Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Nov 23.
Published in final edited form as: JAMA Psychiatry. 2015 Jul;72(7):697–705. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0575

Psychotic experiences in the general population: a cross-national analysis based on 31,261 respondents from 18 countries

John J McGrath 1,2,3,*, Sukanta Saha 1,2,3, Ali Al-Hamzawi 4, Jordi Alonso 5,6, Evelyn J Bromet 7, Ronny Bruffaerts 8, José Miguel Caldas-de-Almeida 9, Wai Tat Chiu 10, Peter de Jonge 11, John Fayyad 12, Silvia Florescu 13, Oye Gureje 14, Josep Maria Haro 15, Chiyi Hu 16, Viviane Kovess-Masfety 17, Jean Pierre Lepine 18, Carmen W Lim 19, Maria Elena Medina Mora 20, Fernando Navarro-Mateu 21, Susana Ochoa 22, Nancy Sampson 10, Kate Scott 19, Maria Carmen Viana 23, Ronald C Kessler 10
PMCID: PMC5120396  NIHMSID: NIHMS830489  PMID: 26018466

Abstract

IMPORTANCE

Community-based surveys find that many otherwise healthy individuals report histories of hallucinations and delusions. To date, most studies have focused on the overall lifetime prevalence of ever having any of these psychotic experiences (PEs), possibly masking important features related to types and frequencies of PEs.

OBJECTIVE

To explore detailed epidemiological information of PEs in a large cross-national sample.

DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS

Data came from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) Surveys, a coordinated set of community epidemiological surveys of the prevalence and correlates of mental disorders in representative household samples in countries throughout the world. 31 261 adult (aged 18 and older) WMH respondents across 18 countries were asked about lifetime and 12-month prevalence and frequency of six types of PEs (two hallucinatory experiences [HEs] and four delusional experiences [DEs]).

MAIN OUTCOMES

Prevalence, frequency, and correlates of PEs.

RESULTS

Mean lifetime prevalence (standard error) of ever having a lifetime PE was 5.8% (0.2), with hallucinatory experiences (5.2% [0.2]) much more common than delusional experiences (1.3% [0.1]), More than two-thirds (72.0%) of respondents with lifetime PEs reported experiencing only one type. PEs were typically infrequent, with 32.2% of respondents with lifetime PEs reporting only one occurrence and an additional 31.8% only 2–5 occurrences. There was a significant relationship between having more than one type of PE and having more frequent PE episodes. Lifetime prevalence estimates were significantly higher among respondents in middle and high income countries than low income countries, women than men, the non-married than the married, not employed and those with low family income.

CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE

The epidemiology of PEs is more nuanced than previously thought. Research is needed that focuses on similarities and differences in predictors of the onset, course, and consequences of distinct PEs.


There has been a growing interest in recent years in the epidemiology of hallucinations and delusions.1 These psychotic experiences (PEs) are reported by a sizeable minority of the general population. A recent meta-analysis based on 61 studies reported that the median lifetime prevalence of PE was 7.2%.2 Because this is substantially higher than the lifetime morbid risk (MR) of psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia (median MR 0.7%),3 the field of psychiatric epidemiology has been forced to rethink how PEs ‘fit’ into the epidemiologic landscape of psychotic disorders. The terminology to describe these experiences has also evolved over time. Sometimes referred to as psychotic-like experiences, we will use the general term psychotic experiences to encompass both hallucinatory experiences (HEs) and delusional experiences (DEs).2

Early work on the epidemiology of PEs focused on these experiences as risk indicators for later conversion to full psychosis. There is an appealing logic to this type of research, as many of the risk factors associated with PEs are also associated with schizophrenia/psychosis.4 More recently, evidence has accumulated that PEs are also associated with the subsequent onset of a wide array of common mental disorders including anxiety, mood, and substance use disorders,57 as well as with an increased risk of suicidal ideation and intent.810 Thus, there is a growing awareness that the presence of PEs may reflect a vulnerability to a wide range of adverse mental health outcomes (in addition to psychotic disorders).1116 These findings, and the concern that antipsychotic medications may be inappropriately used to treat individuals with isolated PEs, may have influenced the decision to exclude ‘Attenuated Psychosis Syndrome’ in recently revised diagnostic criteria.17

As the empirical data have accumulated, systematic reviews have pooled prevalence estimates and applied meta-regression techniques in order to explore the socio-demographic correlates of PEs.2,15,18,19 These reviews provide valuable clues to the nature of PEs, but also highlight important gaps in the literature. Four of these gaps are of special importance for the current study.

Firstly, the use of pooling in systematic reviews of PEs has encouraged the use of coarse dichotomous measures (e.g. lifetime prevalence present/absent) in order to harmonize the wide array of scales and diagnostic instruments used to assess PEs.2 This has reduced the subtlety of the associations examined in these reviews. Secondly, the studies included in the systematic reviews have varied in many key design elements. As noted by Linscott and van Os 2 substantial heterogeneity in the data has hampered analyses related to the relationship between PEs and socio-demographic variables. Thirdly, the vast majority of community studies of PE prevalence and correlates have been carried out in high income countries. A major exception is the World Health Survey (WHS), which included four brief PE questions in surveys of 52 nations.20 However, the WHS assessment of PEs had several limitations (e.g. it lacked information on frequency of PE occurrence; and questions about DEs were not asked in a fashion that excluded experiences related to alcohol, illicit drugs or sleep). Finally, in order to allow pooling of data from different studies, some reviews have collapsed different variables across orthogonal axes. For example, Kaymaz et al15 compiled composite variables related to ‘weak’ and ‘strong’ PEs, which in theory could be built from data related to: (1) the count of different types of PEs, (2) the frequency of occurrence, (3) associated distress, (4) comorbidity, and/or (5) ‘certainty’ (e.g. confidence in the psychotic nature of the experience).

