Abstract
Cognitive difficulties manifested by the growing elderly population with cirrhosis could be amnestic (memory-related) or non-amnestic (memory-unrelated). The underlying neuro-biological and gut-brain changes are unclear in this population. We aimed to define gut-brain axis alterations in elderly cirrhotics compared to non-cirrhotic individuals based on presence of cirrhosis and on neuropsychological performance. Age-matched outpatients with/without cirrhosis underwent cognitive testing (amnestic/non-amnestic domains), quality of life (HRQOL), multi-modal MRI (fMRI go/no-go task, volumetry and MR spectroscopy), blood (inflammatory cytokines) and stool collection (for microbiota). Groups were studied based on cirrhosis/not and also based on neuropsychological performance (amnestic-type, amnestic/non-amnestic-type and unimpaired). Cirrhotics were impaired on non-amnestic and selected amnestic tests, HRQOL and systemic inflammation compared to non-cirrhotics. Cirrhotics demonstrated significant changes on MR spectroscopy but not on fMRI or volumetry. Correlation networks showed that Lactobacillales members were positively while Enterobacteriaceae and Porphyromonadaceae were negatively linked with cognition. Using the neuropsychological classification amnestic/non-amnestic-type individuals were majority cirrhosis and had worse HRQOL, higher inflammation and decreased autochthonous taxa relative abundance compared to the rest. This classification also predicted fMRI, MR spectroscopy and volumetry changes between groups. We conclude that gut-brain axis alterations may be associated with the type of neurobehavioral decline or inflamm-aging in elderly cirrhotic subjects.
The rapidly aging population, with the accompanying neuro-cognitive sequelae such as dementia, is a major medical and psychosocial burden1. This maturing of the population has also affected patients with cirrhosis, who, in addition to the cognitive issues related to aging, are also prone to hepatic encephalopathy (HE)2. Elderly subjects can have issues with amnestic (memory-related) and non-amnestic (unrelated to memory) cognitive dysfunction, which may be reflected in geriatric cirrhotic patients as well. The subclinical phase, known as covert HE (CHE) usually affects non-amnestic domains such as attention, visuo-motor coordination and executive function3. This has been shown in prior studies using mixed tests of memory and attention in which younger cirrhotics are more likely to be impaired on attention and visuo-spatial domains rather than amnestic domains such as delayed memory4. Further characterization of the interaction between dementia (mostly associated with amnestic issues) and HE in elderly cirrhotics is critical because an increasing number of these patients are being evaluated for liver transplant5. Patients with HE, unlike those with dementia, can expect a reasonable improvement in cognition after transplant and are considered appropriate for listing6. The initial differentiation between these groups of patients is often performed at the level of neuro-psychological evaluation but the utility of this in elderly cirrhotics is unclear.
There is also emerging evidence regarding the impaired gut-brain axis in the setting of systemic inflammation in younger cirrhotics and in non-cirrhotic patients with amnestic impairment7,8,9,10,11. The concept of “inflamm-aging” has been studied in elderly patients with a strong focus on an altered gut-brain axis12. However these inflammatory and gut microbial changes need to be studied in the context of elderly cirrhotic patients.
We hypothesized that elderly cirrhotic patients with concomitant amnestic and non-amnestic cognitive impairment, will have poor HRQOL, higher systemic inflammation, and decreased relative abundance of beneficial gut microbiota, associated with changes in multi-modal brain MRI compared to unimpaired subjects.
Results
93 potential patients were referred by the geriatric and gastroenterology clinics for possible inclusion from January 2015 to March 2016. Eight were not able to complete the MMSE and had diagnosed dementia. Six patients had co-morbid conditions that precluded participation, and three were on psycho-active medications. Ultimately 76 patients were included, 39 of whom were cirrhotic and 37 were non-cirrhotic. Based on the cognitive algorithm the neuropsychologist divided patients into unimpaired (n = 23), amnestic-type (n = 25), and amnestic/non-amnestic type (n = 28). The kappa of this classification between the two psychologists was 0.89. While the neuropsychologist classified one patient as amnestic/non-amnestic type, the psychologist considered him amnestic type, while another patient classified as amnestic type was considered unimpaired.
Cognitive performance and HRQOL
Cirrhotic and non-cirrhotic patients had similar demographics and MMSE (Table 1). Cirrhotics demonstrated worse overall RBANS performance, but not delayed memory or language subtests. Cirrhotics performed worse on PHES and EncephalApp compared to non-cirrhotics. Portions of the other amnestic tests (HVLT total recall and Similarities test) were also impaired in cirrhotic patients, who also had a worse HRQOL on both SIP and PROMIS (Table 1).
Table 1. Clinical Comparison between Cirrhotic and Non-Cirrhotic Subjects.
No Cirrhosis (n = 37) | Cirrhosis (n = 39) | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 73.9 ± 5.9 | 72.9 ± 5.4 | 0.43 |
Gender (% men) | 68% | 77% | 0.36 |
Education (years) | 15.2 ± 3.6 | 14.9 ± 2.7 | 0.11 |
Diabetes (%) | 12 (32%) | 16 (41%) | 0.58 |
Alcohol abuse history (%) | 6 (16%) | 13 (33%) | 0.09 |
Hypertension (%) | 15 (41%) | 12 (31%) | 0.33 |
Depression on SSRI (%) | 9 (24%) | 7 (18%) | 0.50 |
Hyperlipidemia (%) | 16 (43%) | 12 (31%) | 0.26 |
Mini-mental status | 28.8 ± 1.4 | 28.4 ± 1.4 | 0.22 |
HVLT | |||
HVLT Total recall | 22.7 ± 5.6 | 19.1 ± 5.8 | 0.04 |
HVLT delayed recall | 7.1 ± 2.9 | 6.9 ± 3.1 | 0.79 |
HVLT retention % | 74.1 ± 22.5 | 82.7 ± 33.3 | 0.21 |
Similarities test | 27.7 ± 4.6 | 24.3 ± 6.3 | 0.01 |
RBANS | |||
Total score | 103.4 ± 18.7 | 91.3 ± 15.5 | 0.004 |
Visuospatial | 109.7 ± 17.5 | 94.4 ± 19.8 | 0.001 |
Immediate Memory | 101.7 ± 16.6 | 90.6 ± 17.6 | 0.04 |
Language | 98.2 ± 13.5 | 95.3 ± 8.5 | 0.29 |
Attention | 106.3 ± 17.7 | 96.1 ± 17.4 | 0.02 |
Delayed Memory | 98.0 ± 18.8 | 92.1 ± 16.9 | 0.09 |
EncephalApp Stroop | |||
Stroop Off Time (sec) | 86.1 ± 27.7 | 100.5 ± 28.4 | 0.05 |
On Time (sec) | 105.6 ± 44.3 | 127.8 ± 43.6 | 0.06 |
Median runs On | 5.0 | 6.0 | 0.02 |
Median runs Off | 6.0 | 6.0 | 0.47 |
OffTime + OnTime (sec) | 182.6 ± 68.7 | 214.5 ± 66.5 | 0.05 |
PHES | |||
Number connection-A (sec) | 45.0 ± 29.4 | 52.7 ± 28.3 | 0.25 |
Number connection-B (sec) | 114.8 ± 81.9 | 145.0 ± 102.0 | 0.15 |
Digit Symbol (raw score) | 54.3 ± 19.1 | 45.2 ± 17.9 | 0.04 |
Line Tracing test (sec) | 106.4 ± 59.1 | 114.3 ± 59.5 | 0.56 |
Line Tracing errors | 30.9 ± 31.9 | 41.4 ± 31.4 | 0.16 |
Serial dotting (sec) | 57.2 ± 21.2 | 82.9 ± 38.1 | <0.001 |
Median PHES | −1 | −7 | 0.01 |
HRQOL assessments | |||
SIP | |||
Total score | 3.1 ± 5.8 | 10.8 ± 11.4 | <0.0001 |
Psychosocial domain | 2.6 ± 4.6 | 9.7 ± 12.8 | 0.001 |
Physical domain | 2.6 ± 5.5 | 10.4 ± 12.0 | <0.0001 |
Median Age-adjusted PROMIS scores | |||
Anger | 39.5 | 58.0 | 0.02 |
Anxiety | 66.0 | 63.0 | 0.63 |
Depression | 45.5 | 49.0 | 0.25 |
Fatigue | 35.0 | 54.0 | 0.05 |
Physical function* | 79.0 | 48.0 | 0.004 |
Social activity* | 76.5 | 47.0 | 0.003 |
Social Role* | 69.0 | 32.0 | <0.0001 |
Sleep disturbance | 44.5 | 70.0 | 0.02 |
Wake disturbances | 29.5 | 58.0 | 0.04 |
P value using ANOVA or Chi-square test as appropriate, *a low score on these PROMIS variables indicates worse function, while a low score on the others indicates good function. PHES: psychometric hepatic encephalopathy score, PROMIS: patient-reported outcome measurement information system, SIP: Sickness Impact Profile, HRQOL: Health-related quality of life, RBANS: repeatable battery for assessment of neuropsychological status, HVLT: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test.
The groups divided according to neuropsychological performance had corresponding cognitive impairments as expected without differences in demographics (Table 2). The amnestic/non-amnestic group had the highest proportion of cirrhotic patients and had the highest HRQOL impairment. In the cirrhosis group, there were no statistically significant correlations between MELD score and cognitive performance or HRQOL.
Table 2. Demographic, cognitive and quality of life variables according to neuropsychological division.
Unimpaired (n = 23) | Amnestic type (n = 25) | Amnestic/non-amnestic type (n = 28) | P value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 73.3 ± 4.4 | 71.9 ± 5.4 | 72.7 ± 5.9 | 0.77 |
Gender (% men) | 55% | 64% | 60% | 0.67 |
Education (years) | 15.8 ± 2.7 | 16.8 ± 2.4 | 15.5 ± 2.9 | 0.12 |
Cirrhosis (%) | 5 (22%) | 12 (48%) | 22 (78%)†,‡ | <0.0001 |
Diabetes (%) | 10 (43%) | 7 (28%) | 11 (39%) | 0.51 |
Alcohol abuse history (%) | 3 (13%) | 6 (21%) | 10 (36%) | 0.18 |
Hypertension (%) | 8 (35%) | 11 (44%) | 10 (36%) | 0.76 |
Depression on SSRI (%) | 4 (17%) | 6 (24%) | 6 (21%) | 0.85 |
Hyperlipidemia (%) | 7 (30%) | 13 (52%) | 8 (28%) | 0.16 |
Mini-mental status | 29 ± 1 | 28 ± 2 | 28 ± 1 | 0.02 |
HVLT | ||||
HVLT Total recall | 25 ± 5 | 18 ± 4 | 19 ± 6‡ | 0.001 |
HVLT delayed recall | 9 ± 2 | 3 ± 2 | 7 ± 2† | <0.0001 |
HVLT retention % | 89 ± 18 | 45 ± 29 | 86 ± 27† | <0.0001 |
Similarities test | 29 ± 4 | 26 ± 6 | 23 ± 6†,‡ | 0.02 |
RBANS | ||||
Total score | 114.3 ± 13.0 | 94.1 ± 13.3 | 88.4 ± 17.6‡ | <0.0001 |
Visuospatial | 117.9 ± 14.0 | 102.1 ± 18.6 | 95.6 ± 14.9†,‡ | <0.0001 |
Immediate Memory | 103.7 ± 14.7 | 89.7 ± 12.1 | 86.5 ± 19.3‡ | 0.004 |
Language | 103.3 ± 10.8 | 95.1 ± 7.3 | 92.5 ± 14.0‡ | 0.01 |
Attention | 110.4 ± 12.9 | 109.1 ± 15.8 | 93.1 ± 15.4†,‡ | 0.001 |
Delayed Memory | 109.3 ± 9.2 | 82.9 ± 18.4 | 88.4 ± 20.5‡ | <0.001 |
EncephalApp Stroop | ||||
Stroop Off Time (sec) | 82.0 ± 16.2 | 92.5 ± 26.1 | 109.4 ± 37.4†,‡ | 0.012 |
On Time (sec) | 104.0 ± 38.9 | 114.0 ± 38.9 | 139.2 ± 58.9†,‡ | 0.05 |
Median runs On | 5.0 | 5.5 | 6.0 | 0.31 |
Median runs Off | 6.0 | 6.0 | 6.5 | 0.38 |
OffTime + OnTime (sec) | 180.4 ± 36.3 | 184.4 ± 51.0 | 248.6 ± 90.4†,‡ | 0.003 |
PHES | ||||
Number connection-A (sec) | 34.7 ± 10.9 | 44.6 ± 22.6 | 65.3 ± 42.5†,‡ | 0.006 |
Number connection-B (sec) | 86.2 ± 33.4 | 111.0 ± 67.8 | 179.5 ± 115.9†,‡ | 0.002 |
Digit Symbol (raw score) | 57.2 ± 14.9 | 48.9 ± 16.1 | 41.9 ± 21.7†,‡ | 0.03 |
Line Tracing test (sec) | 104.8 ± 35.0 | 95.6 ± 42.3 | 121.3 ± 65.5 | 0.32 |
Line Tracing errors | 25.3 ± 17.7 | 44.6 ± 42.4 | 39.5 ± 31.9 | 0.16 |
Serial dotting (sec) | 54.5 ± 15.7 | 63.6 ± 32.9 | 86.6 ± 43.8†,‡ | 0.01 |
Median PHES | 0 | −3 | −6†,‡ | 0.001 |
HRQOL assessments | ||||
SIP | ||||
Total score | 1.4 ± 1.9 | 6.3 ± 7.5 | 11.1 ± 13.3†,‡ | 0.006 |
Psychosocial domain | 1.4 ± 1.8 | 5.5 ± 5.8 | 9.0 ± 13.6†,‡ | 0.03 |
Physical domain | 1.2 ± 2.5 | 5.5 ± 7.3 | 11.2 ± 13.5†,‡ | 0.005 |
Median Age-adjusted PROMIS scores | ||||
Anger | 33.0 | 48.5 | 75.5†,‡ | 0.002 |
Anxiety | 45.0 | 55.0 | 87.0†,‡ | 0.001 |
Depression | 38.0 | 49.0 | 64.0†,‡ | 0.003 |
Fatigue | 26.0 | 41.5 | 92.0†,‡ | 0.006 |
Physical function* | 79.0 | 62.0 | 39.5†,‡ | 0.005 |
Social activity* | 79.0 | 56.5 | 33.0†,‡ | 0.004 |
Social Role* | 62.5 | 53.5 | 8.0†,‡ | 0.008 |
Sleep disturbance | 33.0 | 37.5 | 73.0†,‡ | 0.005 |
Wake disturbances | 26.0 | 62.5 | 85.0†,‡ | 0.003 |
P value using ANOVA or Chi-square test as appropriate, *a low score on these PROMIS variables indicates worse function, while a low score on the others indicates good function. †p < 0.05 between amnestic/non-amnestic and amnestic type, ‡p < 0.05 between amnestic/non-amnestic and unimpaired. PHES: psychometric hepatic encephalopathy score, PROMIS: patient-reported outcome measurement information system, SIP: Sickness Impact Profile, HRQOL: Health-related quality of life, RBANS: repeatable battery for assessment of neuropsychological status, HVLT: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test.
Multi-modal MRI analysis
fMRI
When cirrhotics were compared to non-cirrhotic patients, there were no significant differences in brain activation extent. However, based on cognitive divisions, correct inhibition revealed widespread activation in areas within the dorsolateral prefrontal, paracingulate, posterior cingulate, precuneous, supplementary motor area and posterior parietal cortices in all groups. Amnestic/non-amnestic patients had significantly higher activation during inhibition compared to the other groups in several regions including central opercular cortex, postcentral gyrus and superior parietal lobule. No differences were found between amnestic and unimpaired subjects (Table 3, Fig. 1A,B).
Table 3. Activation table of brain areas showing group differences during correct inhibition to lures; no differences were found between cirrhosis vs. no-cirrhosis and between amnestic group and unimpaired patients.
Cluster Index | Z-score | MNI (mm) |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
X | y | z | |||
Amnestic/Non-amnestic >Unimpaireda | |||||
Cluster 2: 2252 voxels | |||||
Central Opercular Cortex, L | 2 | 3.50 | −42 | −22 | 20 |
Postcentral Gyrus, L | 2 | 3.20 | −38 | −24 | 40 |
Insular Cortex, L | 2 | 3.12 | −36 | −10 | 8 |
Cluster 1: 1199 voxels | |||||
Postcentral Gyrus, R | 1 | 3.16 | 34 | −32 | 40 |
Posterior Cingulate Gyrus, R | 1 | 3.15 | 14 | −36 | 42 |
Superior Parietal Lobule, R | 1 | 2.62 | 32 | −40 | 58 |
Amnestic/Non-amnestic >Unimpairedb | |||||
Cluster 4: 5395 voxels | |||||
Temporal Occipital Fusiform Cortex, R | 4 | 3.63 | 38 | −60 | −10 |
Occipital Pole, L | 4 | 3.54 | −22 | −102 | 4 |
Occipital Pole, R | 4 | 3.40 | 32 | −90 | 30 |
Cluster 3: 2670 voxels | |||||
Central Opercular Cortex, L | 3 | 3.70 | −40 | −20 | 22 |
Insular Cortex, L | 3 | 3.57 | −38 | −12 | 14 |
Superior Parietal Lobule, L | 3 | 3.16 | −36 | −42 | 58 |
Postcentral Gyrus, L | 3 | 3.02 | −58 | −14 | 46 |
Cluster 2: 1585 voxels | |||||
Parietal Operculum Cortex, R | 2 | 3.37 | 48 | −24 | 30 |
Postcentral Gyrus, R | 2 | 3.35 | 56 | −18 | 40 |
Cluster 1: 1029 voxels | |||||
Precuneous Cortex, R | 1 | 3.25 | 14 | −36 | 44 |
Precuneous Cortex, L | 1 | 3.20 | −10 | −52 | 66 |
Posterior Cingulate Gyrus, R | 1 | 3.10 | 6 | −36 | 46 |
Superior Parietal Lobule, R | 1 | 3.01 | 14 | −50 | 64 |
Lateral Occipital Cortex (Sup), R | 1 | 2.91 | 24 | −58 | 52 |
aPresents the brain areas where the brain activation to correct inhibition to lures was significantly greater in the Amnestic/Non-Amnestic compared to Amnestic type groups on fMRI. bPresents the brain areas where the brain activation to correct inhibition to lures was significantly greater in the Amnestic/Non-Amnestic compared to the Unimpaired type group on fMRI. A ‘Cluster’ is a group of anatomically adjacent voxels found to be statistically significant after thresholding with z-threshold = 2.3 and p = 0.05. Z-score represents the magnitude of group differences in activation and Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates (x, y and z axis) represent a standardized way of presenting the anatomical location of the peak Z-score on a normalized brain template.
MR Spectroscopy
Cirrhotic subjects had lower creatine ratios of myoinositoI and NAA + NAAG, and higher creatine ratio of Glutamate + Glutamine in the anterior cingulate cortex. Using the neuropsychological classification, both amnestic/non-amnestic patients had a lower mI/creatine ratio and a lower NAA + NAAG creatine ratio than amnestic patients (Table 4). Amnestic/non-amnestic patients also had a lower NAA + NAAG creatine ratio than unimpaired subjects (p = 0.005).
Table 4. Group differences in brain metabolite creatine ratios in the anterior cingulate cortex.
Metabolite creatine ratios | Based on underlying disease |
Based on neuropsychological division |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Non-cirrhotic | Cirrhosis | Unimpaired | Amnestic type | Amnestic+ Non-amnestic type | |
MyoinositoI | 0.794 ± 0.08 | 0.593 ± 0.19‡‡ | 0.727 ± 0.15 | 0.824 ± 0.10 | 0.611 ± 0.19† |
N-Acetyl Aspartate | 1.303 ± 0.07 | 1.222 ± 0.12‡ | 1.294 ± 0.11* | 1.266 ± 0.09 | 1.173 ± 0.09†† |
Glutamate+ Glutamine | 1.812 ± 0.25 | 2.105 ± 0.47‡ | 1.955 ± 0.30 | 1.796 ± 0.29 | 1.897 ± 0.57 |
‡p < 0.05, ‡‡p < 0.01 between cirrhotic and non-cirrhotic, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. Unimpaired vs. amnestic + non-amnestic type; †p < 0.05, ††p < 0.01 amnestic vs. amnestic + non−amnestic type.
Volumetric analysis
Total brain volumes were similar between cirrhotic compared to non-cirrhotic subjects. When divided based on neuropsychology, the amnestic-type patients had lower white matter, gray matter and total brain volume as well as Hippocampal and left thalamic volume compared to the other two groups (Table 5). Right thalamic volumes in amnestic group were only lower than the amnestic/non-amnestic group.
Table 5. Group differences on brain volumetric analysis.
Brain volume region | Based on the disease | Brain volume region | Based on neuro-psychological performance | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Non-cirrhotic | Cirrhosis | Unimpaired | Amnestic type | Amnestic+ Non-amnestic | ||
White matter | 6.87 ± 0.4 | 6.68 ± 0.5 | White matter | 6.99 ± 0.2** | 6.34 ± 0.6† | 6.88 ± 0.2 |
Gray matter | 6.41 ± 0.5 | 6.34 ± 0.7 | Gray matter | 6.59 ± 0.5* | 5.99 ± 0.6† | 6.55 ± 0.4 |
Total brain | 13.3 ± 0.7 | 13.0 ± 1.2 | Total brain | 13.6 ± 0.6** | 12.4 ± 1.25† | 13.4 ± 0.8 |
Hippocampus L | 3.63 ± 0.4** | 3.14 ± 0.5†† | 3.68 ± 0.3 | |||
Hippocampus R | 3.80 ± 0.5 | 3.50 ± 0.4† | 3.86 ± 0.3 | |||
Thalamus L | 6.75 ± 0.5* | 6.09 ± 0.7†† | 7.05 ± 0.6 | |||
Thalamus R | 6.53 ± 0.5 | 6.18 ± 0.4†† | 6.81 ± 0.5 |
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. Unimpaired vs. amnestic type; †p < 0.05, ††p < 0.01 amnestic vs. amnestic/non-amnestic type. For total brain volumes (white matter, gray matter and total brain) data are presented as x 10−5 mm3 while for local volumes (Hippocampus and Thalamus) data are presented as x 10−3 mm3. All subjects’ volumes are reported relative to a normalized skull size.
Systemic inflammatory cytokines
Serum IL-6 was higher in cirrhotic compared to non-cirrhotic subjects (median 2.6 vs 1.0 pg/ml, p = 0.002), as was serum endotoxin (median 0.50 vs. 0.34 Eu/ml, p = 0.05). On the other hand serum IFN-γ (0.55 vs 1.10 pg/ml, p = 0.48) and PGE2 (146.7 vs. 125.2 pg/ml, p = 0.73) were similar. Using the neuropsychological performance, a progressive significant increase in IFN-γ (0.0 vs 1.1 vs 1.4 pg/ml, p = 0.007) was seen. IL-6 levels were only high in patients with amnestic/non-amnestic impairment (1.0 vs. 1.0 vs. 2.6 pg/ml, p = 0.02). There was no significant difference in PGE2 levels (140.0 vs. 121.3 vs 101.6, p = 0.89) or serum endotoxin between the groups based on neuropsychological performance (0.36 vs. 0.38 vs. 0.45 Eu/ml, p = 0.64).
Gut microbiota changes and correlation networks
Cirrhotic subjects showed a higher relative abundance of members of Lactobacillales (Carnobacteriaceae, Streptococcaceae and Verrucomicrobiae), and a lower relative abundance of Synergisticeae and Peptococcaceae (Fig. 2A). Using neuropsychological divisions, amnestic/non-amnestic-type patients compared to amnestic-type had a significantly lower relative abundance of genera belonging to autochthonous (Subdoligranulum, Oscillibacter) and oral-origin families (Porphyromonadaceae and Prevotellaceae), and a higher Bacteroides abundance. These genera were also present in amnestic-type patients compared to unimpaired; however unimpaired subjects had a higher relative abundance of Fecalibacterium. A higher relative abundance of a butyrate-producing genus (Butyricicoccus) was found in unimpaired compared to amnestic/non-amnestic -type subjects (Fig. 2B–D).
Correlation networks
In the cirrhosis group, members of Lactobacillales (Streptoccaceae, Carnobacteriaceae and Lactobacillaceae) were linked with good cognition on memory-based tests, while beneficial autochthonous taxa such as Ruminococcaceae and Lachnospiraceae were linked with memory-unrelated test performance. In contrast potentially pathogenic taxa such as Enterobacteriaceae were negatively linked with memory-unrelated performance (Fig. 3A,B). In the non-cirrhosis group, the linkage pattern between cognition, autochthonous and potentially pathogenic taxa was similar to cirrhotic group. In addition, LEfSe-discriminated taxa associated with the non-cirrhosis group in comparison with cirrhotics, Synergisticaeae and Peptococcaceae, were associated with poor cognitive performance (negatively associated with HVLT score).
Discussion
This study demonstrates that elderly cirrhotic patients have a significantly impaired cognitive performance and HRQOL, associated with systemic inflammation, gut dysbiosis and altered MR spectroscopic findings compared to age-matched non-cirrhotic subjects. Furthermore, this altered gut-liver-brain axis, especially from a multi-modal MRI perspective, is refined when neuropsychological profile rather than cirrhosis itself is used as a classifier. Changes in systemic inflammation and gut microbiota that track the neuropsychological classification suggest a potential biological basis for this division that transcends the diagnosis of cirrhosis. Similarly, neuropsychological impairment was found to be the earliest biomarker for predicting progression of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease13,14.
The growing elderly cirrhotic population, who could have concomitant amnestic and non-amnestic cognitive deficits, remains an important concern for clinicians and neuropsychologists. The converging effects of CHE and age-related changes suggestive of mild cognitive impairment in these patients could impact the HRQOL and potentially disease progression. Our findings demonstrated that the neurocognitive profile of elderly cirrhotic patients was largely similar to what could be expected in younger cirrhotics compared to non-cirrhotic individuals with a few exceptions3. Elderly cirrhotic subjects had impairment on selected memory-related tests that would not be expected in younger cirrhotic patients3. The relationship between cirrhosis and aging was therefore further explored using the neuropsychological classification. This classification showed that almost half of the patients classified as amnestic type and more than three quarters of amnestic/non-amnestic type had underlying cirrhosis. This higher burden of cognitive impairment in cirrhotic patients, regardless of classification modality, translated into a worse HRQOL that spanned most areas of daily functioning. Therefore elderly cirrhotic patients, even without prior overt HE, are likely to have greater amnestic impairment, which translates into a poor HRQOL compared to non-cirrhotic individuals.
While the relatively simple classification according to underlying cirrhosis was important to define HRQOL changes, it did not readily translate into corresponding changes in fMRI or volumetric analyses. On the other hand the neuropsychological classification determined that patients with amnestic/non-amnestic impairment required greater neuronal recruitment of the visuo-spatial network to achieve the same response compared those with amnestic impairment alone, or unimpaired subjects regardless of cirrhosis15. A similar pattern was observed on volumetry where cirrhosis vs. no-cirrhosis did not significantly differ while the neuropsychological classification clearly separated the groups. Interestingly, hippocampal and brain volume decrease was found in those with predominant amnestic type. A lower brain volume has been associated with non-cirrhotic amnestic impairment and our study found similar trends in patients with amnestic impairment, which included cirrhotic patients13. The relative higher brain volumes in the predominantly cirrhotic amnestic/non-amnestic group likely reflect cirrhosis-associated brain edema16. These findings were further extended by the MR spectroscopy data. This showed that elderly cirrhotic patients, in addition to demonstrating ammonia-associated changes of higher creatine-adjusted values of Glx and lower mI, and had a lower creatine-adjusted NAA + NAAG, which is a neuronal marker described again in non-cirrhotic amnestic impairment17. This MR spectroscopic classification was further refined by the cognitive classification that reaffirmed the lower creatine ratios of NAA + NAAG and mI in the most impaired groups. Therefore, from a multi-modal MRI perspective, where changes are often subtle and may be subclinical, a division based on neuropsychological performance may help define brain dysfunction better than a simplistic cirrhosis/no-cirrhosis classification.
However, the pathophysiology of this cognitive impairment could span several other organ systems, given the complex interaction between systemic inflammation, gut microbial dysbiosis and brain function in younger cirrhotic patients8,18,19. When taken as a whole group, patients with cirrhosis regardless of neuropsychological performance had higher relative abundances of Lactobacillales members. These taxa were positively linked with good performance on memory-based tests, which could potentially explain the lack of major differences on these tests between the cirrhotic and non-cirrhotic groups. However, the non-cirrhotic group had higher relative abundances of Synergistaceae and Peptococcaceae, which were related to poor amnestic performance. Prior known potentially beneficial, autochthonous families such as Ruminococcaceae and Lachnospiraceae were positively linked, while potentially pathogenic taxa such as Enterobacteriaceae were negatively linked to good cognition regardless of cirrhosis19.
Intriguingly changes in gut microbiota persisted when groups were classified neuropsychologically. Fecalibacterium and Butyricoccus, which are potentially probiotic genera associated with inflammation suppression and butyrate production respectively, were higher in cognitively unimpaired subjects20,21,22. On the other hand, genera belonging to beneficial, autochthonous families were less likely in patients with amnestic/non-amnestic type compared to those with amnestic type alone19. Interestingly, Porphromomadaceae, has been associated with neuronal dysfunction in younger cirrhotics, were more likely to be present in amnestic patients and was associated with poor cognition in the cirrhosis correlation network9,11, alluding to the predominant neuronal role in the progression to dementia. Interestingly, unlike in younger cirrhosis vs. no-cirrhosis comparisons, we did not find members of Proteobacteria phylum as differentiators. This could reflect prior studies that show that elderly subjects have a baseline higher Proteobacteria abundance overall, and could also explain the relatively insignificant overall changes in endotoxin23. The other inflammatory markers were chosen to represent innate immunity (IFN-γ), prostaglandin pathway (PGE2) and systemic inflammation (IL-6). PGE2 was analyzed because prior non-cirrhotic human and animal reports have alluded to it as a marker of pre-clinical dementia, but it was not able to differentiate the current population24. A higher IL-6 is often seen in cirrhotic patients, which could explain the higher values in the amnestic/non-amnestic group25. A higher IFN-γ in both cognitively impaired groups could reflect a non-specific innate response to the bacterial dysbiosis26.
This differential systemic milieu could link microbial and brain changes but endotoxemia does not seem to play an important role27. However, further mechanistic studies are needed. Beneficial changes in butyrate and inflammation have been shown with use of synbiotics in elderly patients, noting that these factors may be another aspect to improve the impaired inflammatory-immune basis of inflamm-aging8,12,28. Further studies focusing on improving the altered gut-brain axis in cirrhotic elderly patients and potentially using specific stool bacteria as biomarkers for disease progression are needed.
We excluded OHE patients, who by definition have worse cognition, brain MRI results and gut dysbiosis compared to others, because it could have influenced the interpretation of cognitive tests, and gut microbiota analysis19,27. However, we were still able to find differences in the population with subclinical cognitive dysfunction, and would expect these changes to be magnified in future studies with elderly OHE patients. Another limitation of this study is that it represents a highly selected subject sample without significant co-morbid conditions. The relative contribution of cirrhosis, dysbiosis and aging on the ultimate cognitive and brain MR function is difficult to delineate given the sample size, and larger studies are required.
Our results suggest that an altered gut-brain axis occurs in elderly patients with differential changes based on the underlying cirrhosis and the type of cognitive dysfunction. Elderly cirrhotic patients are likely to have significant amnestic or memory-related impairment, which is usually associated with dementia and translates into a poor HRQOL. A classification based on neuropsychological performance further refines changes in multi-modal MRI markers of functional and structural cortical connectivity. Further studies are needed in more advanced patients to define the effect of these changes on outcomes, and to utilize the gut microbiota as an alternative approach to diagnose and treat cognitive disorders in the growing elderly cirrhotic population.
Methods
We enrolled consecutive outpatients between the 65 and 85 years of age after informed consent. We excluded patients with significant physical (congestive heart failure, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, HIV, malignancies, dialysis-dependent, diabetes with HbA1c > 7%) and neuropsychiatric conditions (established dementia, prior stroke, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, Parkinson’s disease, multiple sclerosis, seizures). We also excluded all on anti-psychotic, anti-seizure, benzodiazepines and chronic anti-depressants other than selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI). We did not exclude patients on chronic SSRI given the drugs’ minimal effect on cognitive performance in cirrhosis29. We also excluded subjects with alcohol misuse or illicit drug use within 3 months, and those who had used laxatives, probiotics and antibiotics within the last six weeks. Cirrhotic patients were excluded if they had current/prior overt hepatic encephalopathy (OHE)19,27. After enrollment, all subjects underwent mini-mental status testing (MMSE) and the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). A score <25 on MMSE and >12 on BDI-II excluded them from further study30. Demographic information and severity of cirrhosis were recorded. Patients then underwent the following (1) Cognitive testing (2) HRQOL assessment (3) Stool/Blood Sample Collection (4) Multi-modal brain MRI and (5) Psychologist interview.
Cognitive Battery
These batteries characterize (a) predominantly non-amnestic impairment [Psychometric hepatic encephalopathy score (PHES), and EncephalApp Stroop]3,31, (b) predominantly amnestic impairment [Hopkins Verbal Learning test (HVLT)32] and (c) batteries that evaluate both amnestic and non-amnestic impairment [Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS)33 and Similarities subtests34]. The RBANS33 assesses 5 cognitive domains (immediate and delayed memory, visuo-spatial skills, attention, and language). While it was originally developed for screening of dementia, it has also been used in HE, with a predominant “subcortical” pattern of dysfunction33,4.
HRQOL assessment was performed using the Sickness Impact Profile (SIP) and the computerized adaptive PROMIS tools35. We collected serum for inflammatory cytokines, and stool for microbiota analysis using published methods36.
Inflammatory cytokines analyzed were IL-6, Interferon-γ(IFN-γ) and prostaglandin E2(PGE2)24 using ELISA and endotoxin was assessed using LAL assay (Assaygate, Ijamsville, MD)19. Stool microbiota composition was determined using published multi-tagged sequencing techniques37 and analyzed using LEFSe (Linear Discriminant Analysis Effect Size) between the study groups38. Correlation network analysis between cognitive testing results, microbiota and inflammatory cytokines was performed in the cirrhotic group and in the non-cirrhotic group using a customized R package11. Only correlations that were p < 0.01 and r > 0.6 or <−0.6 were studied.
