Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2016 Dec 8;11(12):e0166779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166779

Cross Sectional Associations between Socio-Demographic Factors and Cognitive Performance in an Older British Population: The European Investigation of Cancer in Norfolk (EPIC-Norfolk) Study

Shabina A Hayat 1,*, Robert Luben 1, Nichola Dalzell 1, Stephanie Moore 1, Serena Anuj 1, Fiona E Matthews 2, Nick Wareham 3, Carol Brayne 1, Kay-Tee Khaw 1
Editor: Jung Eun Lee4
PMCID: PMC5145160  PMID: 27930656

Abstract

Background

Cognition covers a range of abilities, such as memory, response time and language, with tests assessing either specific or generic aspects. However differences between measures may be observed within the same individuals.

Objective

To investigate the cross-sectional association of cognitive performance and socio-demographic factors using different assessment tools across a range of abilities in a British cohort study.

Methods

Participants of the European Prospective Investigation of Cancer (EPIC) in Norfolk Study, aged 48–92 years, underwent a cognitive assessment between 2006 and 2011 (piloted between 2004 and 2006) and were investigated over a different domains using a range of cognitive tests.

Results

Cognitive measures were available on 8584 men and women. Though age, sex, education and social class were all independently associated with cognitive performance in multivariable analysis, different associations were observed for different cognitive tests. Increasing age was associated with increased risk of a poor performance score in all of the tests, except for the National Adult Reading Test (NART), an assessment of crystallized intelligence. Compared to women, men were more likely to have had poor performance for verbal episodic memory, Odds Ratio, OR = 1.99 (95% Confidence Interval, 95% CI 1.72, 2.31), attention OR = 1.62, (95% CI 1.39, 1.88) and prospective memory OR = 1.46, (95% CI 1.29, 1.64); however, no sex difference was observed for global cognition, OR = 1.07 (95%CI 0.93, 1.24). The association with education was strongest for NART, and weakest for processing speed.

Conclusion

Age, sex, education and social class were all independently associated with performance on cognitive tests assessing a range of different domains. However, the magnitude of associations of these factors with different cognitive tests differed. The varying relationships seen across different tests may help explain discrepancies in results reported in the current literature, and provides insights into influences on cognitive performance in later life.

Introduction

Cognitive ability covers a range of domains, which together form the basis of cognitive function. These domains include recall, learning, understanding, encoding and recognition, most of which require prior knowledge and experience. Measuring cognitive abilities is not straightforward because they are not clearly distinct from one another. One ability may have an impact on the performance of another, or different abilities work in conjunction to execute a function, thus making selection of cognitive tests and the interpretation of results difficult.

There is a growing interest in the heterogeneity of cognitive performance observed in the ageing population [1] and what might constitute ‘normal cognitive ageing’ and why decline occurs [2,3]. There are many studies, both longitudinal and cross-sectional investigating the effects of age on cognition or cognitive ageing, however discrepant age trends have been reported where age-related changes observed in cross-sectional studies have not always been observed in longitudinal analyses. It cannot be assumed that results from longitudinal studies will reflect more accurate age related trends than cross-sectional analyses, as selective attrition and practice effects in longitudinal comparisons may under-estimate age-related cognitive decline [2,4].

There is also uncertainty about patterns of decline across different cognitive abilities. Certain abilities such as memory, spatial ability and processing speed have been observed to decline more readily than others, such as comprehension and vocabulary which tend to remain stable for longer [5,6]. Some suggest that decline in mental speed contributes to the decline seen in other abilities [7,8]. Studies have also indicated that decline occurs at global and at domain specific level [912] and so both should be tested. As yet, there does not appear to be an agreement on the classification of the cognitive domains and there is a lack of consensus on which abilities are most important in testing cognitive impairment or decline [5].

The difficulties and limitations associated with assessment of cognitive function as a result of the variability in cognitive testing and methodologies restricting cross study comparisons are well known [13,14]. Although a plethora of cognitive assessment tools exist, test performances vary depending on the populations in which they are being used. Low levels of accuracy for detecting mild impairment, demographic biases and a lack of an agreed battery of tests appropriate for use in differing situations, all add to the complexities [13]. It is therefore essential to also consider the psychometric properties of the assessment tool and how well it measures the construct of interest within a particular setting.

To better understand when and where decline begins it is necessary to first identify potential factors for the range of results observed. This includes recognizing the reason for the variability observed between different cognitive tests. The aim of this analysis is to gain further insight into how performance on a range of widely used cognitive assessment tools may relate to the socio-demographic factors; age, sex, social class and education.

Methods

Ethics Statement

This research was conducted in compliance with the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. This study was approved by the Norfolk Local Research Ethics Committee (05/Q0101/191) and East Norfolk and Waveney NHS Research Governance Committee (2005EC07L). Participants provided signed informed consent.

Participants

Detailed descriptions of recruitment and study methods have been reported elsewhere [15]. Briefly, men and women then aged 40–79 years were recruited at baseline between 1993 and 1997 through registers in thirty-five general practices in Norfolk. The cohort was similar to the national population samples studied in the Health Survey of England, in terms of anthropometry, serum lipids and blood pressure [15]. Such registers function as population based registers in the UK National Health Service. The data presented here are from the third health examination (3HC or EPIC-Norfolk 3), a face to face assessment conducted between 2006 and 2011 including data from the pilot phase between 2004 and 2006 [16] when the participants were aged 48 to 92 years. Although the EPIC-Norfolk 3 (including the pilot) ran over 7 years, the administration of the health examination remained unchanged with staff tightly following a protocol thus minimizing variation, differences in interpretation and reducing subjectivity [17]. Details on the standardized protocol for both test administration and scoring have been described previously [17].

Assessment of cognition

Cognitive performance was assessed using a battery of tests aiming to cover a range of abilities. These included memory (retrospective and prospective), executive function, attention, language, reading ability, and reaction time, as well as calculation, registration, abstract thinking, learning and visuospatial ability. The test battery took on average 35 minutes to complete.

