Abstract
Background
Sedentary behaviours (SB) are highly prevalent in young people and may be adversely associated with physical and mental health. Understanding of the modifiable determinants of SB is necessary to inform the design of behaviour change interventions but much of the existing research is cross-sectional and focussed upon screen-based behaviours.
Purpose
To examine the social, psychological and environmental determinants of change in children’s objectively measured sedentary time from age 11 to 14 years.
Methods
Data are from the second (2008) and third (2011) waves of assessment in the Sport, Physical Activity, and Eating Behaviour: Environmental Determinants in Young People (SPEEDY) study, conducted in the county of Norfolk, United Kingdom. Longitudinal data on accelerometer assessed sedentary time were available for 316 (53.5% female, 11.2±0.3 years at baseline) and 264 children after-school and at the weekend respectively. Information on 14 candidate determinants, including school travel mode and electronic media ownership, was self-reported. Change in the proportion of registered time spent sedentary was used as the outcome variable in cross-classified linear regression models, adjusted for age, sex, body mass index and baseline sedentary time. Simple and multiple models were run and interactions with sex explored.
Results
Daily sedentary time increased by 30–40 minutes after-school and at the weekend from baseline to follow-up. Participants who travelled to school by cycle exhibited smaller increases in after-school sedentary time (beta; 95%CI for change in % time spent sedentary: -3.3;-6.7,-0.07). No significant determinants of change in weekend sedentary time were identified.
Conclusions
Time spent sedentary increased during the three-year duration of follow-up but few of the variables examined were significantly associated with changes in sedentary time. Children’s mode of school travel may influence changes in their sedentary time over this period and should be examined further, alongside broader efforts to identify modifiable determinants of SB during childhood.
Introduction
Public health guidelines in the United Kingdom (UK) and other countries recommend that children and adolescents should limit their engagement in screen-based or overall sedentary behaviour [1–3]. These guidelines draw upon an emerging body of epidemiological evidence indicating that sedentary behaviour, particularly television (TV) viewing, may be an independent risk factor for physical and mental health in this population [4,5]. Whilst interest in this field has expanded rapidly in recent years, much of the existing research remains cross-sectional and reliant upon subjective assessments of behaviour, limiting our ability to make causal inferences [6]. Nonetheless, concern amongst policy-makers and public health scientists over the potentially detrimental effect of sedentary behaviour on health has prompted the development of interventions to modify these behaviours in young people. Review evidence indicates that these programmes have been somewhat effective to date, but effects appear to be small, are often limited to particular subsets of behaviour and long-term evaluations are lacking [7]. Further research to inform the development of behaviour change interventions in this population is required.
Surveillance data indicates that sedentary behaviour is highly prevalent in young people and that participation varies according to a range of socio-demographic factors, including sex, ethnicity and socio-economic position [8–11]. There is also consistent evidence that, whilst moderate to vigorous physical activity decreases, sedentary behaviour increases with age, particularly during the transition from childhood to adolescence [12,13]. These data are valuable in highlighting population groups that might benefit from targeted intervention programmes. However, intervention developers also require information on the modifiable determinants of behaviour, to inform the design of intervention strategies [14]. Such factors may operate at the individual, social, environmental and policy-levels, as hypothesised within the ecological model [15]. Although a large number of studies have investigated the factors that influence sedentary behaviour in young people, much of this research has been cross-sectional (correlates) and focussed upon TV viewing or other screen-based behaviours [16,17]. Although such behaviours are prevalent in this population, they are just one subset of sedentary behaviour and only weakly predictive of overall sedentary time [18].
A recent review of longitudinal studies confirmed the previously observed variation in sedentary behaviour at the individual level but highlighted a lack of consistent evidence on modifiable determinants within the social and environmental domains [19]. An accompanying commentary called for further studies utilising objective measures of behaviour and that focussed explicitly on devising and testing determinants that are specific to sedentary behaviour [20]. In a previous analysis in the SPEEDY cohort we identified a small number of home environmental and familial factors that were associated with changes in children’s sedentary time over 1 year (age 10 at baseline) [21]. Building upon this previous work, the aim of the current study was to examine the association of sedentary behaviour specific social, psychological and environmental characteristics with change in children’s objectively measured sedentary time from age 11 to 14 years.
Methods
Design and ethics
The Sport, Physical Activity, and Eating Behaviour: Environmental Determinants in Young People (SPEEDY) study is a population based cohort study investigating factors associated with physical activity, sedentary behaviour and diet in children from the county of Norfolk, UK [22]. Ethical approval for each assessment wave was obtained from the University of East Anglia research ethics committee.