Leading commentators have repeatedly called for more fine-grained analyses of PE in order to guide the field.1,21 The current report presents initial results of analyses designed to address the above limitations by examining data collected in the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) Surveys, a series of population-based surveys carried out in many countries using consistent instruments and field procedures designed to facilitate pooled cross-national analyses of the prevalence and correlates of mental disorders. These data provide an unprecedented opportunity to explore the epidemiologic landscape of PEs.

Methods

Participants

The WMH surveys are a coordinated set of community epidemiological surveys administered in probability samples of the household population in countries throughout the world (www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/WMH). Eighteen of the 26 WMH surveys completed up to now administered the CIDI Psychosis Module. These 18 countries are distributed across North and South America (Colombia, Mexico, Peru, Sao Paulo in Brazil, USA); Africa (Nigeria); the Middle East (Iraq, Lebanon); Asia (Shenzhen in the People’s Republic of China [PRC]); the South Pacific (New Zealand); and Europe (Belgium, France, Germany, Italy, the Netherlands, Portugal, Romania, Spain). All 18 surveys were based on multi-stage, clustered area probability household sampling designs (Table 1). The weighted average response rate across all 18 countries was 72.1%. Most surveys were based on nationally representative sample frames, but a few excluded rural areas (Colombia, Mexico), or focused on particular regions (Nigeria, Shenzhen), or cities (Sao Paulo). Participating sites were grouped into 3 country-level income strata according to World Bank criteria22 - ‘low and lower-middle income’ countries (Colombia, Iraq, Nigeria, PRC-Shenzhen, Peru), upper-middle income (Sao Paulo, Lebanon, Mexico, Romania), and high-income countries (the European countries, New Zealand, USA). The age ranges reported here include 18 years and over except in three countries (Mexico, Colombia, Peru) where 65 years was the upper age limit.

Table 1.

World Mental Health (WMH) sample characteristics by World Bank income categoriesa, and sample for psychotic experiences (PEs) after exclusion of 140 respondents who were screen-positive for possible schizophrenia/psychosisb

Country by income categorya Sample characteristicsc, d Field dates Age range Sample size
Response ratee
Part I PEs sample
I. Low and lower middle income countries
Colombia All urban areas of the country 2003 18–65 4,426 722 87.7
Iraq Nationally representative 2006–7 18–96 4,332 4,329 95.2
Nigeria 21 of the 36 states in the country 2002–3 18–100 6,752 1,417 79.3
PRCf – Shenzheng Shenzhen metropolitan area 2006–7 18–88 7,132 2,468 80.0
Peru Nationally representative 2004–5 18–65 3,930 530 90.2
TOTAL 26,572 9,466 84.7

II. Upper-middle income countries
Brazil - São Paulo São Paulo metropolitan area 2005–7 18–93 5,037 2,922 81.3
Lebanon Nationally representative 2002–3 18–94 2,857 1,029 70.0
Mexico All urban areas of the country 2001–2 18–65 5,782 715 76.6
Romania Nationally representative 2005–6 18–96 2,357 2,357 70.9
TOTAL 16,033 7,023 75.8

III. High-income countries
Belgium Nationally representative 2001–2 18–95 2,419 319 50.6
France Nationally representative 2001–2 18–97 2,894 301 45.9
Germany Nationally representative 2002–3 18–95 3,555 408 57.8
Italy Nationally representative 2001–2 18–100 4,712 617 71.3
Netherlands Nationally representative 2002–3 18–95 2,372 348 56.4
New Zealandg Nationally representative 2003–4 18–98 12,790 7,263 73.3
Portugal Nationally representative 2008–9 18–81 3,849 2,053 57.3
Spain Nationally representative 2001–2 18–98 5,473 1,159 78.6
United States Nationally representative 2002–3 18–99 9,282 2,304 70.9
TOTAL 47,346 14,772 65.5

IV. Total 89,951 31,261 72.1
a

Based on World Bank country level of economic development (The World Bank. (2008). Data and Statistics. Accessed May 12, 2009 at: http://go.worldbank.org/D7SN0B8YU0)

b

Respondents were excluded if they endorsed any PE but also (a) reported (1) schizophrenia/psychosis or (2) manic-depression/mania” in response to the question “What did the doctor say was causing (this/these) experiences?”; and (b) those who ever took any antipsychotic medications for these symptoms. This resulted in the exclusion of 140 respondents (0.4% of all respondents), leaving 31,261 respondents for this study.

c

NSMH (The Colombian National Study of Mental Health); IMHS (Iraq Mental Health Survey); NSMHW (The Nigerian Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing); EMSMP (La Encuesta Mundial de Salud Mental en el Peru); LEBANON (Lebanese Evaluation of the Burden of Ailments and Needs of the Nation); M-NCS (The Mexico National Comorbidity Survey); RMHS (Romania Mental Health Survey); ESEMeD (The European Study Of The Epidemiology Of Mental Disorders); NZMHS (New Zealand Mental Health Survey); NMHS (Portugal National Mental Health Survey); NCS-R (The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication).

d

Most WMH surveys are based on stratified multistage clustered area probability household samples in which samples of areas equivalent to counties or municipalities in the US were selected in the first stage followed by one or more subsequent stages of geographic sampling (e.g., towns within counties, blocks within towns, households within blocks) to arrive at a sample of households, in each of which a listing of household members was created and one or two people were selected from this listing to be interviewed. No substitution was allowed when the originally sampled household resident could not be interviewed. These household samples were selected from Census area data in all countries other than France (where telephone directories were used to select households) and the Netherlands (where postal registries were used to select households). Several WMH surveys (Belgium, Germany, and Italy) used municipal resident registries to select respondents without listing households. 13 of the 18 surveys are based on nationally representative household samples.

e

The response rate is calculated as the ratio of the number of households in which an interview was completed to the number of households originally sampled, excluding from the denominator households known not to be eligible either because of being vacant at the time of initial contact or because the residents were unable to speak the designated languages of the survey. The weighted average response rate is 72.1%.

f

People’s Republic of China

g

For the purposes of cross-national comparisons we limit the sample to those 18+.

In keeping with previous studies of PEs,9,11,2328 we made the a priori decision to exclude individuals who had PEs but who also screened positive for possible schizophrenia/psychosis, and manic-depression/mania (i.e. respondents who (a) reported (1) schizophrenia/psychosis or (2) manic-depression/mania” in response to the question “What did the doctor say was causing (this/these) experiences?”; and (b) those who ever took any antipsychotic medications for these symptoms). This resulted in the exclusion of 140 respondents (0.4% of all respondents), leaving 31,261 respondents for this study (see Table 1).

Measures and Assessments

All WMH surveys were conducted face-to-face in the homes of respondents by trained lay interviewers. Informed consent was obtained before beginning interviews in all countries. Procedures for obtaining informed consent and protecting individuals (ethical approval) were approved and monitored for compliance by the institutional review boards of the collaborating organisations in each country.29 Full details of these procedures are described elsewhere.30,31

All WMH interviews had two parts. Part I, administered to all respondents, contained assessments related to core mental disorders. Part II included additional information relevant to a wide range of survey aims, including assessment of PEs. All Part I respondents who met criteria for any Part I DSM IV mental disorder as well as a probability sample of other respondents were administered Part II. Part II respondents were weighted by the inverse of their probability of selection for Part II to adjust for differential sampling. Within the different sites, questions related to PEs were either administered to all respondents or a random sample of those administered Part II. Analyses in this article were based on the weighted Part II subsample of respondents administered the CIDI Psychosis Module. Additional weights were used to adjust for differential probabilities of selection within households, nonresponse, and to match the samples to population socio-demographic distributions.

The instrument used in the WMH surveys was the WHO Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI),32 a validated fully-structured diagnostic interview designed to assess the prevalence and correlates of a wide range of mental disorders according to the definitions and criteria of both the DSM-IV and ICD-10 diagnostic systems.

The CIDI Psychosis Module included questions about 6 PE types – 2 related to HEs (visual hallucinations, auditory hallucinations) and 4 related to DEs (two ‘bizarre’ delusional items - thought insertion/withdrawal, mind control/passivity; two ‘paranoid’ delusional items - ideas of reference, plot to harm/follow) (See Appendices A1 & A2). For example, respondents were asked if they ever experienced PEs (e.g. “Have you ever heard any voices that other people said did not exist?”). This was followed by a probe question to determine if the reported PEs ever occurred when the person was ‘not dreaming, not half-asleep, or not under the influence of alcohol or drugs’. Only responses of the latter type are considered here. The sequence of these follow-up probe types differed slightly between the first 6 WMH surveys, which were carried out in Europe (Belgium, France, Germany, Italy, the Netherlands, Spain), and the remaining 12 countries (See eTables 1 and 2).

Respondents who reported PEs were then asked about: (a) presence of the PEs in the past 12 months; and (b) frequency/occurrences of the PEs in their lifetime. In this paper we present prevalence estimates for any PE, any HE (with or without associated DEs), any DE (with or without associated HEs), ‘pure’ HE (without DEs) and ‘pure’ DE (without HEs). In addition, we will present two key PE-related metrics: (a) count of types of PEs (henceforth referred to as PE type metric); and (b) frequency of occurrence of PE episodes (henceforth referred to as PE frequency metric). Respondents may have had more than one hallucination and/or delusion type associated with a single episode of PEs. For the PE frequency metric, reported frequency of lifetime PE episodes was divided into 5 categories: only once, 2–5 times, 6–10 times, 11–100 times, and 101 and above. This five-category scheme was collapsed into 1–10 versus 11+ in analyses of socio-demographic correlates of PE frequency among respondents with lifetime PEs.

The socio-demographic factors considered here include: gender, age, number of years of education, employment history, marital status, family income, and nativity (i.e. born inside the country of assessment). For the bivariate and multivariate analyses, the socio-demographic variables were stratified into broad categories based on methodology described elsewhere.29

Statistical Analysis

Weighted prevalence estimates were calculated for the various PE types and related metrics. Odds ratios (ORs) and design-corrected 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are reported. Because the WMH survey data featured geographical clustering and weighting, standard errors (se) of parameter estimates were generated using the design-based Taylor series linearization method implemented in a SAS macro (SAS Institute inc. version 9.4). Multivariate significance was evaluated using Wald χ2 tests based on design-corrected coefficient variance–covariance matrices. The association between the PE type metric versus the PE frequency metric was evaluated using the Cochran-Armitage test. Statistical significance was evaluated consistently using two-tailed .05-level tests.

Results

Prevalence of PEs

Table 2 presents country-specific lifetime PE prevalence estimates. Lifetime prevalence (se) of at least 1 PE was reported by 5.8% (0.2) of the 31 261 respondents. Lifetime prevalence of any HE was 5.2% (0.2) and of any DE 1.3% (0.1). The median and interquartile range (IQR) of lifetime PEs, HEs and DEs were 5.5% (IQR: 2.8–7.5), 4.4% (IQR: 1.8–6.5), and 1.3% (IQR: 0.9–1.8), respectively (see eFigures 1–3 for cumulative distribution of PE, HE and DE estimates respectively). Twelve-month prevalence (se) of any PE was 2.0% (0.1), while the median (IQR) was 1.4% (1.0–2.8).

Table 2.

Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of psychotic experiences in the World Mental Health surveys

Lifetime Any PEb Lifetime Any HEc Lifetime Any DEd 12-month PE Total Sample

Income categorya Country n1e (%f, SE) n1e (%f, SE) n1e (%f, SE) n1e (%f, SE) Ng
Low and Lower-Middle Colombia 73 (7.5,1.2) 68 (7.1,1.2) 11 (0.9,0.3) 25 (2.1,0.5) 722
Iraq 51 (1.2,0.2) 46 (1.1,0.2) 13 (0.4,0.2) 25 (0.7,0.2) 4329
Nigeria 39 (2.2,0.5) 32 (1.7,0.4) 16 (1.0,0.4) 15 (1.0,0.4) 1417
PRC-Shenzhen 151 (5.3,0.8) 116 (3.8,0.6) 54 (1.8,0.4) 45 (1.4,0.3) 2468
Peru 36 (6.4,1.4) 33 (6.1,1.4) 7 (1.1,0.4) 18 (3.3,0.9) 530
Total Low and Lower-Middle 350 (3.2,0.3) 295 (2.6,0.2) 101 (0.9,0.1) 128 (1.2,0.2) 9466

Upper-Middle Brazil-Sao Paulo 548 (14.9,0.9) 471 (13.3,0.9) 183 (3.6,0.3) 230 (5.6,0.4) 2922
Lebanon 37 (1.9,0.4) 30 (1.6,0.4) 14 (0.6,0.3) 15 (0.9,0.4) 1029
Mexico 53 (4.1,1.0) 49 (3.6,0.9) 12 (0.8,0.4) 22 (1.4,0.4) 715
Romania 24 (1.0,0.4) 21 (0.9,0.4) 5 (0.1,0.1) 9 (0.3,0.1) 2357
Total Upper-Middle 662 (7.2,0.4) 571 (6.4,0.4) 214 (1.7,0.1) 276 (2.7,0.2) 7023

High Belgium 32 (8.3,2.5) 19 (5.0,1.6) 20 (5.7,2.3) 11 (4.1,2.4) 319
France 27 (5.7,1.4) 23 (4.9,1.3) 7 (1.6,0.6) 6 (1.3,0.7) 301
Germany 25 (2.8,0.5) 16 (1.8,0.4) 13 (1.3,0.3) 6 (1.0,0.2) 408
Italy 38 (4.5,0.8) 31 (3.5,1.0) 16 (1.9,0.6) 12 (1.3,0.5) 617
Netherlands 47 (10.8,2.5) 41 (10.1,2.5) 11 (1.6,0.5) 13 (3.0,1.2) 348
New Zealand 724 (6.9,0.4) 667 (6.5,0.4) 134 (0.9,0.1) 271 (2.4,0.2) 7263
Portugal 140 (5.2,0.7) 106 (3.9,0.5) 66 (2.6,0.5) 43 (1.7,0.3) 2053
Spain 91 (6.7,1.5) 77 (5.8,1.5) 35 (1.4,0.4) 19 (0.9,0.2) 1159
United States 249 (8.6,0.9) 232 (8.2,0.9) 41 (1.3,0.2) 79 (2.8,0.4) 2304
Total High 1373 (6.8,0.3) 1212 (6.2,0.3) 343 (1.4,0.1) 460 (2.2,0.2) 14772

All Countries 2385 (5.8,0.2) 2078 (5.2,0.2) 658 (1.3,0.1) 864 (2.0,0.1) 31261
a

Income = Based on World Bank country level of economic development (The World Bank. (2008). Data and Statistics. Accessed May 12, 2009 at: http://go.worldbank.org/D7SN0B8YU0);

b

PE = Psychotic experience (any of the six types);

c

HE = Hallucinatory experience (either of the two types);

d

DE = Delusional experience (any of the four types);

e

n1 = Unweighted number of respondents who reported the PEs;

f

Prevalence estimates are based on weighted data;

g

N = The total unweighted number of respondents who were asked about PEs.

Lifetime PEs prevalence (se) was significantly higher among women than men (6.6% [0.2] vs. 5.0% [0.3]; χ21=16.0, p< .001). Similar gender differences were found for prevalence of HEs (5.9% [0.2] vs. 4.3% [0.3]; χ21 =19.4, p< .001) but not DEs (1.4% [0.1] vs. 1.3% [0.1]; χ21=0.3, p=.61). The significant gender difference was also found for respondents with ‘pure’ HEs (5.2% [0.2] vs. 3.7% [0.3]; χ21=19.3, p < .001), but not ‘pure’ DEs (0.7% [0.1] vs. 0.7% [0.1]; χ21=0.1, p = .80).

Significant differences were found across the three country-level income strata in lifetime prevalence of any PE, any HE and any DE – in each comparison the prevalence estimates were significantly higher among respondents in middle and high income countries than low income countries (χ22 ranged from 7.1 to 58.2, each p < .001).

The prevalence of individual PEs and the distribution of the PE type metric

Table 3 shows the lifetime prevalence estimates of individual PE types and counts of different PE types. The most common PE type overall was visual hallucinations (3.8% [0.2]) followed by auditory hallucinations (2.5% [0.1]). Prevalence estimates of individual DE types were low (0.3– 0.7%). Among those with any lifetime PE, 72.0% (representing 4.2% of the total sample) reported only 1 PE type, 21.1% (representing 1.2% of the total sample) exactly 2 types, and 6.8% (representing 0.4% of the total sample) 3 or more types.

Table 3.

Lifetime prevalence of individual psychotic experiences in the World Mental Health surveys

Type Lifetime prevalence
na (%b, SE)
HE - visual 1545 (3.8,0.2)
HE - auditory (verbal) 1051 (2.5,0.1)
Any HEc 2078 (5.2,0.2)
DE - thought insertion/withdrawal 193 (0.4,0.0)
DE - mind control/passivity 148 (0.3,0.0)
DE - ideas of reference 209 (0.4,0.0)
DE - plot to harm/follow 328 (0.7,0.1)
Any DEd 658 (1.3,0.1)
Any PEe 2385 (5.8,0.2)
Exactly 1 PE type 1631 (4.2,0.2)
Exactly 2 PE types 544 (1.2,0.1)
3 or more PE types 210 (0.4,0.0)
Total Sample (Nf) 31261 (100,0.0)
a

n = Unweighted number of respondents who reported the PEs;

b

Prevalence estimates are based on weighted data;

c

HE = Hallucinatory experience (either of the two items);

d

DE = Delusional experience (any of the four items);

e

PE = Psychotic experience (any of the six items);

f

N = The total unweighted number of respondents who were asked about PEs.

The distribution of the PE frequency metric, and the relationship between PE type and PE frequency metrics

PEs were typically infrequent, with 32.2% of the respondents with lifetime PEs reporting only one solitary episode (Table 4). An additional 31.8% of respondents with lifetime PEs experienced only 2–5 PE episodes. Thus, for nearly two-thirds of respondents (64.0%) with lifetime PEs, these experiences occurred only 1–5 times in their lives. An additional 10.0% of respondents with lifetime PEs reported 6–10 lifetime episodes, 20.0% 11–100 episodes, and 6.0% 101 or more episodes. The relationship between the PE type metric and the PE frequency metric is best displayed in Table 5. Those with more PE types are disproportionately more likely to have more PE episodes (Cochran-Armitage z = −10.0, p <.001).

Table 4.

Frequency of lifetime occurrence of psychotic experiences (PEs) among respondents who reported ever having one or more PE in the World Mental Health surveys

Sample Type PE frequency metric
Row Sample Size 1 2–5 6–10 11–100 101 or more

na %b (SE) %b (SE) %b(SE) %b(SE) %b(SE)
All countries (except for ESEMed#) HE - visual 1379 31.9 (1.7) 32.0 (1.7) 10.5 (1.0) 18.7 (1.4) 6.7 (0.9)
HE - auditory (verbal) 965 20.8 (1.9) 31.7 (2.2) 11.1 (1.3) 26.5 (1.9) 9.9 (1.3)

Any HEc 1871 30.4 (1.4) 32.4 (1.4) 10.0 (0.8) 20.6 (1.3) 6.6 (0.8)

DE - thought insertion/withdrawal 162 28.9 (4.1) 27.1 (3.9) 9.7 (2.0) 15.6 (2.9) 18.8 (3.3)
DE - mind control/passivity 136 27.1 (5.0) 18.2 (3.5) 11.8 (2.5) 24.2 (4.5) 18.8 (4.6)
DE - ideas of reference 169 15.5 (2.6) 24.5 (4.5) 13.2 (2.8) 23.7 (4.0) 23.1 (5.2)
DE - plot to harm/follow 278 36.4 (3.5) 26.2 (2.8) 12.6 (2.2) 18.7 (2.4) 6.0 (1.8)

Any DEd 556 29.1 (2.5) 27.6 (2.1) 12.6 (1.5) 18.2 (1.8) 12.5 (2.0)

Any PEe 2125 31.7 (1.4) 31.9 (1.3) 10.1 (0.7) 20.0 (1.2) 6.4 (0.7)
Exactly 1 PE type 1452 37.5 (1.7) 32.3 (1.5) 8.9 (0.8) 18.0 (1.4) 3.3 (0.7)
Exactly 2 PE types 491 16.7 (3.3) 35.3 (3.6) 12.8 (2.5) 24.9 (2.5) 10.3 (1.9)
3 or more PE types 182 17.2 (2.8) 17.6 (3.5) 13.8 (3.4) 25.6 (3.9) 25.8 (4.3)

ESEMed# only Any PEe 260 36.4 (3.3) 30.7 (2.9) 9.5 (1.5) 20.3 (2.3) 3.2 (0.9)
Exactly 1 PE type 179 43.0 (3.9) 29.6 (3.7) 4.3 (1.4) 21.0 (2.1) 2.1 (0.9)
Exactly 2 PE types 53 23.6 (8.6) 33.7 (4.7) 32.3 (5.7) 6.8 (1.3) 3.6 (1.4)
3 or more PE types 28 2.1 (0.6) 34.0 (12.1) 2.5 (1.0) 48.2 (12.5) 13.2 (5.6)

All countries Any PEe 2385 32.2 (1.3) 31.8 (1.2) 10.0 (0.7) 20.0 (1.1) 6.0 (0.6)
Exactly 1 PE type 1631 38.1 (1.5) 32.0 (1.4) 8.4 (0.7) 18.3 (1.2) 3.2 (0.6)
Exactly 2 PE types 544 17.4 (3.1) 35.1 (3.3) 14.6 (2.3) 23.2 (2.2) 9.7 (1.8)
3 or more PE types 210 15.6 (2.5) 19.3 (3.3) 12.7 (3.1) 27.9 (3.9) 24.5 (3.9)
a

n = Unweighted number of respondents who reported the PEs;

b

Prevalence estimates are based on weighted data;

c

HE = Hallucinatory experience (either of the two types);

d

DE = Delusional experience (any of the four types);

e

PE = Psychotic experience (any of the six types);

#

ESEMeD = European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders.

Table 5.

Cross table of psychotic experiences (PE) frequency metric and PE type metric in the World Mental Health surveys

PE frequency metric
Row Sample Size 1 2–5 6 or more Cochran- Armitage test Chi-Square test

Sample Type na %b (SE) %b (SE) %b (SE) z (p-value) χ22 (p-value)
All countries Exactly 1 PE type 1631 38.1 (1.5) 32.0 (1.4) 29.9 (1.5) −10.0* (<.001) 32.1* (<.001)
2 or more PE types 754 16.9 (2.4) 31.2 (2.6) 51.8 (2.9)
a

n = Unweighted number of respondents who reported the PEs;

b

Prevalence estimates are based on weighted data;

*

Significant at 0.05 level, two-sided test (p value shown in table).

Associations between socio-demographic factors with lifetime PEs, HEs and DEs

eTable 3 shows the association of socio-demographic variables with lifetime PEs, HEs and DEs in bivariate and multivariate models. Several socio-demographic variables were associated with increased Odds Ratios for PEs, HEs and DEs in both models: (a) being a homemaker or classified as ‘other’ employment (looking for work, disabled etc.) (versus employed); (b) being non-married (never married or separated/widowed/divorced) (versus married), and (c) lower household income (versus high income). In addition to these findings, several socio-demographic variables were associated with only one type of PE. Young respondents (18–29 years) were significantly more likely to have DEs (compared to those over 60 years), while age was unrelated to HEs (and overall PEs). While female sex was associated with an increased prevalence of PEs (in both models), this finding was driven by an increased risk of HEs (but not DEs). Low education, in comparison, was associated with an increased risk of DEs, but not HEs. Unexpectedly, those born outside the country (i.e. migrants) were significantly less likely than the native born to report HEs (but not DEs) in both bivariate and multivariate models.

Associations of socio-demographic factors with PE type and PE frequency metrics

Concerning factors that influence the PE type metric (in those who had experienced PEs), in the multivariate model, the three younger age strata (i.e. that spanned 18 to 59 years) were significantly more likely to have more than one PE type (compared to those aged 60+ years) (eTable 4). None of the other socio-demographic characteristics was associated with the PE type metric. Concerning the correlates of the PE frequency metric, student status was significantly associated with lower frequency of PE occurrence. None of the other socio-demographic variables was associated with PE frequency (eTable 5).

Discussion

Based on cross-national samples from 18 countries, we found that 5.8% of respondents reported having one or more PEs at least once in their lifetime and 2.0% in the previous year. These overall estimates are broadly consistent with the previous literature.2 In addition, though, our study foregrounds important new information regarding the count of PE types and frequency of PEs that go beyond the issues considered in previous community-based studies of PEs.

Perhaps the most striking finding is that these experiences are infrequent for the majority of individuals who experience PEs, with 32.2% reporting only one PE episode in their life and 64% reporting no more than 5 lifetime occurrences.

In the general population, those with 2 or more types of PEs are also significantly more likely to have more PE episodes. For example, of those who reported 3 or more PE types, nearly a quarter (24.5%) reported more than 101 occurrences.

Our findings provide an empirical foundation upon which to investigate factors that influence the persistence of PEs.1 When viewed within the context of the gap between 12-month and lifetime PE estimates from the current study (i.e. 2.0% versus 5.8%), we can infer that most individuals do not have persistent PEs. Mindful that lifetime prevalence estimates for mental health disorders are often downward-biased due to under-reporting.33 Linscott and van Os2 estimated that of those who report any PEs, approximately 80% would have had transient experiences. This estimate is consistent with our empirical finding that about 64% of individuals with PEs report only 1–5 lifetime occurrences.

Based on the set of PEs examined, our study confirms that hallucinations were more common than delusions (5.2% versus 1.3%), and this general pattern was consistent across the 3 country-level income strata. We note that the lifetime prevalence of PEs was lower in the low-lower middle income countries (3.2%) compared to the upper-middle and high income countries (7.2%, 6.8% respectively). While we cannot directly compare our results with the one previous cross-national study of PEs 20 due to differences in how PEs were assessed, we note that both studies (optimized for consistent design and PE assessment) provided insights in variation between sites.

One of the strengths of cross-national studies such as the WMH Survey is that they are able to identify risk factors that exist consistently across countries despite site-specific cultural factors. We found an increased prevalence of both HEs and DEs associated with being unmarried, not employed, and having low household income. However, certain demographic features were differentially associated with HEs but not DEs, and vice versa. For example, women had a significantly higher prevalence of HEs but not DEs. We found a significant relationship between younger age and DEs, but not HEs. Unexpectedly, migrants in our study were significantly less likely to report lifetime HEs (compared to native born respondents). These novel findings provide important points of distinction between the epidemiology of psychotic disorders and PEs.4,34

It is of interest to note that while several socio-demographic variables were significantly associated with the lifetime prevalence of PEs, these features were not associated with the PE type or PE frequency metrics. We speculate that comorbid psychiatric illness (e.g. depression, anxiety disorders) and other risk factors known to be associated with PEs and mental disorders (e.g. family history, substance use, trauma exposure) may contribute to these PE-related metrics. The comprehensive nature of the WMH survey will allow us to explore these hypotheses in future analyses.

While the study has many strengths (e.g. large sample size, range of countries, uniform methodology for data collection, innovative analysis of PE-related metrics), there are several limitations. In keeping with other population-based surveys, we relied on trained lay interviewers to administer the questionnaire. While we excluded those who were screen-positive for possible psychotic disorders, we did not have access to valid measures of clinical psychotic disorders. Lifetime prevalence estimates are prone to under-reporting.33 We only assessed four types of DEs, and these probes may have been insensitive to culture-specific delusional beliefs.16

Conclusions

We have provided the most comprehensive description of the epidemiology of PEs published to date. While the lifetime prevalence of PEs is 5.8%, these are mostly rare events. For nearly a third of those who have PEs (i.e. 32.2%), these were solitary (i.e. one-off) events. In the general population there is a small subgroup of individuals who have multiple types of PEs and who experiences these PEs more frequently. The research community needs to leverage this fine-grained information in order to better determine how PEs reflect risk status. Our study highlights the subtle and variegated nature of the epidemiology of PEs, and provides a solid foundation upon which to explore the bi-directional relationship between PEs and mental health disorders.

Supplementary Material

Appendix Tables

Acknowledgments

The surveys discussed in this article were carried out in conjunction with the World Health Organization (WHO) World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative. We thank the WMH staff for assistance with instrumentation, fieldwork, and data analysis. These activities were supported by the US National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH: R01MH070884), the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Pfizer Foundation, the US Public Health Service (R13-MH066849, R01-MH069864, and R01 DA016558), the Fogarty International Center (FIRCA R01- TW006481), the Pan American Health Organization, Eli Lilly and Company, Ortho-McNeil Pharmaceutical Inc., GlaxoSmithKline, and Bristol-Myers Squibb. A complete list of WMH publications can be found at www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmh/.

Each WMH country obtained funding for its own survey. The São Paulo Megacity Mental Health Survey is supported by the State of São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) Thematic Project Grant 03/00 204-3. The Shenzhen, People’s Republic of China Mental Health Survey is supported by the Shenzhen Bureau of Health and the Shenzhen Bureau of Science, Technology and Information. The Colombian National Study of Mental Health (NSMH) is supported by the Ministry of Social Protection, with supplemental support from the Saldarriaga Concha Foundation. The ESEMeD project is funded by the European Commission (Contracts QLG5–1999–01042; SANCO 2004123), the Piedmont Region (Italy), Fondo de Investigación Sanitaria, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spain (FIS 00/0028), Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología, Spain (SAF 2000–158-CE), Departament de Salut, Generalitat de Catalunya, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III (CIBER CB06/02/0046, RETICS RD06/0011 REM-TAP), and other local agencies and by an unrestricted educational grant from GlaxoSmithKline. Implementation of the Iraq Mental Health Survey (IMHS) and data entry were carried out by the staff of the Iraqi MOH and MOP with direct support from the Iraqi IMHS team with funding from both the Japanese and European funds through United Nations Development Group Iraq Trust Fund (UNDG ITF). The Lebanese National Mental Health Survey (LEBANON) is supported by the Lebanese Ministry of Public Health, the WHO (Lebanon), anonymous private donations to IDRAAC, Lebanon, and unrestricted grants from Janssen Cilag, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Roche, and Novartis. The Mexican National Co-Morbidity Survey (MNCS) is supported by The National Institute of Psychiatry Ramon de la Fuente (INPRFMDIES 4280) and by the National Council on Science and Technology (CONACyT-G30544-H), with supplemental support from the Pan American Health Organization (PAHO), Te Rau Hinengaro: The New Zealand Mental Health Survey (NZMHS), is supported by the New Zealand Ministry of Health, Alcohol Advisory Council, and the Health Research Council. The Nigerian Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing (NSMHW) is supported by the WHO (Geneva), the WHO (Nigeria), and the Federal Ministry of Health, Abuja, Nigeria. The Peruvian World Mental Health Study was funded by the National Institute of Health of the Ministry of Health of Peru. The Portuguese Mental Health Study was carried out by the Department of Mental Health, Faculty of Medical Sciences, NOVA University of Lisbon, with the collaboration of the Portuguese Catholic University, and was funded by the Champalimaud Foundation, the Gulbenkian Foundation, the Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT) and the Ministry of Health. The Romania WMH study projects ‘Policies in Mental Health Area’ and ‘National Study regarding Mental Health and Services Use’ were carried out by the National School of Public Health and Health Services Management (former the National Institute for Research and Development in Health, present National School of Public Health Management and Professional Development, Bucharest), with technical support from Metro Media Transylvania, the National Institute of Statistics – National Center for Training in Statistics, Cheyenne Services SRL, Statistics Netherlands and were funded by the Ministry of Public Health (formerly the Ministry of Health) with supplemental support from Eli Lilly Romania SRL. The US National Co-Morbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) is supported by the US NIMH (U01- MH60220) with supplemental support from the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA), the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (RWJF; Grant 044708), and the John W. Alden Trust. JMcG was funded by the John Cade Fellowship from the National Health and Medical Research Council (APP1056929).

The views and opinions expressed in this report are those of the authors and should not be construed to represent the views or policies of any of the sponsoring organizations, agencies, or the WHO.

The funders had no input into the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis and interpretation of the data; or preparation, review or approval of the manuscript.

Footnotes

Potential Conflict of Interests

Over the past three years RK has been a consultant for Hoffman-La Roche Inc, Johnson & Johnson Wellness and Prevention, Sonofi-Aventis Group. RK has served on advisory boards for Mensante Corporation, Plus One Health Management, Lake Nona Institute and US Preventive Medicine. There are no other competing interests.

References

  • 1.van Os J. The Many Continua of Psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Linscott RJ, van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med. 2013;43(6):1133–1149. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Saha S, Chant D, Welham J, McGrath J. A Systematic Review of the Prevalence of Schizophrenia. PLoS Med. 2005;2(5):e141. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kelleher I, Cannon M. Psychotic-like experiences in the general population: characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychol Med. 2010:1–6. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Freeman D, Fowler D. Routes to psychotic symptoms: trauma, anxiety and psychosis-like experiences. Psychiatry Res. 2009;169(2):107–112. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Varghese D, Scott J, Welham J, et al. Psychotic-like experiences in major depression and anxiety disorders: a population-based survey in young adults. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(2):389–393. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Johns LC, Cannon M, Singleton N, et al. Prevalence and correlates of self-reported psychotic symptoms in the British population. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;185:298–305. doi: 10.1192/bjp.185.4.298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nishida A, Sasaki T, Nishimura Y, et al. Psychotic-like experiences are associated with suicidal feelings and deliberate self-harm behaviors in adolescents aged 12–15 years. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;121(4):301–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01439.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Saha S, Scott JG, Johnston AK, et al. The association between delusional-like experiences and suicidal thoughts and behaviour. Schizophr Res. 2011;132(2–3):197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kelleher I, Devlin N, Wigman JT, et al. Psychotic experiences in a mental health clinic sample: implications for suicidality, multimorbidity and functioning. Psychol Med. 2014;44(8):1615–1624. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Saha S, Scott JG, Varghese D, McGrath JJ. The association between general psychological distress and delusional-like experiences: a large population-based study. Schizophr Res. 2011;127(1–3):246–251. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Stochl J, Khandaker GM, Lewis G, et al. Mood, anxiety and psychotic phenomena measure a common psychopathological factor. Psychol Med. 2014:1–11. doi: 10.1017/S003329171400261X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Armando M, Nelson B, Yung AR, et al. Psychotic-like experiences and correlation with distress and depressive symptoms in a community sample of adolescents and young adults. Schizophr Res. 2010;119(1–3):258–265. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Werbeloff N, Drukker M, Dohrenwend BP, et al. Self-reported Attenuated Psychotic Symptoms as Forerunners of Severe Mental Disorders Later in Life. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012 doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kaymaz N, Drukker M, Lieb R, et al. Do subthreshold psychotic experiences predict clinical outcomes in unselected non-help-seeking population-based samples? A systematic review and meta-analysis, enriched with new results. Psychol Med. 2012:1–15. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.DeVylder JE, Burnette D, Yang LH. Co-occurrence of psychotic experiences and common mental health conditions across four racially and ethnically diverse population samples. Psychol Med. 2014;44(16):3503–3513. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Carpenter WT, van Os J. Should attenuated psychosis syndrome be a DSM-5 diagnosis? Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(5):460–463. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Linscott RJ, van Os J. Systematic reviews of categorical versus continuum models in psychosis: evidence for discontinuous subpopulations underlying a psychometric continuum. Implications for DSM-V, DSM-VI, and DSM-VII. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:391–419. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. 2008:1–17. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Nuevo R, Chatterji S, Verdes E, Naidoo N, Arango C, Ayuso-Mateos JL. The Continuum of Psychotic Symptoms in the General Population: A Cross-national Study. Schizophr Bull. 2010 doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.David AS. Why we need more debate on whether psychotic symptoms lie on a continuum with normality. Psychol Med. 2010:1–8. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.World Bank. World Development Indicators. Washington, D.C: World Bank; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Saha S, Scott J, Varghese D, McGrath J. The association between physical health and delusional-like experiences: a general population study. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(4):e18566. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Saha S, Scott J, Varghese D, McGrath J. Anxiety and depressive disorders are associated with delusional-like experiences: a replication study based on a National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing. BMJ Open. 2012;2(3) doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Saha S, Scott JG, Varghese D, Degenhardt L, Slade T, McGrath JJ. The association between delusional-like experiences, and tobacco, alcohol or cannabis use: a nationwide population-based survey. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;11:202. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Saha S, Scott JG, Varghese D, McGrath JJ. Socio-economic disadvantage and delusional-like experiences: A nationwide population-based study. Eur Psychiat. 2013;28(1):59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Scott J, Chant D, Andrews G, Martin G, McGrath J. Association between trauma exposure and delusional experiences in a large community-based sample. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190:339–343. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.026708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Varghese D, Saha S, Scott JD, Chan RCK, McGrath JJ. The Association Between Family History of Mental Disorder and Delusional-Like Experiences: A General Population Study. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 2011;156B(4):478–483. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kessler RC, Ustun TB. The WHO World Mental Health Surveys: Global Perspectives on the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kessler RC, Haro JM, Heeringa SG, Pennell BE, Ustun TB. The World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2006;15(3):161–166. doi: 10.1017/s1121189x00004395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kessler RC, Üstün TB. The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview. In: Kessler RC, Üstün TB, editors. The WHO World Mental Health Surveys: Global Perspectives on the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2008. pp. 58–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kessler RC, Ustun TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2004;13(2):93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, et al. How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychol Med. 2010;40(6):899–909. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kelleher I, Cannon M. Whither the psychosis-neurosis borderline. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(2):266–268. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix Tables

RESOURCES