The cognition battery used in EPIC-Norfolk 3, (EPIC-COG), comprised of seven tests used previously in other studies that are aimed at particular domains of cognitive function. These included testing for i) global cognitive function, using a shortened version of the Extended Mental State Exam (SF-EMSE) [18] a brief pen and paper test; ii) verbal episodic memory, assessed with the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT) [19], with words displayed on a screen but requiring only a verbal response; iii) non-verbal episodic memory, using a computerized touch screen test called Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Paired Associates Learning Test (CANTAB-PAL) [20,21], using the first trial memory score (FTMS) as the outcome measure; iv) attention, assessed with a pen and paper letter cancellation task [22] as used in the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (MRC CFAS) [23]; using accuracy score (PW-Accuracy), which is the number of correctly identified target letters (Ps and Ws) minus all potential target letters missed up to the point scanned by the participant as the outcome measure; v) prospective memory, an event and time based task (also used in MRC CFAS) [24]); vi) processing speed (reaction time in milliseconds), using the computerized Visual Sensitivity Test (VST) [25,26] and finally vii) reading ability and crystallized intelligence that reflects accumulated knowledge [27] using a shortened version of the National Adult Reading Test [28] or Short-NART [29]. Here we used the NART Error Score as the outcome measure, where a higher score indicates poorer performance. The NART words were presented on a computer screen, but only required a verbal response scored by the researcher as correct or incorrect. The tests as they were used in this population have been described in considerable detail previously [17], and so further details on these tests will not be given here.

All the above measures of cognitive function were offered to the individuals taking part. Fig 1 shows the range of cognitive abilities purported to be assessed from the literature describing these tests and the predominant ability that is reflected in the score as reported here. Table 1 presents descriptive data showing the characteristics of the men and women in the EPIC-Norfolk cohort.

Fig 1. Cognitive abilities assessed by individual test of the EPIC-COG Battery.

Fig 1

Cognitive abilities assessed by each test of the EPIC-COG Battery used in the EPC-Norfolk 3 Cohort, 2006–2011 (including data from the pilot phase 2004–2006)

Table 1. Characteristics of the 8584 individuals with cognitive measures participating in EPIC-Norfolk 3, and the pilot phase.

  Men Women
  (N = 3840) (N = 4744)
Mean Age (SD) 69.4 (8.1) 68.1 (8.0)
Frequency % (N)
Age Band
<60 years 10.3 (397) 14.0 (665)
60-<65 years 22.7 (871) 26.4 (1252)
65-<70 years 20.9 (802) 20.8 (985)
70-<75 years 19.8 (760) 17.4 (825)
75-<80 years 15.4 (591) 12.9 (613)
80+ years 10.9 (419) 8.5 (404)
Marital status
Single 3.1 (116) 4.0 (184)
Married 86.6 (3244) 71.5 (3314)
Widowed 6.2 (234) 16.3 (754)
Separated or Divorced 4.0 (152) 8.3 (386)
Education
Degree Level 20.1 (772) 15.6 (741)
A level and equivalent 48.0 (1844) 41.1 (1951)
O Level and equivalent 9.8 (377) 13.6 (647)
No Qualifications 22.0 (846) 29.6 (1404)
Social Class
Professional 9.5 (364) 8.2 (384)
Managerial 42.7 (1628) 39.8 (1869)
Skilled Non Manual 12.2 (466) 19.1 (898)
Skilled Manual 22.7 (865) 18.8 (883)
Semi-Skilled Manual 11.0 (418) 11.3 (532)
Non-skilled 1.9 (72) 2.7 (125)
Mean Cognitive Test Score (SD)
SF-EMSE 32.5 (3.2) 32.7 (3.0)
HVLT 23.9 (5.5) 26.1 (5.4)
FTMS 15.2 (4.6) 15.6 (4.5)
PW-Accuracy 11.6 (6.1) 13.1 (6.0)
VST, Rxn Time
(milliseconds) 2282.5 (430.6) 2229.9 (455.4)
NART Error Score 17.9 (10.3) 16.6 (9.5)
Frequency % (N)
Prospective Memory Task 77.7 (2912) 84.1 (3915)
(Successful)

Abbreviations: A Level, Advanced Level; CANTAB-PAL, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Paired Associates Learning Test; FTMS, First Trial Memory Score; HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; N, Number; NART, National Adult Reading Test; O Level, Ordinary Level; Rxn, Reaction; SF-EMSE:, Shortened version (Short form) of the Extended Mental State Exam; SD, Standard deviation; VST, Visual Sensitivity Test

Covariates

Age was categorized into 5-year age bands. For marital status, very few participants reported being separated (less than one percent), so this category was combined with the divorced group giving four categories; married, single, widowed and divorced or separated.

Education was obtained from the baseline questionnaire, based on the highest qualification attained, categorized into 4 groups: degree or equivalent, Advanced (‘A’) level or equivalent (educational attainment to the equivalent of completing schooling up to the age of 18 years), Ordinary (‘O’) level or equivalent (a level lower than ‘A’ level, or completing schooling up to the age of 16 years) and finally less than ‘O’-level or no qualifications.

Social class, also from baseline for men was coded using current occupation except if participants reported being unemployed in which case their partner’s social class was used. Last employment was used for men who were retired. Unemployed men without partners were unclassified. Social class in women was graded using the “conventional” approach [30], which uses their partner’s social class except when the partner’s social class was unclassified or missing, or they had no partner in which case social class was based on their own occupation. An unemployed woman without a partner was coded as unclassified. Personal measures for social class of individual’s own occupation were also available.

Social class was classified according to the Registrar General’s occupation-based classification scheme into 5 main categories [31]. Social class I consists of professionals, class II includes managerial and technical occupations, class III is subdivided into non-manual and manual skilled workers (III non-manual and III manual), class IV consists of partly skilled workers, and class V comprises unskilled manual workers. Information on marital status was obtained from the health questionnaire completed near the time of the health examination.

Variables were further grouped as follows: Marital status into Married and Single (combining, single, separated, divorced and widowed categories); Social Class into non manual (combining classes I, II and III non-manual) and manual classes (combining manual classes III manual, IV and V). To give further insight into the effects of lower and higher levels of education, this variable was categorized into three groups; (i) No qualification (less than obtaining an ‘O’ level or equivalent or not completing school up to the age of 16), (ii) Completion of school up to the age of 16 or up to the age of 18 (by combining the two categories of attaining ‘O’ level or equivalent and ‘A’ level or equivalent) and finally (iii) those obtaining an education to graduate level (those with a degree or equivalent) or above. Due to low numbers with poor performance in the highest education group, we used the lowest qualification category as the reference category. However, to allow for a more direct comparison of the odds ratios with the other co-variates, we also used education as a dichotomized variable, comparing no qualification with any qualification (combining ‘O’, ‘A’ and degree level).