Data collection procedures
The SPEEDY study included three waves of assessment: baseline (T0; age 9/10y; April-July 2007), 1-year follow-up (T1; age 10/11y; April-July 2008) and 4-year follow-up (T2; age 13/14y; April-July 2011). Full details of participant recruitment and procedures for baseline data collection have been reported previously [22]. Participation at T0 was prerequisite for recruitment to either of the subsequent waves. Data from waves T1 and T2 are used in the current study. Wave T1 was used as baseline in this analysis due to the inclusion of items related to the determinants of sedentary time in the T1 questionnaire that had not been included in the previous wave. As noted above, we have previously explored the social, familial and environmental determinants of change in sedentary time from T0 to T1 [21]. At T1, study information sheets and consent forms were mailed to all 2064 participants from T0. Those who consented were mailed an accelerometer, instruction sheet and questionnaire. Participants were asked to wear the accelerometer for one week and to return it by mail, using an addressed, pre-paid envelope. At T2 all participants with a valid home address from T0 (n = 1964) were mailed information sheets and consent forms. Through local administrative authorities, we ascertained the number of participants attending each secondary school in Norfolk, but our original consent did not allow us to trace individual participants. We presented the study in Year 9 assemblies at secondary schools attended by at least five original participants. Consent forms were returned to the study office by mail. Subsequent measurements were taken at school following similar procedures as at baseline. To increase recruitment, an extra invitation letter was sent home prior to the holiday (July 2011), resulting in an additional 62 participants being assessed by mail, following the same methodology as T1.
Sedentary time measurement
Sedentary time was measured objectively using an Actigraph (GT1M; Pensacola, FL) accelerometer [23,24], set to record at 5-second epochs. Children were instructed to wear the monitor during waking hours for seven days and to remove it while bathing, showering and swimming. Accelerometer data were analysed using a batch processing program (MAHUffe: http://www.mrc-epid.cam.ac.uk/physical-activity-downloads). A count threshold of 100 counts per minute (cpm) was used to define sedentary time [25,26]. Periods of ≥10 minutes of consecutive zero counts [27,28] and days with <500 minutes of recording between 6am–11pm were excluded [28,29]. Two sedentary time outcome variables were derived and analysed separately; (1) after-school (3–11pm, Monday-Friday) and (2) at the weekend (6am– 11pm, Saturday/Sunday). To account for differences in accelerometer wear time between baseline and follow-up, outcome variables were constructed as change in the proportion of time spent sedentary, calculated as follows: [(T2 sedentary time/T2 wear time)—(T1 sedentary time/ T1 wear time)]*100. A minimum of 2 days of weekday data and 1 day of weekend data was required for inclusion in the after-school and weekend analyses respectively.
Exposures
Fourteen determinants were included in the analysis, grouped under the following headings: behavioural, environmental, social, psychological (Table 1). Information on putative determinants was self-reported by children at T1 using previously tested items where possible [30,31].
Table 1. Description of candidate determinants.
Variable | Description | Summary†(% or mean(SD)) | |
---|---|---|---|
Behavioural | |||
School travel mode | Item: How do you usually travel to school? Response options: Car (ref), Bus/Train, Cycle, Walk. | Car | 37.2 |
Bus/Train | 8.3 | ||
Cycle | 7.1 | ||
Walk | 47.4 | ||
After school destination | Item: Do you usually go anywhere else on your way home from school? Response options: Home only (ref), Friend’s house, Shops/Park/Other. | Home | 72.4 |
Friends house | 7.4 | ||
Other | 20.2 | ||
Environmental | |||
Home location | Home located in rural (ref) / urban location. Derived from home postcode using methods described by Bibby and Shephard [44]. Four density profiles were collapsed: ‘city’/‘town and fringe’ classified as Urban, ‘hamlets and isolated dwellings’/‘villages’ classified as Rural. | Urban | 37.1 |
Rural | 62.9 | ||
Car ownership | Item: Which of the following things do you have at home? Prompt: More than 1 car. Response options: No (ref), Yes. | No | 34.3 |
Yes | 65.7 | ||
Home—games console | Item: Which of the following things do you have at home? Prompt: A games console. Response options: No (ref), Yes. | No | 20.7 |
Yes | 79.3 | ||
Home—active games console | Item: Which of the following things do you have at home? Prompt: An active games console. Response options: No (ref), Yes. | No | 54.2 |
Yes | 45.8 | ||
Bedroom—electronic media | Number of electronic media items present in participant’s bedroom: television, DVD/video player, personal computer, video games console. Sum score: range 0–4. | 1.5 (1.3) | |
Bedroom—active games console | Item: Which of these do you have in your bedroom? Prompt: An active games console. Response options: No (ref), Yes. | No | 92.6 |
Yes | 7.4 | ||
Social | |||
School travel—social context | Item: Who do you usually travel to school with? Response options: Accompanied by an adult (ref), Alone, Friend/Sibling. | Adult | 45.5 |
Alone | 5.8 | ||
Friend/Sibling | 48.7 | ||
Social support for limiting SB* | Seven items indicating social and environmental restrictions on SB. Example items: ‘My parents tell me to watch less TV’ (reverse coded), ‘I can play a computer game for as long as I want’. Response options: Never (coded 2), Sometimes (coded 1), Always (coded 0). Sum score: range 0–14. Cronbach’s alpha: 0.4. | 7.5 (1.8) | |
Psychological | |||
Negative perception of SB* | Four items indicating participant’s negative perception of screen-based SB. Example items: ‘I think TV and video games are boring’, ‘Watching TV takes time away from other fun activities’. Response options: Yes (coded 2), Don’t know (coded 1), No (coded 0). Sum score: range 0–8. Cronbach’s alpha: 0.4. | 4.3 (1.8) | |
Positive perception of SB* | Four items indicating participant’s positive perception of screen-based SB. Example items: ‘I enjoy playing computer games for many hours in a row’, ‘Watching TV is my favourite pastime’. Response options: Yes (coded 2), Don’t know (coded 1), No (coded 0). Sum score: range 0–8. Cronbach’s alpha: 0.4. | 3.3 (1.8) | |
Self-efficacy for SB change* | Five items assessing perceived ability to limit SB. Example items: ‘I can turn off the TV even when there is a programme on that I enjoy’, ‘I can leave the room when others are watching TV’. Response options: Yes (coded 2), Don’t know (coded 1), No (coded 0). Sum score: range 0–10. Cronbach’s alpha: 0.4. | 7.9 (1.6) | |
Enjoyment of SB | Item: I enjoy sedentary activities. Response options: No (ref), Don’t know, Yes. | No | 26.1 |
Don’t know | 30.1 | ||
Yes | 43.8 |
Covariates
Date of birth and sex were self-reported. Height and weight were measured by trained research assistants at baseline (T0) and used to calculate body mass index (BMI, kg/m2).
Statistical analysis
Analyses were conducted using Stata (version 13.0) in 2015. We compared demographic characteristics and responses to exposure variables between those included in the analysis and those lost to follow-up using Student’s t tests and chi-squared tests. Cross-classified linear regression models were used to examine associations between candidate determinants (assessed at T1) and changes in the proportion of time spent sedentary after-school and at the weekend. The cross-classified model accounts for the clustering of participants within primary (T1) and secondary (T2) schools but does not assume a hierarchical structure. This is because children from any given primary school attended several different secondary schools, and each secondary school received pupils from several different primary schools. Initially, associations between determinants and sedentary time outcomes were examined with adjustment for age, sex, BMI and baseline level of the outcome variable (simple models) [32]. Determinants associated at P<0.1 were retained for inclusion in a multivariable model [21]. Subsequently, interaction terms were added to simple regression models to examine effect modification by sex. Interactions were retained for inclusion in the multivariable model where they met the following criteria: 1) the interaction term P-value was <0.1; and 2) the determinant was associated at P<0.05 in either boys or girls. This strategy was employed to simplify interpretation of the final multivariable model and to reduce the risk of type 1 error resulting from multiple hypothesis testing. In the multivariable model, statistical significance was set at P<0.05.
Results
Data for the current analysis are from the second (T1) and third (T2) waves of assessment in the SPEEDY study. Of the 2064 children invited to participate at T1, 1019 (49.4% of invited) obtained parental consent and took part in assessments. Three years later, 1964 children who had valid contact details at T1 were invited to participate in the final wave of assessment (T2), of which 480 (24.4% of invited) obtained consent. Valid accelerometer data on changes in after-school and weekend sedentary time were obtained from 316 and 264 participants respectively. Participants included in the after-school analysis were more likely to have a mother who remained in education beyond 16 years of age (47.0% vs. 37.3%, p = 0.01) and less likely to live in rented accommodation (15.2% vs. 25.1%, p<0.01) than those who took part at T1 but did not provide valid after-school outcome data. There were, however, no differences in age or sex between the analytical sample and those lost to follow-up. Participants included in the after-school analysis were less likely to travel home from school alone (5.8% vs. 11.4%, p = 0.02), less likely to own a video games console (79.3% vs. 84.6%, p = 0.04), had fewer items of electronic media in the bedroom (1.5. vs. 1.9 item, p<0.01) and had higher levels of social support for limiting their sedentary behaviour (score 7.5 vs. 7.2, p = 0.03) than those who took part at T1 but did not provide valid after-school outcome data. Demographic characteristics of the 316 participants that provided valid data on after-school sedentary time at T1 and T2 are reported in Table 2. Over 3 years, daily sedentary time increased by approximately 30–40 minutes after-school and at the weekend (Table 3).
Table 2. Baseline characteristics of participants with valid after-school accelerometer data at baseline and follow-up.
All | Girls | Boys | |
---|---|---|---|
Sex, n (%) | 316 | 169 (53.5) | 147 (46.5) |
Age, mean(SD) | 11.2 (0.3) | 11.2 (0.3) | 11.2 (0.3) |
Maternal education, n (%) | |||
Left education <16y | 115 (37.3) | 69 (41.3) | 46 (32.6) |
Left education >16y | 193 (62.7) | 98 (58.7) | 95 (67.4) |
House tenure, n (%) | |||
Renting | 47 (15.2) | 28 (16.7) | 19 (13.5) |
Buying | 262 (84.8) | 140 (83.3) | 122 (86.5) |
SD, standard deviation.
Maternal education: All n = 308, boys n = 141, girls n = 167.
House tenure: All n = 309, boys n = 141, girls n = 168.
Table 3. Accelerometer wear time and sedentary time after-school and at the weekend.
After-schoola | Weekend | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
T1 | T2 | Change | T1 | T2 | Change | |
Sedentary time,(min/day) | ||||||
All | 206.1 (41.2) | 242.4 (48.1) | +36.4 (55.5) | 444.6 (71.1) | 482.7 (79.6) | +38.1 (89.4) |
Girls | 210.8 (40.7) | 243.7 (44.0) | +32.8 (53.1) | 445.3 (67.2) | 476.6 (69.7) | +31.3 (86.9) |
Boys | 200.5 (41.2) | 241.0 (52.5) | +40.4 (58.1) | 443.8 (75.4) | 489.3 (88.9) | +45.5 (91.7) |
Wear time,(min/day) | ||||||
All | 323.8 (55.4) | 341.8 (58.5) | +18.1 (69.3) | 691.0 (79.7) | 682.1 (93.4) | -9.0 (112.9) |
Girls | 327.8 (55.0) | 339.8 (54.3) | +12.0 (65.9) | 691.8 (75.0) | 672.3 (88.0) | -19.5 (107.8) |
Boys | 319.1 (55.7) | 344.2 (63.0) | +24.0 (72.8) | 690.2 (84.8) | 692.6 (98.2) | +2.4 (117.5) |
Sedentary time, % of wear time | ||||||
All | 63.7 (6.8) | 70.9 (7.1) | +7.2 (8.4) | 64.4 (7.8) | 70.9 (8.2) | +6.6 (9.1) |
Girls | 64.4 (6.7) | 71.7 (6.4) | +7.3 (7.8) | 64.4 (7.3) | 70.8 (9.1) | +6.7 (8.5) |
Boys | 62.9 (6.9) | 69.9 (7.7) | +7.0 (9.2) | 64.3 (8.3) | 71.1 (7.3) | +6.4 (9.8) |
SD, standard deviation.
a After-school defined as 3–11pm Monday to Friday.
After-school: All n = 316, boys n = 147, girls n = 169.
Weekend: All n = 264, boys n = 127, girls n = 137.
Simple associations between candidate determinants and changes in the proportion of time spent sedentary after-school and at the weekend are presented in Table 4. Three variables were associated with change in after-school sedentary time in simple models and carried over for inclusion in a multivariable model (Table 5). One variable remained significant in the multivariable model. Participants who reported that they travelled to school by cycle exhibited smaller increases in their after-school sedentary time. The presence of an active games console in the home (p = 0.068) was positively associated with change in weekend sedentary time. No other exposures were associated with change in weekend sedentary time. A multivariable model for change in weekend sedentary time, therefore, was not required.
Table 4. Simple associations of behavioural, environmental, social and psychological factors with changes in after-school and weekend sedentary time.
Variable | After-schoola | Weekend | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
β | 95% CI | β | 95% CI | |
Behavioural | ||||
School travel mode | ||||
Car (ref) | ||||
Bus/Train | 0.82 | (-2.1, 3.7) | 1.5 | (-2.0, 5.0) |
Cycle | -3.4 | (-6.6, -0.20)** | -1.4 | (-5.2, 2.5) |
Walk | -0.53 | (-2.2, 1.1) | 0.81 | (-1.3, 2.9) |
After school destination | ||||
Home (ref) | ||||
Friends house | 1.3 | (-1.8, 4.4) | 0.28 | (-3.4, 3.9) |
Park/Shops/Other | 0.52 | (-1.5, 2.5) | -0.57 | (-3.0, 1.8) |
Environmental | ||||
Home location | -0.70 | (-2.3, 0.94) | 0.60 | (-1.6, 2.8) |
Car ownership | 0.62 | (-1.0, 2.3) | -1.3 | (-3.3, 0.67) |
Home—games console | 0.90 | (-1.0, 2.8) | -0.91 | (-3.2, 1.4) |
Home—active games console | -0.89 | (-2.4, 0.65) | 1.7 | (-0.13, 3.5)* |
Bedroom—electronic media | 0.0026 | (-0.62, 0.62) | 0.16 | (-0.59, 0.91) |
Bedroom—active games console | 1.3 | (-1.6, 4.2) | 2.4 | (-0.95, 5.8) |
Social | ||||
School travelȁsocial context | ||||
Accompanied by an adult (ref) | ||||
Alone | 0.11 | (-3.2, 3.4) | -0.63 | (-4.5, 3.3) |
Friend/Sibling | 0.37 | (-1.2, 2.0) | 0.73 | (-1.2, 2.7) |
Social support for limiting SB | -0.16 | (-0.59, 0.27) | 0.34 | (-0.20, 0.89) |
Psychological | ||||
Negative perception of SB | -0.12 | (-0.56, 0.31) | 0.00044 | (-0.53, 0.54) |
Positive perception of SB | 0.25 | (-0.19, 0.70)† | -0.082 | (-0.63, 0.47) |
Self-efficacy for SB change | 0.08 | (-0.39, 0.55) | -0.27 | (-0.85, 0.31) |
Enjoyment of SB | ||||
No (ref) | ||||
Don’t know | 0.79 | (-1.3, 2.9) | 1.0 | (-1.5, 3.6) |
Yes | 2.0 | (0.15, 3.9)** | 0.69 | (-1.7, 3.0) |
β, beta coefficient; 95% CI, 95% confidence interval; ref, reference group.
* p<0.1;
** p<0.05;
† evidence of interaction with sex.
a After-school defined as 3–11pm Monday to Friday.
Models adjusted for: age (continuous), sex (male, female), body mass index (continuous).
After-school (n = 289–306) and weekend (n = 238–251) analytical samples vary due to missing data for individual determinants.
Table 5. Final multivariable model for association of candidate determinants with change in the proportion of after-school time spent sedentary.
Variable | β | 95% CI |
---|---|---|
School travel mode | ||
Car (ref) | ||
Bus/Train | 1.2 | (-1.7, 4.1) |
Cycle | -3.3 | (-6.5, -0.07)* |
Walk | -0.18 | (-1.9, 1.5) |
Positive perception of SB (boys)a | 0.63 | (-0.08, 1.3) |
Sex interaction | -0.85 | (-1.7, 0.04) |
Enjoyment of SB | ||
No (ref) | ||
Don’t know | 0.29 | (-1.8, 2.4) |
Yes | 1.6 | (-0.39, 3.6) |
β, beta coefficient; 95% CI, 95% confidence interval; SB, sedentary behaviour; ref, reference group.
* p<0.05,
** p<0.01.
a β (95% CI) for association in girls: -0.22 (-0.82, 0.37).
After-school defined as 3–11pm Monday to Friday.
Models adjusted for: age (continuous), sex (male, female), body mass index (continuous) and mutually adjusted for named determinants.
Discussion
Between the ages of 11 and 14 years, participants in the current study increased their daily sedentary time by approximately 30–40 minutes after-school and at the weekend. Overall, few of the candidate determinants examined were significantly associated with these changes in sedentary time, but participants that used a bicycle for their journey to school showed smaller increases in sedentary time over three years. No significant determinants of change in weekend sedentary time were observed.
Over the three-year follow-up period, travelling by cycle for the journey to school was associated with a smaller increase in children’s overall sedentary time after school. Relative to those who travelled by car and assuming monitor wear time of 12 hours/day, use of a cycle for the journey to school was associated with an approximately 24 minute smaller increase in sedentary time. That is, where the sample mean change in sedentary time after-school was an increase of approximately 36 minutes/day, travel by cycle to school was associated with an increase of approximately 12 minutes/day. To our knowledge, the role of school travel as a determinant of change in children’s sedentary time has not been examined previously. There is evidence that active school travel is associated with higher levels of physical activity, though associations are typically stronger and more consistent for walking than cycling [33–35]. This may partially be a reflection of the generally low prevalence of cycling to school and the methodological challenges associated with assessing cycling-based activity. Given the paucity of research in this area and the associated methodological challenges, our findings warrant further investigation but should be interpreted cautiously at present.
In the simple regression model, boys who expressed a stronger positive attitude towards sedentary behaviour exhibited larger increases in after-school sedentary time, but this association was attenuated, and no longer significant, in the multivariable model. Given the strict criteria we employed for the exploration of interactions, the role of attitudes in shaping children’s sedentary behaviour, the assessment thereof and possible effect modification by sex, may be worthy of further investigation. This would be most valuable in large population based cohorts with sufficient participants to enable exploration of interactions. It is unclear why the observed trend was limited to boys, but it is possible that the items used to assess attitudes towards sedentary behaviour better captured this construct in boys than girls. Individual items focussed specifically upon TV viewing and computer use, for example, which are typically more prevalent in boys than girls [10,18]. Previous longitudinal research examining the association of children’s attitudes with their overall sedentary time is lacking and evidence linking attitudes with screen-based sedentary behaviour and physical activity in this population is mixed [16,17,36]. Our findings indicate that exposure measurement tools should be developed with consideration to possible differential functioning between sexes and that statistical testing for interactions with sex should be included in future studies exploring the determinants of sedentary time.
No significant determinants of change in weekend sedentary time were identified. There was a trend for participants that reported having an active games console in the home to exhibit larger increases in sedentary time but the association did not reach our a priori determined level of significance (p<0.05). Despite the intuitive appeal, none of the four exposures related to the home media environment were strongly associated with changes in sedentary time. Previous experimental work has indicated that the introduction of an active games console into the home may reduce passive gaming, increase physical activity and reduce overall sedentary time, but results are inconsistent and effects appear to wane over time [37,38]. Interestingly, a recent observational study in Irish children (age 12 years) noted that active games consoles were used predominantly to play passive games and that active video game play was positively correlated with TV viewing [39]. These complex interrelations between electronic media and sedentary behaviour in children suggest that interventions that focus solely upon the provision or removal of electronic devices from the home may be overly simplistic and unlikely to produce consistent changes in behaviour. Indeed, qualitative studies have shown that diverse features of the home social and physical environment interact to influence children’s physical activity and sedentary behaviour [40,41]. Researchers and practitioners will need to acknowledge these issues during intervention design. It may, for example, be necessary to enable some degree of family-level tailoring of intervention content to account for the unique characteristics of individual homes.
A total of 14 exposure variables from the behavioural, environmental, social and psychological domains were examined for their association with sedentary time in the current study, but few significant associations were identified. The reasons for this are unclear but a number of explanations may be hypothesised. Firstly, it may be that changes in discretionary sedentary time are driven by a small number of key variables. Whilst this is plausible, few candidates have emerged from the research conducted to date, which is characterised by considerable inconsistency between studies [19]. An alternative explanation is that there was a lack of specificity between exposure and outcome variables used in this analysis [42]. Differential associations between candidate exposures and individual sedentary behaviours may result in associations being attenuated to the null when an aggregate-level outcome (i.e. total sedentary time) is used. We attempted to address this limitation by examining after-school and weekend behaviour separately but this may not have been sufficient. Continued effort to develop objective measures of individual sedentary behaviours is necessary. Consistent with a recent call in the literature, the exposures used in this analysis were devised specifically for the exploration of sedentary behaviour [20]. Nonetheless, further development and testing may be required to refine and optimise these items, particularly those related to social or psychological constructs.
A key strength of this study is the use of accelerometry to obtain objective assessments of sedentary time in a population based cohort of children. This analysis adds to a small body of longitudinal evidence examining the determinants of change in children’s sedentary time. Exposure variables were derived for multiple levels of the ecological model and statistical analyses accounted for the clustering of participants in primary and secondary schools at baseline and follow-up. Multivariable models were constructed to control for confounding and to identify independent associations amongst exposure variables where appropriate. The following limitations are acknowledged. Firstly, although our analytical strategy accounted for a number of potential confounders, we were not able to adjust for sexual maturation, which may have resulted in residual confounding. In addition, the analytical sample was relatively small due to participant attrition between baseline and follow-up assessments. Although sample retention was similar to that in other cohorts in this population [43] it is possible that some of our analyses were underpowered. However, most regression estimates were small, suggesting that the predominantly null findings cannot be attributed solely to a lack of statistical power. There was evidence that participants from families of lower socio-economic position were more likely to drop-out and that the analytical sample generally lived in homes that had fewer electronic media or were more supportive of limiting sedentary behaviour, potentially limiting the generalisability of our findings. Some of our social and environmental exposures were assessed with items that pertained to screen-based behaviours specifically, rather than overall sedentary time, which may have limited their predictive capacity. In addition, the internal consistency of composite exposure measures was lower than ideal, likely due to our use of a limited range of response options intended to maximise participant comprehension. We recognise the need for further development of these tools, but contend that there inclusion is justified given the exploratory nature of the analysis and limited available evidence on this topic. Lastly, data used in this analysis were collected in 2008 / 2011 and it is possible that behaviour patterns and exposure characteristics may have changed since this time. It is unknown, however, whether exposure-outcome associations changed commensurately over this period.
Conclusion
In this population-based cohort of English children, sedentary time increased by 30–40 minutes per day between the ages of 11 and 14 years. Few of the exposure variables examined were predictive of changes in sedentary time, though the role of school travel mode as a determinant of change in children’s sedentary behaviour is worthy of additional examination. Further work to identify the modifiable determinants of change in sedentary behaviour during the transition from childhood to adolescence is required. Methodological developments related to the assessment of sedentary behaviour specific exposures and outcomes may be beneficial to this process.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The SPEEDY study is a collaboration between the MRC Epidemiology Unit and the School of Environmental Sciences and the School of Medicine, Health Policy and Practice at the University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK. The contribution of our collaborators at the University of East Anglia in obtaining funding and ethical approval, data collection and data processing is gratefully acknowledged. We thank the schools, children, and parents for their participation, everyone who helped with the data collection and Norfolk Children’s Services for invaluable input and support. In addition, we thank Kate Westgate and Stefanie Mayle from the physical activity technical team at the MRC Epidemiology Unit for their assistance in processing the accelerometer data.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
The work was supported by the Centre for Diet and Activity Research (CEDAR), a UK Clinical Research Collaboration Public Health Research Centre of Excellence (RES-590-28-0002). Funding from the British Heart Foundation, Economic and Social Research Council, Medical Research Council, the National Institute for Health Research, and the Wellcome Trust, under the auspices of the UK Clinical Research Collaboration, is gratefully acknowledged. The SPEEDY study was funded by the Medical Research Council (Unit Programme number MC_UU_12015/7, MC_UU_12015/3, MC_UU_12015/4) and the National Prevention Research Initiative, consisting of the following funding partners: British Heart Foundation; Cancer Research United Kingdom; Department of Health; Diabetes United Kingdom; Economic and Social Research Council; Medical Research Council; Health and Social Care Research and Development Office for Northern Ireland; Chief Scientist Office, Scottish Government Health Directorates; Welsh Assembly Government; and World Cancer Research Fund.
References
- 1.Start Active Stay Active (2011) A report on physical activity for health from the four home countries’ Chief Medical Officers. The Department of Health.
- 2.Tremblay MS, Leblanc AG, Janssen I, Kho ME, Hicks A, Murumets K, et al. Canadian sedentary behaviour guidelines for children and youth. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2011;36: 59–64. 10.1139/H11-012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.American Academy of Pediatrics C on C and M, Mulligan DA, Altmann TR, Brown A, Christakis DA, Clarke-Pearson K, et al. Children, adolescents, obesity, and the media. Pediatrics. 2011;128: 201–8. 10.1542/peds.2011-1066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mitchell JA, Byun W. Sedentary Behavior and Health Outcomes in Children and Adolescents. Am J Lifestyle Med. 2014;8: 173–199. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Suchert V, Hanewinkel R, Isensee B. Sedentary behavior and indicators of mental health in school-aged children and adolescents: A systematic review. Prev Med (Baltim). 2015;76: 48–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chinapaw M, Altenburg T, Brug J. Sedentary behaviour and health in children—Evaluating the evidence. Prev Med (Baltim). 2015;70: 1–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Biddle SJH, Petrolini I, Pearson N. Interventions designed to reduce sedentary behaviours in young people: a review of reviews. Br J Sports Med. 2014;48: 182–186. 10.1136/bjsports-2013-093078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kaiser Family Foundation. Generation M2: Media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-olds. 2010.
- 9.Craig R, Shelton N, editors. Health Survey for England 2007 Volume 1: Healthy lifestyles: knowledge, attitudes and behaviour. Leeds: The Health and Social Care Information Centre; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Atkin AJ, Sharp SJ, Corder K, van Sluijs EMF. Prevalence and Correlates of Screen time in Youth: An International Perspective. Am J Prev Med. 2014; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Griffiths LJ, Cortina-Borja M, Sera F, Pouliou T, Geraci M, Rich C, et al. How active are our children? Findings from the Millennium Cohort Study. BMJ Open. 2013;3: e002893 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cooper AR, Goodman A, Page AS, Sherar LB, Esliger DW, van Sluijs EM, et al. Objectively measured physical activity and sedentary time in youth: the International children’s accelerometry database (ICAD). Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12: 113 10.1186/s12966-015-0274-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Corder K, Sharp SJ, Atkin AJ, Griffin SJ, Jones AP, Ekelund U, et al. Change in objectively measured physical activity during the transition to adolescence. Br J Sports Med. 2015;49: 730–736. 10.1136/bjsports-2013-093190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Baranowski T, Anderson C, Carmack C. Mediating variable framework in physical activity interventions. How are we doing? How might we do better? Am J Prev Med. 1998;15: 266–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sallis J, Owen N. Ecological models of health behavior In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Lewis FM, editors. Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research and Practice,. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2002. pp. 462–485. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pate RR, Mitchell JA, Byun W, Dowda M. Sedentary behaviour in youth. Br J Sports Med. 2011/August/13 2011;45: 906–13. 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Uijtdewilligen L, Nauta J, Singh AS, van Mechelen W, Twisk JWR, van der Horst K, et al. Determinants of physical activity and sedentary behaviour in young people: a review and quality synthesis of prospective studies. Br J Sports Med. 2011/August/13 2011;45: 896–905. 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Klitsie T, Corder K, Visscher TL, Atkin AJ, Jones AP, van Sluijs EM. Children’s sedentary behaviour: descriptive epidemiology and associations with objectively-measured sedentary time. BMC Public Health. 2013;13: 1092 10.1186/1471-2458-13-1092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stierlin AS, De Lepeleere S, Cardon G, Dargent-Molina P, Hoffmann B, Murphy MH, et al. A systematic review of determinants of sedentary behaviour in youth: a DEDIPAC-study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12: 133 10.1186/s12966-015-0291-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Brug J, Chinapaw M. Determinants of engaging in sedentary behavior across the lifespan; lessons learned from two systematic reviews conducted within DEDIPAC. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12: 134 10.1186/s12966-015-0293-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Atkin AJ, Corder K, Ekelund U, Wijndaele K, Griffin SJ, van Sluijs EMF. Determinants of change in children’s sedentary time. PLoS One. 2013;8: e67627 10.1371/journal.pone.0067627 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.van Sluijs EMF, Skidmore PML, Mwanza K, Jones AP, Callaghan AM, Ekelund U, et al. Physical activity and dietary behaviour in a population-based sample of British 10-year old children: the SPEEDY study (Sport, Physical activity and Eating behaviour: environmental Determinants in Young people). BMC Public Health. 2008;8: 388 10.1186/1471-2458-8-388 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Evenson KR, Catellier DJ, Gill K, Ondrak KS, McMurray RG. Calibration of two objective measures of physical activity for children. J Sports Sci. 2008;26: 1557–1565. 10.1080/02640410802334196 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Treuth MS, Schmitz K, Catellier DJ, McMurray RG, Murray DM, Almeida MJ, et al. Defining accelerometer thresholds for activity intensities in adolescent girls. Med Sci Sport Exerc. 2004;36: 1259–1266. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ridgers ND, Salmon J, Ridley K, O’Connell E, Arundell L, Timperio A. Agreement between activPAL and ActiGraph for assessing children’s sedentary time. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9: 15 10.1186/1479-5868-9-15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Trost SG, Loprinzi PD, Moore R, Pfeiffer KA. Comparison of accelerometer cut points for predicting activity intensity in youth. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43: 1360–8. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318206476e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sardinha LB, Andersen LB, Anderssen SA, Quitério AL, Ornelas R, Froberg K, et al. Objectively measured time spent sedentary is associated with insulin resistance independent of overall and central body fat in 9- to 10-year-old Portuguese children. Diabetes Care. 2008;31: 569–75. 10.2337/dc07-1286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Corder K, van Sluijs EMF, Ekelund U, Jones AP, Griffin SJ. Changes in children’s physical activity over 12 months: longitudinal results from the SPEEDY study. Pediatrics. 2010;126: e926–35. 10.1542/peds.2010-0048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Basterfield L, Adamson AJ, Frary JK, Parkinson KN, Pearce MS, Reilly JJ. Longitudinal study of physical activity and sedentary behavior in children. Pediatrics. 2011;127: e24–30. 10.1542/peds.2010-1935 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Norman GJ, Vaughn AA, Roesch SC, Sallis JF, Calfas KJ, Patrick K. Development of decisional balance and self-efficacy measures for adolescent sedentary behaviors. Psychol Health. Taylor & Francis Group; 2004;19: 561–575. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Norman GJ, Schmid B a, Sallis JF, Calfas KJ, Patrick K. Psychosocial and environmental correlates of adolescent sedentary behaviors. Pediatrics. 2005;116: 908–16. 10.1542/peds.2004-1814 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fitzmaurice G. A conundrum in the analysis of change. Nutrition. 2001;17: 360–1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Davison KK, Werder JL, Lawson CT. Children’s active commuting to school: current knowledge and future directions. Prev Chronic Dis. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2008;5: A100. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.van Sluijs EMF, Fearne VA, Mattocks C, Riddoch C, Griffin SJ, Ness A. The contribution of active travel to children’s physical activity levels: Cross-sectional results from the ALSPAC study. Prev Med (Baltim). 2009;48: 519–524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cooper AR, Andersen LB, Wedderkopp N, Page AS, Froberg K. Physical Activity Levels of Children Who Walk, Cycle, or Are Driven to School. Am J Prev Med. 2005;29: 179–184. 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Craggs C, Corder K, van Sluijs EMF, Griffin SJ. Determinants of change in physical activity in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Am J Prev Med. 2011/May/14 Elsevier Inc.; 2011;40: 645–58. 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.02.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Maitland C, Stratton G, Foster S, Braham R, Rosenberg M. A place for play? The influence of the home physical environment on children’s physical activity and sedentary behaviour. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. BioMed Central Ltd; 2013;10: 99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Straker LM, Abbott R a, Smith AJ. To remove or to replace traditional electronic games? A crossover randomised controlled trial on the impact of removing or replacing home access to electronic games on physical activity and sedentary behaviour in children aged 10–12 years. BMJ Open. 2013;3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Forde C, Hussey J. How Children Use Active Videogames and the Association Between Screen Time and Physical Activity. Games Health J. 2015;4: 312–7. 10.1089/g4h.2014.0135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Maitland C, Stratton G, Foster S, Braham R, Rosenberg M. The Dynamic Family Home: a qualitative exploration of physical environmental influences on children’s sedentary behaviour and physical activity within the home space. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11: 157 10.1186/s12966-014-0157-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Granich J, Rosenberg M, Knuiman M, Timperio A. Understanding children’s sedentary behaviour: a qualitative study of the family home environment. Health Educ Res. 2010;25: 199–210. 10.1093/her/cyn025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Atkin AJ, van Sluijs EMF, Dollman J, Taylor WC, Stanley RM. Identifying correlates and determinants of physical activity in youth: How can we advance the field? Prev Med (Baltim). 2016;87: 167–169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Harding SK, Page AS, Falconer C, Cooper AR. Longitudinal changes in sedentary time and physical activity during adolescence. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12: 44 10.1186/s12966-015-0204-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bibby P, Shepherd J. Developing a new classification of urban and rural areas for policy purposes: The methods. London, England: University of London; 2004. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.