Abstract
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies regularly use univariate general-linear-model-based analyses (GLM). Their findings are often inconsistent across different studies, perhaps because of several fundamental brain properties including functional heterogeneity, balanced excitation and inhibition (E/I), and sparseness of neuronal activities. These properties stipulate heterogeneous neuronal activities in the same voxels and likely limit the sensitivity and specificity of GLM. This paper selectively reviews findings of histological and electrophysiological studies and fMRI spatial independent component analysis (sICA) and reports new findings by applying sICA to two existing datasets. The extant and new findings consistently demonstrate several novel features of brain functional organization not revealed by GLM. They include overlap of large-scale functional networks (FNs) and their concurrent opposite modulations, and no significant modulations in activity of most FNs across the whole brain during any task conditions. These novel features of brain functional organization are highly consistent with the brain’s properties of functional heterogeneity, balanced E/I, and sparseness of neuronal activity, and may help reconcile inconsistent GLM findings.
Keywords: fMRI, GLM, ICA, functional network overlap, default mode network, balanced excitation and inhibition, sparseness of neuronal activity
1. Introduction
1.1. Overview
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies regularly use univariate general-linear-model-based analyses (GLM) to interrogate blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) signal from each voxel separately (Pernet, 2014; Poline and Brett, 2012; Xu et al., 2015). This GLM approach and its findings are in general consistent with a modular view of brain functional organization that different mental processes operate in different brain regions (Donoso et al., 2014; Kanwisher, 2010; Pessoa, 2014; Rowland and Moser, 2014; Szentagothai, 1983). However, some findings from different studies appear highly inconsistent and may generate confusion for the field. The present paper continues our recent efforts for addressing inconsistent GLM findings and for better understanding brain functional organization (Xu, 2015; Xu et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2013b). The manuscript consists of two parts. The first part reviews histological, electrophysiological, and fMRI findings supporting extensive functional network (FN) overlaps that contrast with a modular functional organization of the brain. The second part uses spatial independent component analysis (sICA) to generate new evidence for further support of our hypothesis that large-scale FN overlap is a general property of brain functional organization, and that analytical approaches capable of revealing this property might reconcile previously inconsistent findings generated by GLM. The following sections of the introduction will present examples of inconsistent fMRI findings, discuss why GLM may generate inconsistencies, review evidence for FN overlap, and introduce findings from a new study that further support our theory.
1.2. Examples of inconsistent fMRI findings
One example is related to the so-called default mode network (DMN) (Fox et al., 2005; Mazoyer et al., 2001; Raichle et al., 2001; Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos, 2007). Around the year of 2000, Raichle and colleagues found that several brain regions often exhibited reduced activities during cognitive tasks relative to rest condition (Gusnard and Raichle, 2001; Raichle et al., 2001; Shulman et al., 1997). For example, Gusnard et al used fMRI and GLM to assess brain activity in healthy participants while they viewed pictures selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lane et al., 1997) under two different task conditions (Gusnard and Raichle, 2001). For the internally cued condition (ICC), participants were instructed to respond to their feeling (i.e., pleasant, unpleasant, or neutral) induced by the pictures. In the externally cued condition (ECC), participants responded to the scene depicted by the picture (i.e., indoors, outdoors, or uncertain). Both ICC- and ECC-induced activity decreases in the ventral medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) relative to a condition of passive visual fixation, and ECC induced a greater deactivation relative to ICC. Based on these findings, these investigators proposed that the brain regions with a greater activity during rest condition formed a default mode network (DMN). Furthermore, a later study from the same group analyzed correlations of BOLD signal timecourse in different brain regions during a resting condition (Fox et al., 2005). Three seeds (i.e., ROI, region of interest) were selected in the brain regions often showing increased activity during cognitive tasks (i.e., task-positive regions), and another three seeds were selected in the brain regions often showing decreased activity (i.e., DMN). They found that BOLD signals in the task-positive regions positively correlated with each other and formed a so-called task-positive network, that BOLD signals in the DMN positively correlated with each other and formed a so-called task-negative network, and that the BOLD signal in the two networks negatively correlated with each other. Their findings received extensive support from other fMRI studies using GLM (Buckner et al., 2008; Kelly et al., 2008; McKiernan et al., 2003; Raichle, 2015). These GLM studies together demonstrated that the brain consists of a lateral task-positive network and a medial task-negative network or DMN with anti-correlated functional activities between them, and that the task-positive network responds to external goal-directed tasks while the task-negative network relates to mentalizing (i.e., theory of mind), spontaneous mind-wandering, and internally oriented self-related mental processes (Fox et al., 2005; Gusnard et al., 2001; Mason et al., 2007; Raichle, 2015; Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos, 2007).
However, not all findings from GLM studies support the DMN concept. For example, Gilbert et al studied task-related changes in BOLD signal while healthy participants performed stimulus-oriented tasks by monitoring stimuli on the screen and stimulus-independent tasks by monitoring internally generated information (e.g., adding 7 repeatedly) (Gilbert et al., 2012; Gilbert et al., 2006). The rostral MPFC, a main region of the DMN, showed reduced BOLD signal in the stimulus-independent tasks relative to stimulus-oriented tasks. In another study, healthy participants performed a working memory tasks with parametric task loads of social information (Meyer et al., 2012). For each trial of this task, participants started to remember (i.e., encoding phase) 2–4 names of friends on the screen for 4 s. Then a word for personality trait (e.g., funny) appeared for 1.5 s after the names disappeared from the screen. Participants were instructed to remember the names and rank these friends based on the given trait for 6 s (i.e., delay phase), and finally to press a button to respond to a probe that appeared on the screen (i.e., retrieving phase). During both the delay and retrieving periods, participants showed a task-load dependent increases in BOLD signal not only in the so-called task-positive network such as the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex and superior parietal lobule, but also in the task-negative network or DMN including the dorsal and ventral MPFC, precuneus/posterior cingulate, and temporal parietal junction (Meyer et al., 2012). Several other studies further investigated the above-discussed finding of anti-correlations of task-positive and task-negative networks during rest condition by analyzing simulated and real fMRI data acquired during a resting condition (Anderson et al., 2011; Murphy et al., 2009). These studies did not find anti-correlations between BOLD signals from the two networks if they did not perform global signal regression during data preprocessing, and found such anti-correlations only after global signal regression. The authors of these studies proposed that the previous reported anti-correlations were largely artifacts relating to global signal regression (Anderson et al., 2011; Murphy et al., 2009). Furthermore, multiple studies report that the external goal-directed tasks activate not only the lateral regions (i.e., the task-positive network), but also the medial regions (i.e., the task-negative network), and that mind-wandering and self-related mental processes also activate both the medial and lateral regions (Anderson et al., 2011; Chai et al., 2012; Fox et al., 2015; Murphy et al., 2009; Spreng et al., 2014; Spreng et al., 2009; Spreng et al., 2010).
A second example is related to reported hyper-activity and hypo-activity in older relative to younger adults during cognitive tasks. Some studies found a greater task-related activity in older relative to younger adults during cognitive tasks such as memory encoding and retrieving. For example, younger and older adults were instructed in one study to view (i.e., encode) color photographs and were tested whether they could recognize viewed pictures about 10 minutes later (Gutchess et al., 2005). Relative to the younger adults, the older adults showed a greater activation in the prefrontal and parietal cortices but a smaller activation in the parahippocampal gryus for remembered relative to forgotten pictures during the encoding phase. Another study assessed task-related brain activity while younger and older adults viewed words and judged their living/nonliving categorization (Kennedy et al., 2015). During easy trials of unambiguous words relative to fixation baseline, the older adults showed a greater increase in BOLD signal in the right middle frontal gyrus and the bilateral parietal cortices, but no smaller increases in any regions relative to the younger adults. The task-related hyper-activation in the older relative to the younger adults has been hypothesized to reflect a compensation of reduced functional efficiency in the aging brain (Reuter-Lorenz and Park, 2014; Turner and Spreng, 2015).
However, several other studies reported that older adults exhibited a smaller task-related brain activity relative to younger adults during cognitive tasks. For example, one study assessed task-related brain activity in younger and older adults while they viewed a list of words and decided whether the words were abstract or concrete in meaning (Stebbins et al., 2002). Relative to the young adults, the older adults showed a smaller task-related increase in activity in the left superior, middle, and inferior frontal gyri. In a series of studies, younger and older adults performed a Sternberg working memory task with two task loads (Rypma and D'Esposito, 2000; Rypma et al., 2001). They needed to remember two and six letters during the low- and high-load conditions, respectively. The older and younger groups did not show significant difference in task-related activity in any brain regions during the low-load condition, but the older showed a smaller task-related activity in the dorsal prefrontal cortex during the high-load condition. In a more recent study, younger and older adults were instructed to read and remember lists of word pairs, and their recollections were tested about 15 minutes after reading (de Chastelaine et al., 2016). For remembered relative to forgotten word pairs, older adults relative to younger adults exhibited a smaller task-related activity in the anterior cingulate cortex. The task-related hypoactivation in older adults has been hypothesized to reflect impaired brain function (Kaup et al., 2014; Li et al., 2015; Nagel et al., 2011; Sala-Llonch et al., 2015). Furthermore, similar inconsistencies in task-related hyper- versus hypo-activation have been reported in almost all neuropsychiatric disorders in fMRI studies using GLM, including depression, schizophrenia, and substance-use disorders (Balodis and Potenza, 2014; Hommer et al., 2011; Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2013; Mayberg, 2014; Parens and Johnston, 2014; Whalley et al., 2012; Xu, 2015a).
1.3. Limited sensitivity and specificity of GLM
Multiple factors, such as task context, sample size, individual variability, and the spatial and temporal resolutions of fMRI may contribute to inconsistent fMRI findings (Mayberg, 2014; Parens and Johnston, 2014; Xu, 2015). In addition, we propose that the limited sensitivity and specificity of GLM in detecting neuronal activity is a critical factor. GLM is typically a univariate approach, analyzes BOLD signal from each voxel separately, and reports significant or no significant changes in the signal under different task conditions at different time points (Poline and Brett, 2012). GLM studies regularly interpret signal increases as increased neuronal activities in the voxel, signal decreases as reduced neuronal activities, and no signal change as no changes in neuronal activity. This practice implicitly follows the afore-mentioned modular view of brain functional organization and assumes that BOLD signal increases and decreases in different voxels reflect activation and deactivation of neurons related to different mental processes, respectively. However, as discussed below, it oversimplifies the relationship between changes in BOLD signal and heterogeneous activities of neuronal populations in a voxel and neglects several fundamental brain properties including functional heterogeneity, balanced excitation and inhibition (E/I), and sparseness of neuronal activity (Logothetis, 2008; Serences and Saproo, 2012; Xu, 2015).
Histological studies report extensive convergent and divergent fiber projections among cortical regions and between cortical and subcortical structures (Boucsein et al., 2011; Cappe et al., 2009; Dancause et al., 2006; Driver and Noesselt, 2008; Padberg et al., 2009; Schnepel et al., 2015; Stepniewska et al., 2015). These data indicate that intermixed neurons in the same cortical regions receive inputs from and send outputs to extensive cortical and subcortical areas. Consistent with these histological findings, electrophysiological studies report that intermixed neurons respond to different afferents and exhibit highly heterogeneous functional activities (Funahashi, 2013; Fuster, 2009; Swindale, 1998). These findings do not support the modular theory of brain functional organization. Instead, they indicate that functional heterogeneity of intermixed cortical neurons within the same region is a fundamental property of the cortex (Fuster and Bressler, 2015; Harris and Mrsic-Flogel, 2013; Hermansen et al., 2007; Perin et al., 2011). A typical fMRI voxel in the cortex with 3–5 mm in each dimension contains at least hundreds of thousands of neurons (Druga, 2009; Logothetis, 2008; Roth and Dicke, 2012). Therefore, the BOLD signal from each voxel reflects a highly heterogeneous mixture of functional activities of the entire neuronal population in a voxel, and analyzing this heterogeneous nature of BOLD signal from each voxel is not a strength of the standard univariate GLM.
Furthermore, about 15~20% of cortical neurons are GABAergic inhibitory interneurons and 80–85% excitatory pyramidal neurons (Druga, 2009). These inhibitory and excitatory neurons form feedforward and feedback inhibitory circuits, the building blocks in the cortex (Fig. 1) (Haider et al., 2013; Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Okun and Lampl, 2008; Ren et al., 2007; Silberberg and Markram, 2007). Even though inhibitory neurons are fewer than excitatory neurons in the cortex, they form extensive synapses onto adjacent pyramidal neurons, as if to exert a 'blanket of inhibition’ (Fino et al., 2013; Fino and Yuste, 2011; Inan et al., 2013; Karnani et al., 2014; Markram et al., 2004; Packer and Yuste, 2011). The inhibitory and excitatory neurons maintain a balanced E/I in the cortex; i.e., increased activity in some neurons relates to reduced activity in adjacent neurons, and neurons in the same voxels may show concurrent, colocalized activation and deactivation (CCAD) (Logothetis, 2008; Xu, 2015). Closely related to the property of balanced E/I is sparseness of neuronal activities, another fundamental property in the brain (Barth and Poulet, 2012; Wolfe et al., 2010). This property indicates that only a fraction of cortical neurons in the same regions may fire action potentials at any instant, while most neurons are silent (Barth and Poulet, 2012; Crochet et al., 2011; Tolhurst et al., 2009). Again, GLM cannot differentiate concurrent increase, decrease, and no changes in activity from intermixed neurons in a voxel.
Fig. 1.
Schematic demonstration of feedforward and feedback inhibition circuits. Feedforward inhibition circuit: Excitatory inputs from the thalamus activate both pyramidal neurons and inhibitory interneurons, with the latter in turn suppressing activated pyramidal neurons. Feedback inhibition circuit: Activated pyramidal neurons excite inhibitory interneurons, which in turn suppress pyramidal neurons. The inhibitory interneurons form extensive synapses onto adjacent pyramidal neurons, as if to exert a 'blanket of inhibition’. They inhibit not only the activated neurons, but also other neurons. Please note that different colors of pyramidal neurons indicate potential heterogeneous functional properties.
If increases and decreases in neuronal activities indeed induce BOLD signal increase and decrease, respectively, as reported in many studies (Goense et al., 2012; Logothetis, 2008; Mullinger et al., 2013, 2014; Shmuel et al., 2006), BOLD signals related to opposite changes in neural activities in the same voxels may cancel each other. Therefore, BOLD signal changes in each voxel probably reflect changes in differences (or ratios) between CCAD, not in activation or deactivation alone (Logothetis, 2008; Xu, 2015). A voxel may not show any BOLD signal change if its difference in CCAD does not change, even though individual neurons may change activity significantly. It may express a greater BOLD signal if its neuronal population reduces total deactivation without an increase in activation (Logothetis, 2008; Xu, 2015). Therefore, increases or decreases in BOLD signal do not reliably indicate a greater activation or smaller deactivation, and this uncertainty may contribute to inconsistent findings from fMRI studies using GLM (Logothetis, 2008; Xu, 2015).
1.4. Functional network overlap
The convergent and divergent axonal projections in the brain and heterogeneous activities of intermixed neurons in the same brain region demonstrate that cortical microcircuits are not independent and segregated in space, but they rather overlap and interdigitate with each other (Harris and Mrsic-Flogel, 2013; Hermansen et al., 2007; Perin et al., 2011). Based on these findings, some investigators have explicitly proposed extensive overlap of largescale FNs in the brain (Fuster, 2009; Fuster and Bressler, 2015; Hermansen et al., 2007; Swindale, 1998). Supporting this proposal, multiple fMRI studies from our and other groups demonstrate extensive FN overlap by using sICA or analytical techniques other than GLM.
Independent component analysis (ICA) is a computational technique for extracting hidden signals from observed signal mixtures (Beckmann, 2012; Calhoun and Adali, 2012; Calhoun et al., 2009; Comon, 1994; McKeown et al., 1998b). Different from univariate GLM, sICA is multivariate and treats BOLD signal from each voxel as a signal mixture of different source signals, and separates it into spatially independent components (ICs) by using higher-order statistics (Calhoun and Adali, 2012; Calhoun et al., 2002; Calhoun et al., 2009; McKeown et al., 1998a; McKeown et al., 1998b; McKeown and Sejnowski, 1998). This advantage of sICA was one of the reasons for introducing sICA to fMRI (McKeown et al., 1998b). In an early sICA study, both sICA and GLM were used to extract simulated signals from a synthetic dataset (McKeown et al., 1998b). Synthetic fMRI data were generated by adding simulated signals with known timecourses to acquired real BOLD data in arbitrarily selected brain regions. SICA generated an IC with a timecourse consistent with the simulated signals and a spatial map consistent with the selected brain regions, and another IC with a timecourse consistent with the actual task-related activity. However, GLM recovered only a small proportion of voxels with the simulated signal. Findings from this study indicated that sICA had a greater sensitivity and specificity than GLM in detecting brain regions of simulated signal (McKeown et al., 1998b).
SICA groups all voxels with synchronized source signals into one temporally coherent FN and is a popular method for studying large-scale FNs in fMRI (Beckmann, 2012; Calhoun and Adali, 2012; Calhoun et al., 2009; Comon, 1994; McKeown et al., 1998b). Because of its unique capacity of separating signal mixture from each voxel into source signals, investigators have used sICA to de-noise fMRI data by separating artifacts from signals (Aron and Poldrack, 2006; Beckmann, 2012; Brooks et al., 2013; Du et al., 2016; Griffanti et al., 2014; Tohka et al., 2008; Yakunina et al., 2013). Furthermore, some fMRI sICA studies have described spatial overlap of two or more FNs, indicating that sICA can separate BOLD signal mixtures from individual voxels into two or more FNs in the overlapping regions (Calhoun et al., 2008; Domagalik et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2009a; Kim et al., 2009b; Menz et al., 2009; St Jacques et al., 2011; van Wageningen et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2009; Zhang and Li, 2012). Very recently, we and at least three other groups systematically described FN overlap generated by sICA (Beldzik et al., 2013; Braga et al., 2013; Geranmayeh et al., 2014; Leech et al., 2012; Xu et al., 2014a; Xu et al., 2013a; Xu et al., 2013b; Yeo et al., 2013).
For example, a recent study used sICA to extract FNs from fMRI datasets related to speech (Speech), counting (Count), and decision-making (Decision) tasks (Geranmayeh et al., 2014). SICA generated one FN at the left frontal-temporal-parietal (FTP) and another FN at the right FTP mirroring the left one. The two FNs overlapped with each other at the left parietal and frontal cortices. The left FN showed a task-related activity increase during Speech but an activity decrease during Count and Decision. However, the right FN showed opposite changes in task-related activity; i.e., reduced activity during Speech and increased activity during Count and Decision. Furthermore, a third FN extensively overlapped with the FN at the left FTP and showed reduced activation during Speech and Count. Therefore, three overlapping FNs showed task-related, concurrent, opposite modulations during Speech and Count (Geranmayeh et al., 2014). These findings demonstrate that the BOLD signal from the same brain regions may consist of heterogeneous source signals each with independent unique task-related modulations.
In another study, both GLM and sICA were used to interrogate fMRI data acquired during pro-saccadic (PS) and anti-saccadic (AS) tasks (Beldzik et al., 2013; Domagalik et al., 2012). sICA extracted 11 FNs which showed extensive overlap at multiple brain regions including the intraparietal sulcus, frontal eye field, precuneus and posterior cingulate (PCC). Some of these overlapping FNs showed synergistic changes in task-related activity; i.e., increased activity during AS relative to PS. The overlapping regions of synergistic FNs also exhibited increased activity as revealed by GLM. However, the FNs overlapping at the PCC showed opposite changes in task-related activity during AS relative to PS; i.e., increased in some FNs but decreased in others. These opposite changes in activity in the PCC appeared to cancel each other because GLM did not detect significant changes in activity in these brain regions.
Over the last several years, we have applied both GLM and sICA to five task-related datasets acquired from different participants at four different institutes (Xu, 2015; Xu et al., 2014a; Xu et al., 2014b; Xu et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2013a; Xu et al., 2013b). These tasks involve visual attention, working memory, interference inhibition, mentalizing, and monetary motivation and rewards. Consistent with data from other GLM studies (Fox et al., 2005; Gusnard et al., 2001; Mason et al., 2007; Raichle, 2015; Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos, 2007), our GLM application revealed that the visual attention and working memory tasks activated the lateral task-positive network and deactivated the medial task- negative network, and that the mentalizing task activated the task-negative network. Different from GLM findings, sICA consistently demonstrated that: 1) FNs overlapped with each other extensively at both medial and lateral brain regions, 2) overlapping FNs may show concurrent task-related increases and decreases in activity in both medial and lateral brain regions, 3) FNs did not show task-related changes in activity (i.e., task-neutral FNs) occupied most or all brain volumes and overlapped with FNs showing increases or decreases in activity, 4) brain regions exhibiting task-related increases in activity as revealed by GLM may involve overlapping FNs exhibiting concurrent task-related increases and decrease in activity as revealed by sICA, with the increases dominating over the decreases, 5) brain regions exhibiting task-related decreases in activity as revealed by GLM may involve overlapping FNs exhibiting concurrent task-related increases and decrease in activity as revealed by sICA, with the decreases dominating over the increases, and 6) brain regions that did not exhibit task-related changes in activity as revealed by GLM may show overlapping FNs with concurrent opposite changes in activity, indicating these opposite changes in activity may have cancelled each other in GLM.
Consistent with these sICA findings, multiple studies using several other multivariate analytic methods also reported extensive FN overlap either during resting state or task performance. These methods include temporal ICA (Smith et al., 2012), Connected Iterative Scan (CIS) (Yan et al., 2011), Link Cluster (Madhyastha et al., 2013), Latent Dirichlet Allocation (LDA) (Yeo et al., 2013), Sparse Coding And Dictionary Learning (Lv et al., 2015), Stable Overlapping Replicator Dynamics (SORD) (Yoldemir et al., 2015), and Innovation-Driven Co- Activation Patterns (iCAPs) (Karahanoglu and Van De Ville, 2015). These consistent findings from different approaches indicate that extensive FN overlap is a general property of large-scale brain functional organization, not a unique feature of specific cognitive tasks or specific population. We proposed that overlapping FNs with simultaneous but different task-related modulations probably reflect the fundamental properties of functional heterogeneity, balanced E/I, and sparseness of neuronal activity in the brain (Xu, 2015; Xu et al., 2015).
1.5. Reconciling inconsistent GLM findings
The sICA findings of extensive FN overlap may help reconcile some inconsistent GLM finding from a novel theoretical viewpoint (Xu, 2015). For example, some studies proposed that the finding of anti-correlated task-positive and task-negative FNs was an artifact introduced by removing global signals during data preprocessing, because it could not be found in studies without this preprocessing procedure (Anderson et al., 2011; Carbonell et al., 2011; Raichle, 2015). On the other hand, other investigators suggested that removing global signals could remove signals common to all brain regions and thus reveal the anti-correlation hidden in mixed raw signals (Anderson et al., 2011; Carbonell et al., 2011; Raichle, 2015). As reviewed in the sections above, FNs generated by sICA overlap extensively in both medial and lateral brain regions, and they may show task-related concurrent synergistic or opposite modulations. This finding indicates that both correlated and anti-correlated signals exist in the same brain regions, regardless of medial or lateral locations, and between different brain regions (e.g., medial relative to lateral). This interpretation of sICA findings is supported by the brain’s property of balanced E/I, which stipulates that both correlated and anti-correlated activities exist among adjacent neurons in the same brain regions. Therefore, FN overlap revealed by sICA may help reconcile conflicting findings related to the anti-correlations between activities in the medial and lateral brain regions.
Using sICA to separate overlapping FNs may also help reconcile inconsistent GLM findings of hyper- vs. hypo-activity in older adults and psychiatric patients. There is strong evidence that glutamatergic excitation, GABAergic inhibition and balanced E/I are altered in older adults and psychiatric patients relative to younger adults and healthy controls (HCs), respectively, and that unbalanced E/I contributes to impaired cognitive functioning (Brady et al., 2013; Caspary et al., 2008; Edden et al., 2012; El Idrissi et al., 2013; Ende et al., 2016; Gonzalez-Burgos et al., 2015; Legon et al., 2015; Luscher and Fuchs, 2015; Murray et al., 2014; Nelson and Valakh, 2015; Pehrson and Sanchez, 2015; Purkayastha et al., 2015; Riese et al., 2015; Robertson et al., 2016; Roux and Buzsaki, 2015; Schmidt and Mirnics, 2015; Stebbings et al., 2016; Tse et al., 2015). This pathophysiological mechanism may contribute importantly to inconsistent GLM findings because they may lead to BOLD signal decrease, increase, or no change in different brain regions in older adults and patients, depending on the relative extent of changes in excitation and inhibition in each region. SICA’s unique capacity of separating overlapping FNs enables sICA to reduce the cancellation of opposite signals by separately measuring concurrent increases and decreases in source signals from the same voxels; sICA may also avoid confusion relating to the potential conflation of reduced deactivation and increased activation. Thus, sICA may help reconcile inconsistent GLM findings of hyper- vs. hypo-activity.
The remaining sections of this paper will report findings by applying sICA to two fMRI datasets downloaded from public databases. These findings will provide further support to our hypothesis that extensive overlap of large-scale FNs is a general property of brain functional organization, and that using sICA to separate overlapping FNs may help reconcile inconsistent GLM findings of hyper- versus hypo-activation in older relative to younger adults. The two datasets were acquired from younger and older adults during an audiovisual speech perception task (AVSPT) and a false-belief task (FBT). The AVSPT was a typical external goal-directed task and required participants to identify external targets by pressing a button. The FBT involved mentalizing and required participants to answer question regarding the mental state of characters described in stories. We made the following predictions regarding sICA findings based on recent sICA data and the fundamental brain properties discussed above. 1) FNs generated by sICA would overlap extensively at both the lateral and medial parts of the brain, because of the functional heterogeneity property of the brain. 2) Overlapping FNs may show different, or even opposite (i.e., up versus down), task-related modulations in timecourses, because of the balanced E/I property. 3) FNs not showing task-related modulations over time may occupy most or the whole brain volume and extensively overlap with FNs exhibiting significant modulations, because of the sparseness of neuronal activity property. 4) Older participants would show smaller, but not greater, modulations in FN timecourse relative to the younger adults at selected contrasts of task conditions, because of altered excitation and inhibition and impaired cognitive function in older relative to younger adults. Findings from sICA would demonstrate that reduced task-related suppression of FNs contributed to the GLM finding of hyperactivation in older relative to younger adults. For qualitative comparison of the findings from sICA and GLM, we also applied a GLM to the two datasets and present the GLM findings in the supplementary materials.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and fMRI tasks
Two fMRI datasets were downloaded from public repositories. The first dataset related to manual responses to auditory stimuli and the second to a false-belief task (FBT). Previous GLM findings indicate that tasks used in the first and second dataset should activate the lateral (i.e., task-positive network) and the medial (i.e., default mode network (DMN) or task-negative network) part of the brain, respectively (Fox et al., 2005; Mason et al., 2007; Raichle, 2015). We selected the two tasks that should activate different brain regions to demonstrate extensive overlap of large-scale FNs representing a general property of brain functional organization.
Audiovisual speech perception task (AVSPT)
This dataset was downloaded from figshare (http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.1181943) (Baum and Beauchamp, 2014), and was acquired from 14 younger (20–39 years, 6 female, mean age 26.1 years) and 19 older adults (53–70 years, 12 female, mean age 63.0 years) during performance of an AVSPT. The task used a rapid event-related design, and consisted of McGurk (auditory “pa” + visual “ka”, auditory “ba” + visual “ga”), non-McGurk congruent (“ba”, “ga”, “da”, “pa”, “ka” and “ta”), non-McGurk incongruent (auditory “ka” + visual “pa”, auditory “ga” + visual “ba”), target (audiovisual “ma” in younger subjects or “press” in older adults), and fixation (fixing on crosshairs) trials. Participants were required to press a button as a response to the targets and do nothing in response to other stimuli. Behavioral data were missing for two younger subjects. Available data indicated that the accuracy was near ceiling (10/12 younger adults at 100% accuracy, 18/19 older adults at 100% accuracy, with no significant difference between groups, t29 = 0.6, p = 0.57), and that all participants were alert in the scanner. The duration for each stimulus varied from 1.7 to 1.8 seconds followed by fixation crosshairs for the remainder of the trial. Each participant had two functional runs. The duration for each run was 456 ~ 596 seconds and consisted of 10 ~ 20 target trials, 25 ~ 55 McGurk trials, and 40 ~ 75 non-McGurk trials. Please see (Baum and Beauchamp, 2014) for a more detailed description of participant characteristics, task conditions, and task performance.
False-belief task (FBT)
This dataset was downloaded from https://openfmri.org (ds000109) (Moran et al., 2012) and was acquired from healthy younger and older participants. The younger group includes 21 subjects (mean age = 22.7 years, range: 18–37; female = 15) while the older group includes 12 subjects (mean age = 72 years; range: 65 ~ 88; female = 5). The participants performed a FBT using a block design. The task required the participants read a short story, remember the content, and answer questions about the mental status of a character described in the story (Moran et al., 2012). The duration for each story-reading and question-answering block was 10 and 6 seconds, respectively. There was a variable delay of 0–6 seconds between the two blocks. The crucial test in the FBT was for participants to answer the question from the perspective of the story character and not from their own perspective. The control condition, i.e., false photograph stories task (FPT), was isomorphic to the FBT in that participants again read stories describing a state of the world and answered questions about that content after a variable delay. The critical difference between conditions was that the FPT contained an incorrect physical description of the world rather than a false belief (or mental state) about the world. During scanning, participants answered questions more accurately for the FPT relative to the FBT, and the older group showed a trend reflecting worse performance on the FBT relative to the younger group, but not on the FPT. Please see (Moran et al., 2012) for a more detailed description of task conditions, participant characteristics, and task performance. In addition, some sICA findings from this dataset have been reported (Xu, 2015). However, the data reported in the current paper were not described in the previous publication.
2.2. Imaging data acquisition
AVSPT
The imaging data were acquired using a 3T scanner (Phillips Medical Systems). A T2* weighed gradient echo-planar imaging sequence (TR= 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle =90°, in plane resolution: 2.75 × 2.75 mm2) was used to acquire blood-oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signal. Each participant performed two functional runs in the scanner. T1-weighted MP-RAGE high-resolution images were also acquired.
FBT
The imaging data were acquired using a 3T Trio scanner (Siemens). BOLD signals were acquired using a gradient echo-planar sequence (TR=2000 ms, TE = 35 ms, in plane resolution: 3 × 3 mm3, slice thickness: 3 mm with 0.54 mm skip, and 36 slice). Each participant had two runs and each run acquired 179 volumes.
2.3. ICA procedures
SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Welcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London) was used to preprocess imaging data. Each BOLD timeseries in each dataset was motion-corrected, normalized to the MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) template, and smoothed with a 6-mm Gaussian kernel. The group ICA algorithm (GIFT, http://mialab.mrn.org/software/gift, version1.3h) was used to extract spatially independent components (ICs) from the preprocessed fMRI timeseries of each dataset (Calhoun et al., 2001; Calhoun et al., 2009). Data from all participants of each dataset were concatenated into a single dataset and reduced using two stages of principal component analysis (PCA) (Calhoun et al., 2001). We extracted 75 ICs from each dataset by using the Infomax algorithm (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995). This high-model-order ICA generated refined components consistent with known anatomical and functional segmentations of the brain (Abou-Elseoud et al., 2010; Allen et al., 2011; Balsters et al., 2013; Ciccarelli et al., 2009; Kiviniemi et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2009; Ystad et al., 2010). The Infomax algorithm generated a spatial map and a timecourse of the source signal changes for each IC. For each dataset, this analysis was repeated 50 times using ICASSO for assessing the repeatability of ICs (Supplemental Fig. 1) (Himberg et al., 2004), and the most representative run was selected for further analysis. Finally, subject-specific IC timecourses and spatial maps were back-reconstructed using the GICA3 method (Calhoun et al., 2001; Erhardt et al., 2011; Meda et al., 2009). GICA3 derives subject-specific timecourses and spatial maps directly from the PCA and ICA. The product of PCA and ICA is used to predict the subject data to the accuracy of information retained from the PCA. The aggregate spatial map is the sum of the subjectspecific spatial maps, analogous to a random effects model where the subjectspecific effects are zero-mean distributed deviations from the group mean effect. GICA3 provides a natural interpretation for the subject-specific timecourses and spatial maps (Allen et al., 2011; Erhardt et al., 2011). The detailed algorithms of GICA3 were presented in a previous paper (Erhardt et al., 2011).
Each IC was visually inspected for separating artifacts from functional networks (FNs). Our diagnostic criteria for artifacts were consistent with the criteria used previously (Allen et al., 2011). ICs with peak voxels in white matter and/or cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) were diagnosed as artifacts. A small dynamic range of power spectra and/or small ratio of the integral of spectral power below 0.10 Hz to the integral of power between 0.15 and 0.25 Hz were used as indicators of artifacts (Allen et al., 2011; Robinson et al., 2009). For defining brain regions associated with each IC, we used the GIFT one-sample t-test tool to create a group-level t-map for each IC. The significance threshold was set at voxel height p<0.001, false-discovery-rate (FDR)-corrected for multiple comparisons of voxel-wise whole-brain analysis.
2.4. Assessing FN overlap
Since each IC has a positive and a negative element, the above-described t-map of each IC contains significant clusters with positive and negative t values. In this paper, we call the positive and negative clusters in the t-map as positive and negative sub-networks, respectively, and each sub-network represents one FN. In the following text of this paper, the term IC refers to a component extracted by sICA, and each IC includes both positive and negative subnetworks. In contrast, FN refers to either the positive or negative sub-network of each IC. We converted each sub-network into a binary mask. Only significant voxels in each sub-network surviving the statistical threshold described above were converted into voxels with a value of one in the output mask, while all other voxels were converted into voxels with values of zero. Two masks were generated for each t-map: one for positive clusters and one for negative clusters. We added these masks together for assessing FN overlap in each dataset. Within the output map, any voxels with a value of two or higher indicated that two or more FNs overlapped at this voxel.
2.5. Assessing task-related modulation over timecourses
For sICA temporal sorting, SPM12 was used to create a design matrix for each subject. The design matrix represents the onset of each trial or task block during fMRI. For the AVSPT, McGurk, non-McGurk, and target trials were modeled explicitly as different event-related trials, and fixations were modeled implicitly as baseline. For the FBT, blocks related to reading FBT and FPT were explicitly modeled as different task blocks, as were the blocks for answering related questions, while the resting blocks were implicitly modeled as baseline conditions. The temporal sorting function from GIFT was used to perform a multiple regression analysis between the IC timecourse and the design matrix for each participant. For each IC, this regression generated a beta-weight value for each trial type or block of each functional run. These beta-weight values represent the correlations between IC timecourses and the canonical hemodynamic response model of task conditions, and index the engagement of ICs during specific task conditions (Meda et al., 2009). An increase or decrease in beta-weight values in one task condition relative to another indicates an increase or decrease in task-related activity in the IC.
The beta-weights of each IC for each task condition across multiple runs for each subject were averaged to generate mean beta-weights for each participant, which were used to calculate the group means for each task condition. We used SPSS (v. 21, IBM SPSS) GLM for repeated measure to assess the main effect of task condition and group (i.e., younger vs. older group), and the two-way interaction effect of group-by-task condition. In this study, we assessed beta-weight changes between McGurk vs. target trials for the AVSPT, and FBT blocks versus FPT blocks for the FBT. The significance threshold was p<0.05, using FDR algorithm for correction of multiple comparisons due to multiple ICs. A significant increase in beta-weight indicates a task-related upmodulation of the timecourse or an increase in activity of the positive subnetwork of the IC during a specific task condition relative to another task condition. Therefore, the positive sub-network of this IC was designated as a positive FN, and the negative sub-network as a negative FN. A significant decrease in beta-weight indicates a task-related down-modulation of the timecourse or a decrease in activity of the positive sub-network of the IC. Therefore, the positive sub-network of this IC was designated as a negative FN, and the negative sub-network as a positive FN. If the beta-weight was not significantly different between the two task conditions after correction for multiple comparisons, then the IC is a neutral IC and both of its sub-networks are neutral FNs. For assessing the volume of all positive FNs, we grouped all positive FNs into one brain space, and the total number of voxels covered by any positive FN represented the total volume of all positive FNs. A similar method was used to calculate the total volume of negative FNs and neutral FNs.
3. Results
3.1. Audiovisual speech perception task (AVSPT)
We analyzed task-related modulations of 36 ICs after excluding ICs diagnosed as artifacts and ICs mainly involving the cerebellum. The reason for excluding cerebellum ICs was to reduce the number of comparisons. Sixteen of the ICs (IC1, 4, 8, 17, 25, 27, 32, 33, 36, 39, 41, 42, 44, 45, 50, and 51) showed a significant main effect of task (please see Supplemental Fig. 2 and Table 2 for detailed spatial maps and statistics). All ICs, except IC44, exhibited upmodulation in timecourses for target vs. McGurk trials (Fig. 2A, B & Fig. 3). Therefore, they were task-positive ICs as defined in the current study, and their positive and negative sub-networks were task-positive and task-negative FNs, respectively. IC44 showed significant down-modulation at target versus McGurk trials (Fig. 2C). Therefore, it was a task-negative IC and its positive and negative sub-networks were task-negative and task-positive FNs, respectively.
Fig. 2.
Examples of ICs exhibiting significant modulations during AVSPT performance. Each of rows A, B, & C presents an IC exhibiting significant taskrelated modulations at Target relative to McGurk trials. The first column shows the spatial pattern of each labeled IC, the second column shows the mean and SE of beta weights at McGurk and Target trials, and the third column shows the mean timecourses of BOLD signals within 15 seconds after the onset of McGurk or Target trials.
Fig. 3.
An IC showing different modulations between the younger and older groups during AVSPT performance. A presents the spatial pattern of the labeled IC. B shows the mean and SE of beta weights at McGurk and Target trials. C shows the mean timecourses of BOLD signals within 15 seconds after the onset of McGurk or Target trials.
One task-positive IC (i.e., IC27) showed a significant main effect of group and two-way interaction effect of task-by-group (Fig. 3 & Supplemental Table 2). The effects were mainly due to a greater task-related up-modulation of its timecourse in the younger relative to the older group at target versus McGurk trials (Fig. 3). Therefore, its positive and negative sub-network showed a greater increase and decrease, respectively, in BOLD signal in the younger relative to the older group at target versus McGurk trials. The positive sub-network of this IC mainly involved the anterior insula and adjacent ventrolateral PFC, medial PFC, temperoparietal junction (TPJ), and caudate heads, bilaterally, while its negative sub-network mainly involved the bilateral superior frontal sulcus. The remaining 20 of the 36 ICs were task-neutral ICs; i.e., they did not show significant taskrelated modulations. Therefore, their positive and negative sub-networks were task-neutral FNs.
All task-positive FNs combined together covered about 64.9% of all brain volume analyzed in this study and included both medial and lateral parts of each cortical lobe and subcortical regions such as the thalamus, basal ganglia, and midbrain (Fig. 4A & Table 1). Furthermore, two or more task-positive FNs overlapped extensively at multiple cortical and subcortical regions including the dorsomedial PFC, precuneus/posterior cingulate (PCC), insula, and TPJ, bilaterally. These regions probably represent the interaction areas of synergistic FNs related to different mental processes for successful task performance.
Fig. 4.
Spatial distributions of task-positive, task-negative, and task-neutral functional networks (FNs). A, B, and C. The color on the brain images shows regions covered by task-positive, task-negative, and task-neutral FNs, respectively. The color bar indicates the number of overlapping FNs.
Table 1.
Volumes of different FNs and their overlaps (cm3)
Tasks* | Positive FNs |
Negative FNs |
Neutral FNs |
Posi & Nega |
Posi & Neut |
Nega & Neut |
PNN |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AVSPT (%) |
985.9 (64.9) |
451.3 (29.7) |
1184.5 (78.0) |
275.5 (18.1) |
699.9 (46.1) |
340.1 (22.4) |
212.5 (14.0) |
FBT (%) |
552.6 (33.3) |
277.1 (16.7) |
1628.9 (98.2) |
139.8 (8.4) |
528.4 (31.9) |
268.1 (16.2) |
135.1 (8.1) |
For AVSPT and FBT, the total brain volumes occupied by all selected ICs (i.e., 36 for AVSPT and 40 for FBT) are 1518.8 cm3 and 1658.6 cm3, respectively. Number in each pair of parenthesis indicates the % of total brain volumes of all selected ICs of each dataset.
Abbreviations: Nega & Neut: overlap of negative and neutral FNs; Posi & Nega: overlap of positive and negative FNs; Posi & Neut: overlap of positive and neutral FNs; PNN: overlap of positive, negative, and neutral FNs.
Task-negative FNs were less extensive than the task-positive FNs. They covered about 29.7% of the studied brain volume and involved all major divisions of the brain, including the medial, lateral, dorsal, and ventral cortical regions (Fig. 4B & Table 1). Similar to the task-positive FNs, two or more task-negative FNs may overlap at the same locations. In addition, task-negative and task-positive FNs overlapped at multiple regions including the medial PFC, frontal eye fields, PCC/precuneus, ventrolateral PFC, and posterior insula (Fig. 4A, B). This finding indicates that these regions probably represent interaction areas of FNs involved in different functions.
Relative to the task-positive and task-negative FNs, the task-neutral FNs occupied the most extensive brain regions (i.e., ~78.0% of all analyzed brain volume), including most cortical and subcortical regions (Fig. 4C & Table 1). Many of them overlapped with each other extensively at multiple cortical and subcortical locations, including the intraparietal sulcus, medial frontoparietal cortex, and dorsolateral PFC. Furthermore, the task-neutral, task-positive, and task-negative FNs overlapped at several brain regions including the medial and ventrolateral PFC, frontal eye fields, and PCC, as revealed by visual inspection of Fig. 4A, B, and C and indicated in Table 1.
3.2. False-belief task (FBT)
We analyzed task-related modulations of 40 ICs after exclusion of ICs diagnosed as artifacts and ICs mainly involving the cerebellum. Five (IC 25, 42, 52, 57, and 60) of the 40 ICs showed a significant main effect of task (please see Supplemental Fig. 3 and Table 2 for spatial maps and statistics). They exhibited up-modulation in their timecourses during FBT versus FPT blocks and therefore were task-positive ICs (Fig. 5). The remaining 35 of the 40 ICs did not show significant task-related modulations and therefore were task-neutral ICs.
Fig. 5.
Examples of ICs exhibiting significant modulations during FBT performance. Each of the rows A, B, & C presents an IC exhibiting significant task-related modulations during FBT relative to FPT blocks. The first column shows the spatial pattern of each labeled IC, the second column the mean and SE of beta weights during FBT and FPT blocks, and the third column the mean timecourses of BOLD signals within 30 seconds after the onset of FBT or FPT blocks.
Three task-neutral ICs (IC12, 62, and 66) showed a significant effect of group (please see Supplemental Fig. 3 and Table 2 for spatial maps and statistics). Two of them (IC12 and 62) exhibited a greater up-modulation in the younger relative to the older group during both FBT and FPT blocks (Fig. 6A, B). The positive sub-network of IC12 mainly involves the medial visual cortex. The positive sub-network of IC62 mainly involves the left temporal cortex and bilateral dorsolateral PFC, while its negative sub-network is the right TPJ. The remaining IC (IC66) showed a greater down-modulation in the younger relative to the older group during both task blocks (Fig. 6C). The positive sub-network of this IC mainly involves the TPJ and lateral PFC, while its negative sub-network mainly involves the precuneus and medial PFC. No ICs showed a significant two-way interaction of task-by-group.
Fig. 6.
ICs showing different modulations between the younger and older groups during FBT. A, B, and C each presents an IC showing a significant difference in task-related modulations during FBT and FPT blocks. Please see the legend for Fig. 5 for more details.
All task-positive FNs combined together covered about 33.3% of the studied brain volumes (Table 1), and they covered more posterior portions of the brain such as the parietal and temporal cortices than the anterior part of the brain such as the PFC (Fig. 7A). Consistent with the findings from the AVSPT, sICA showed extensive overlap of task-positive FNs at multiple cortical and subcortical regions including the precuneus and TPJ, bilaterally, indicating interaction of synergistic FNs. Task-negative FNs covered about 16.7% of the studied brain volume including frontal, parietal, and temporal cortices (Fig. 7B & Table 1). Consistent with findings from the first task, some task-negative FNs overlapped with each other, and overlapped with task-positive FNs at multiple regions including the precuneus/PCC and parietal and temporal cortices (Fig. 7A, B). Relative to the task-positive and task-negative FNs, the task-neutral FNs occupied the most extensive brain regions (i.e., ~98.2% of all studied brain volume), including almost all cortical and subcortical volumes (Fig. 7C & Table 1). They showed extensive overlap with each other, and overlap with task-positive and task-negative FNs.
Fig. 7.
Spatial distribution of task-positive, task-negative, and task-neutral functional networks (FNs). A, B, and C. The color on the brain images shows regions covered by task-positive, task-negative, and task-neutral FNs, respectively. The color bar above row A indicates the number of overlapping FNs for task-positive and task-negative FNs, and the color bar beneath row C indicates the number of overlapping task-neutral FNs.
4. Discussion
4.1. Main findings
The main findings revealed by sICA are highly consistent with our predictions and they include: 1) extensive overlap of task-positive, task-negative, and task-neutral FNs, 2) extensive task-neutral FNs covering most of the analyzed brain volumes, and 3) smaller task-related modulation of FNs in the older relative to the younger group.
4.2. FN overlap
Two lines of data are regularly cited to support the modular theory of brain functional organization. The first is related to cortical columns; i.e., cortical neurons in a vertical cluster often respond to the same attributes of peripheral stimulation (Horton and Adams, 2005; Mountcastle, 1997; Szentagothai, 1983). The second is related to topographic maps of peripheral receptive fields in the sensory cortex, such as the retinotopic map in the primary visual cortex (Hubel and Wiesel, 1968). However, many studies find that dense horizontal connections exist between cortical columns, that intermixed neurons in the same columns respond to different attributes of peripheral stimulation, and that multiple microcircuits occupy the same space and interlace with each other (Boucsein et al., 2011; Harris and Mrsic-Flogel, 2013; Hermansen et al., 2007; Horton and Adams, 2005; Perin et al., 2011). Therefore, both histological and electrophysiological studies indicate that neurons in the same cortical columns are heterogeneous in functional activities and may join in different microcircuits. Furthermore, the same sensory cortex may contain multiple topographic maps representing different attributes of external inputs. For example, topographic maps for space, color, ocular dominance, motion orientation, and spatial frequency superimpose on each other in the primary visual cortex (Swindale, 1998, 2000). These data strongly indicate that large-scale FNs overlap with each other, instead of being isolated from each other (Fuster, 2009; Fuster and Bressler, 2015; Swindale, 1998, 2000). Consistent with these histological and electrophysiological findings, extensive overlap of large-scale FNs has been reported by recent fMRI studies using sICA and several other multivariate analytical approaches capable of separating intermixed signals as reviewed in the introduction (Beldzik et al., 2013; Geranmayeh et al., 2014; Xu et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2013b). The extensive FN overlap revealed in the current studies provide further support to our hypothesis that large-scale FN overlap is a general property of brain functional organization.
The current and previous sICA studies find that more FNs overlap in the higher-order association cortex relative to the primary sensory cortex (Beldzik et al., 2013; Geranmayeh et al., 2014; Xu et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2013b). These brain regions often exhibit more “edges” (i.e., functional connectivity) with more diverse brain regions relative to the primary sensory cortex in functional connectivity studies using seed-based approaches (Crossley et al., 2014; van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2013). It has been proposed that these brain regions represent “cores or hubs” of brain FNs, and that they may play central roles in brain communication and neuropsychiatric disorders (Crossley et al., 2014; Rubinov and Bullmore, 2013). The co-localization of FN overlap and “cores or hubs” indicates that common neural substrates may underlie these different findings using different analytical methods. Relative to the primary sensory cortex, the association cortex contains more heterogeneous neurons, receiving inputs from and sending outputs to more diverse brain regions (Augustine, 1996; Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Katsuki and Constantinidis, 2012; Petrides, 2005). We propose that the highly heterogeneous neurons and their divergent and convergent inputs and outputs in the association cortex probably underlie more FN overlap as revealed by sICA and more functional connectivity (i.e., edges) as revealed by seed-based functional connectivity analyses. However, we acknowledge that the exact relations between brain microcircuits and large-scale FNs generated by fMRI are not clear, in part related to an incomplete understanding of neurovascular coupling.
FN overlap suggests that local connections, not just long-rang projections, contribute to functional connectivity among different FNs. Local fibers are much denser than long-range fibers and may play a greater role in FN connectivity than long-rang fibers (Buzsaki et al., 2004; Levy and Reyes, 2012; Watkins et al., 2014). Therefore, changes in functional and anatomical connectivity measured using BOLD signal and diffusion MRI (dMRI), respectively, may demonstrate different patterns in patients with neuropsychiatric disorders (Fornito and Bullmore, 2015), because BOLD signal correlations probably reflect both local and long-range connectivity, but dMRI may preferentially reflect long-range (i.e., white-matter) fiber tracts (Alexander et al., 2007; Mori et al., 2009).
4.3. FN modulation
Overlapping FNs may show modulations in the same direction; e.g., up-modulations at target trials relative to McGurk trials. In this scenario, a GLM may detect task-related changes in BOLD within the overlapping regions, and thus the two approaches may reveal consistent findings. On the other hand, overlapping FNs may show opposite modulations during the same task conditions. This finding is novel relative to most fMRI studies using GLM but consistent with the properties of balanced E/I and functional heterogeneity in the brain (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Swindale, 1998). These task-related concurrent co-localized opposite changes may cancel each other, and thus a GLM may not show significant changes in BOLD signal in the overlapping regions. Therefore, sICA may reveal functional activities hidden from GLM.
Previous fMRI studies using GLM have repeatedly reported task-related opposite modulations of BOLD signal in different brain regions; e.g., in the default mode vs. task-positive networks (Fox et al., 2006; Raichle, 2015). However, sICA findings indicate that opposite-modulated FNs do not need to be separated in space, and they actually overlap at both medial and lateral brain regions extensively. We posit that this sICA finding is more consistent with the known brain anatomy relative to the GLM findings of segregated FNs. For example, most cortical inhibitory neurons are local interneurons and do not project to other brain regions (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Okun and Lampl, 2008). The inhibitory interactions among different cortical regions are mainly performed by excitatory pyramidal neurons. It is logical to predict that, for one cortical region to exert inhibitory effect on another region, it sends long-range axons to activate local inhibitory interneurons in the target brain region, and the activation of the inhibitory neurons in turn suppresses the activity of adjacent pyramidal neurons in the target brain regions. Therefore, excitation and inhibition among adjacent cortical neurons should correlate with each other as stipulated by balanced E/I (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Okun and Lampl, 2008). The task-related BOLD signal reduction in a region should be interpreted as a decrease in the total activities of its neuronal populations; however, some neurons in the region (e.g., the inhibitory interneurons) may increase activity. Therefore, overlapping FNs with different task-related modulations as revealed by sICA probably more accurately reflect functional activities in the brain relative to segregated FNs as revealed by GLM, and this knowledge may help reconcile conflicting GLM findings related to anti-correlation between activities in the lateral vs. medial frontoparietal cortex as reviewed in the introduction.
4.4. Neutral FNs
SICA found that most FNs were task-neutral and did not show significant task-related modulations. These task-neutral FNs covered most of the brain and overlapped with task-positive and task-negative FNs. This finding is different from typical GLM findings of brain regions exhibiting no task-related activity as being restricted and separated from brain regions showing task-related activity, but is consistent with the property of sparseness of neuronal responses in the brain. This brain property indicates that only a small percentage (<10%) of the entire neuronal population in any brain region may be able to fire action potentials at any moment (Barth and Poulet, 2012; Crochet et al., 2011; Tolhurst et al., 2009). Therefore, most large-scale FNs may not show taskrelated changes in timecourses during any task.
4.5. Older vs. younger groups
As reviewed in the introduction, fMRI studies using GLM have reported both greater and smaller task-related activities in older relative to younger adults, and have interpreted the two opposite findings as compensation of impaired functional efficiency and impaired function, respectively (Reuter-Lorenz and Cappell, 2008; Toepper et al., 2014). However, most of these studies did not consider the possibility of balanced E/I in the same voxels and that increased BOLD signal may relate to reduced deactivation. In the current study, the GLM found that, relative to the younger group, the older group exhibited a smaller deactivation or greater activation during different task conditions (please see supplementary result and Fig. 4 and 5). These findings are consistent with the interpretation of functional compensation in the older adults. GLM also found a smaller task-related increase in activation in the older relative to the younger adults (please see supplementary result and Fig. 4 and 5), consistent with the interpretation of impaired function in older adults. However, sICA found reduced activation and deactivation, not increased activation and deactivation, in the older relative to the younger group at all task conditions. The findings from applying sICA to both tasks are highly consistent, suggest impaired function in older relative to younger adults, and do not support the functional compensation theory, while GLM revealed findings suggesting both impaired function and functional compensation. Therefore, the consistent findings from sICA may help reconcile inconsistent findings from GLM. We acknowledge that comparing findings from sICA and GLM alone may not determine whether sICA or GLM findings more accurately reflect real events in the brain, and this issue should be further investigated in future studies.
4.6. Methodological considerations
Most fMRI studies have used GLM since the invention of fMRI about 25 years ago, and multiple studies have assessed the test-retest reliability of GLM. The overall conclusion is that GLM findings at the group level are highly reliable (Brandt et al., 2013; Caceres et al., 2009; Gountouna et al., 2010; Plichta et al., 2012; Upadhyay et al., 2015). While sICA is less frequently employed relative to GLM, group sICA is one of the two most popular methods (the other is seed-based approach) for studying largescale brain functional networks in fMRI (Erhardt et al., 2011; Franco et al., 2013). Multiple studies have consistently reported high test-retest reliability of spatial maps and related timecourses generated by sICA (Franco et al., 2013; Moodie et al., 2014; Poppe et al., 2013; Wisner et al., 2013; Zuo et al., 2010; Zuo and Xing, 2014). Therefore, both GLM and sICA are reliable analytical approaches for fMRI studies. However, they have different assumptions on relationships between BOLD signal and neuronal activities from the same voxels. GLM treats each voxel as a single unit and assumes the increases and decreases in BOLD signal reflect increases and decreases in neuronal activity, respectively. Therefore, it is not particularly sensitive to functional heterogeneity, balanced E/I, and sparseness of responses in a voxel. In contrast, sICA assumes that the BOLD signal from each voxel represents a linear mixture of different source signals in the voxel, and separates the source signals into maximally independent components using higher-order statistics (Calhoun et al., 2002; McKeown et al., 1998b), and generates overlapping FNs with different timecourses. However, the exact relationship between this sICA finding and heterogeneous neuronal activities and balanced E/I in the same brain regions has not been validated and should be investigated further in future studies.
SICA is arguably more popular than temporal ICA (tICA), perhaps in part because fMRI has many more voxels than time points, and ICA needs a large number of samples for optimal performance (Calhoun and Adali, 2012; Smith et al., 2012). SICA enforces spatial independence and has several limitations. The first is that the optimal number of ICs existing in each dataset is not known and different participants may have different optimal number of ICs. However, several studies indicated that a small change (5–10) in the number of ICs does not appear to change the pattern of each IC, and that ICA can extract physiologically interpretable ICs across a wide range of decomposing dimensions (Abou-Elseoud et al., 2010; Calhoun et al., 2008; Kiviniemi et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2009). While the number of ICs may influence the extent of FN overlap, the proposed framework should be supported if we used a smaller number of ICs because extensive FN overlaps were demonstrated in previous studies using a lower dimension of sICA (Beldzik et al., 2013; Geranmayeh et al., 2014; Xu et al., 2015; Xu et al., 2013b). The second is the uncertainty of an optimal statistical threshold for defining task-positive, task-negative, and task-neutral FNs. Using a strict threshold for positive and negative FNs may lead to a liberal threshold for neutral FNs and vice versa. Therefore, the number of FNs in each category may change depending on the selection of different thresholds. However, the final conclusion of extensive overlap of different FNs still holds at different thresholds. The third is that sICA may not always detect significant changes in BOLD signal revealed by a GLM, because sICA may split the synergistic changes into several ICs and make them less detectable. Therefore, GLM and sICA each appear to have advantages and disadvantages relative to the other in detecting different aspects of brain functional activity. Findings from the two approaches may complement each other and provide a more complete and consistent picture regarding brain functional organization than either method alone. In addition to the limitation of sICA, the findings of this study were limited by the small sample size of the younger group in the AVSPT dataset and the older group in the FBT dataset. Therefore, these findings should be verified in larger samples in future studies.
5. Summary
This study provides further evidence supporting our hypothesis that sICA may reconcile inconsistent GLM findings on the anti-correlations of functional activities at the medial and lateral brain regions, and that large-scale FN overlap is a general property of brain functional organization. This property does not support the traditional modular view of brain functional organization, but is compatible with several fundamental properties in the brain including functional heterogeneity, balanced E/I, and sparseness of neuronal responses. Furthermore, this is the first study to demonstrate that sICA may reconcile inconsistent GLM findings on task-related hyper- vs. hypo-activation in older relative to the young adults. By separating concurrent co-localized opposite signal, sICA revealed a novel framework for brain functional organization. It indicated more extensive brain regions and more complicated neuronal events (e.g., CCAD) in each region underlying cognitive functions relative to the picture provided by GLM. However, we acknowledge that the relationships between FN overlap, balanced E/I and functional heterogeneity are hypothetical at present. Since these brain properties have been defined based on functional activity in microcircuits, their relationships with large-scale networks as defined by fMRI should be further investigated in future studies.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
SICA reveals extensive overlap of functional networks (FNs) in the brain.
FN overlap probably reflects a fundamental property of the brain.
Knowledge of FN overlap may help reconcile previous inconsistent fMRI findings.
FN overlap represents a novel framework for brain functional organization.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported in part by grants K01DA027750, R01DA039136, R01DA020908, R01DA035058, P20DA027844, P20GM103472, and P50DA09241 from the National Institutes of Health; the Connecticut State Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services; the Connecticut Mental Health Center; CASAColumbia; and a Center of Excellence in Gambling Research Award from the National Center for Responsible Gaming. Thanks Drs. Sarah H. Baum and Michael S. Beauchamp for helping understand and analyze AVSPT dataset. The openfmri was funded by a grant from the National Science Foundation (OCI-1331441). The funding agencies did not have input into the content of this manuscript.
Dr. Potenza has consulted for Ironwood, Lundbeck, Shire, INSYS, Rivermend Health, Opiant/Lakelight Therapeutics, and Pfizer; has received research support from the Pfizer, Mohegan Sun Casino and the National Center for Responsible Gaming; has participated in surveys, mailings or telephone consultations related to drug addiction, impulse-control disorders or other health topics; has consulted for gambling and legal entities on issues related to impulse-control/addictive disorders; provides clinical care in a problem gambling services program; has performed grant reviews for the National Institutes of Health and other agencies; has edited journals and journal sections; has given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events and other clinical or scientific venues; and has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Disclosures: None of the authors have any relevant financial disclosures.
References
- Abou-Elseoud A, Starck T, Remes J, Nikkinen J, Tervonen O, Kiviniemi V. The effect of model order selection in group PICA. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31:1207–1216. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Field AS. Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics. 2007;4:316–329. doi: 10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen EA, Erhardt EB, Damaraju E, Gruner W, Segall JM, Silva RF, Havlicek M, Rachakonda S, Fries J, Kalyanam R, Michael AM, Caprihan A, Turner JA, Eichele T, Adelsheim S, Bryan AD, Bustillo J, Clark VP, Feldstein Ewing SW, Filbey F, Ford CC, Hutchison K, Jung RE, Kiehl KA, Kodituwakku P, Komesu YM, Mayer AR, Pearlson GD, Phillips JP, Sadek JR, Stevens M, Teuscher U, Thoma RJ, Calhoun VD. A baseline for the multivariate comparison of resting-state networks. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5:2. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JS, Druzgal TJ, Lopez-Larson M, Jeong EK, Desai K, Yurgelun-Todd D. Network anticorrelations, global regression, and phase-shifted soft tissue correction. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:919–934. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Poldrack RA. Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: Role of the subthalamic nucleus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:2424. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Augustine JR. Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain Research Reviews. 1996;22:229. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(96)00011-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balodis IM, Potenza MN. Anticipatory Reward Processing in Addicted Populations: A Focus on the Monetary Incentive Delay Task. Biol Psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balsters JH, Robertson IH, Calhoun VD. BOLD Frequency Power Indexes Working Memory Performance. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:207. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barth AL, Poulet JF. Experimental evidence for sparse firing in the neocortex. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baum SH, Beauchamp MS. Greater BOLD variability in older compared with younger adults during audiovisual speech perception. PLoS One. 2014;9:e111121. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beckmann CF. Modelling with independent components. Neuroimage. 2012;62:891–901. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beldzik E, Domagalik A, Daselaar S, Fafrowicz M, Froncisz W, Oginska H, Marek T. Contributive sources analysis: a measure of neural networks' contribution to brain activations. Neuroimage. 2013;76:304–312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. 1995;7:1129–1159. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.6.1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boucsein C, Nawrot MP, Schnepel P, Aertsen A. Beyond the cortical column: abundance and physiology of horizontal connections imply a strong role for inputs from the surround. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2011;5:32. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady RO, Jr, McCarthy JM, Prescot AP, Jensen JE, Cooper AJ, Cohen BM, Renshaw PF, Ongur D. Brain gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) abnormalities in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2013;15:434–439. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braga RM, Sharp DJ, Leeson C, Wise RJ, Leech R. Echoes of the Brain within Default Mode, Association, and Heteromodal Cortices. J Neurosci. 2013;33:14031–14039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0570-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brandt DJ, Sommer J, Krach S, Bedenbender J, Kircher T, Paulus FM, Jansen A. Test-Retest Reliability of fMRI Brain Activity during Memory Encoding. Front Psychiatry. 2013;4:163. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks JC, Faull OK, Pattinson KT, Jenkinson M. Physiological noise in brainstem FMRI. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:623. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsaki G, Geisler C, Henze DA, Wang XJ. Interneuron Diversity series: Circuit complexity and axon wiring economy of cortical interneurons. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caceres A, Hall DL, Zelaya FO, Williams SC, Mehta MA. Measuring fMRI reliability with the intra-class correlation coefficient. Neuroimage. 2009;45:758–768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T. Multisubject independent component analysis of fMRI: a decade of intrinsic networks, default mode, and neurodiagnostic discovery. IEEE Rev Biomed Eng. 2012;5:60–73. doi: 10.1109/RBME.2012.2211076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, Pekar JJ. A method for making group inferences from functional MRI data using independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2001;14:140–151. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Adali T, Pearlson GD, van Zijl PC, Pekar JJ. Independent component analysis of fMRI data in the complex domain. Magn Reson Med. 2002;48:180–192. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD. Modulation of temporally coherent brain networks estimated using ICA at rest and during cognitive tasks. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29:828–838. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun VD, Liu J, Adali T. A review of group ICA for fMRI data and ICA for joint inference of imaging, genetic, and ERP data. Neuroimage. 2009;45:S163–S172. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cappe C, Morel A, Barone P, Rouiller EM. The thalamocortical projection systems in primate: an anatomical support for multisensory and sensorimotor interplay. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2025–2037. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carbonell F, Bellec P, Shmuel A. Global and system-specific resting-state fMRI fluctuations are uncorrelated: principal component analysis reveals anti-correlated networks. Brain Connect. 2011;1:496–510. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspary DM, Ling L, Turner JG, Hughes LF. Inhibitory neurotransmission, plasticity and aging in the mammalian central auditory system. J Exp Biol. 2008;211:1781–1791. doi: 10.1242/jeb.013581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129:564–583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chai XJ, Castanon AN, Ongur D, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. Neuroimage. 2012;59:1420–1428. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciccarelli O, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Talbot K, Orrell RW, Howard RS, Nunes RG, Miller DH, Matthews PM, Thompson AJ, Smith SM. Investigation of white matter pathology in ALS and PLS using tract-based spatial statistics. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:615–624. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Comon P. Independent component analysis — a new concept? Signal Process. 1994;36:287–314. [Google Scholar]
- Crochet S, Poulet JF, Kremer Y, Petersen CC. Synaptic mechanisms underlying sparse coding of active touch. Neuron. 2011;69:1160–1175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crossley NA, Mechelli A, Scott J, Carletti F, Fox PT, McGuire P, Bullmore ET. The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders. Brain. 2014;137:2382–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dancause N, Barbay S, Frost SB, Plautz EJ, Popescu M, Dixon PM, Stowe AM, Friel KM, Nudo RJ. Topographically divergent and convergent connectivity between premotor and primary motor cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:1057–1068. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Chastelaine M, Mattson JT, Wang TH, Donley BE, Rugg MD. The relationships between age, associative memory performance, and the neural correlates of successful associative memory encoding. Neurobiol Aging. 2016;42:163–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.03.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domagalik A, Beldzik E, Fafrowicz M, Oginska H, Marek T. Neural networks related to pro-saccades and anti-saccades revealed by independent component analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;62:1325–1333. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donoso M, Collins AG, Koechlin E. Human cognition. Foundations of human reasoning in the prefrontal cortex. Science. 2014;344:1481–1486. doi: 10.1126/science.1252254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Driver J, Noesselt T. Multisensory interplay reveals crossmodal influences on 'sensory-specific' brain regions, neural responses, and judgments. Neuron. 2008;57:11–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Druga R. Neocortical inhibitory system. Folia biologica. 2009;55:201–217. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Du Y, Allen EA, He H, Sui J, Wu L, Calhoun VD. Artifact removal in the context of group ICA: A comparison of single-subject and group approaches. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:1005–1025. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edden RA, Crocetti D, Zhu H, Gilbert DL, Mostofsky SH. Reduced GABA concentration in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:750–753. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- El Idrissi A, Shen CH, L'Amoreaux WJ. Neuroprotective role of taurine during aging. Amino Acids. 2013;45:735–750. doi: 10.1007/s00726-013-1544-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ende G, Cackowski S, Van Eijk J, Sack M, Demirakca T, Kleindienst N, Bohus M, Sobanski E, Krause-Utz A, Schmahl C. Impulsivity and Aggression in Female BPD and ADHD Patients: Association with ACC Glutamate and GABA Concentrations. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:410–418. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erhardt EB, Rachakonda S, Bedrick EJ, Allen EA, Adali T, Calhoun VD. Comparison of multi-subject ICA methods for analysis of fMRI data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:2075–2095. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fino E, Packer AM, Yuste R. The logic of inhibitory connectivity in the neocortex. Neuroscientist. 2013;19:228–237. doi: 10.1177/1073858412456743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fino E, Yuste R. Dense inhibitory connectivity in neocortex. Neuron. 2011;69:1188–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Reconciling abnormalities of brain network structure and function in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015;30:44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox KC, Spreng RN, Ellamil M, Andrews-Hanna JR, Christoff K. The wandering brain: meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage. 2015;111:611–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:10046–10051. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604187103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franco AR, Mannell MV, Calhoun VD, Mayer AR. Impact of analysis methods on the reproducibility and reliability of resting-state networks. Brain Connect. 2013;3:363–374. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Funahashi S. Space representation in the prefrontal cortex. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;103:131–155. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM. Cortex and memory: emergence of a new paradigm. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:2047–2072. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuster JM, Bressler SL. Past makes future: role of pFC in prediction. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015;27:639–654. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geranmayeh F, Wise RJ, Mehta A, Leech R. Overlapping Networks Engaged during Spoken Language Production and Its Cognitive Control. J Neurosci. 2014;34:8728–8740. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0428-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert SJ, Bird G, Frith CD, Burgess PW. Does "task difficulty" explain "task-induced deactivation?". Frontiers in psychology. 2012;3:125. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert SJ, Simons JS, Frith CD, Burgess PW. Performance-related activity in medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) during low-demand tasks. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2006;32:45–58. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goense J, Merkle H, Logothetis NK. High-resolution fMRI reveals laminar differences in neurovascular coupling between positive and negative BOLD responses. Neuron. 2012;76:629–639. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez-Burgos G, Cho RY, Lewis DA. Alterations in cortical network oscillations and parvalbumin neurons in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:1031–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gountouna VE, Job DE, McIntosh AM, Moorhead TW, Lymer GK, Whalley HC, Hall J, Waiter GD, Brennan D, McGonigle DJ, Ahearn TS, Cavanagh J, Condon B, Hadley DM, Marshall I, Murray AD, Steele JD, Wardlaw JM, Lawrie SM. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) reproducibility and variance components across visits and scanning sites with a finger tapping task. Neuroimage. 2010;49:552–560. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffanti L, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Beckmann CF, Auerbach EJ, Douaud G, Sexton CE, Zsoldos E, Ebmeier KP, Filippini N, Mackay CE, Moeller S, Xu J, Yacoub E, Baselli G, Ugurbil K, Miller KL, Smith SM. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. Neuroimage. 2014;95:232–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2001;98:4259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:685–694. doi: 10.1038/35094500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutchess AH, Welsh RC, Hedden T, Bangert A, Minear M, Liu LL, Park DC. Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17:84–96. doi: 10.1162/0898929052880048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haider B, Hausser M, Carandini M. Inhibition dominates sensory responses in the awake cortex. Nature. 2013;493:97–100. doi: 10.1038/nature11665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris KD, Mrsic-Flogel TD. Cortical connectivity and sensory coding. Nature. 2013;503:51–58. doi: 10.1038/nature12654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermansen TD, Ventegodt S, Kandel I. Human development XI: the structure of the cerebral cortex. Are there really modules in the brain? The Scientific World Journal. 2007;7:1922–1929. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2007.256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himberg J, Hyvarinen A, Esposito F. Validating the independent components of neuroimaging time series via clustering and visualization. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1214–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hommer DW, Bjork JM, Gilman JM. Imaging brain response to reward in addictive disorders. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1216:50–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05898.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horton JC, Adams DL. The cortical column: a structure without a function. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005;360:837–862. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol. 1968;195:215–243. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inan M, Blazquez-Llorca L, Merchan-Perez A, Anderson SA, DeFelipe J, Yuste R. Dense and overlapping innervation of pyramidal neurons by chandelier cells. J Neurosci. 2013;33:1907–1914. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4049-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacson JS, Scanziani M. How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron. 2011;72:231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N. Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:11163–11170. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005062107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karahanoglu FI, Van De Ville D. Transient brain activity disentangles fMRI resting-state dynamics in terms of spatially and temporally overlapping networks. Nature communications. 2015;6:7751. doi: 10.1038/ncomms8751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karnani MM, Agetsuma M, Yuste R. A blanket of inhibition: functional inferences from dense inhibitory connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;26:96–102. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katsuki F, Constantinidis C. Unique and shared roles of the posterior parietal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitive functions. Frontiers in integrative neuroscience. 2012;6:17. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaup AR, Drummond SP, Eyler LT. Brain functional correlates of working memory: reduced load-modulated activation and deactivation in aging without hyperactivation or functional reorganization. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20:945–950. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. Neuroimage. 2008;39:527–537. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy KM, Rodrigue KM, Bischof GN, Hebrank AC, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. Age trajectories of functional activation under conditions of low and high processing demands: an adult lifespan fMRI study of the aging brain. Neuroimage. 2015;104:21–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim DI, Manoach DS, Mathalon DH, Turner JA, Mannell M, Brown GG, Ford JM, Gollub RL, White T, Wible C, Belger A, Bockholt HJ, Clark VP, Lauriello J, O'Leary D, Mueller BA, Lim KO, Andreasen N, Potkin SG, Calhoun VD. Dysregulation of working memory and default-mode networks in schizophrenia using independent component analysis, an fBIRN and MCIC study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009a;30:3795–3811. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim DI, Mathalon DH, Ford JM, Mannell M, Turner JA, Brown GG, Belger A, Gollub R, Lauriello J, Wible C, O'Leary D, Lim K, Toga A, Potkin SG, Birn F, Calhoun VD. Auditory oddball deficits in schizophrenia: an independent component analysis of the fMRI multisite function BIRN study. Schizophr Bull. 2009b;35:67–81. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiviniemi V, Starck T, Remes J, Long X, Nikkinen J, Haapea M, Veijola J, Moilanen I, Isohanni M, Zang YF, Tervonen O. Functional segmentation of the brain cortex using high model order group PICA. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3865–3886. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lane RD, Reiman EM, Bradley MM, Lang PJ, Ahern GL, Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE. Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:1437–1444. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(97)00070-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leech R, Braga R, Sharp DJ. Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32:215–222. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3689-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legon W, Punzell S, Dowlati E, Adams SE, Stiles AB, Moran RJ. Altered Prefrontal Excitation/Inhibition Balance and Prefrontal Output: Markers of Aging in Human Memory Networks. Cereb Cortex. 2015 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy RB, Reyes AD. Spatial profile of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic connectivity in mouse primary auditory cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32:5609–5619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5158-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li HJ, Hou XH, Liu HH, Yue CL, Lu GM, Zuo XN. Putting age-related task activation into large-scale brain networks: A meta-analysis of 114 fMRI studies on healthy aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;57:156–174. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Limbrick-Oldfield EH, van Holst RJ, Clark L. Fronto-striatal dysregulation in drug addiction and pathological gambling: Consistent inconsistencies? NeuroImage. Clinical. 2013;2:385–393. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis NK. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature. 2008;453:869–878. doi: 10.1038/nature06976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luscher B, Fuchs T. GABAergic control of depression-related brain states. Adv Pharmacol. 2015;73:97–144. doi: 10.1016/bs.apha.2014.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lv J, Jiang X, Li X, Zhu D, Chen H, Zhang T, Zhang S, Hu X, Han J, Huang H, Zhang J, Guo L, Liu T. Sparse representation of whole-brain fMRI signals for identification of functional networks. Med Image Anal. 2015;20:112–134. doi: 10.1016/j.media.2014.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madhyastha T, Cao Y, Sujitnapisatham S, Presnyakov G, Chaovalitwongse WA, Grabowski T for the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging, I. Link Clustering to Explore Brain Dynamics Using Resting State Functional MRI. Journal of radiology and radiation therapy. 2013;1:1012. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markram H, Toledo-Rodriguez M, Wang Y, Gupta A, Silberberg G, Wu C. Interneurons of the neocortical inhibitory system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:793–807. doi: 10.1038/nrn1519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason MF, Norton MI, Van Horn JD, Wegner DM, Grafton ST, Macrae CN. Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science. 2007;315:393–395. doi: 10.1126/science.1131295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayberg HS. Neuroimaging and psychiatry: the long road from bench to bedside. The Hastings Center report Spec No. 2014:S31–S36. doi: 10.1002/hast.296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazoyer B, Zago L, Mellet E, Bricogne S, Etard O, Houde O, Crivello F, Joliot M, Petit L, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Research Bulletin. 2001;54:287. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(00)00437-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeown MJ, Jung TP, Makeig S, Brown G, Kindermann SS, Lee TW, Sejnowski TJ. Spatially independent activity patterns in functional MRI data during the Stroop color-naming task. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1998a;95:803. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeown MJ, Makeig S, Brown GG, Jung TP, Kindermann SS, Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. Analysis of fMRI data by blind separation into independent spatial components. Hum Brain Mapp. 1998b;6:160–188. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:3<160::AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKeown MJ, Sejnowski TJ. Independent component analysis of fMRI data: examining the assumptions. Hum Brain Mapp. 1998;6:368–372. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:5/6<368::AID-HBM7>3.0.CO;2-E. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. J Cog Neuroscience. 2003;15:394. doi: 10.1162/089892903321593117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meda SA, Stevens MC, Folley BS, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD. Evidence for anomalous network connectivity during working memory encoding in schizophrenia: an ICA based analysis. PLoS One. 2009;4:e7911. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menz MM, McNamara A, Klemen J, Binkofski F. Dissociating networks of imitation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3339–3350. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer ML, Spunt RP, Berkman ET, Taylor SE, Lieberman MD. Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:1883–1888. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121077109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moodie CA, Wisner KM, MacDonald AW., 3rd Characteristics of canonical intrinsic connectivity networks across tasks and monozygotic twin pairs. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:5532–5549. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moran JM, Jolly E, Mitchell JP. Social-cognitive deficits in normal aging. J Neurosci. 2012;32:5553–5561. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5511-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mori S, Oishi K, Faria AV. White matter atlases based on diffusion tensor imaging. Curr Opin Neurol. 2009;22:362–369. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32832d954b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mountcastle VB. The columnar organization of the neocortex. Brain. 1997;120(4):701–722. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.4.701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullinger KJ, Mayhew SD, Bagshaw AP, Bowtell R, Francis ST. Poststimulus undershoots in cerebral blood flow and BOLD fMRI responses are modulated by poststimulus neuronal activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:13636–13641. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221287110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullinger KJ, Mayhew SD, Bagshaw AP, Bowtell R, Francis ST. Evidence that the negative BOLD response is neuronal in origin: A simultaneous EEG-BOLD-CBF study in humans. Neuroimage. 2014;94C:263–274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.02.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage. 2009;44:893–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray JD, Anticevic A, Gancsos M, Ichinose M, Corlett PR, Krystal JH, Wang XJ. Linking microcircuit dysfunction to cognitive impairment: effects of disinhibition associated with schizophrenia in a cortical working memory model. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:859–872. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagel IE, Preuschhof C, Li SC, Nyberg L, Backman L, Lindenberger U, Heekeren HR. Load modulation of BOLD response and connectivity predicts working memory performance in younger and older adults. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:2030–2045. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson SB, Valakh V. Excitatory/Inhibitory Balance and Circuit Homeostasis in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neuron. 2015;87:684–698. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Okun M, Lampl I. Instantaneous correlation of excitation and inhibition during ongoing and sensory-evoked activities. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:535–537. doi: 10.1038/nn.2105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packer AM, Yuste R. Dense, unspecific connectivity of neocortical parvalbumin-positive interneurons: a canonical microcircuit for inhibition? J Neurosci. 2011;31:13260–13271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3131-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padberg J, Cerkevich C, Engle J, Rajan AT, Recanzone G, Kaas J, Krubitzer L. Thalamocortical connections of parietal somatosensory cortical fields in macaque monkeys are highly divergent and convergent. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:2038–2064. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parens E, Johnston J. Neuroimaging: beginning to appreciate its complexities. The Hastings Center report Spec No. 2014:S2–S7. doi: 10.1002/hast.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pehrson AL, Sanchez C. Altered gamma-aminobutyric acid neurotransmission in major depressive disorder: a critical review of the supporting evidence and the influence of serotonergic antidepressants. Drug design, development and therapy. 2015;9:603–624. doi: 10.2147/DDDT.S62912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perin R, Berger TK, Markram H. A synaptic organizing principle for cortical neuronal groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:5419–5424. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016051108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pernet CR. Misconceptions in the use of the General Linear Model applied to functional MRI: a tutorial for junior neuro-imagers. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2014;8:1. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessoa L. Understanding brain networks and brain organization. Physics of life reviews. 2014;11:400–435. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2014.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005;360:781–795. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plichta MM, Schwarz AJ, Grimm O, Morgen K, Mier D, Haddad L, Gerdes AB, Sauer C, Tost H, Esslinger C, Colman P, Wilson F, Kirsch P, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Test-retest reliability of evoked BOLD signals from a cognitive-emotive fMRI test battery. Neuroimage. 2012;60:1746–1758. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poline JB, Brett M. The general linear model and fMRI: does love last forever? Neuroimage. 2012;62:871–880. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poppe AB, Wisner K, Atluri G, Lim KO, Kumar V, Macdonald AW., 3rd Toward a neurometric foundation for probabilistic independent component analysis of fMRI data. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2013;13:641–659. doi: 10.3758/s13415-013-0180-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purkayastha P, Malapati A, Yogeeswari P, Sriram D. A Review on GABA/Glutamate Pathway for Therapeutic Intervention of ASD and ADHD. Curr Med Chem. 2015 doi: 10.2174/0929867322666150209152712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME. The brain's default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2015;38:433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ren M, Yoshimura Y, Takada N, Horibe S, Komatsu Y. Specialized inhibitory synaptic actions between nearby neocortical pyramidal neurons. Science. 2007;316:758–761. doi: 10.1126/science.1135468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reuter-Lorenz PA, Cappell KA. Neurocognitive Aging and the Compensation Hypothesis. CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. 2008;17:177–182. [Google Scholar]
- Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24:355–370. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riese F, Gietl A, Zolch N, Henning A, O'Gorman R, Kalin AM, Leh SE, Buck A, Warnock G, Edden RA, Luechinger R, Hock C, Kollias S, Michels L. Posterior cingulate gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamate/glutamine are reduced in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and are unrelated to amyloid deposition and apolipoprotein E genotype. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36:53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson CE, Ratai EM, Kanwisher N. Reduced GABAergic Action in the Autistic Brain. Curr Biol. 2016;26:80–85. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson S, Basso G, Soldati N, Sailer U, Jovicich J, Bruzzone L, Kryspin-Exner I, Bauer H, Moser E. A resting state network in the motor control circuit of the basal ganglia. BMC Neurosci. 2009;10:137. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth G, Dicke U. Evolution of the brain and intelligence in primates. Prog Brain Res. 2012;195:413–430. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00020-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roux L, Buzsaki G. Tasks for inhibitory interneurons in intact brain circuits. Neuropharmacology. 2015;88:10–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowland DC, Moser MB. From cortical modules to memories. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;24:22–27. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubinov M, Bullmore E. Schizophrenia and abnormal brain network hubs. Dialogues in clinical neuroscience. 2013;15:339–349. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.3/mrubinov. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rypma B, D'Esposito M. Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:509–515. doi: 10.1038/74889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rypma B, Prabhakaran V, Desmond JE, Gabrieli JD. Age differences in prefrontal cortical activity in working memory. Psychol Aging. 2001;16:371–384. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.16.3.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sala-Llonch R, Bartres-Faz D, Junque C. Reorganization of brain networks in aging: a review of functional connectivity studies. Frontiers in psychology. 2015;6:663. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MJ, Mirnics K. Neurodevelopment, GABA system dysfunction, and schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:190–206. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schnepel P, Kumar A, Zohar M, Aertsen A, Boucsein C. Physiology and Impact of Horizontal Connections in Rat Neocortex. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25:3818–3835. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serences JT, Saproo S. Computational advances towards linking BOLD and behavior. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:435–446. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shmuel A, Augath M, Oeltermann A, Logothetis NK. Negative functional MRI response correlates with decreases in neuronal activity in monkey visual area V1. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:569–577. doi: 10.1038/nn1675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997;9:648–663. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silberberg G, Markram H. Disynaptic inhibition between neocortical pyramidal cells mediated by Martinotti cells. Neuron. 2007;53:735–746. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Watkins KE, Toro R, Laird AR, Beckmann CF. Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Miller KL, Moeller S, Xu J, Auerbach EJ, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Andersson J, Glasser MF, Van Essen DC, Feinberg DA, Yacoub ES, Ugurbil K. Temporally-independent functional modes of spontaneous brain activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:3131–3136. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121329109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sonuga-Barke EJ, Castellanos FX. Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31:977–986. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, DuPre E, Selarka D, Garcia J, Gojkovic S, Mildner J, Luh WM, Turner GR. Goal-congruent default network activity facilitates cognitive control. J Neurosci. 2014;34:14108–14114. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2815-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim AS. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21:489–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage. 2010;53:303–317. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St Jacques PL, Kragel PA, Rubin DC. Dynamic neural networks supporting memory retrieval. Neuroimage. 2011;57:608–616. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stebbings KA, Choi HW, Ravindra A, Caspary DM, Turner JG, Llano DA. Ageing-related changes in GABAergic inhibition in mouse auditory cortex, measured using in vitro flavoprotein autofluorescence imaging. J Physiol. 2016;594:207–221. doi: 10.1113/JP271221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stebbins GT, Carrillo MC, Dorfman J, Dirksen C, Desmond JE, Turner DA, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Glover G, Gabrieli JD. Aging effects on memory encoding in the frontal lobes. Psychol Aging. 2002;17:44–55. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.17.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stepniewska I, Cerkevich CM, Kaas JH. Cortical Connections of the Caudal Portion of Posterior Parietal Cortex in Prosimian Galagos. Cereb Cortex. 2015 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swindale NV. Cortical organization: modules, polymaps and mosaics. Curr Biol. 1998;8:R270–R273. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(98)70170-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swindale NV. How many maps are there in visual cortex? Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:633–643. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.7.633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szentagothai J. The modular architectonic principle of neural centers. Reviews of physiology, biochemistry and pharmacology. 1983;98:11–61. doi: 10.1007/BFb0033866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Toepper M, Gebhardt H, Bauer E, Haberkamp A, Beblo T, Gallhofer B, Driessen M, Sammer G. The impact of age on load-related dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tohka J, Foerde K, Aron AR, Tom SM, Toga AW, Poldrack RA. Automatic independent component labeling for artifact removal in fMRI. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1227–1245. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tolhurst DJ, Smyth D, Thompson ID. The sparseness of neuronal responses in ferret primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2009;29:2355–2370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3869-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tse MT, Piantadosi PT, Floresco SB. Prefrontal cortical gamma-aminobutyric acid transmission and cognitive function: drawing links to schizophrenia from preclinical research. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:929–939. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turner GR, Spreng RN. Prefrontal Engagement and Reduced Default Network Suppression Co-occur and Are Dynamically Coupled in Older Adults: The Default-Executive Coupling Hypothesis of Aging. J Cogn Neurosci. 2015:1–15. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Upadhyay J, Lemme J, Anderson J, Bleakman D, Large T, Evelhoch JL, Hargreaves R, Borsook D, Becerra L. Test-retest reliability of evoked heat stimulation BOLD fMRI. J Neurosci Methods. 2015;253:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. An anatomical substrate for integration among functional networks in human cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33:14489–14500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2128-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Wageningen H, Jorgensen HA, Specht K, Eichele T, Hugdahl K. The effects of the glutamate antagonist memantine on brain activation to an auditory perception task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3616–3624. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watkins PV, Kao JP, Kanold PO. Spatial pattern of intra-laminar connectivity in supragranular mouse auditory cortex. Front Neural Circuits. 2014;8:15. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2014.00015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whalley HC, Papmeyer M, Sprooten E, Lawrie SM, Sussmann JE, McIntosh AM. Review of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies comparing bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Bipolar Disord. 2012;14:411–431. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01016.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wisner KM, Atluri G, Lim KO, Macdonald AW., 3rd Neurometrics of intrinsic connectivity networks at rest using fMRI: retest reliability and cross-validation using a meta-level method. Neuroimage. 2013;76:236–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe J, Houweling AR, Brecht M. Sparse and powerful cortical spikes. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20:306–312. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu X, Lu J, Chen K, Long Z, Wang X, Shu H, Li K, Liu Y, Yao L. Multiple neural networks supporting a semantic task: an fMRI study using independent component analysis. Neuroimage. 2009;45:1347–1358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J. Implications of cortical balanced excitation and inhibition, functional heterogeneity, and sparseness of neuronal activity in fMRI. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN. Opposite Modulation of Brain Functional Networks Implicated at Low vs. High Demand of Attention and Working Memory. PLoS One. 2014a;9:e87078. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Calhoun VD, Potenza MN. The absence of task-related increases in BOLD signal does not equate to absence of task-related brain activation. J Neurosci Methods. 2014b doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Calhoun VD, Worhunsky PD, Xiang H, Li J, Wall JT, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN. Functional Network Overlap as Revealed by fMRI Using sICA and Its Potential Relationships with Functional Heterogeneity, Balanced Excitation and Inhibition, and Sparseness of Neuron Activity. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0117029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Potenza MN, Calhoun VD. Spatial ICA reveals functional activity hidden from traditional fMRI GLM-based analyses. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2013a;7:154. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu J, Zhang S, Calhoun VD, Monterosso J, Li CS, Worhunsky PD, Stevens M, Pearlson GD, Potenza MN. Task-related concurrent but opposite modulations of overlapping functional networks as revealed by spatial ICA. Neuroimage. 2013b;79:62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yakunina N, Tae WS, Lee KU, Kim SS, Nam EC. Spatiotemporal Segregation of Neural Response to Auditory Stimulation: An fMRI Study Using Independent Component Analysis and Frequency-Domain Analysis. PLoS One. 2013;8:e66424. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yan X, Kelley S, Goldberg M, Biswal BB. Detecting overlapped functional clusters in resting state fMRI with Connected Iterative Scan: a graph theory based clustering algorithm. J Neurosci Methods. 2011;199:108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2011.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Chee MW, Buckner RL. Estimates of segregation and overlap of functional connectivity networks in the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage. 2013;88C:212–227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoldemir B, Ng B, Abugharbieh R. Stable Overlapping Replicator Dynamics for Brain Community Detection. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2015 doi: 10.1109/TMI.2015.2480864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ystad M, Eichele T, Lundervold AJ, Lundervold A. Subcortical functional connectivity and verbal episodic memory in healthy elderly--a resting state fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2010;52:379–388. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S, Li CS. Functional networks for cognitive control in a stop signal task: independent component analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33:89–104. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuo XN, Kelly C, Adelstein JS, Klein DF, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Reliable intrinsic connectivity networks: test-retest evaluation using ICA and dual regression approach. Neuroimage. 2010;49:2163–2177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuo XN, Xing XX. Test-retest reliabilities of resting-state FMRI measurements in human brain functional connectomics: a systems neuroscience perspective. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;45:100–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.