Patients then underwent multi-modal brain MRI on the same day as these procedures. The MRI consisted of (1) fMRI, (2) MR spectroscopy, and (3) volumetric analysis
fMRI ICT task
During this task the subject was asked to respond to targets and withhold responses to lures, on inhibitory control test, a validated go/no-go task, while in the scanner39. A brief training session was performed prior to the MRI scan. Each subject underwent six runs of the ICT inside the scanner (supplementary information).
fMRI data analysis
fMRI data analysis was carried out using the FMRI Expert Analysis Tool v 5.98 part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library)40. After standard preprocessing, a time-series statistical analysis was carried out with local autocorrelation correction using the General Linear Model. The model included correct response to target and correct inhibition to lures as contrast of interest. Incorrect response to lure, random responses and six motion parameters were added as contrasts of no interest. Group average statistical maps were created in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space for all correct inhibition to lures trials. The MNI template is an internationally recognize standard for anatomical localization of brain activation observed on fMRI. Also, statistical maps to evaluate group differences were generated and thresholded using cluster-based thresholding.
Spectroscopy analysis
The choline, creatine, myo-inositol, N-acetylaspartate + N-acetylaspartate glutamate (NAAA + NAAG) and glutamate + glutamine (Glx) complex peak areas were computed using a quantitative assessment of the metabolite concentration by means of LCModel software with the creatine concentration ratio41. These metabolites were chosen based on prior HE research16.
Volumetric analysis
Brain tissue volume, normalized for subject head size, was estimated with SIENAX42. Additionally, we also estimated hippocampal and thalamus volumes using part of FSL43 and normalized them for head size.
Study Approval and Consents
All participants gave informed consent. The study is approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Virginia Commonwealth University and McGuire VA Medical Center and all experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Analysis of the subjects was performed using two major classifications:
Cirrhotic vs. non-cirrhotic patients and
Subjects without impairment vs. those with predominant amnestic impairment vs. those with both amnestic and non-amnestic impairment regardless of cirrhosis.
In arriving at the classification the neuropsychologist reviewed, and applied age correction, to the subject’s raw cognitive scores and conducted a standardized interview with each subject and their companion, if available. During the interview academic, vocational, and substance abuse history was obtained. Subjects whose cognitive performance on all the measures ranged from –2 standard deviations and above were considered unimpaired. Subjects with focal impairment ≥2 SD on ≥2 learning/memory subtests were classified as amnestic type. Finally, subjects were considered amnestic/non-amnestic type if they were impaired ≥2 SD on measures in >2 domains, such as information/psychomotor speed and learning/memory. The interview notes and cognitive performance were then reviewed by another psychologist (KS) who was blinded to the original division by the neuropsychologist. A Kappa score between the divisions was calculated for concordance.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS (version 12, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) with appropriate tests. Laboratory tests, neuropsychological tests, HRQOL tests, volumetrics and MRS were compared between groups by the two-tailed unpaired t-test or ANOVA based on the divisions. Multiple comparison correction was done wherever applicable using Holm-Bonferroni method. Specialized analyses (microbiota, fMRI, correlation networks are mentioned in individual sections). A sample size of at least 16 in each group was deemed to be appropriate with an 80% power based on our prior studies on gut-brain analysis in younger patients with cirrhosis compared to healthy controls9.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Bajaj, J. S. et al. Elderly patients have an altered gut-brain axis regardless of the presence of cirrhosis. Sci. Rep. 6, 38481; doi: 10.1038/srep38481 (2016).
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Rob Cadrain for help with the brain imaging and Ms Mary Beatty-Brooks for her help with the figures. The work was supported by NIDDK R01DK089713, Veterans Affairs Merit Review CX10076, Pilot funding from IDAS at VCU Medical Center, VCU PERQ Award and Department of Medicine Academic Enhancement Fund.
Footnotes
Author Contributions J.S.B. was responsible for conceptualization and all aspects of the study, V.A., J.L.S., F.G.M. were involved in the MRI scanning, analysis and interpretation, J.B.W. was responsible for neuropsychological evaluation of all patients and provided the classification, K.S. provided additional psychological expertise, M.B.W., E.A.G., D.G., A.F., M.G., S.H., P.A.B., S.H. were responsible for recruitment and study procedures, P.G.M., J.W., E.J.L., R.K., P.B.H. and M.S. were responsible for the microbiota analysis and interpretation; all authors were responsible for drafting and revising the manuscript.
References
- http://www.cdc.gov/aging/aginginfo/alzheimers.htm.Author: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, GA, Title: Alzheimer’s Disease, Year published 2015, last accessed 01/08/2016.
- Rakoski M. O. et al. Burden of cirrhosis on older Americans and their families: analysis of the health and retirement study. Hepatology 55, 184–191, doi: 10.1002/hep.24616 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilstrup H. et al. Hepatic encephalopathy in chronic liver disease: Practice Guideline by the American Association for the Study of Liver Diseases and the European Association for the Study of the Liver. Hepatology 60, 715–735, doi: 10.1002/hep.27210 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mooney S. et al. Utility of the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) in patients with end-stage liver disease awaiting liver transplant. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 22, 175–186, doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.005 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang C. W. et al. Functional impairment in older liver transplantation candidates: From the functional assessment in liver transplantation study. Liver Transpl 21, 1465–1470, doi: 10.1002/lt.24334 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campagna F. et al. Cognitive impairment and electroencephalographic alterations before and after liver transplantation: what is reversible? Liver transplantation: official publication of the American Association for the Study of Liver Diseases and the International Liver Transplantation Society 20, 977–986, doi: 10.1002/lt.23909 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caracciolo B., Xu W., Collins S. & Fratiglioni L. Cognitive decline, dietary factors and gut-brain interactions. Mech Ageing Dev 136–137, 59–69, doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2013.11.011 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quigley E. M. Commentary: synbiotics and gut microbiota in older people–a microbial guide to healthy ageing. Aliment Pharmacol Ther 38, 1141–1142, doi: 10.1111/apt.12495 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahluwalia V. et al. Impaired Gut-Liver-Brain Axis in Patients with Cirrhosis. Sci Rep 6, 26800, doi: 10.1038/srep26800 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bajaj J. S., Betrapally N. S. & Gillevet P. M. Decompensated cirrhosis and microbiome interpretation. Nature, doi: 10.1038/nature14851 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bajaj J. S. et al. Linkage of gut microbiome with cognition in hepatic encephalopathy. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 302, G168–175 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cevenini E., Monti D. & Franceschi C. Inflamm-ageing. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 16, 14–20, doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e32835ada13 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heister D. et al. Predicting MCI outcome with clinically available MRI and CSF biomarkers. Neurology 77, 1619–1628, doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182343314 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau S. M. et al. Comparing predictors of conversion and decline in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 75, 230–238, doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e8e8b8 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McPhail M. J. et al. Modulation of neural activation following treatment of hepatic encephalopathy. Neurology 80, 1041–1047, doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828726e1 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarma M. K. et al. Multi-dimensional MR spectroscopy: towards a better understanding of hepatic encephalopathy. Metab Brain Dis 26, 173–184, doi: 10.1007/s11011-011-9250-1 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang H. et al. Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy in Alzheimer’s Disease: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J Alzheimers Dis 46, 1049–1070, doi: 10.3233/JAD-143225 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claesson M. J. et al. Gut microbiota composition correlates with diet and health in the elderly. Nature 488, 178–184, doi: 10.1038/nature11319 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bajaj J. S. et al. Altered profile of human gut microbiome is associated with cirrhosis and its complications. J Hepatol 60, 940–947, doi: 10.1016/j.jhep.2013.12.019 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miquel S. et al. Faecalibacterium prausnitzii and human intestinal health. Curr Opin Microbiol 16, 255–261, doi: 10.1016/j.mib.2013.06.003 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eeckhaut V. et al. Butyricicoccus pullicaecorum in inflammatory bowel disease. Gut 62, 1745–1752, doi: 10.1136/gutjnl-2012-303611 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geirnaert A. et al. Butyricicoccus pullicaecorum, a butyrate producer with probiotic potential, is intrinsically tolerant to stomach and small intestine conditions. Anaerobe 30, 70–74, doi: 10.1016/j.anaerobe.2014.08.010 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biagi E. et al. Through ageing, and beyond: gut microbiota and inflammatory status in seniors and centenarians. PLoS One 5, e10667, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010667 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson J. U. et al. Prostaglandin signaling suppresses beneficial microglial function in Alzheimer’s disease models. J Clin Invest 125, 350–364, doi: 10.1172/JCI77487 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shawcross D. L., Davies N. A., Williams R. & Jalan R. Systemic inflammatory response exacerbates the neuropsychological effects of induced hyperammonemia in cirrhosis. J Hepatol 40, 247–254 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capobianchi M. R., Uleri E., Caglioti C. & Dolei A. Type I IFN family members: similarity, differences and interaction. Cytokine Growth Factor Rev 26, 103–111, doi: 10.1016/j.cytogfr.2014.10.011 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shawcross D. L. Is it time to target gut dysbiosis and immune dysfunction in the therapy of hepatic encephalopathy? Expert Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol 9, 539–542, doi: 10.1586/17474124.2015.1035257 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macfarlane S., Cleary S., Bahrami B., Reynolds N. & Macfarlane G. T. Synbiotic consumption changes the metabolism and composition of the gut microbiota in older people and modifies inflammatory processes: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study. Aliment Pharmacol Ther 38, 804–816, doi: 10.1111/apt.12453 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bajaj J. S. et al. Cognitive performance as a predictor of hepatic encephalopathy in pretransplant patients with cirrhosis receiving psychoactive medications: a prospective study. Liver Transpl 18, 1179–1187, doi: 10.1002/lt.23484 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck A. T., Ward C. H., Mendelson M., Mock J. & Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 4, 561–571 (1961). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allampati S. et al. Diagnosis of Minimal Hepatic Encephalopathy Using Stroop EncephalApp: A Multicenter US-Based, Norm-Based Study. Am J Gastroenterol 111, 78–86, doi: 10.1038/ajg.2015.377 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hogervorst E. et al. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test and screening for dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 13, 13–20 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randolph C., Tierney M. C., Mohr E. & Chase T. N. The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS): preliminary clinical validity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 20, 310–319, doi: 10.1076/jcen.20.3.310.823 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (Psychological Corporation, 2002). [Google Scholar]
- Bajaj J. S. et al. PROMIS computerised adaptive tests are dynamic instruments to measure health-related quality of life in patients with cirrhosis. Aliment Pharmacol Ther 34, 1123–1132, doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2036.2011.04842.x (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillevet P., Sikaroodi M., Keshavarzian A. & Mutlu E. A. Quantitative assessment of the human gut microbiome using multitag pyrosequencing. Chem Biodivers 7, 1065–1075 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bajaj J. S., Betrapally N. S. & Gillevet P. M. Decompensated cirrhosis and microbiome interpretation. Nature 525, E1–2, doi: 10.1038/nature14851 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segata N. et al. Metagenomic biomarker discovery and explanation. Genome Biol 12, R60, doi: 10.1186/gb-2011-12-6-r60 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garavan H., Ross T. J. & Stein E. A. Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: an event-related functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96, 8301–8306 (1999). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich M. W., Ripley B. D., Brady M. & Smith S. M. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. NeuroImage 14, 1370–1386 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Provencher S. W. Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in vivo proton NMR spectra. Magn Reson Med 30, 672–679 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S. M. et al. Accurate, robust, and automated longitudinal and cross-sectional brain change analysis. Neuroimage 17, 479–489 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patenaude B., Smith S. M., Kennedy D. N. & Jenkinson M. A Bayesian model of shape and appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. Neuroimage 56, 907–922, doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.046 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.