from this information that it is reasonable to propose neuromodulation as a mechanism for transitioning between a circuit's modes of activity and that the mechanism may involve a change of synaptic coupling strengths. We further infer that our model's requirement of a roughly threefold increase in Tmax to effect such a change falls well within the bounds set by the current level of understanding.

Timescales of neuromodulatory action.

Burst times of HVCRA neurons during song have a typical precision of ∼1 ms (Kozhevnikov and Fee 2007). Our model thus implies that neurotransmitter rise and decay timescales must be that precise if neuromodulation is to serve as the mode-switching mechanism. We also have implied that the temporary elevation of neurotransmitter can sustain an FSU's active mode for hundreds of milliseconds, a typical syllable duration. How reasonable are these requirements?

Rise times of neurotransmitter concentrations on the order of milliseconds have been reported (e.g., Robinson et al. 2003). In addition, routine injected neurotransmitter “pulses” that are designed to simulate real synaptic action typically are delivered over 0.1 ms, implying that neurotransmitter concentrations in real biological circuits are believed to be capable of changing significantly over 0.1 ms.

How long can a transient's effect on a circuit last? The time course can vary from one to hundreds of milliseconds, depending on the magnitude of the initial injection and other modulatory factors such as the presence of particular receptors. Scimemi and Beato (2009), for example, showed that the duration of postsynaptic glutamatergic currents can vary over two orders of magnitude, depending on whether they are mediated by AMPA receptors alone (tdecay ∼2 ms) or if NMDA receptors are also recruited (tdecay ∼200 ms).

Finally, we are concerned about the temporal precision of neurotransmitter decay. Can such a process occur reliably with a precision of 1 ms, upon the sounding of each syllable? The consideration here is not the shape of neurotransmitter fall-off, but rather that the fall-off reach one critical value at a reliable time following injection. That is: in our model, the series that occurs during active mode does not slow gradually as neurotransmitter concentration decreases. Rather, the series occurs at a constant rate above the threshold concentration, below which there occurs a sharp transition back to quiescence. An answer to our question here would be speculative, however, given the current experimental uncertainties.

Possible Use of Excitatory-to-Excitatory Connections

Some monosynaptic connections between HVCRA neurons have been identified experimentally (KVL15), although at a rate roughly 100 times lower than that of the reciprocal inhibitory-excitatory connections. While our model does not require excitatory-excitatory monosynaptic connections, it does allow for them. Here we note how excitatory-excitatory connections might arise within our FSU framework, and we offer a possible biophysical use for such connections.

We have identified two possible locations for excitatory-excitatory connections in our model. One is within a particular ensemble of an FSU. Such connections do not strike us as important, however, as unconnected HVCRA neurons in one ensemble may be active simultaneously even if not directly connected. A second, and possibly useful, location for such connections could be as follows: between FSUs.

Consider two FSUs, where there exists a unidirectional monosynaptic connection from FSU 1 to FSU 2. Imagine that FSU 1 lies within the spatial region receiving a neurotransmitter injection, while FSU 2 does not. When FSU 1 receives the injection, its ensembles may now participate in the corresponding syllable. Given the connection to FSU 2, the ensembles of FSU 2 may also be recruited to participate in the syllable, even though FSU 2 has not received the injection directly. In this way, excitatory-to-excitatory connections might serve to reduce the required spatial extent of the injection. This suggestion constitutes little more than speculation; we offer it to address the observed low rate of such connections.

Attempts to Incite FSU Mode Switching via Electrical Manipulation

We describe briefly an alternative method we investigated to achieve an active series of firings of HVCI neurons: electrical stimulation. Electrical signaling can occur faster than chemical signaling and on a timescale whose precision has been established well beyond the current state of understanding of the temporal precision of neuromodulatory action. We initially considered it to be the most likely candidate for an FSU mode-switching mechanism.

Beginning from the quiescence regime, we attempted to incite series activity via current pulses delivered to particular cells in the circuit. We gave a current pulse (shorter than 10 ms) atop the background current to various subsets of neurons in one FSU. The pulse-receiving neuron(s) spiked once in response; the other neurons did not respond to the pulse for any biophysically realistic values of injected current (<700 pA) or synapse strength (<100 μS). The circuit activity resumed its previous behavior within 10 ms. We concluded that if electrical stimulation is to effect mode-switching, more elaborate injection designs would be required. Seeking a simpler solution, we looked elsewhere.

The Silent Intersyllable Gaps

We chose not to include the silent intersyllable gaps in our model of HVC, because at this time we find no particularly compelling representation for a gap. We suspect, however, that the gaps are integral to song production and have a temporal representation at the level of HVC. Cooling HVC uniformly stretches syllables and gaps while cooling the nucleus RA had no effect on song (Long and Fee 2008). More importantly, correlations between respiratory action and activity in HVC have been demonstrated (Andalman et al. 2011; Amador et al. 2013). We speculate that the duration of each silence conveys part of the informational content of song.

Structural vs. Functional Connectivity Rates

We comment briefly on relating our model's functional connectivity to structural connectivity rates, where functional connections are defined as the subset of structural connections that are currently in use by the circuit. Our model requires high rates of functional connectivity within an FSU. We would like to directly compare these rates to observed structural connectivity rates (e.g., Mooney and Prather 2005; KVL15). Such a comparison, however, would require knowledge of the learning stage (see Okubo et al. 2015 for evidence of synapse pruning during learning in HVC) and the rate at which a formerly used synaptic connection dissolves (see Luo and O'Leary 2005 and Walsh and Lichtman 2003 for evidence that axons projecting to unused synapses have some timescale of retraction or degeneration). Calcium imaging techniques appear to hold the greatest promise for illuminating the relationship between structural and functional connections. Until then, we will withhold speculation.

Spatial Considerations

The spatial organization of FSUs, which we have not considered in this paper, is an important topic to consider. There exists evidence for such organization in HVC (Stauffer et al. 2012; Day et al. 2013; Poole et al. 2012), and efforts are underway to incorporate evidence of spatial organization into a model of functional connectivity (Markowitz et al. 2015).

A Concern Regarding Model Robustness to Small Differences Among Synapse Strengths

As noted, in our numerical simulations the relative values of the six interneuron-to-interneuron gij values must be identical to 1 part in 20 in order for the series WLC activity to occur reliably. This is a worrisome weakness. In a follow-up paper we will examine possible reasons for this sensitivity and possible methods to remedy it.

Model Predictions and Suggested Experiment

Here we suggest experimental tests of 1) the FSU model predictions, and 2) the suggestion that neuromodulatory action initiates song. These tests can currently be performed in the laboratory.

Testing FSU model predictions.

The plot of FSU behavior as a function of Tmax and gij (Fig. 6) reveals five additional modes of behavior that reside within the slender transition regions between quiescent and active mode. This finding predicts that one should occasionally find HVCRA neurons exhibiting activity that is neither strictly below action potential threshold nor strictly a one-time-burst event. Furthermore, such atypical behavior of a particular neuron may be expected to occur reliably at a particular temporal location during song. Hahnloser et al. (2002) might have encountered such an event, depicted by their raster plot of neuron 5 (Fig. 8). A deeper examination of this possibility requires the recording of a significantly larger number of HVCRA neurons during song.

Testing the proposal that neuromodulation initiates song.

To examine whether neuromodulation plays a role in initiating song, it might be helpful to examine whole cell recordings in the awake bird, where, in place of an external stimulus, a neuromodulatory agent is delivered to HVC. The aim here is to identify some neuromodulatory agent that can act as the external stimulus to initiate song. Heimovics and Riters (2008) performed such an experiment in the starling HVC and found that infusing dopamine agonists stimulated song while antagonists hindered it. This experiment merits repeating. Furthermore, it would be instructive to compare the result of infusing such a neurotransmitter both globally and locally to HVC. In our model, a global infusion would ignite all FSUs in HVC simultaneously; whether it would be possible for patterned activity to arise in that scenario, we would be interested to learn.

A second suggestion is to specifically target VTA, with experiments similar to the work of Kapur (2008). That author found that VTA activity is associated with the onset and offset of song bouts. The nature of that association was not determined, and the finding merits a follow-up examination. As noted earlier, we are making no claim that VTA is the probable origin of the process that modulates Tmax in HVC. Rather, as VTA has been identified as a modulator of numerous brain areas, we are suggesting that it would be worthwhile to examine its possible role in the scenario presented in this paper.

A LOOK FORWARD

We have offered a biophysically based model of a fundamental bimodal unit of song representation in the avian HVC. The two modes are robust to small variations in values of the parameters governing their dynamics. When considering a population of such FSUs, much of the experimentally observed population activity of HVCI and HVCRA neurons can be reproduced qualitatively. Additional modes of activity can occur during the transition regions between the two dominant modes, and on occasion these should be identifiable in the laboratory. We have also discovered that excitatory feedback to the inhibitory population is necessary for a stable propagation of series firings within the framework of winnerless competition, a finding that calls for a physical explanation.

We have offered no details of how the FSU represents the temporal and spectral content of a syllable. Could the rate of series propagation be related? Could the number of HVCRA ensembles firing per syllable be related? Both possibilities might be addressed by performing tests akin to the raster plot of Hahnloser et al. (2002) during an experiment in which the ambient temperature is varied. Such “cooling studies” have revealed direct relationships among temperature, rate of song production, and spectral content (e.g., Long and Fee 2008).

Many questions arise regarding our proposed mechanism for effecting a bimodal FSU. If an injection of inhibitory neurotransmitter is responsible, then by what mechanism is the “donor” inhibitory cell triggered to release neurotransmitter? Is this donor cell internal or external to HVC? How does the neurotransmitter injection find its target FSU? What spatial precision is required of the injection? It would be fascinating to ultimately engineer an experiment capable of targeting individual FSUs with a neurotransmitter injection. It would serve as an ultimate test of our model if, for example, in the zebra finch brain the “FSU 2” of Fig. 9 could be reliably identified, and reliably elicit the bird's “Syllable B” repeatedly, upon repeated targeted injections to that FSU.

Finally, there are the critical questions of model robustness on large scales and in the presence of high noise. How strictly must our “scalability rules” be followed by a circuit consisting of thousands of cells and synaptic connections? Do the rules permit some reasonable degree of flexibility? In follow-up papers we will address these questions and describe a method of statistical data assimilation that can, in principle, determine whether a proposed functional connectivity accurately represents data from a real biophysical circuit.

GRANTS

This research was funded by the Office of Naval Research Grant N00014-13-1-0205.

DISCLOSURES

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s).

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

E.A. created the model, performed the numerical calculations, and wrote the majority of the paper. H.D.I.A. focused and guided the writing of the paper, in terms of presentation, pedagogical considerations, and emphasis on particular model components so as to broaden the impact of the work. Both authors contributed to the exploration of the biophysical implications of the model and the interpretation for functional birdsong vocalization.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We appreciate Arij Daou, Daniel Margoliash, and Michael Long for enlightening explanations, perspectives, and translations. Misha Rabinovich, Daniel Breen, and Nirag Kadakia offered useful conversations. Thanks also to three anonymous reviewers for valuable feedback on an earlier version of this paper.

Footnotes

1

Throughout this paper, we will adopt the custom of referring to concentration in moles. The custom exists because of the many uncertainties involved in determining synapse volumes (see discussion).

2

As noted earlier, in this model we assume that the maximum neurotransmitter concentration governing the excitatory connections is a constant. It is, of course, possible that a modulation of the excitatory synaptic connections is also occurring. This consideration will enter into the construction of a more generalized model.

3

In addition, not all HVCRA neurons in one active FSU must receive a turn to fire. We suspect that a given FSU contains a reservoir of HVCRA ensembles: significantly more than are required to generate one syllable in the trained bird.

4

Regarding the noise terms and the consistency with which neuron 3 fires slightly before neuron 2 across trials in Fig. 9: For the creation of Fig. 9, each ensemble contained two HVCRA neurons. Within each ensemble, one of the HVCRA neurons received a noise term of mean magnitude 0.005 nA, and the second received a noise term of mean magnitude 0.03 nA; each noise term was assigned a variation of 10%. It is the difference in mean magnitude that simulated the consistent offset in firing times of neurons 2 and 3.

REFERENCES

  1. Afraimovich V, Zhigulin V, Rabinovich M. On the origin of reproducible sequential activity in neural circuits. Chaos 14: 1123–1129, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amador A, Perl YS, Mindlin GB, Margoliash D. Elemental gesture dynamics are encoded by song premotor cortical neurons. Nature 495: 59–64, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andalman AS, Foerster JN, Fee MS. Control of vocal and respiratory patterns in birdsong: dissection of forebrain and brainstem mechanisms using temperature. PLoS One 6: e25461, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashmore RC, Wild JM, Schmidt MF. Brainstem and forebrain contributions to the generation of learned motor behaviors for song. J Neurosci 25: 8543–8554, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barberis A, Petrini EM, Mozrzymas JW. Impact of synaptic neurotransmitter concentration time course on the kinetics and pharmacological modulation of inhibitory synaptic currents. Front Cell Neurosci 5: 6, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Breen D, Shirman S, Armstrong E, Daou A, Margoliash D, Abarbanel HD. Dynamical models for interneurons in the birdsong system. Biol Cybern. To be submitted: summer 2016. [Google Scholar]
  7. Cannon J, Kopell N, Gardner T, Markowitz J. Neural sequence generation using spatiotemporal patterns of inhibition. PLoS Comput Biol 11: e1004581, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cynx J. Experimental determination of a unit of song production in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). J Comp Psychol 104: 3–10, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Daou A, Ross MT, Johnson F, Hyson RL, Bertram R. Electrophysiological characterization and computational models of HVC neurons in the zebra finch. J Neurophysiol 110: 1227–1245, 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Day NF, Terleski KL, Nykamp DQ, Nick TA. Directed functional connectivity matures with motor learning in a cortical pattern generator. J Neurophysiol 109: 913–923, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Destexhe A, Mainen ZF, Sejnowski TJ. Synthesis of models for excitable membranes, synaptic transmission and neuromodulation using a common kinetic formalism. J Comput Neurosci 1: 195–230, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Destexhe A, Sejnowski TJ. Thalamocortical Assemblies: How Ion Channels, Single Neurons and Large-Scale Networks Organize Sleep Oscillations. Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dutar P, Vu HM, Perkel DJ. Multiple cell types distinguished by physiological, pharmacological, and anatomic properties in nucleus HGC of the adult zebra finch. J Neurophysiol 80: 1828–1838, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gibb L, Gentner TQ, Abarbanel HD. Brain stem feedback in a computational model of birdsong sequencing. J Neurophysiol 102: 1763–1778, 2009a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gibb L, Gentner TQ, Abarbanel HD. Inhibition and recurrent excitation in a computational model of sparse bursting in song nucleus hvc. J Neurophysiol 102: 1748–1762, 2009b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hahnloser RH, Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. An ultra-sparse code underlies the generation of neural sequences in a songbird. Nature 419: 65–70, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Heimovics SA, Riters LV. Evidence that dopamine within motivation and song control brain regions regulates birdsong context-dependently. Physiol Behav 95: 258–266, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Huerta R, Varona P, Rabinovich MI, Abarbanel HD. Topology selection by chaotic neurons of a pyloric central pattern generator. Biol Cybern 84: L1–L8, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kadakia N, Armstrong E, Breen D, Morone U, Daou A, Margoliash D, Abarbanel HD. Nonlinear statistical data assimilation for HVCRA neurons in the avian song system. Biol Cybern. First published September 29, 2016; doi: 10.1007/s00422-016-0697-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kapur R. Contributions of the Avian VTA to Behavioral Switching (PhD dissertation). San Francisco, CA: Univ. of California, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  21. Kosche G, Vallentin D, Long MA. Interplay of inhibition and excitation shapes a premotor neural sequence. J Neurosci 35: 1217–1227, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kozhevnikov AA, Fee MS. Singing-related activity of identified HVC neurons in the zebra finch. J Neurophysiol 97: 4271–4283, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Laurent G, Stopfer M, Friedrich RW, Rabinovich MI, Volkovskii A, Abarbanel HD. Odor encoding as an active, dynamical process: experiments, computation, and theory. Annu Rev Neurosci 24: 263–297, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Li M, Greenside H. Stable propagation of a burst through a one-dimensional homogeneous excitatory chain model of songbird nucleus HVC. Phys Rev E Stat Nonlin Soft Matter Phys 74: 011918, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Long MA, Fee MS. Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature 456: 189–194, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Long MA, Jin DZ, Fee MS. Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation. Nature 468: 394–399, 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Luo L, O'Leary DD. Axon retraction and degeneration in development and disease. Annu Rev Neurosci 28: 127–156, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Markowitz JE, Liberti WA 3rd, Guitchounts G, Velho T, Lois C, Gardner TJ. Mesoscopic patterns of neural activity support songbird cortical sequences. PLoS Biol 13: e1002158, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mooney R, Prather JF. The hvc microcircuit: the synaptic basis for interactions between song motor and vocal plasticity pathways. J Neurosci 25: 1952–1964, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mozrzymas JW. Dynamism of GABE A receptor activation shapes the personality of inhibitory synapses. Neuropharmacology 47: 945–960, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Okubo TS, Mackevicius EL, Payne HL, Lynch GF, Fee MS. Growth and splitting of neural sequences in songbird vocal development. Nature 528: 352–357, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Phillipson O. Afferent projections to the ventral tegmental area of Tsai and interfascicular nucleus: a horseradish peroxidase study in the rat. J Comp Neurol 187: 117–143, 1979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Poole B, Markowitz JE, Gardner TJ. The song must go on: resilience of the songbird vocal motor pathway. PLoS One 7: e38173, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rabinovich M, Tristan I, Varona P. Neural dynamics of attentional cross-modality control. PLoS One 8: e64406, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Rabinovich M, Volkovskii A, Lecanda P, Huerta R, Abarbanel H, Laurent G. Dynamical encoding by networks of competing neuron groups: winnerless competition. Phys Rev Lett 87: 068102, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Rabinovich MI, Varona P, Selverston AI, Abarbanel HD. Dynamical principles in neuroscience. Rev Modern Phys 78: 1213, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  37. Robinson DL, Venton BJ, Heien ML, Wightman RM. Detecting subsecond dopamine release with fast-scan cyclic voltammetry in vivo. Clin Chem 49: 1763–1773, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Schmidt MF, Perkel DJ. Slow synaptic inhibition in nucleus hvc of the adult zebra finch. J Neurosci 18: 895–904, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Scimemi A, Beato M. Determining the neurotransmitter concentration profile at active synapses. Mol Neurobiol 40: 289–306, 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Solis MM, Perkel DJ. Rhythmic activity in a forebrain vocal control nucleus in vitro. J Neurosci 25: 2811–282, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Stamatakis AM, Jennings JH, Ung RL, Blair GA, Weinberg RJ, Neve RL, Boyce F, Mattis J, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K, Stuber GD. A unique population of ventral tegmental area neurons inhibits the lateral habenula to promote reward. Neuron 80: 1039–1053, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stauffer TR, Elliott KC, Ross MT, Basista MJ, Hyson RL, Johnson F. Axial organization of a brain region that sequences a learned pattern of behavior. J Neurosci 32: 9312–9322, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Vallentin D, Long MA. Motor origin of precise synaptic inputs onto forebrain neurons driving a skilled behavior. J Neurosci 35: 299–307, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Varona P, Rabinovich MI, Selverston AI, Arshavsky YI. Winnerless competition between sensory neurons generates chaos: a possible mechanism for molluscan hunting behavior. Chaos 12: 672–677, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Verduzco-Flores SO, Bodner M, Ermentrout B. A model for complex sequence learning and reproduction in neural populations. J Comp Neurosci 32: 403–423, 2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Vu ET, Mazurek ME, Kuo YC. Identification of a forebrain motor programming network for the learned song of zebra finches. J Neurosci 14: 6924–6934, 1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Walsh MK, Lichtman JW. In vivo time-lapse imaging of synaptic takeover associated with naturally occurring synapse elimination. Neuron 37: 67–73, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Yildiz IB, Kiebel SJ. A hierarchical neuronal model for generation and online recognition of birdsongs. PLoS Comput Biol 7: e1002303, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Zeeman ML. Hopf bifurcations in competitive three-dimensional Lotka-Volterra systems. Dynam Stability Syst 8: 189–216, 1993. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurophysiology are provided here courtesy of American Physiological Society

RESOURCES