Analyses

In this population where the prevalence of dementia and cognitive impairment using accepted standard diagnostic criteria was very low, we were only able to use poor performance as an indicator, where poor performance was defined (on any test) as obtaining a score less than a cutoff point corresponding to the 10th percentile of the population distribution in each of the seven cognitive tests individually. It was not possible to define a cut-point with 10% of the population distribution for the prospective memory task and as 19% of the population failed the task, this was used as the lower cut-point. Participants were then classified into two groups based on the cut-off for each of the tests.

Obtaining a score less than a cutoff point corresponding to the 10th percentile of the population distribution (or failing the prospective memory task) was first examined by univariate analysis, using the chi-square test to observe differences between the groups for each socio-demographic variable. The associations between each variable and being in the poor performance group were assessed using logistic regression analysis adjusting for co-variates; age (per 5 years), sex (women being the reference group), marital status (being married as the reference group), educational level (no qualification being the reference group for the three level education variable, and with qualifications being the reference group for the dichotomized education variable) and social class (non-manual, as the reference group) where social class was examined using both the “conventional” method using a woman’s partner’s occupation, as well as personal measures according to a woman’s own occupation. Data was further examined stratified into two age groups split at age 65 years (creating two groups, those under age 65 and those 65 years and older).

Age was also included in a model as a continuous variable and the unstandardized (Beta) coefficient were examined to compare differences in terms of chronological years of age for education level (comparing those with no qualifications with those completing school to the age of 16 or 18 and secondly, those with no qualifications with those educated to graduate level).

In addition, we included a further model, adding the interaction term age group (< 65 and those ≥65 years) x education (entered as the dichotomized variable, no qualification compared to those with any qualification) to examine difference in poor cognitive performance for age depending on education level. Tests showing a significant interaction between age group and education (no qualification compared to those with any qualification), were then stratified to examine the magnitude of differences in associations for these two age groups.

Although intelligence and education, are said to be correlated [27], we did not assume our tests of crystalized intelligence, NART, to be measuring the same exposure and so we tested this further as a secondary analysis and associations were examined by further adjusting for the NART Error Score.

Missing data in cognitive tests

To explore the effect of missing data, a sensitivity analysis was carried out only in individuals with complete data on all seven cognitive tests and the specified covariates (n = 5727). All P values reported are 2 sided. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS version 21.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

Of the 8623 participants attending the third examination, cognitive data were available on 8584 men and women. Compliance was high with over 90% (n = 7773) attempting at least six of the seven tests in the battery and almost 70% (n = 6011) having measures for all seven tests. Less than 5% (n = 223) of participants attempted fewer than four tests. The CANTAB-PAL and VST had lower completion rates than the other tests, partly due to a technical computer failure, which resulted in loss of data. Fig 2 shows the distribution of the EPIC-Norfolk participants with poor cognitive performance across the seven tests. Over 50% of participants in the cohort were not in the poor performing group for any test and less than 10% of the cohort had a poor performance score in three or more tests.

Fig 2. Distribution of participants in the poor performance group across all tests in the EPIC-COG Battery.

Fig 2

Distribution of participants in the poor performance group (defined as obtaining a score less than a cut-off point corresponding to the 10th percentile of the population distribution in each of the seven cognitive tests individually.

The distribution of poor cognitive performance by the socio-demographic variable (age, sex, marital status, education and social class) in each of the tests are shown in Tables 2, 3 and 4.

Table 2. Distribution of participants in the bottom tenth percentile in each cognitive test by age.

  SF-EMSE HVLT FTMS PW_Acc Prospective Memory VST NART Error Score
Freq. Freq. Freq. Freq. Freq. Freq. Freq.
N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%)
Score< = 29 Total Score< = 18 Total Score< = 10 Total Score< = 5 Total Failed task Total > = 2702.46 ms Total Score> = 31
All 8483 1092 (12.9) 8081 1042 (12.9) 7281 870 (11.9) 8410 915 (10.9) 8403 1576 (18.8) 7144 714 (10.0) 8112 849 (10.5)
Age
<60
years 1055 49 (4.6) 1022 30 (2.9) 959 39 (4.1) 1048 51 (4.9) 1049 92 (8.8) 890 59 (6.6) 999 80 (8.0)
60-<65
years 2105 159 (7.6) 2061 118 (5.7) 1877 125 (6.7) 2098 125 (6.0) 2095 246 (11.7) 1846 112 (6.1) 2062 196 (9.5)
65-<70
years 1767 200 (11.3) 1700 146 (8.6) 1529 136 (8.9) 1756 169 (9.6) 1751 286 (16.3) 1522 124 (8.1) 1716 201 (11.7)
70-<75
years 1572 221 (14.1) 1496 263 (17.6) 1339 190 (14.2) 1563 181 (11.6) 1558 336 (21.6) 1323 128 (9.7) 1508 180 (11.9)
75-<80
years 1183 244 (20.6) 1109 257 (23.2) 985 207 (21.0) 1170 203 (17.4) 1170 325 (27.8) 996 142 (14.3) 1107 131 (11.8)
80+
years 801 219 (27.3) 693 228 (32.9) 592 173 (29.2) 775 186 (24.0) 780 291 (37.3) 567 149 (26.3) 720 61 (8.5)
p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.001 p = 0.001

P-value for trends across the groups using the chi-square test

Abbreviations: A Level, Advanced Level; FTMS, Freq, Frequency; First Trial Memory Score (outcome measure for CANTAB-PAL, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Paired Associates Learning Test); HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; Ms, millisecond; NART, National Adult Reading Test; N, Number; O Level, Ordinary Level; SF-EMSE:, PW-Accuracy (Accuracy Score, for the letter cancellation task) Shortened version (Short form) of the Extended Mental State Exam; VST, Visual Sensitivity Test

Table 3. Distribution of participants in the bottom tenth percentile in each cognitive test by sex and marital status.

  SF-EMSE HVLT FTMS PW_Acc Prospective Memory Visual sensitivity NART Error Score
Freq Freq. Freq. Freq. Freq. Freq. Freq.
N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%)
Score< = 29 Total Score< = 18 Total Score< = 10 Total Score< = 5 Total Failed task Total > = 2702.46 ms Total Score> = 31
Sex
Women 4681 580 (12.4) 4476 424 (9.5) 4011 442 (11.0) 4650 409 (8.8) 4653 738 (15.9) 3928 364 (9.3) 4504 397 (8.8)
Men 3802 512 (13.5) 3605 618 (17.1) 3270 428 (13.1) 3760 506 (13.5) 3750 838 (22.3) 3216 350 (10.9) 3608 452 (12.5)
p = 0.14 p<0.001 p = 0.007 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.02 p<0.001
Marital
Status
Single 295 42 (14.2) 282 35 (12.4) 251 50 (19.9) 291 35 (12.0) 239 54 (18.4) 241 28 (11.6) 283 27 (9.5)
Married 6494 777 (12.0) 6225 766 (12.3) 5620 597 (10.6) 6445 638 (9.9) 6439 1149 (17.8) 5499 521 (9.5) 6244 656 (10.5)
Widowed 964 173 (17.9) 883 165 (18.7) 786 150 (19.1) 948 163 (17.2) 948 244 (25.7) 779 112 (14.4) 895 93 (10.4)
Separated
or divorced 533 68 (12.8) 499 43 (8.6) 445 37 (8.3) 530 53 (10.0) 529 86 (16.3) 450 34 (7.6) 503 40 (8.0)
  p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.3

P-value for trends across the groups using the chi-square test

Abbreviations: A Level, Advanced Level; Freq, Frequency, FTMS, First Trial Memory Score (outcome measure for CANTAB-PAL, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Paired Associates Learning Test); HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; Ms, millisecond; NART, National Adult Reading Test; N, Number; O Level, Ordinary Level; SF-EMSE:, PW-Accuracy (Accuracy Score, for the letter cancellation task) Shortened version (Short form) of the Extended Mental State Exam; VST, Visual Sensitivity Test

Table 4. Distribution of participants in the bottom tenth percentile in each cognitive test by social class and education.

  SF-EMSE HVLT FTMS PW_Acc Prospective Memory VST NART Error Score
Freq Freq Freq Freq Freq Freq Freq
N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%) N N (%)
(Total) Score< = 29 (Total) Score< = 18 (Total) Score< = 10 (Total) Score< = 5 (Total) Failed task (Total) > = 2702.46 ms (Total) Score> = 31
Social Class
Professional 734 58 (7.9) 694 59 (8.5) 649 55 (8.5) 728 55 (7.6) 727 102 (14.0) 646 58 (9.0) 709 15 (2.1)
Managerial 3445 342 (9.9) 3306 336(10.2) 2957 316 (10.7) 3416 312 (9.1) 3416 603 (17.7) 2893 266 (9.2) 3311 188 (5.7)
Skilled
  Non Manual 1350 149 (11.0) 1286 168 (13.1) 1139 140 (12.3) 1342 142 (10.6) 1342 233 (17.4) 1145 114 (10.0) 1301 92 (7.1)
    Manual 1736 291 (16.8) 1644 267 (16.2) 1493 187 (12.5) 1722 228 (13.2) 1715 377 (22.0) 1453 148 (10.2) 1633 332 (20.3)
Semi-skilled
    Manual 943 192 (20.4) 893 163 (18.3) 811 132 (16.3) 932 130 (13.9) 932 203 (21.8) 782 90 (11.5) 900 161 (17.9)
Non-skilled 196 48 (24.5) 185 41 (22.2) 165 30 (18.2) 193 30 (15.5) 194 41 (21.1) 157 24 (15.3) 178 51 (28.7)
p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.08 p<0.001
Education
Degree Level 1491 73 (4.9) 1415 71 (5.0) 1283 86 (6.7) 1484 101 (6.8) 1484 196 (13.2) 1264 97 (7.7) 1447 15 (1.0)
A level
  and equiv. 3749 429 (11.4) 3596 449 (12.5) 3233 352 (10.9) 3719 412 (11.1) 3720 668 (18.0) 3195 295 (9.2) 3604 289 (8.0)
O Level
  and equiv. 1019 76 (7.5) 976 77 (7.9) 895 87 (9.7) 1013 60 (5.9) 1011 163 (16.1) 870 74 (8.5) 966 42 (4.3)
No
Qualifications 2222 512 (23.0) 2092 443 (21.2) 1868 344 (18.4) 2192 341 (15.6) 2186 547 (25.0) 1813 248 (13.7) 2093 502 (24.0)
p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001

P-value for trends across the groups using the chi-square test

Abbreviations: A Level, Advanced Level; Freq, Frequency; FTMS, First Trial Memory Score (outcome measure for CANTAB-PAL, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Paired Associates Learning Test); eqiv, equivalent; HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; Ms, millisecond; NART, National Adult Reading Test; N, Number; O Level, Ordinary Level; SF-EMSE:, PW-Accuracy (Accuracy Score, for the letter cancellation task) Shortened version (Short form) of the Extended Mental State Exam; VST, Visual Sensitivity Test

Associations adjusted for the covariates age, sex, education and social class are shown in Table 5 (associations adjusted for age only are given in S1 Table).

Table 5. Odds ratios for poor performance adjusted for age, sex, marital status, education and social class.

  SF-EMSE HVLT FTMS PW-Accuracy Prospective Memory VST NART Error Score
  OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
Number of Participants 8208 7817 7036 8138 8403 6902 7846
Age 1.43 1.37, 1.50 1.68 1.60, 1.77 1.5 1.42, 1.58 1.38 1.32, 1.45 1.38 1.33, 1.43 1.37 1.30, 1.45 0.96 0.91, 1.007
(per 5 year increase) p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.09
Sex 1.07 0.93, 1.24 1.99 1.72, 2.31 1.17 1.00, 1.36 1.62 1.39, 1.88 1.46 1.29, 1.64 1.16 0.98, 1.37 1.77 1.51, 2.08
(Women a) 0.3 p<0.001 p = 0.06 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.08 p<0.001
Marital status 1.08 0.92, 1.28 0.99 0.83,1.19 1.26 1.05, 1.50 1.35 1.14, 1.61 1.11 0.96, 1.27 1.04 0.85, 1.27 1.04 0.85, 1.27
(Married a) p = 0.3 p = 0.9 p = 0.01 p = 0.001 p = 0.2 p = 0.7 p = 0.7
Social Class 1.68 1.45, 1.94 1.52 1.31, 1.77 1.24 1.06, 1.46 1.42 1.22, 1.66 1.25 1.10, 1.42 1.1 0.92, 1.31 2.63 2.23, 3.09
(Non-manual a) p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.01 p<0.001 p = 0.001 p = 0.3 p<0.001
Education
  Up to16 or 18 years 0.5 0.43,0.58 0.59 0.51, 0.69 0.65 0.55,0.77 0.72 0.62, 0.85 0.74 0.65, 0.84 0.74 0.62,0.89 0.27 0.23, 0.32
(No Qual a) p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.002 p<0.001
Graduate Level 0.25 0.19, 0.33 0.26 0.20, 0.35 0.43 0.33,0.57 0.54 0.41, 0.69 0.56 0.46, 0.69 0.67 0.51, 0.88 0.05 0.003,0.008
(No Qual a) p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p = 0.004 p<0.001

a Reference category

CI, Confidence Interval; FTMS, First Trial Memory Score (outcome measure for CANTAB-PAL, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Paired Associates Learning Test); HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; N, Number; NART, National Adult Reading Test; OR, Odds ratio; PW-Accuracy (Accuracy Score, for the letter cancellation task) Qual, Qualifications; SF-EMSE, Shortened version (Short form) of the Extended Mental State Exam; SD, Standard deviation, VST, Visual Sensitivity Test

Odds Ratios for poor performance (defined as obtaining a score less than a cut-off point corresponding to the 10th Percentile of the population distribution) for each test in the cognition battery used in EPIC-Norfolk 3, adjusted for covariates (age, sex, marital Status, education and social class)

Age

Table 5 indicates that increasing age was associated with being in the poor performance group for all tests, except the NART error score (intelligence) for which there was no significant trend with age in the multivariable model, Odds Ratio (OR) = 0.96 (95% Confidence Interval (CI) 0.91, 1.01 P = 0.09). The strongest association was observed for HVLT OR = 1.68 (95% CI 1.60, 1.77 P<0.001) followed by FTMS OR = 1.50 (95% CI 1.42, 1.58 P<0.001), both testing for episodic memory. The OR observed for the tests for the other domains were more comparable to each other.

Stratifying data by age group (<65 and ≥65 years of age)

When the data were stratified into the two age groups (<65 years and ≥65 years), the most striking difference in association between the age groups was observed for the VST for both age and sex, where for the under 65 group, there was no association between age and poor performance, OR = 0.9 (95% CI 0.75, 1.15 P = 0.5) or for sex, OR = 0.99 (95% CI 0.71, 1.37 P = 0.9), but statistically significant associations observed in the 65+ years age group for age, OR = 1.52 (95% CI 1.40, 1.66 P<0.001) and sex OR = 1.23 (95% CI 1.01, 1.50 P = 0.04). The results for the stratified analyses are shown in S2 Table.

Sex

There were noticeable differences in the odds ratios for gender across the different tests. Men were more likely to be in the poor performance group for HVLT, PW-Accuracy, the prospective memory task and NART. Weaker, but not statistically significant associations were observed for FTMS and VST. There were no differences observed between men and women for SF-EMSE.

Marital status

Marital status was significantly associated with cognitive performance for FTMS and PW-Accuracy, with poor performance more likely in single than married individuals. The other tests showed no evidence of associations with marital status.

Education

More education was strongly associated with decreased poor performance across all the tests. However, the strength of association varied from one test to another, with NART being most strongly associated, particularly at graduate level.

When education was examined as a dichotomized variable (so that the ORs for education were directly comparable with the other covariates), this time comparing no qualification with any qualification (combining ‘O’, ‘A’ and degree level), and using the qualification group as the reference, the OR observed were; SF-EMSE OR = 2.22 (95% CI 1.92, 2.56 P<0.001); HVLT OR = 1.89 (95% CI 1.62, 2.20 P<0.001); FTMS OR = 1.63 (95% CI 1.38, 1.93 P<0.001); Prospective memory OR = 1.42 (95% CI 1.24, 1.62 P<0.001); PW-Accuracy OR = 1.45 (95% CI 1.23, 1.70 P<0.001); VST OR = 1.37 (95% CI 1.14, 1.65 P = 0.001) and NART 4.31 (95% CI 3.66, 5.08 P<0.001). Again, any qualification compared to no qualification having the strongest association for the NART (data not shown).

Using the unstandardized regression coefficients (from the model adjusting for covariates in Table 5, unstandardized coefficients not shown), we observed that the likelihood of being in the poor performance group for those with an education up to ‘O’ or ‘A’ level compared to no qualification was equivalent to being just over 5 years younger for HVLT, FTMS, PW-Accuracy, prospective memory task, and VST. Comparing graduate level to no qualification, the likelihood was equivalent to being almost 10 years younger for most of the tests and 13 years younger for the HVLT. For the SF-EMSE, comparing those with an education up to ‘O’ or ‘A’ level compared to those with no qualification, was equivalent to 9.9 years younger and for graduate level, compared to no qualifications the likelihood was equivalent to those 19.6 years younger (data not shown).

There was significant interaction between education (no qualification compared to those with any qualification) for HVLT (p = 0.01) and FTMS (p = 0.01) but not for SF-EMSE, PW_Accuracy, prospective memory, VST or NART. On stratifying the data for both HVLT and FTMS (complete data not shown), the associations were stronger with education for the under 65 age group for HVLT, OR = 3.42 (95% CI 2.35, 4.99 P<0.001) than for the ≥65 year age group OR = 1.94 (95% CI 1.65, 2.28 P<0.001); and for FTMS, the under 65 age group OR = 2.50 (95% CI 1.73, 3.62 P<0.001) and ≥65 year age group OR = 1.66 (95% CI 1.39, 1.99 P<0.001). This can be seen in the associations observed for education in the age group stratified data (S2 Table), particularly for education to 16 or 18 years for HVLT and FTMS. Associations were considerably stronger for the <65 years age group as compared to the ≥65 years for both these tests. The association with education did not differ greatly in the two age groups for the remaining tests.

Social Class

Manual social class was independently associated with poor performance for all the tests apart from VST. On repeating the model but using individuals’ own social class or occupation rather than the ‘conventional’ method, the associations observed were slightly stronger, however, there were no qualitative differences observed in the relationship. The data for both measures of social class are shown in S3 Table.

Adjustment for NART

When the models were additionally adjusted for the NART error score as a secondary analysis (S4 Table), the odds were attenuated, but a higher likelihood of poor performance per 5 year increase in age was still observed for all remaining tests. The relationship observed for SF-EMSE reversed, with men 27% less likely to be in the poor performance group than women OR = 0.83, (95% CI 0.71, 0.97) P = 0.02. Little change seen was observed in the associations for any of the tests for marital status.

Associations with education at both ‘O’ or ‘A’ level and at graduate level were attenuated but still observed for SF-EMSE and FTMS after adjusting for NART. Graduate level education remained inversely associated with for poor cognitive performance. Associations were no longer significant for education and PW-Accuracy or the prospective memory task, For HVLT, associations with education were observed at graduate level but not at ‘O’ and ‘A’ level. For VST, associations were only observed for those completing school to ‘O’ and ‘A’ level but did not remain at graduate level. For social class, additionally adjusting for NART substantially attenuated associations, with just SF-EMSE still statistically significant association.

Missing data in cognitive tests

The results from the sensitivity analysis carried out only on individuals with complete data on all tests were similar to earlier results presented. The sensitivity analyses are shown in S5 and S6 Tables.

Discussion

This paper presents the results of the association of age, sex, education, social class and (as a secondary analysis), crystallized intelligence on different cognitive abilities using a range of assessment tools in a healthy population of older men and women. We observed that the relationship of sociodemographic factors with cognitive function does depend on the assessment tool used.

Older age was found to be associated with poor performance in all of the cognitive tests (abilities), except with the NART score (crystallized intelligence), confirming in this larger study that age has little or no association with NART performance [32]. The greatest age-related differences were observed for HVLT (verbal episodic memory). Episodic memory deficits have been reported to be associated with strongest and most persistent risk of cognitive decline [33,34] and are the most common and earliest complaints. Our findings are consistent with previous work, here presented in terms of poorer performance observed in multiple domains across age [6,11,35] and stability observed in crystallized intelligence, or knowledge that is learned and acquired over years which has shown to be more resistant to the effect of age [28,36,37].

Men were significantly more likely to be poor performers compared to women for HVLT, PW-Accuracy (attention), prospective memory and NART. The sex differences were greatest for HVLT. In contrast, there was no significant sex difference for SF-EMSE (global function). Differences in men and women have been shown across various domains [38], and our findings, were consistent with other studies [39]. Dementia can be considered as the extreme of poor cognitive performance. Though the overall prevalence of dementia has been reported to be higher in women [40] this could be explained by a higher proportion of older women in the general population. Findings from large population-based studies report no sex differences in the rates of dementia up to high age[4143] with higher rates only observed in women compared to men in the oldest old [43]. The higher prevalence of dementia observed in women may be due to the fact that age is the strongest risk factor for dementia and women make up the majority of the older population due to their increased life expectancy relative to men. No significant differences between men and women were observed for VST (processing speed), although previous studies have found men to perform better [44]. This could be partly explained in the composition of the test which relies on the accurate recognition of a triangle shape as well as speed. Hence accuracy (performed better by women) and reaction time or processing speed (performed better by men), when combined, result in men and women shown to be performing similarly.

As shown previously, [45] education and social class were independently associated with cognitive performance. This was observed across all the domains even when adjusting for age and the other covariates. Although associations were strengthened when assigning social class to women using their own social class, we found no qualitative differences in associations from this and the ‘conventional’ method and the prediction of poor performance in all the tests.

When estimating the potential impact of education on the age-related likelihood of poor performance, this was noticeable for the SF-EMSE test for global cognition, where the risk of being in the poor performance group for those with an education up to ‘O’ and ‘A’ level compared to no qualification was equivalent to being almost 10 years younger and at graduate level compared to no qualification, this difference was even greater, with the risk equivalent to being almost 20 years younger.

We also found in this study that the Influence of education was stronger in younger age group (<65 years) than in ≥65 year age group. Not having qualifications in those <65 years was associated with greater risk of being in the poor performance group compared to people aged 65 and over. Plausible explanations for this difference include the likelihood that other factors such as co morbidities common in older people could have a greater influence on cognitive performance in older people.

NART, as would be expected, showed the greatest association with education, however when controlling for the NART, associations were substantially attenuated but still observed for the SF-EMSE and FTMS for the levels of education examined. The change of direction of the association observed for SF-EMSE and sex after adjusting for NART may be because men performed worse than women on the NART and this was observed in the change in association on adjusting for NART.

Education was associated with a lowered risk of poor performance for HVLT at graduate level but not at ‘O’ and ‘A’ level in the fully adjusted model. Associations were no longer present for PW-Accuracy and little for prospective memory. Education and Intelligence (of which NART is a proxy measure), even though known closely related variables, are not perfectly correlated, and cannot be substituted for each other [27]. Our findings support this and indicate that whatever NART assesses, whether it is a surrogate for prior ability including childhood intelligence or a composite indicator of education; adjusting for NART has a material influence on the independent association of social class and education on cognitive performance measures. Though NART does not completely remove association in all the tests here, it is an important exposure to consider and adjust for when analyzing cognitive function.

Another noteworthy point is that tests purporting to measure similar domains, such as FTMS and HVLT; both measures of episodic memory (one verbal and one non-verbal), showed quite different relationships, particularly for sex, with men showing greater likelihood of poor performance for HVLT than the FTMS. One possible explanation is that tasks requiring verbal and semantic knowledge are considered to be more cognitively demanding, requiring greater self-initiation and encoding than non-verbal tasks such as the FTMS of the CANTAB-PAL [11,46]. This observation, as well as that seen for processing speed, highlights the complex nature for assessing cognitive function, because different abilities do not work in isolation. Each test actually assesses more than the one ability (as shown in Fig 1) and to execute a task successfully, abilities work in conjunction and not independently of each other. While the score focuses on the performance of a single ‘main’ ability, it actually reflects a range of other abilities that are being utilized.

There are a number of strengths to this study. The data are collected from a single large population with individuals being assessed under the same conditions. This reduces the variation in methodology seen in other studies that have combined data from various sources. We also examined ability separately across a wider range covering six domains including a test for global function and not restricted to a few domains, just episodic memory or simply global cognitive function as in many studies. Furthermore, we assessed differences in performance at individual test level rather than as aggregated test scores which allowed us to observe differences even in those tests intending to measure the same ability.

Our cohort is of course confined those with capacity to consent and those willing to participate in the follow up examination, so it is unlikely to include those with major ill health including established dementia. However unlike other studies that are more restrictive in age, sex or using a cohort that is not representative of the general population [3,23,45], our cohort comprises a large number of participants from the general population at baseline, includes men and women, covers a broad range in age, education and social class. Even in this relatively well-functioning group [16], we observed variability in function across all domains. While cross-sectional studies cannot look at longitudinal change, the recruitment bias are similar to longitudinal studies, with healthier, younger individuals more likely to take part. Although longitudinal studies have the advantage of allowing temporal relationship to be examined more accurately, they still have the issue of attrition and drop out which if uncorrected reduces the observed decline.

As mentioned above, a limitation of our study is that we have not tested for other risk factors for poor performance in particular comorbidities, all which influence cognitive performance. These will be examined in further detail in the future as the main focus of this paper was on differences across assessment tools. This study is cross-sectional, and reflects a more ‘healthy profile’ as it consists of survivors and those who make it to the health check and get tested. There are plans to add longitudinal measures to examine the pattern of age related changes in this cohort.

In summary, we observed that the likelihood of poorer cognitive performance increases with age for all measures, in particular for HVLT (verbal episodic memory) but not for NART which is reported to measure crystallized intelligence, or accumulated knowledge. Men were more likely to have poor performance than women in all tests apart from SF-EMSE (global cognition) and marital status having only moderate influence on the FTMS (non-verbal episodic memory) and PW_Accuracy (attention) but not on the other tests. Social class was shown to have the strongest association with the NART (crystallized intelligence) and least with VST (processing speed) and education was associated with performance across all tests, again with the strongest for the NART In conclusion, we report that age, sex, education, social class and crystallized intelligence though all independently related to cognitive performance, varied in their association with the different tests. To further investigate “true” age related changes, characteristics of tests must also be considered when investigating determinants of cognitive function.

While the relationship between cognitive performance and the functioning of an individual or health outcomes is beyond the scope of this paper, these results provide further understanding of the influences and characteristics of these tests. This may enable greater insight into the determinants of cognitive function in older people and how to maintain cognitive health in later life.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Age adjusted odds ratios for poor performance for each test in the EPIC-COG battery.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance stratified by those over and under 65 years.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance using two separate measures of social class for women.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance adjusted for all covariates plus NART Error Score.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance for participants with complete data (N = 5727).

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance for participants with complete data also adjust for NART.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all study participants, general practitioners and the EPIC Norfolk study team for their contribution to this work.

Data Availability

The EPIC Norfolk study depends on data from NHS digital or its previous equivalent bodies. NHS Digital do not allow the sharing of data at individual record level without having a data sharing agreement in place. Although our data are anonymised, the volume of data required for this analysis and the recruitment of participants through general practices from a single region, makes data sharing without a data sharing agreement a risk. The EPIC Norfolk study data sharing agreement ensures that collaborating researchers will not make contact with participants or their GPs Researchers wishing to request data can contact the corresponding author Shabina A Hayat, at Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Strangeways Research Laboratory, Wort’s Causeway, Cambridge, UK. Email:Sah63@medschl.cam.ac.uk, or contact the Head of Informatics at EPIC-Norfolk, Robert Luben E mail: Robert.luben@phpc.cam.ac.uk.

Funding Statement

The infrastructure for this study was supported by the Medical Research Council, UK http://www.mrc.ac.uk/ (Ref: G0401527) and Cancer Research UK http://www.cancerresearchuk.org/ (CRUK, Ref: C864/A8257). The clinic for EPIC-Norfolk 3 was funded by Research into Ageing, now known as Age UK http://www.ageuk.org.uk/ (Grant Ref: 262). The pilot phase was supported by MRC (Ref: G9502233) and CRUK (Ref: C864/A2883). KK received the above funding. FEM Is supported by MRC (Ref: U105292687). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Weaver Cargin J, Collie A, Masters C, Maruff P. The nature of cognitive complaints in healthy older adults with and without objective memory decline. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2008;30: 245–257. 10.1080/13803390701377829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Salthouse T. When does age-related cognitive decline begin. Neurbiol Aging. 2009;30: 507–514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, et al. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2012;344: d7622 10.1136/bmj.d7622 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sliwinski M, Buschke H. Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships among age, cognition, and processing speed. Psychol Aging. 1999;14: 18–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Deary IJ, Batty GD. Cognitive epidemiology. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2007;61: 378–384. 10.1136/jech.2005.039206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wilson RS, Beckett L A., Barnes LL, Schneider J A., Bach J, Evans D A., et al. Individual differences in rates of change in cognitive abilities of older persons. Psychol Aging. 2002;17: 179–193. A [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Salthouse T A. What and When of Cognitive Aging. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2004;13: 140–144. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Salthouse T A. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. 1996;103: 403–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cullum S, Huppert F A, McGee M, Dening T, Ahmed A, Paykel ES, et al. Decline across different domains of cognitive function in normal ageing: results of a longitudinal population-based study using CAMCOG. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;15: 853–862. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Williams JG, Huppert FA, Matthews FE, Nickson J. Performance and normative values of a concise neuropsychological test (CAMCOG) in an elderly population sample. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;18: 631–44. 10.1002/gps.886 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bäckman L, Jones S, Berger A-K, Laukka EJ, Small BJ. Multiple cognitive deficits during the transition to Alzheimer’s disease. J Intern Med. 2004;256: 195–204. 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01386.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Tucker-Drob EM. Global and domain-specific changes in cognition throughout adulthood. Dev Psychol. 2011;47: 331–343. 10.1037/a0021361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cullen B, O’Neill B, Evans JJ, Coen RF, Lawlor B A. A review of screening tests for cognitive impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2007;78: 790–799. 10.1136/jnnp.2006.095414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Dangour AD, Allen E. Do omega-3 fats boost brain function in adults? Are we any closer to an answer? Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97: 909–910. 10.3945/ajcn.113.061168 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Day N, Oakes S, Luben R, Khaw K-T, Bingham S WA and W. EPIC-Norfolk: study design and characteristics of the cohort. European Prospective Investigation of Cancer. Br J Cancer. 1999;80: 95–103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hayat S A., Luben R, Keevil VL, Moore S, Dalzell N, Bhaniani A, et al. Cohort profile: A prospective cohort study of objective physical and cognitive capability and visual health in an ageing population of men and women in Norfolk (EPIC-Norfolk 3). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43: 1063–1072. 10.1093/ije/dyt086 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hayat S A, Luben R, Moore S, Dalzell N, Bhaniani A, Anuj S, et al. Cognitive function in a general population of men and women: a cross sectional study in the European Investigation of Cancer-Norfolk cohort (EPIC-Norfolk). BMC Geriatr. 2014;14: 142 10.1186/1471-2318-14-142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Huppert F A, Cabelli ST, Matthews FE. Brief cognitive assessment in a UK population sample—distributional properties and the relationship between the MMSE and an extended mental state examination. BMC Geriatr. 2005;5: 7 10.1186/1471-2318-5-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brandt J, Brandt J. The hopkins verbal learning test: Development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin Neuropsychol. Routledge; 1991;5: 125–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sahakian BJ, Morris RG, Evenden JL, Heald A, Levy R, Philpot M, et al. a Comparative Study of Visuospatial Memory and Learning in Alzheimer-Type Dementia and Parkinson’S Disease. Brain. 1988;111: 695–718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fowler KS, Saling MM, Conway EL, Semple JM LW.: Computerized neuropsychological tests in the early detection of dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002;3: 139–146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Richards M., Kuh D., Hardy R. and Wadsworth MEJ. Lifetime cognitive function and timing of the natural menopause. Neurology. 1999;53: 308–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Medical THE, Function C. Cognitive function and dementia in six areas of England and Wales: the distribution of MMSE and prevalence of GMS organicity level in the MRC CFA Study. Psychol Med. 1998;28: 319–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Huppert F a, Johnson T, Nickson J. High Prevalence of Prospective Memory Impairment in the Elderly and in Early-stage Dementia : Findings from a Population-based Study. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2000;14: S63–S81. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bandelow S, Hogervorst E, Smith AD. The development of computational tools for dementia diagnosis. Proceedings of the International Conference on Advanced Computational Intelligence and its Applications. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hogervorst EEF, Bandelow S, Schmitt J, Jentjens ROY, Oliveira M, Allgrove J, et al. Caffeine improves physical and cognitive performance during exhaustive exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40: 1841–1851. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31817bb8b7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Deary IJ, Johnson W. Intelligence and education: Causal perceptions drive analytic processes and therefore conclusions. Int J Epidemiol. 2010;39: 1362–1369. 10.1093/ije/dyq072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Nelson HE, McKenna P. The use of current reading ability in the assessment of dementia. Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1975;14: 259–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Beardsall L. & Brayne C. Estimation of verbal intelligence in an elderly community: A prediction analysis using a shortened NART. Br J Clin Psychol. 1990;29: 83–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mcfadden E, Luben R. Different measures of social class in women and mortality. 2009; 231–236. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 31.Shohaimi S, Welch A, Bingham S, Luben R, Day N, Wareham N, et al. Residential area deprivation predicts fruit and vegetable consumption independently of individual educational level and occupational social class: a cross sectional population study in the Norfolk cohort of the European Prospective Investigation into Canc. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004;58: 686–91. 10.1136/jech.2003.008490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Crawford JR, Stewart LE, Garthwaite PH, Parker DM, Besson J a. The relationship between demographic variables and NART performance in normal subjects. Br J Clin Psychol. 1988;27 (Pt 2): 181–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Aggarwal NT, Wilson RS, Beck TL, Bienias JL, Bennett D A. Mild cognitive impairment in different functional domains and incident Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76: 1479–1484. 10.1136/jnnp.2004.053561 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.De Jager C., Schrijnemaekers A-C CMC, Honey TEM, Budge MM. Detection of MCI in the clinic: evaluation of the sensitivity and specificity of a computerised test battery, the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test and the MMSE. Age Ageing. 2009;38: 455–60. 10.1093/ageing/afp068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Salthouse T A., Ferrer-Caja E. What needs to be explained to account for age-related effects on multiple cognitive variables? Psychol Aging. 2003;18: 91–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Beardsall L. & Brayne C, Beardsall L, Brayne C. Estimation of verbal intelligence in an elderly community: A prediction analysis using a shortened NART. Br J Clin Psychol. 1990;29: 83–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brayne C and Beardsall L. Estimation of verbal intelligence in an elderly community: An epidemiological study using NART. British journal of Clinical Psychology. 1990. pp. 83–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gerstorf D, Herlitz A, Smith J. Stability of sex differences in cognition in advanced old age: the role of education and attrition. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2006;61: P245–P249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Meinz EJ, Salthouse T A. Is age kinder to females than to males? Psychon Bull Rev. 1998;5: 56–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Thies W, Bleiler L. 2011 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s Dement. Elsevier Ltd; 2011;7: 208–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hofman a, Rocca W a, Brayne C, Breteler MMB, Clarke M, Cooper B, et al. The Prevalence of Dementia in Europe: A Collaborative Study of 1980–1990 Findings. Int J Epidemiol. 1991;20: 736–748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ruitenberg A, Ott A, Van Swieten JC, Hofman A, Breteler MMB. Incidence of dementia: Does gender make a difference? Neurobiol Aging. 2001;22: 575–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bamford SM, Walker T. Women and dementia—Not forgotten. Maturitas. Elsevier Ireland Ltd; 2012;73: 121–126. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2012.06.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Der G, Deary IJ. “Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood: Results from the United Kingdom Health and Lifestyle Survey”: Correction. Psychol Aging. 2009;24: 229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gallacher JEJ, Elwood PC, Hopkinson C, Rabbitt PM a, Stollery BT, Sweetnam PM, et al. Cognitive function in the Caerphilly study: Associations with age, social class, education and mood. Eur J Epidemiol. 1999;15: 161–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Fowler KS, Saling MM, Conway EL, Semple JM, Louis WJ. Paired associate performance in the early detection of DAT. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002;8: 58–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 Table. Age adjusted odds ratios for poor performance for each test in the EPIC-COG battery.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance stratified by those over and under 65 years.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance using two separate measures of social class for women.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance adjusted for all covariates plus NART Error Score.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance for participants with complete data (N = 5727).

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Odds ratios for poor performance for participants with complete data also adjust for NART.

(DOCX)

Data Availability Statement

The EPIC Norfolk study depends on data from NHS digital or its previous equivalent bodies. NHS Digital do not allow the sharing of data at individual record level without having a data sharing agreement in place. Although our data are anonymised, the volume of data required for this analysis and the recruitment of participants through general practices from a single region, makes data sharing without a data sharing agreement a risk. The EPIC Norfolk study data sharing agreement ensures that collaborating researchers will not make contact with participants or their GPs Researchers wishing to request data can contact the corresponding author Shabina A Hayat, at Department of Public Health and Primary Care, Strangeways Research Laboratory, Wort’s Causeway, Cambridge, UK. Email:Sah63@medschl.cam.ac.uk, or contact the Head of Informatics at EPIC-Norfolk, Robert Luben E mail: Robert.luben@phpc.cam.ac.uk.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES