Abstract
The objective of this study was to examine whether cognitive change and age predicted work outcome in the context of supported employment (SE) and Compensatory Cognitive Training (CCT) in severe mental illness (SMI). Forty unemployed outpatients receiving SE (7 young [20–35]; 15 middle-aged [36–50]; 18 older [51–66]) completed cognitive assessments at baseline and after 12 weeks of CCT. Logistic regression analyses showed that improvement in attention/vigilance significantly predicted work attainment (B=2.35, SE=1.16, p=0.043). Young and older participants were more likely to obtain work than middle-aged participants (B=4.03, SE=1.43, p=0.005; B=2.16, SE=0.93, p=0.021, respectively). Improved attention and age-group (young and old) were associated with better work outcomes after SE+CCT. Improving attention may be an important target for improving work outcome in SMI. Middle-aged individuals may need additional support to return to work.
Keywords: cognition, vocational outcome, cognitive training
1. Introduction
Cognitive impairments limit vocational functioning in patients with severe mental illness (SMI, including schizophrenia-spectrum disorders, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder; McGurk and Mueser, 2004; Tsang et al., 2010). Interventions combining supported employment (SE) with cognitive training appear to improve both cognitive and work outcomes in this population (Bell et al., 2005a; McGurk et al., 2009). However, not all patients obtain work, so identifying predictors of response could help focus therapeutic efforts to improve outcomes. Global cognition, executive functioning, and learning/memory may be modifiable factors that predict vocational outcomes in patients with SMI (McGurk and Mueser, 2006), and cognitive improvements during treatment may better predict work outcome than does baseline cognitive functioning (McGurk et al., 2009). From a lifespan perspective, age is another important factor when studying work outcomes in people with SMI (Luciano and Meara, 2014). Although age is not necessarily a barrier to employment (Bell et al., 2005b; Catty et al., 2008; Twamley et al., 2012a) or cognitive training improvement (Bell et al., 2005a; Twamley et al., 2011; Wykes et al., 2009), some studies have shown that age moderates the effect of cognitive training on cognitive outcomes, with younger patients showing greater improvements in cognition than older patients (Bowie et al., 2014; Kontis et al., 2013). Similar results have been found when cognitive training has been combined with supported employment (McGurk and Mueser, 2008). Work skills assessed by an observer-rated scale seem to improve more in patients who are within their first years of the first-episode psychosis than patients with a more chronic course of the illness (Bowie et al., 2014). However, to our knowledge the effect of age on work outcomes in terms of job attainment and in the context of combined SE and cognitive training is unknown.
We aimed to explore cognitive changes and lifespan periods as predictors of work outcomes in a combined SE and cognitive training intervention for people with SMI. Given the exploratory nature of the research question and the scarcity of previous literature, we did not have an a priori hypothesis regarding which cognitive changes would be related to work outcomes. With regard to lifespan periods, we hypothesized that younger participants would have better work outcomes.
2. Methods
The current study was part of a two-year randomized controlled trial comparing the effects of SE with and without a 12-week Compensatory Cognitive Training intervention (SE+CCT) in outpatients with SMI. Results regarding age as a moderator of cognitive change within the CCT condition have been published previously (Thomas et al., in press). The inclusion criteria for the study were: (1) DSM-IV diagnosis of SMI, (2) unemployed but stating a goal of work, (3) age 18 or older, (4) English-speaking, and (5) no presence of dementia or intellectual disability. Diagnoses were confirmed via comprehensive chart reviews by trained raters. Participants were recruited via clinician referral at the Outpatient Psychiatric Services clinic at the University of California, San Diego, and continued to receive their usual psychiatric care during the trial. All participants gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by the IRB. Participants included in the current analysis were those randomized to the SE+CCT group who completed the CCT intervention and for whom work outcome data were available (n=40; 25 with severe mood disorders, 15 with psychosis; mean age=47.2, SD=10.15; 60% male). Participants received 12 individual 1-hour sessions of CCT from their employment specialist, in addition to SE sessions, during the first 12 weeks of the study. CCT focuses on compensatory strategy teaching and habit learning in the domains of prospective memory, attention, learning, and executive functioning (Mendella et al., 2015; Twamley et al., 2012b). CCT is a brief intervention that teaches strategies in the domains above via interactive, game-like activities to maintain interest and increase focus and motivation. Therapists elicit clients’ personal goals and link the strategies taught to their specific goals in order to enhance intrinsic motivation. Home exercises are assigned to promote habit learning and strategy use in the real world.
Participants were administered an expanded MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (Nuechterlein et al., 2008) at baseline and after CCT (3-months) and followed until 24-months. Work outcome was defined as obtaining a competitive job during the 24-month study. Cognitive predictors included the change between the standardized raw pre and post-CCT (12 week follow up) scores in the following cognitive domains: processing speed (Trail Making Test, part A; Symbol Coding), attention/vigilance (Continuous Performance Test–Identical Pairs), spatial working memory (Spatial Span), learning (Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised; Brief Visual Memory Test–Revised), executive functioning (Neuropsychological Assessment Battery–Mazes; Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–64 card version (Kong et al., 2000), total correct and perseverative errors; Trail Making Test, B minus A; Letter fluency, FAS) and prospective memory (Memory for Intentions Screening Test, Raskin S, 2004). Premorbid IQ was assessed with the Wide Range Achievement Test–III (Wilkinson, 1993). Additional measures included the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (Kay et al., 1987) and the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Hamilton, 1967).
Participants were grouped into three developmental stages accordantly to the lifespan period: young adults (20–35 years), middle-aged adults (36–50 years) or older participants (51–66 years). Age groups included 7 young, 15 middle-age, and 18 older participants. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to examine baseline differences among age-groups. Logistic regression analyses using forward stepwise entry were conducted with job attainment as the dependent variable and change in cognitive domains, the age group and the interaction of these variables as potential predictors. Several covariates were included in subsequent logistic regression analyses to control for potential confounders.
3. Results
No significant differences between age groups were found in any sociodemographic, clinical, or work-related variables beyond the expected differences related to age (Table 1). Regarding baseline cognition, younger participants had higher scores than the both middle-aged and older participants in the global composite score (mean difference=0.52, p=0.018; 0.60, p=0.005 respectively) and in the learning domain (mean difference=0.89, p=0.045; 0.92, p=0.030 respectively) (Table 1). During the study, 19 participants (47.5%) obtained a job. Regression analysis showed a statistically significant model (X2=16.02, p=0.001) in which variance in job attainment was predicted by improvement in attention/vigilance (B=2.35, SE=1.16, p=0.043) and age, with younger and older participants being more likely to obtain work compared with middle-aged participants (B=4.03, SE=1.43, p=0.005; B=2.16, SE=0.93, p=0.021 respectively). The age group was the first variable introduced in the model and explained a 29% of the variance in job attainment. The final model including both variables explained the 44% of the variance in the outcome. Changes in the other cognitive domains did not add significant predictive value to the model (ps>0.064) and neither the interaction of change in global cognition with age group nor the interaction of any specific cognitive domain with age group were statistically significant (ps>0.057), suggesting that age did not have a differential effect on work outcome as a function of change in cognition. The main results were maintained after controlling for baseline differences between groups (global cognition, p=0.132; learning, p=0.154) and other potential confounders such as baseline attention/vigilance (p=0.296) and premorbid IQ (p=0.549). Among clinical variables, diagnostic, baseline depressive symptom severity, and positive and negative symptom severity were not significant predictors (ps>0.456), nor were baseline differences in number of years worked during adult life (p=0.862).
Table 1.
Variables, mean (sd) | Young adults | Medium-age adults | Old adults | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Premorbid IQ | 106 (6.51) | 103.53 (5.63) | 106.50 (6.29) | 1.03 | 0.368 |
Years of education | 12.43 (2.44) | 13.07 (2.58) | 13.61 (1.82) | 0.75 | 0.480 |
Clinical variables | |||||
Duration of illness, years | 15.86 (6.31) | 21.27 (11.0) | 34.22 (13.90) | 8.06 | 0.001 |
Number of hospitalizations | 3.29 (2.43) | 3.07 (2.97) | 2 (2.20) | 0.99 | 0.379 |
Total CPZE | 257.14 (252.37) | 200 (428.54) | 262.96 (654.31) | 0.06 | 0.938 |
PANSS Positive symptoms | 13.86 (7.71) | 12.73 (4.67) | 11.33 (4.81) | 0.64 | 0.532 |
PANSS Negative symptoms | 12.14 (3.98) | 11.73 (3.92) | 13.44 (5.81) | 0.53 | 0.591 |
PANSS General symptoms | 29.29 (9.45) | 28.71 (5.01) | 27.72 (8.33) | 0.13 | 0.876 |
HDRS Depressive symptoms | 15.71 (9.01) | 19.40 (8.97) | 15.83 (10.15) | 0.67 | 0.519 |
Work-related variables | |||||
Number of years worked during adult life | 5.86 (3.63) | 14.23 (6.70) | 27.19 (11.66) | 16.55 | <0.001 |
Months worked in past 5 years | 23.14 (17.61) | 18.71 (17.99) | 29.83 (20.79) | 1.33 | 0.277 |
Cognitive domains a | |||||
Processing speed | 0.02 (0.29) | −0.08 (0.58) | 0.03 (0.48) | 0.23 | 0.797 |
Attention/vigilance | 0.65 (0.53) | 0.02 (0.84) | −0.05 (0.81) | 2.10 | 0.137 |
Spatial working memory | 0.57 (0.94) | 0.03 (0.67) | −0.28 (0.96) | 2.55 | 0.092 |
Learning | 0.47 (0.47) | −0.42 (0.71) | −0.46 (0.88) | 4.12 | 0.024 |
Executive functioning | 0.06 (0.24) | −0.09 (0.30) | −0.06 (0.25) | 0.78 | 0.465 |
Prospective memory | 0.62 (0.58) | −0.15 (0.94) | −0.26 (0.85) | 2.78 | 0.075 |
Global composite score | 0.40 (0.35) | −0.12 (0.35) | −0.19 (0.44) | 6.07 | 0.005 |
Abbreviations: IQ = intelligence quotient; CPZE = Chlorpromazine equivalence; PANSS = Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale; HDRS = Hamilton Depression Rating Scale.
Standardized raw scores.
4. Discussion
The main results of this study were that improved attention and age group (younger and older participants) were associated with better vocational outcome in terms of work attainment after a combined treatment of SE+CCT. These results, if replicated, could help to tailor vocational interventions in patients with SMI. The results support previous literature showing that cognitive functioning at follow-up is a stronger predictor of vocational outcomes than is baseline cognitive performance (McGurk and Mueser, 2006; McGurk et al., 2009). They suggested that improving attention may be an important target. It is known that attention is one of the most important cognitive skills in community and work functioning (Bowie et al., 2008; Green et al., 2000).
Taking into account that even small changes in cognition may have an impact on functioning when the right cognitive domain is improved (Wykes and Spaulding, 2011), the results suggested that strategies focused in learning to reduce distractions and maintain attention are important skills to teach during CCT. The results also showed that younger and older participants seemed to benefit more from the SE+CCT intervention compared with the middle-aged participants. Previous literature has shown greater improvements in cognition among young patients than in older patients receiving cognitive training alone (Bowie et al., 2014; Kontis et al., 2013) or in combination with SE (McGurk and Mueser, 2008). Also, work skills assessed by an observer-rated scale seem to improve more in patients who are within their first years of the illness (Bowie et al., 2014). Our results added that the positive effect of being young was also found in terms of work outcomes when CCT is provided with SE. People in their twenties and early thirties are in a critical developmental period in terms of completion of education and starting work and the onset of SMI during this period often interrupts the work trajectory. Thus, it is worthwhile to focus on improving work outcomes in this population. The results also showed that older participants did better than middle-aged participants in terms of work attainment after the treatment. This is important because older people with SMI have the highest rates of unemployment (Luciano and Meara, 2014) and may face specific age-related barriers when searching a job. Previous studies have shown that, although to a lesser extent, older people benefit from cognitive training (Bell et al., 2005a; Kontis et al., 2013; Twamley et al., 2011; Wykes et al., 2009) and can return to work when provided evidence-based SE (Twamley et al., 2012a). Bowie et al. (2014) found that real-world work skills did not improve in patients with more than 15 years of psychosis after CT. However, it has to be noted that the mean age in this study for the “long-term course” group was 45 years, which would correspond to “middle-age” adults in our study. It may be that compared to those in the middle-age group, younger individuals have an advantage in being hired, while older individuals may represent a “survivor” cohort of individuals with SMI who are highly motivated to work.
Our study has several limitations; most notably, the small sample size limited the size of the separate age groups. This limitation is relevant given that it is possible that some effects, such as the interactions between age and cognitive domains, were not detectable due to an insufficient statistical power. Also, our results may not generalize those individuals who receive SE without CCT. Due to the lack of control group, the specific treatment effects of SE and CCT remain unknown and future work should examine this relationship in the context of a control condition. Although most of the selected cognitive measures have low ceiling/floor and practice effects as shown by the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery studies (Nuechterlein et al., 2008), we cannot rule out the possibility that psychometric characteristics influenced the results, especially for measures not included in the MATRICS (e.g., prospective memory). It should also be noted that the results provide information about whether someone obtained work, but did not examine the duration of employment, number of hours worked, or the wages earned. Despite these limitations, our results suggest that improving attention may improve job attainment rates in the context of SE for individuals with SMI, and that middle-aged individuals with SMI may need additional support to obtain employment.
5. Conclusions
Improved attention and age-group (young and old) were associated with better work outcomes after SE+CCT. Improving attention may be an important target for improving work outcome in SMI. Middle-aged individuals may need additional support to return to work.
Acknowledgments
Source of Funding: This study was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH080150 to EWT). The authors also acknowledge Fundación Alicia Koplowitz (FAK) for support of Dr. Puig.
Footnotes
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
- Bell MD, Bryson GJ, Greig TC, Fiszdon JM, Wexler BE. Neurocognitive enhancement therapy with work therapy: Productivity outcomes at 6- and 12-month follow-ups. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2005a;42:829–838. doi: 10.1682/jrrd.2005.03.0061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell MD, Fiszdon JM, Greig TC, Bryson GJ. Can older people with schizophrenia benefit from work rehabilitation? J Nerv Ment Dis. 2005b;193:293–301. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000161688.47164.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Leung WW, Reichenberg A, McClure MM, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Harvey PD. Predicting schizophrenia patients’ real-world behavior with specific neuropsychological and functional capacity measures. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:505–511. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowie CR, Grossman M, Gupta M, Oyewumi LK, Harvey PD. Cognitive remediation in schizophrenia: efficacy and effectiveness in patients with early versus long-term course of illness. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2014;8:32–38. doi: 10.1111/eip.12029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catty J, Lissouba P, White S, Becker T, Drake RE, Fioritti A, Knapp M, Lauber C, Rössler W, Tomov T, van Busschbach J, Wiersma D, Burns T EQOLISE Group. Predictors of employment for people with severe mental illness: results of an international six-centre randomised controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192:224–231. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.041475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizoph Bull. 2000;26:119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton M. Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6:278–296. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1967.tb00530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong SK, Thompson LL, Iverson GL, Heaton RK, editors. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-64 Card Version. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Kontis D, Huddy V, Reedere C, Landau S, Wykes T. Effects of age and cognitive reserve on cognitive remediation therapy outcome in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;21:218–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luciano A, Meara E. Employment status of people with mental illness: national survey data from 2009 and 2010. Psychiatr Serv. 2014;65:1201–1209. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.201300335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendella PD, Burton CZ, Tasca GA, Roy P, St Louis L, Twamley EW. Compensatory cognitive training for people with first-episode schizophrenia: results from a pilot randomized controlled trial. Schizophr Res. 2015;162:108–111. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGurk SR, Mueser KT. Cognitive functioning, symptoms, and work in supported employment: A review and heuristic model. Schizophr Res. 2004;70:147–173. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGurk SR, Mueser KT. Cognitive and clinical predictors of work outcomes in clients with schizophrenia receiving supported employment services: 4-year follow-up. Adm Policy Ment Health. 2006;33:598–606. doi: 10.1007/s10488-006-0070-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGurk SR, Mueser KT. Response to cognitive rehabilitation in older vs. younger persons with severe mental illness. Am J Psychiatr Rehabil. 2008;11:90–105. [Google Scholar]
- McGurk SR, Mueser KT, DeRosa T, Wolfe R. Work, recovery, and comorbidity in schizophrenia: A randomized controlled trial of cognitive remediation. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:319–335. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD, Essock S, Fenton WS, Frese FJ, Gold JM, Goldberg T, Heaton RK, Keefe RSE, Kraemer H, Mesholam-Gately R, Seidman LJ, Stover E, Weinberger DR, Young AS, Zalcman S, Marder SR. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:203–213. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raskin S. Memory for intentions screening test. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(Suppl 1):110. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas KR, Puig O, Twamley EW. Age as a moderator of change following Compensatory Cognitive Training in severe mental illness. Psychiatr Rehabil J. doi: 10.1037/prj0000206. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsang HW, Leung AY, Chung RC, Bell M, Cheung WM. Review on vocational predictors: a systematic review of predictors of vocational outcomes among individuals with schizophrenia: an update since 1998. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2010;44:495–504. doi: 10.3109/00048671003785716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Twamley EW, Burton CZ, Vella L. Compensatory cognitive training for psychosis: who benefits? Who stays in treatment? Schizophr Bull. 2011;37:55–62. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Twamley EW, Vella L, Burton CZ, Becker DR, Bell MD, Jeste DV. The efficacy of supported employment for middle-aged and older people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012a;135:100–104. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.11.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Twamley EW, Vella L, Burton CZ, Heaton RK, Jeste DV. Compensatory cognitive training for psychosis: effects in a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry. 2012b;73:1212–1219. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m07686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson GS, editor. WRAT3: Wide Range Achievement Test Administration Manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range Inc; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Wykes T, Reeder C, Landau S, Matthiasson P, Haworth E, Hutchinson C. Does age matter? Effects of cognitive rehabilitation across the age span. Schizophr Res. 2009;113:252–258. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wykes T, Spaulding WD. Thinking about the future cognitive remediation therapy--what works and could we do better? Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(Suppl 2):80–90. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]