Skip to main content
BMC Neurology logoLink to BMC Neurology
. 2016 Nov 25;16:241. doi: 10.1186/s12883-016-0749-2

Help seeking behavior and onset-to-alarm time in patients with acute stroke: sub-study of the preventive antibiotics in stroke study

E Zock 1,5,, H Kerkhoff 1, R P Kleyweg 1, T B V van Bavel-Ta 2, S Scott 3, N D Kruyt 4, P J Nederkoorn 5, D van de Beek 5
PMCID: PMC5123223  PMID: 27884126

Abstract

Background

Patients with acute stroke often do not seek immediate medical help, which is assumed to be driven by lack of knowledge of stroke symptoms. We explored the process of help seeking behavior in patients with acute stroke, evaluating knowledge about stroke symptoms, socio-demographic and clinical characteristics, and onset-to-alarm time (OAT).

Methods

In a sub-study of the Preventive Antibiotics in Stroke Study (PASS), 161 acute stroke patients were prospectively included in 3 Dutch hospitals. A semi-structured questionnaire was used to assess knowledge, recognition and interpretation of stroke symptoms. With in-depth interviews, response actions and reasons were explored. OAT was recorded and associations with socio-demographic, clinical parameters were assessed.

Results

Knowledge about stroke symptoms does not always result in correct recognition of own stroke symptoms, neither into correct interpretation of the situation and subsequent action. In our study population of 161 patients with acute stroke, median OAT was 30 min (interquartile range [IQR] 10–150 min). Recognition of one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss (p = 0.046) and adequate interpretation of the stroke situation (p = 0.003), stroke at daytime (p = 0.002), severe stroke (p = 0.003), calling the emergency telephone number (p = 0.004), and transport by ambulance (p = 0.040) were associated with shorter OAT.

Conclusion

Help seeking behavior after acute stroke is a complex process. A shorter OAT after stroke is associated with correct recognition of one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss, adequate interpretation of the stroke situation by the patient and stroke characteristics and logistics of stroke care, but not by knowledge of stroke symptoms.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s12883-016-0749-2) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Stroke, Illness Behavior, Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice, Time-to Treatment, Thrombolytic Therapy

Background

In patients with acute stroke, intravenous thrombolysis with recombinant tissue plasminogen activator and endovascular treatment are effective, but only within 6 h after stroke onset [1, 2]. Unfortunately, many patients with acute stroke do not seek immediate medical help [3, 4]. This knowledge has prompted several public campaigns to increase public awareness of stroke symptoms. However, these campaigns have limited or no sustained effect on the proportion of patients eligible for acute stroke treatments [58].

It has been presumed that in case of a stroke situation the patient or bystander has to recognize stroke symptoms, attribute these symptoms to acute stroke, but also has to recognize the importance to seek immediate help [9]. Other factors of importance in the process of help seeking are age, stroke severity and ethnicity [1012]. The interactions between these factors are unknown. We explored the process of help seeking behavior in patients with acute stroke, evaluating knowledge about stroke symptoms, socio-demographic and clinical characteristics, and onset-to-alarm time (OAT).

Methods

Patients

The current prospective observational study was performed as a sub-study of the Preventive Antibiotics in Stroke Study (PASS) [13]. In this multicenter, prospective, randomized, open-label, masked endpoint trial including 2550 patients the clinical benefit of ceftriaxone versus standard stroke care was investigated. Details of the protocol were described previously [14]. In short, eligible patients were aged 18 years or older, had a stroke since less than 24 h and a score of 1 or more on the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS). For the current sub-study we recruited consecutive patients from 3 participating centers from Nov-1–2011 to May-1–2014. Patients unable to comprehend interviews due to aphasia or language barrier were excluded.

Methods

Informed consent for this sub-study was obtained and patients were interviewed within 72 h after admission using a questionnaire with 23 closed and open-ended questions based on a pilot study (Additional file 1) [4]. Hospitals were selected on their geographical representation and ability to invest extra time for our questionnaire. Patients were selected on their physical state and the ability to understand and answer the questions of the questionnaire. Knowledge of stroke symptoms was evaluated twofold. Firstly, in open-ended questions patients had to name any stroke signs. Secondly, five symptoms were verbally presented and patients were asked to recall which of these could be signs of stroke. These five symptoms were one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss, any speech disturbance, vision loss with one or both eyes, dizziness and a severe, unusual headache. Knowledge of the three major stroke symptoms together, usually used in stroke campaigns, was also investigated: knowledge of one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss, face asymmetry and speech disturbance. In an open-ended question we explored knowledge about stroke treatment options. Patients were asked whether a therapy for stroke exists, and if yes, what kind of therapy. Exploration of help seeking behavior was subcategorized by recognition, interpretation and action. Recognition was defined as being aware of body cues and realizing that something was wrong and was determined by asking patients which symptoms had been experienced [9]. Interpretation was defined as interpreting symptoms in terms of a disease [9]. Action was defined as the first action taken within one hour after symptom onset (i.e., contacting family, the general practitioner, the emergency medical number or otherwise); if no action was undertaken, reasons were explored. Help seeking behavior was defined as the interaction between knowledge, recognition and interpretation leading to the presence or absence of any action.

Factors previously associated with patient delay (socio-demographic and clinical stroke characteristics, mode of transport) were obtained prospectively. OAT was defined as the time interval between the first moment patients or bystander(s) witnessed symptoms to first action of help seeking. Stroke severity was defined by the National Institute of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS)-score, categorized in 4 severities, because of low frequencies of the higher scores (defined as minor with a score 1 to 4, moderate 5 to 15, moderate to severe 16 to 20 and severe >20) [15]. Daytime was defined as hours between 6:01 am and 22:59 pm.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to describe the results of semi-structured and in-depth interviews. Non-parametric tests for dichotomous (Mann Whitney U) and categorical (Kruskal Wallis) variables were used to explore the associations between OAT and socio demographic, clinical characteristics, knowledge and the abovementioned elements of help seeking behavior. A p value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Baseline characteristics

Between November 1st, 2011, and May 1st, 2014, 161 patients with acute stroke were included. Not all patients were able to answer all questions. Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. Mean age was 72 years and 53% were men. The majority of patients had a cerebral infarction (90%) and stroke severity was relatively mild with a median NIHSS score of 4 (IQR 3–6). There were no differences in clinical characteristics between patients included in the 3 hospitals (data not shown).

Table 1.

Socio-demographic and clinical characteristics

Characteristic No./No. evaluated (%) Characteristic No./No. evaluated (%)
Male 85/161 (53) Time of stroke onset known
Mean age (yrs) 72  Yes 119/160 (74)
Median pre-stroke mRS (IQR) 0 (0–1)  No 16/160 (10)
Median NIHSS (IQR) 4 (3–6)  Wake up 25/160 (16)
 1–4 102/161 (63) Symptom onset at
 5–15 49/161 (30)  Work 7/161 (4)
 16–20 9/161 (6)  Home 126/161 (78)
 >20 1/161 (1)  Other 28/161 (17)
History Diagnosis at admission
 Stroke 53/161 (33)  Ischemic stroke 145/161 (90)
 Smoking 93/161 (58)  Haemorrhage 12/161 (8)
 Hypertension 81/161 (50)  TIA 4/161 (3)
 Diabetes 33/161 (21) Stroke localization
 Alcoholism 1/161 (1)  Left hemisphere 69/161 (43)
Living together 102/157 (65)  Right hemisphere 73/161 (45)
Bystander present 101/160 (63)  Posterior circulation 17/161 (11)

mRS modified Rankin Scale, IQR Inter Quartile Range, NIHHS National Institute of Health Stroke Scale, TIA Transient Ischemic Attack

Knowledge, recognition and interpretation

Most common knowledge about stroke symptoms was one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss (82 of 158 [52%]), speech disturbance (60 of 158 [38%]) and facial asymmetry (55 of 158 [35%]) (Table 2). Three or more symptoms were simultaneously mentioned by 53 of 159 patients (33%) and 36 of 159 (22%) did not know any stroke symptom. One hundred of 160 patients (62%) could recall three or more symptoms; 8 of 160 patients (5%) could not recall any symptom. Only 16 of 159 patients (10%) knew the three major stroke criteria, one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss, face asymmetry and speech disturbance. Knowledge about treatment options for was present in 77 of 161 patients (48%); suggested treatment options ranged from medication for influencing blood clotting in general sense (18 of 77 [23%]), rehabilitation (16 of 77 [21%]), acute intravenous medication (8 of 77 [10%]) to for example resting and drinking water (3 of 77 [4%]).

Table 2.

Knowledge

Knowledge of stroke symptoms (open-ended) No./no. evaluated (%) Knowledge of stroke symptoms (recall) No./no. evaluated (%)
One-sided weakness and/or sensory loss 82/158 (52%) One-sided weakness and/or sensory loss in arm, leg, or face 127/160 (79%)
Face asymmetry 55/158 (35%)
Any speech disturbance 60/158 (38%) Any speech disturbance 59/158 (37%)
Decreased consciousness 20/158 (13%)
Vision loss 19/158 (12%) Vision loss one or both eyes 63/160 (39%)
Headache 17/158 (11%) Severe, unusual headache 71/160 (44%)
Confusion 14/158 (9%)
Dizziness 10/158 (6%) Dizziness 77/160 (48%)
Nausea/vomiting 7/158 (4%)

Recognition of one’s own one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss and speech disturbance was present in 106 of 158 patients (66%) and 59 of 158 patients (37%) patients respectively. Thirty-three of 157 patients (21%) could not specify the stroke symptom(s) they had experienced (Additional file 2: Table S1) but managed to describe the impact on performance, such as ‘unable to drive a car’ or ‘loss of control’.

Correct interpretation of the stroke situation at symptom onset was made by 46 of 161 patients (29%). However, 66 of 161 patients (41%) had no clue which medical condition caused their symptoms. Another explanation than stroke was given in 49 of 161 patients (30%), such as stress, a heart problem, hypoglycemia, or eye infection.

Help seeking behavior

The elements of help seeking behavior are visualized in Fig. 1 in patients with and without knowledge about one-sided weakness or sensory loss as stroke warning sign. Fifty-seven of 79 patients (73%) with knowledge about this symptom recognized that something was wrong when they experienced these symptoms themselves. Twenty-eight of these patients (49%) interpreted this as a stroke. When interpreted as a stroke, 21 patients (75%) undertook action within the first hour after symptom onset. In the group of patients without knowledge about stroke symptoms (48%), who did not recognize this symptom (38%) and without a correct interpretation (86%), more than half (60%) undertook action within the first hour after symptom onset. The process for other symptoms as speech disturbance and facial asymmetry showed similar results (Additional file 3: Figure S1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Process of help seeking behavior

Action and onset-to-alarm time

Action was undertaken in 87 of 161 patients (54%) within the first hour after symptom onset (Fig. 2). The median OAT was 30 min (interquartile range [IQR] 10–150 min). Many of them contacted a relative or friend (28 of 87 [32%]; median OAT 25 min.) or the general practitioner (27 of 87 [31%]; median OAT 15 min.). The emergency medical service was called in 18 of 87 (21%; median OAT 5 min.) of these patients. Some patients (14 of 87 [16%]; median OAT 10 min.) described a combination of several actions, for example, finishing work, going home and consulting family. In some cases family undertook action after for example accidentally calling the patient and noticing that something was wrong. No direct action was undertaken by 72 of 161 patients (45%): many did nothing at all (48 of 72 [67%]; median OAT 2 h, 30 min.), others went to bed (15 of 72 [21%]; median OAT 5 h, 10 min.) or waited for evolvement of symptoms (9 of 72 [13%]; median OAT 2 h, 30 min.). Reasons for not seeking help were: no health problem present (30 of 72 [42%]) or pre-assumption that symptoms would disappear spontaneously (18 of 72 [25%]; data not shown).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

First action and OAT

Univariate analysis showed that more severe stroke (p = 0.003), daytime occurrence of symptoms (median time 30 vs. 270 min; p = 0.002), calling the emergency telephone number (p = 0.004), and transport by ambulance (median time 30 vs. 75 min; p = 0.040) were associated with shorter OAT (Table 3). Knowledge about the three major stroke criteria together was not associated with OAT (median time 30 vs. 30 min; p = 0.460). Correct recognition of one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss was associated with shorter OAT (median time 30 vs. 60 min; p = 0.046). Patients who interpreted their situation as a stroke had a shorter median OAT than those without correct interpretation (median time 15 vs. 45 min; p = 0.003).

Table 3.

Associations between socio-demographic, clinical characteristics, elements of help seeking behavior and OAT

Socio-demographic, clinical characteristics No./No. evaluated (%) Median OAT (min) p value
Gender Male 75/142 (52) 30 0.732
Female 67/142 (47) 30
Age in years <50 7/142 (5) 5 0.451
51–60 22/142 (16) 30
61–70 28/142 (20) 43
71–80 56/142 (39) 43
81–90 23/142 (16) 30
>91 6/142 (4) 60
Stroke in history Yes 46/142 (32) 60 0.397
No 96/142 (68) 30
Living together Yes 91/138 (66) 30 0.066
No 47/138 (34) 60
Diagnosis Ischemic stroke 126/142 (89) 30 0.475
Hemorrhage 12/142 (8) 23
TIA 4/142 (3) 135
Localization Left hemisphere 65/140 (46) 50 0.167
Right hemisphere 58/140 (42) 30
Posterior circulation 17/140 (12) 30
Aphasia Yes 24/132 (18) 43 0.948
No 108/132 (82) 30
NIHSS 1–4 90/142 (63) 50 0.003*
5–15 45/142 (32) 30
16–20 6/142 (4) 4
>20 1/142 (1) 15
Stroke at daytime Yes 24/142 (17) 30 0.002*
No 118/142 (83) 270
Bystander present Yes 91/140 (65) 30 0.096
No 49/140 (35) 60
Type of referral Emergency medical number 39/142 (28) 15 0.004*
General practitioner 83/142 (58) 50
Family 12/142 (9) 90
Other 4/142 (3) 23
Mode of transport Ambulance 103/140 (73) 30 0.044*
Own transport 33/140 (24) 75
Other 4/140 (3) 68
Elements of help seeking behavior
Knowledge of 3 stroke criteriaa
Recognition one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss
Yes 15/140 (11) 30 0.460
No 125/140 (89) 30
0.046*
Yes 94/139 (68) 30
No 45/139 (32) 60
Recognition face asymmetry Yes 17/139 (12) 30 0.654
No 122/139 (88) 33
Recognition speech disturbance Yes 51/139 (37) 30 0.276
No 88/139 (63) 40
Interpretation stroke situation Yes 40/142 (28) 15 0.003*
No 102/142 (72) 45

Associations calculated with Mann Whitney U test or Kruskal Wallis test were applicable

OAT onset-to-alarm time, NIHHS National Institute of Health Stroke Scale

*Statistically significant at p < 0.05

a3 stroke criteria: one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss, face asymmetry and speech disturbance

Discussion

In our prospective observational study, a shorter OAT after stroke was associated with correct recognition of one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss, correct interpretation of the stroke situation by the patient, stroke characteristics and logistics of stroke care. This is concordant with other studies [9, 16, 17]. In a qualitative study different aspects of interpretation influenced action or no action [9]. One of these aspects was the presence and influence of another person at stroke onset. Our study showed no relation between the presence and influence of another person and OAT. In the study of Faiz et al. stroke severity, transport by ambulance and lower age were significantly associated with earlier admission [17]. We did not find an association between age and OAT. In contrast, another study reported that older patients were more likely to call the emergency medical number [10]. The crucial role of ambulance services has been described [16]. Not only transport by ambulance, but also items as pre-notification to the receiving hospital and telemedicine-based interaction between the hospital and ambulance are suggested for further reduction in treatment time.

Knowledge about stroke symptoms was not associated with shorter OAT. This is in contrast with other studies [3, 1820]. In a quantitative study of 113 patients awareness of stroke symptoms and signs was associated with earlier hospital arrival [20]. However, awareness was investigated with a structured questionnaire. In our study knowledge was investigated open-ended, for patients could name any symptom they thought about. We showed that recalling stroke symptoms is much easier instead of open-ended questioning. The definition of knowledge about stroke symptoms depends on the research method. In another study with 150 stroke patients, different cognitive aspects were associated with delay [3]. Not only poor knowledge of stroke symptoms, but not realizing the importance of these symptoms resulted in delay. It seems contradictory that knowledge was not associated with OAT in our study, while a correct interpretation of the stroke situation was. However, the interpretation in the setting of an acute stroke situation is different from evaluating knowledge by reproducing single stroke symptoms in the office.

Our findings show that help seeking behavior after stroke is not a fixed process starting with having knowledge about stroke symptoms in general, followed by correct recognition of one’s own stroke symptoms, interpretation as a stroke and finally action. Nearly half of our patients with a stroke undertook no action within the first hour after symptom onset, despite having knowledge or recognizing symptoms. On the other hand, many patients still undertook action without knowledge, incorrect recognition and incorrect interpretation (shown in Fig. 1). A qualitative study proposed a model of help seeking behavior at the time of stroke [9]. Patients seemed to follow fixed steps from knowledge of stroke symptoms towards action in the process of help seeking. Our study does not support this hypothesis. Additional factors, for instance fear, ignorance, ideas about seriousness or access to medical services, may play a role in the decision to seek help [21]. Many studies focused on some isolated factors responsible for patient delay. Demographic factors [22, 23], logistic factors [16, 17] and social or psychological factors [2427] were studied. All these elements are of influence and may interact with each other, but a complete insight in the process of help seeking behavior has not been found yet. We believe a more complete insight is needed for more successful stroke campaigns. Differentiating the focus and strategies of these campaigns could reach and educate more future stroke patients. Reaching different age groups or people with different education and interest in information should probably be addressed to in different ways. Changing behavior of people with fear for hospitals or denial to be ill are other important elements to be taken into account.

Our study had several limitations. Firstly, our study was performed within a randomized clinical study [13]. The inclusion criteria used in this study led to selection bias. Patients had to be included within 24 h after symptom onset. This will lead to underrepresentation of patients with very long delays in help seeking behavior. Some patients are waiting for days and sometimes even for weeks before consulting a doctor [4, 17]. Secondly, included patients were interviewed after stroke onset, which may have caused a bias towards a better knowledge of stroke symptoms. Recall bias may cause overestimation of stroke symptoms knowledge. Studies have been performed in control populations to assess knowledge of stroke [28, 29]. These studies give insight in knowledge, but no information about the process leading to help seeking in a real stroke situation, in which social and emotional factors may play a decisive role [11, 22, 30, 31]. Patients with aphasia or a language barrier were not included, possibly leading to selection bias. Due to investment of extra time for completing the questionnaire and other logistical factors, not all patients eligible for this sub-study could be interviewed. However, we believe that our results are still generalizable to the overall stroke cohort, because the patients were randomly included. Finally, we investigated help seeking action within the first hour after stroke onset. Over the past 5 years, much effort has been made in shortening the onset to treatment time [16, 32, 33].

Conclusion

A shorter OAT after stroke is associated with correct recognition of one-sided weakness and/or sensory loss, correct interpretation of the stroke situation by the patient, stroke characteristics and logistics of stroke care, but not by knowledge of stroke symptoms. Our data support the assumption that help seeking behavior after stroke is not a fixed process, starting with having knowledge and followed by correct recognition and interpretation. This process is complex and seems influenced by many factors. Future research must focus on the type of behavior and its motivation when experiencing a stroke. This is crucial to design appropriate stroke campaigns for changing patient behavior after stroke and thereby reducing patient delay optimally.

Acknowledgements

We thank all our colleagues of the Neurology department for their inclusions in the PASS study and completing the questionnaire of this sub-study.

Funding

None.

Availability of data and materials

PASS data, Data of all patients are available on paper CRF in AMC, as well as in a SPSS database.

Sub-study data, Data of all patients are available on paper CRF in ASz, as well as in a SPSS database. Same patient identification numbers are used as in the PASS study.

The datasets during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. In both centers, data will be stored for at least 15 years.

Authors’ contributions

Literature search: EZ, HK, RK, VB. Study design, PASS study: PN, DB. Study design, sub-study: EZ, HK, RK, VB. Data collection: all. Data analysis: EZ, HK, RK, DB, PJ, NK. Data interpretation: EZ, HK, RK, DB, PJ, NK. Writing: all. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics committee (PASS): Research Ethics Committee, Academic Medical Center Amsterdam, Meibergdreef 9, 1100 DD Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

Reference number: MEC AMC, 018.

Local ethics committee: Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek Advies Commissie, Albert Schweitzer hospital, Albert Schweitzerplaats 25, 3318 AT Dordrecht, The Netherlands.

Reference number: NA.

Information about the PASS study and this sub-study was described in two separate patient information letters. All patients gave their informed consent for the PASS study and separately for this sub-study. The author of this manuscript owns the information letter and informed consent form of all patients of this sub-study.

Abbrevations

IQR

Interquartile range

NIHHS

National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale

OAT

Onset-to-alarm time

PASS

Preventive Antibiotics in Stroke Study

Additional files

Additional file 1: (71.2KB, pdf)

Questionnaire. (PDF 71 kb)

Additional file 2: Table S1. (83.3KB, pdf)

Recognition. (PDF 83 kb)

Additional file 3: Figure S1. (424.6KB, pdf)

Face asymmetry and speech disturbance. (PDF 424 kb)

Contributor Information

E. Zock, Phone: +31786523452, Email: e.zock@asz.nl

H. Kerkhoff, Email: h.kerkhoff@asz.nl

R. P. Kleyweg, Email: r.p.kleyweg@asz.nl

T. B. V. van Bavel-Ta, Email: v.v.bavel@elisabeth.nl

S. Scott, Email: saskia.scott@slz.nl

N. D. Kruyt, Email: n.d.kruyt@lumc.nl

P. J. Nederkoorn, Email: p.j.nederkoorn@amc.uva.nl

D. van de Beek, Email: d.vandebeek@amc.uva.nl

References

  • 1.The National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke rt-PA Stroke Study Group Tissue plasminogen activator for acute ischemic stroke. N Engl J Med. 1995;333:1581–7. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199512143332401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Goyal M, Menon BK, van Zwam WH, Dippel DWJ, Mitchell PJ, Demchuk AM, et al. Endovascular thrombectomy after large-vessel ischaemic stroke: a meta-analysis of individual patient data from five randomised trials. Lancet. 2016;387:1723–31. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)00163-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barr J, McKinley S, O rsquo Brien E, Herkes G. Patient recognition of and response to symptoms of tia or stroke. Neuroepidemiology. 2006;26:168–75. doi: 10.1159/000091659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Zock E, Kerkhoff H, Kleyweg RP. [Initial reactions of patients after a stroke: more than half undertake no action] Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd. 2012;156:A4336. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lecouturier J, Rodgers H, Murtagh MJ, White M, Ford GA, Thomson RG. Systematic review of mass media interventions designed to improve public recognition of stroke symptoms, emergency response and early treatment. BMC Public Health. 2010;10:784. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fussman C, Rafferty AP, Lyon-Callo S, Morgenstern LB, Reeves MJ. Lack of association between stroke symptom knowledge and intent to call 911: a population-based survey. Stroke. 2010;41:1501–7. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.110.578195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mikulik R, Goldemund D, Reif M, Brichta J, Neumann J, Jarkovsky J, et al. Calling 911 in response to stroke: no change following a four-year educational campaign. Cerebrovasc Dis. 2011;32:342–8. doi: 10.1159/000330339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mellon L, Hickey A, Doyle F, Dolan E, Williams D. Can a media campaign change health service use in a population with stroke symptoms? Examination of the first Irish stroke awareness campaign. Emerg Med J. 2013;31:536–40. doi: 10.1136/emermed-2012-202280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Moloczij N, McPherson KM, Smith JF, Kayes NM. Help-seeking at the time of stroke: stroke survivors’ perspectives on their decisions. Health Soc Care Community. 2008;16:501–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2524.2008.00771.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kleindorfer DO, Lindsell CJ, Broderick JP, Flaherty ML, Woo D, Ewing I, et al. Community socioeconomic status and prehospital times in acute stroke and transient ischemic attack: do poorer patients have longer delays from 911 call to the emergency department? Stroke. 2006;37:1508–13. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000222933.94460.dd. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mosley I, Nicol M, Donnan G, Patrick I, Dewey H. Stroke symptoms and the decision to call for an ambulance. Stroke. 2007;38:361–6. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000254528.17405.cc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Addo J, Ayis S, Leon J, Rudd AG, McKevitt C, Wolfe CDA. Delay in presentation after an acute stroke in a multiethnic population in South london: the South london stroke register. J Am Heart Assoc. 2012;1:e001685. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.112.001685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Westendorp WF, Vermeij J-D, Zock E, Hooijenga IJ, Kruyt ND, Bosboom HJLW, et al. The Preventive Antibiotics in Stroke Study (PASS): a pragmatic randomised open-label masked endpoint clinical trial. Lancet. 2015;385:1519–26. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)62456-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Nederkoorn PJ, Westendorp WF, Hooijenga IJ, de Haan RJ, Dippel DWJ, Vermeij FH, et al. Preventive antibiotics in stroke study: rationale and protocol for a randomised trial. Int J Stroke. 2010;6:159–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-4949.2010.00555.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Adams RJ. Management issues for patients with ischemic stroke. Am Acad Neurol. 1995;45:S15–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fassbender K, Balucani C, Walter S, Levine SR, Haass A, Grotta J. Streamlining of prehospital stroke management: the golden hour. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:585–96. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70100-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Faiz KW, Sundseth A, Thommessen B, Rønning OM. Prehospital delay in acute stroke and TIA. Emerg Med J. 2013;30:669–74. doi: 10.1136/emermed-2012-201543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Saver JL, Fonarow GC, Smith EE, Reeves MJ, Grau-Sepulveda MV, Pan W, et al. Time to treatment with intravenous tissue plasminogen activator and outcome from acute ischemic stroke. JAMA. 2013;309:2480–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.6959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Iguchi Y, Wada K, Shibazaki K, Inoue T, Ueno Y, Yamashita S, et al. First impression at stroke onset plays an important role in early hospital arrival. Intern Med. 2006;45:447–51. doi: 10.2169/internalmedicine.45.1554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Koksal EK, Gazioglu S, Boz C, Can G, Alioglu Z. Factors associated with early hospital arrival in acute ischemic stroke patients. Neurol Sci. 2014;35(10):1567–72. doi: 10.1007/s10072-014-1796-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mackintosh JE, Murtagh MJ, Rodgers H, Thomson RG, Ford GA, White M. Why people do, or do not, immediately contact emergency medical services following the onset of acute stroke: qualitative interview study. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e46124. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hickey A, O'Hanlon A, McGee H, Donnellan C, Shelley E, Horgan F, et al. Stroke awareness in the general population: knowledge of stroke risk factors and warning signs in older adults. BMC Geriatr. 2009;9:35. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-9-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Goldberg RJ, Steg PG, Sadiq I, Granger CB, Jackson EA, Budaj A, et al. Extent of, and factors associated with, delay to hospital presentation in patients with acute coronary disease (the GRACE registry) Am J Cardiol. 2002;89:791–6. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9149(02)02186-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Moser DK, Kimble LP, Alberts MJ, Alonzo A, Croft JB, Dracup K, et al. Reducing delay in seeking treatment by patients with acute coronary syndrome and stroke: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association Council on cardiovascular nursing and stroke council.[Reprint in J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2007 Jul-Aug;22(4):326–43; PMID: 17589286] Circulation. 2006;114:168–82. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.106.176040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mandelzweig L, Goldbourt U, Boyko V, Tanne D. Perceptual, social, and behavioral factors associated with delays in seeking medical care in patients with symptoms of acute stroke. Stroke. 2006;37:1248–53. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000217200.61167.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dombrowski SU, Mackintosh JE, Sniehotta FF, Araujo-Soares V, Rodgers H, Thomson RG, et al. The impact of the UK “Act FAST” stroke awareness campaign: content analysis of patients, witnessand primary care clinicians’ perceptions. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1–1. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Skolarus LE, Murphy JB, Zimmerman MA, Bailey S, Fowlkes S, Brown DL, et al. Individual and community determinants of calling 911 for stroke among African Americans in an urban community. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2013;6:278–83. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.111.000017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Mata J, Frank R, Gigerenzer G. Symptom recognition of heart attack and stroke in nine European countries: a representative survey. Health Expect. 2014;17:376–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-7625.2011.00764.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Yang J, Zheng M, Cheng S, Ou S, Zhang J, Wang N, et al. Knowledge of stroke symptoms and treatment among community residents in Western Urban China. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis. 2014;23:1216–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2013.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Kurz MW, Kurz KD, Farbu E. Acute ischemic stroke--from symptom recognition to thrombolysis. Acta Neurol Scand. 2013;127(Suppl. 196):57–64. doi: 10.1111/ane.12051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hong KS, Bang OY, Kim JS, Heo JH, Yu KH, Bae HJ, et al. Stroke statistics in Korea: part II stroke awareness and acute stroke care, a report from the Korean stroke society and clinical research center for stroke. J Stroke. 2013;15:67–77. doi: 10.5853/jos.2013.15.2.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Weber MA, Schiffrin EL, White WB, Mann S, Lindholm LH, Kenerson JG, et al. Clinical practice guidelines for the management of hypertension in the community. J Clin Hypertens. 2013;16:14–26. doi: 10.1111/jch.12237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ford AL, Williams JA, Spencer M, McCammon C, Khoury N, Sampson TR, et al. Reducing door-to-needle times using toyota's lean manufacturing principles and value stream analysis. Stroke. 2012;43:3395–8. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.670687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

PASS data, Data of all patients are available on paper CRF in AMC, as well as in a SPSS database.

Sub-study data, Data of all patients are available on paper CRF in ASz, as well as in a SPSS database. Same patient identification numbers are used as in the PASS study.

The datasets during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. In both centers, data will be stored for at least 15 years.


Articles from BMC Neurology are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES