Abstract
Introduction
Callous-unemotional (CU) traits in the presence of conduct problems are associated with increased risk of severe antisocial behavior. Developmentally sensitive methods of assessing CU traits have recently been generated, but their construct validity in relation to neurocognitive underpinnings of CU has not been demonstrated. The current study sought to investigate whether the fear-specific emotion recognition deficits associated with CU traits in older individuals are developmentally expressed in young children as low concern for others and punishment insensitivity.
Methods
A sub-sample of 337 preschoolers (mean age 4.8 years [SD=.8]) who completed neurocognitive tasks was taken from a larger project of preschool psychopathology. Children completed an emotional recognition task in which they were asked to identify the emotional face from the neutral faces in an array. CU traits were assessed using the Low Concern (LC) and Punishment Insensitivity (PI) subscales of the Multidimensional Assessment Profile of Disruptive Behavior (MAP-DB), which were specifically designed to differentiate the normative misbehavior of early childhood from atypical patterns.
Results
High LC, but not PI, scores were associated with a fear-specific deficit in emotion recognition. Girls were more accurate than boys in identifying emotional expressions but no significant interaction between LC or PI and sex was observed.
Conclusions
Fear recognition deficits associated with CU traits in older individuals were observed in preschoolers with developmentally-defined patterns of low concern for others. Confirming that the link between CU-related impairments in empathy and distinct neurocognitive deficits is present in very young children suggests that developmentally-specified measurement can detect the substrates of these severe behavioral patterns beginning much earlier than prior work. Exploring the development of CU traits and disruptive behavior disorders at very early ages may provide insights critical to early intervention and prevention of severe antisocial behavior.
Keywords: disruptive behavior, emotion recognition, developmental, callous-unemotional traits
Callous-unemotional (CU) traits in the presence of conduct problems delineate a group of youth at increased risk of a more severe, chronic and violent pattern of antisocial behavior (Frick, Ray, Thornton, & Kahn, 2014). Accordingly, CU traits have been incorporated into the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-5th Edition as the “with limited prosocial emotions” specifier for Conduct Disorder (APA, 2013). Although the majority of work examining CU traits has involved adolescents and youth in middle to late childhood, there is increasing evidence that CU traits may be identifiable early in life (Frick & Viding, 2009; Kotler & McMahon, 2005; Nichols et al., 2014; Waller, Hyde, Grabell, Alves, & Olson, 2015). Testing theoretical assumptions regarding developmental trajectories of the behavioral problems associated with CU traits (Blair, 2013; Frick & White, 2008; Wakschlag, Tolan, & Leventhal, 2010) requires tools to assess the expression of these traits in a developmentally sensitive manner.
Recently, multiple independent studies have found that callous behaviors can be identified in early childhood, generally using slightly modified downward extensions of measures designed for older individuals (Ezpeleta, de la Osa, Granero, Penelo, & Domenech, 2013; Hyde et al., 2013; Kimonis et al., 2006; Willoughby, Waschbusch, Moore, & Propper, 2011). Yet many of the items useful in assessing CU traits in older individuals may not optimally capture the developmental phenomenology of CU traits in young children (Wakschlag et al., 2010). However, recent work has linked CU traits in preschoolers to decrements in moral regulation, guilt and empathy similar to those seen in older children (Waller et al., 2015).
In prior work in this sample, we have established the validity of a developmentally sensitive, multi-dimensional measure of disruptive behavior in very young children; the Multidimensional Assessment Profile of Disruptive Behavior (MAP-DB; Nichols et al., 2014; Wakschlag et al., 2014; Wakschlag, Henry, et al., 2012). Two MAP-DB subscales were conceptualized as developmental manifestations of CU traits; the Low Concern (LC) subscale and the Punishment Insensitivity (PI) subscale (Nichols et al., 2014). The LC subscale assesses a child’s insensitivity to the feelings of others and includes items such as “Doesn’t seem to care about pleasing others” and “Doesn’t seem to care when others feel sad” (Wakschlag et al., 2014). On the other hand, the PI subscale assesses a child’s failure to demonstrate behavior change following punishment and includes items such as “Blame someone else for something s/he did wrong” and “Keep on misbehaving no matter what you do” (Nichols et al., 2014). Furthermore, we have demonstrated that high PI scores, but not LC scores, are associated with poorer performance on a reinforcement-learning task in very young children (Briggs-Gowan et al., 2014), consistent with passive avoidance learning deficits in older youth with CU traits and adult psychopaths (Blair, 2007). Our recent work in older youth suggests that decision-making, and fear-identification deficits are associated with distinct pathways to antisocial behavior (Blair, Leibenluft, & Pine, 2014). However, the extent to which these differentiated patterns are evident in early childhood has not been tested.
In adolescents and adults, impairment in processing the emotional expressions of others, particularly for distress cues, has been related to CU traits (see Blair et al., 2014). This impairment can be indexed as: (i) impaired facial and vocal distress expression recognition (Dawel, O' Kearney, McKone, & Palermo, 2012; Marsh & Blair, 2008; Stevens, Charman, & Blair, 2001); (ii) reduced psychophysiological responses to distress expressions (Blair, Jones, Clark, & Smith, 1997; Vaidyanathan, Hall, Patrick, & Bernat, 2011); and (iii) reduced amygdala responses (Marsh et al., 2008; Viding et al., 2012; White et al., 2012) to distress expressions. Critically, degree of impairment in response to distress expressions has been shown to be positively associated with level of CU traits (e.g. Blair, Budhani, Colledge, & Scott, 2005; Vaidyanathan et al., 2011; Viding et al., 2012; White et al., 2012).
The goal of the current study was to examine emotional expression recognition deficits in very young children with developmentally-defined patterns of callous behavior as measured by the MAP-DB and a developmentally sensitive emotion-processing task. A large community sample of preschoolers, oversampled for disruptive behavior risk, completed a task in which children discriminated between emotional (either fearful, happy or angry) and neutral faces. The current study investigated the extent to which the LC and PI subscales of the MAP-DB were associated with deficits in emotional expression recognition accuracy and response latency. Based on previous work (e.g. Dawel et al., 2012) and theory (e.g. Blair et al., 2014), we predicted that deficits in emotional expression recognition accuracy and response latency would be fear-specific and uniquely associated with LC.
Methods
Participants
The current study’s participants were drawn from Phase II of the Multidimensional Assessment of Preschoolers (MAPS) Study, which recruited preschoolers from five Chicago-based pediatric clinic waiting rooms (for details see Figure 1 and Nichols et al., 2014). The MAPS project obtained a sample broadly stratified by child age, sex, race/ ethnicity, and poverty. English and Spanish-speaking parents accompanied their 3–5 year old child to the clinic and were invited to participate in a survey study about the MAP-DB if they were the child’s legal guardian. The survey had a response rate of approximately 80%, which yielded 1,857 completed surveys. Survey completion was incentivized by the offer of $20 with a $10 bonus for completion of the survey prior to leaving the clinic.
A stratified, random sample was drawn from the Phase II sample to participate in an intensive laboratory-based protocol to assess correlates and mechanisms of emergent psychopathology pathways. The subsample was restricted to children without significant delays or neurodevelopmental conditions and their English-speaking, biological mothers. We oversampled children with (a) disruptive behavior above the 80th percentile on the MAP-DB (Wakschlag et al., 2014) and (b) children of mothers who reported past-year intimate partner violence exposure (for details see Nichols et al., 2014). The analytic sample for this paper includes children who met all eligibility criteria for the intensive sub-study, participated in the first laboratory visit when the emotion identification task was administered (see Figure 1), and had usable data from this task.
Specifically, 425 children participated in the neurocognitive task component of the intensive sub-study. Twenty-eight participants with developmental delays or neurodevelopmental conditions identified at the visit were excluded from the task-based analyses. Of the 397 remaining participants, usable data were acquired for 84.9% (n=337). Task data were subsequently determined to be unusable for 60 children (see Figure 1).
These 337 children did not differ from the children who attended visits but did not have usable data (n=60) in terms of intimate partner violence exposure [χ2=0.06, p>0.81]. However, they were more likely to be girls [54.9% vs. 36.7%; χ2=6.78, p<.01], less likely to be living in poverty [46.3% vs. 61.7%; χ2=4.82, p=.03] less likely to have mothers with low education (high school or less) [21.2% vs. 37.3%; χ2=7.24, p<.01] and more likely to be Caucasian [22.0% vs. 5.0%; χ2=4.82–9.37, p<.05]. Additionally, these children tended to be older than children without usable data [mean age in years = 4.8(0.8) vs. 3.8(0.5); t=2.32, p=.02], and had lower levels of total disruptive behavior [67.3(51.5) vs. 89.2(69.5); t=12.47; p<.01]. However, relevant to the aims of this paper, they had similar levels of LC and PI scores [t=1.29–1.56, p>.1].
Average child age in the sample was 4.82 years (SD=0.8, range=3.1–7.2). Ninety-two percent of children were 3–5 years of age but 8% had turned 6 and <1% 7 by the time of their laboratory visit. Consistent with the oversampling strategy, 41.8% had high levels of disruptive behavior on the MAP-DB and 21.4% had mothers who had experienced intimate partner violence. Participant characteristics are presented for the full sample and across the PI and LC groupings in Table 1. High and low PI and LC groups were defined as above and below the 80th percentile on these subscales respectively. Differences in gender and, race/ ethnicity breakdown were not observed between high and low PI groups (χ2=.806–3.920, p>.27). There was no difference in proportion of males and females for LC (χ2=1.531, p=0.22). However, those with high LC scores were less likely to be of Hispanic ethnicity (25.2% vs. 34.4%, χ2=8.767, p=0.03) than those with low LC scores (see Table 1).
Table 1.
Analyzed sample | Low Concern Disregard | Punishment Insensitivity | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(N=337) | Low (N=230) |
High (N=107) |
Low (N=241) |
High (N=96) |
||||||||
N(%) | N(%) | N(%) | χ2 | N(%) | N(%) | χ2 | ||||||
Gender | ||||||||||||
Males | 152 (45.1%) | 109 (47.4%) | 43 (40.2%) | 105 (43.6%) | 47 (49.0%) | |||||||
Females | 185 (54.9%) | 121 (52.6%) | 64 (59.8%) | 1.53 | 136 (54.6%) | 49 (51.0%) | 0.81 | |||||
N(%) | N(%) | N(%) | χ2 | N(%) | N(%) | χ2 | ||||||
Ethnicity/Race | ||||||||||||
African-American | 152 (45.1%) | 104 (45.2%) | 48 (44.9%) | 107 (44.4%) | 45 (46.9%) | |||||||
European-American | 74 (31.5%) | 42 (18.3%) | 32 (29.9%) | 49 (20.3%) | 25 (26.0%) | |||||||
Hispanic | 106 (22.0%) | 79 (34.3%) | 27 (25.2%) | 80 (33.2%) | 26 (27.1%) | |||||||
Other | 5 (1.5%) | 5 (2.2%) | 0 (0%) | 8.77* | 5 (2.1%) | 0 (0%) | 3.92 | |||||
N(%) | N(%) | N(%) | χ2 | N(%) | N(%) | χ2 | ||||||
Socio-economic factors | ||||||||||||
Employed mother | 181 (53.9%) | 124 (45.2%) | 57 (53.8%) | <.01 | 129 (53.8%) | 52 (46.9%) | <.01 | |||||
Mother Ed <= HS | 70 (21.2%) | 48 (45.2%) | 22 (20.8%) | 0.02 | 50 (21.2%) | 20 (46.9%) | <.01 | |||||
Living in poverty | 156 (46.3%) | 107 (45.2%) | 49 (45.8%) | 0.02 | 105 (43.6%) | 51 (46.9%) | 2.52 | |||||
Married/cohabitating | 221 (65.6%) | 149 (64.8%) | 72 (67.3%) | 0.20 | 164 (68.1%) | 57 (59.4%) | 2.23 | |||||
M | (SE) | M | (SE) | M | (SE) | F | M | (SE) | M | (SE) | F | |
Differential Ability Scales | 99.47 | (0.98) | 99.31 | (1.05) | 95.57 | (1.98) | 2.79 | 99.33 | (1.04) | 95.34 | (2.02) | 3.08 |
Child-Directed Aggression | 7.98 | (0.66) | 7.30 | (0.81) | 9.43 | (1.17) | 2.24 | 6.58 | (0.64) | 11.47 | (1.65) | 11.35* |
p<.05,
SE=standard error
Procedures
Mothers and children completed a series of observational and behavioral tasks in the course of two three-hour laboratory visits. Informed consent was obtained from mothers. Transportation expenses were reimbursed and participant families were compensated $210 for their time participating in all components of the laboratory visits. Children and mothers were assessed separately. Northwestern University and University of Connecticut Health Center Institutional Review Boards approved all study procedures.
Measures
Emotional Faces Task (LoBue, 2014; Lobue & DeLoache, 2008)
In the Emotional Faces Task, children were presented with an array of images (see Figure 2A). Each array had nine different shapes and the children had to select the target shape. The task was administered using a touchscreen computer and children had to touch the appropriate image to select it and move on to the next trial. If the child’s response was correct, an orange border appeared around the image. If the child’s response was incorrect, a grey border appeared around the image.
Given the age range of the sample, participants completed a baseline array in the first block of the task. Children were instructed to discriminate between a target shape and distractor shapes (see Figure 2–A1). The target shapes were identical in each trial (see Figure 2–A2). Participants completed three practice trials in a 2 × 2 matrix and twenty-seven task trials in a 3 × 3 matrix. This shapes block was included as a covariate in the model. Children with less than 55% accuracy were excluded.
Following the first block, blocks 2–4 were the task conditions of interest and were randomized between participants. Each block presented 27 trials and contained target faces of one emotion to create a fearful expression block, an angry expression block and a happy expression block. Images were taken from the NimStim stimulus set (Tottenham et al., 2009). Participants were presented with arrays displaying nine faces in a 3 × 3 matrix (see Figure 2B). Each array consisted of eight neutral adult faces and one emotional adult face. Participants were always instructed to identify the emotional face (e.g. “look for the angry face; find the angry face as fast as you can”). The task was administered using a touchscreen computer and children had to touch the appropriate emotion to select it and move to the next trial. Again, after a correct response, an orange border appeared around the image; after an incorrect response, a grey border appeared around the image.
Trials that were less than 100 ms, longer than 7 seconds, or more than 4 standard deviations from an individual child’s mean latency were removed. Accuracy and latency were calculated only if there were a minimum of 9 usable trials for that emotion condition. Latency was calculated as the mean response time within emotion condition for accurate trials.
Developmental Expression of Callous/Unemotional Traits
Low Concern
Parents were asked to rate behaviors associated with LC over the previous month on a six-point scale [0=Never; 1=Rarely (less than once per week); 2=Some (1–3 days of the week); 3=Most (4–6 days of the week); 4=Every day of the week; and 5=Many times each day]. The LC subscale contains 9 items (see Table 2). The LC subscale has acceptable internal consistency (α=.81) and test-retest reliability (ICC=.83 over a mean of 161.2 days, ± 84.9) and has been psychometrically validated for use in early childhood (Wakschlag et al., 2014).
Table 2.
Punishment Insensitivity Items | Low Concern Items |
---|---|
1. Act like s/he didn’t hear you when you said “no” | 1. Not care about other’s feelings when frustrated, angry, or upset |
2. Deny he or she did something that was not allowed | 2. Not seem to care about parent’s feelings |
3. Act like rules didn’t matter | 3. Keep on doing something that was scaring or upsetting someone |
4. Keep on misbehaving “no matter what you do” | 4. Not seem to care about other adults’ feelings |
5. Act like s/he did not know “right from wrong” | 5. Act like s/he did not care about pleasing other people |
6. Not care when punished | 6. Act like s/he did not care when someone was mad or upset |
7. Refuse to apologize | 7. Enjoy making others mad |
8. Do things to humiliate or embarrass others | |
9. Act like s/he did not care when someone felt bad or sad |
Punishment Insensitivity
The PI subscale includes seven items (see Table 2) that parents rated based on their child’s response when punished or disciplined in the previous month, using a six-point scale [0=Never, 1=Hardly Ever, 2=Sometimes, 3=Often, 4=Most of the time, 5=All the time] (Nichols, 2014). The PI subscale has acceptable internal consistency (α= .73) and test-retest reliability (ICC=.86 over a mean of 161.2 days, ± 84.9) and has been psychometrically validated for use in early childhood (Nichols et al., 2014).
The correlation between the two scales was .72. For both scales, a two-group categorical variable was calculated to differentiate High (≥80th percentile) and Low (<80th percentile) scores. Thresholds for these categories were defined using the normative distributions from the MAPS Phase II sample (Wakschlag et al., 2014).
Covariates
Other dimensions of disruptive behavior
In order to establish the specificity of the associations between PI, LC and emotion processing, our models also accounted for impulsivity, irritability, aggression and non-verbal reasoning.
Impulsivity
Child impulsivity was assessed via parent ratings of the 10-item Activity/Impulsivity subscale of the preschool version of the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA; Carter & Briggs-Gowan, 2006). Internal consistency in the current sample was acceptable (α = .81).
Irritability
Child irritability was assessed with the MAP-DB Temper Loss dimension, which has demonstrated acceptable psychometric properties (α = .97. test-retest reliability = .72, n = 38; Wakschlag, Choi, et al., 2012) and validity in capturing underlying severity of problems with temper tantrums and irritable mood in children (Wakschlag, Choi, et al., 2012; Wakschlag et al., 2015). Temper loss is a defining feature of preschool disruptive behavior and thus was included as a potential covariate.
Aggression
Child aggression was assessed with the MAP-DB Aggression dimension, which has demonstrated acceptable psychometric properties (α = .94, test-retest reliability = .85) (Wakschlag et al., 2014). The aggression dimension has been shown to capture a wide range of aggressive behaviors as expressed in young children including physical, verbal and relational aggression, and to be useful in discriminating between abnormal aggression and aggressive behavioral within the normal range (Wakschlag et al., 2014). Aggression was entered as a covariate to ensure any findings regarding LC or PI were not due to an association with increased aggression.
Child-directed aggression
Eight items from the Conflict Tactics Scales Parent-Child version (CTSPC; Straus, Hamby, Finkelhor, Moore, & Runyan, 1998) were used to measure child-directed physical aggression by the mother (e.g., You shook your child). Due to concern for compromising reliability of mother’s response, very severe and rare behaviors in the CTSPC were not included. The scale score was calculated, following administration guidelines, by summing the frequency value for each response, using the midpoint (i.e., 0 = Never, 1 = Once, 2 = Twice 4 = 3–5 Times, 8 = 6–10 Times, 15 = 11–20 times, 25 = > 20 Times). Internal consistency was acceptable (α = .61).
Nonverbal reasoning
Child nonverbal reasoning was directly assessed with the Picture Similarities subscale of the Differential Ability Scales-Second Edition (DAS; Elliott, 1983). The DAS yields scaled ability scores (sample MN=99.5, SD=19.9). Nonverbal reasoning was assessed as a proxy for IQ. Significant differences between high and low PI and LC groups were not observed on DAS scores (see Table 1).
Analytic Plan
Due to their potential associations with task performance, child age, sex, Temper Loss, Aggression, impulsivity, Shapes block accuracy and response latency, nonverbal reasoning and socio-demographic characteristics were reviewed to determine whether they should be included as covariates in models (Table 3). Variables associated with either task performance or LC/PI were included as covariates. Models also conservatively included CTSPC child-directed aggression due to oversampling based on family violence. The primary analyses consisted of two sets of analyses to test for the effects of LC for others emotions and PI on (i) accuracy of emotion identification and (ii) latency. Within each set, the first analysis was tested as a repeated-measures Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) via SAS PROC GLM, which is appropriate for unbalanced designs. The three emotion conditions were the repeated dependent variables (i.e., fear accuracy, angry accuracy and happy accuracy). Independent variables were grouping variables reflecting LC, PI and the interaction between LC and PI. Within subject effects were adjusted for sphericity with the Greenhouse-Geisser (GG) adjustment. All models included relevant covariates. To protect against chance findings due to multiple comparisons, univariate analyses were conducted only when there was a significant main effect. Pairwise comparisons among groups were adjusted for multiple comparisons with the Bonferroni adjustment. A p-value of .05 was used for all analyses.
Table 3.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. LC | 1 | .688** | −.088 | .053 | −.051 | .083 | .028 | .044 | −.023 | .734** | .785** | .415** | −.056 | .121* |
2. PI | 1 | −.137* | .121* | −.102a | .174** | .003 | −.047 | −.100a | .681** | .668** | .512** | −.167** | .211** | |
3. Response Accuracy | 1 | −.305** | .573** | −.364** | .151** | .085 | .351** | −.069 | −.143** | −.140* | .401** | −.049 | ||
4. Response Latency | 1 | −.354** | .488** | −.038 | .095a | −.477** | .137* | .058 | .061 | −.238** | −.010 | |||
5. Shapes Accuracy | 1 | −.398** | .078 | .123* | .407** | −.018 | −.132* | −.047 | .457** | .047 | ||||
6. Shapes Latency | 1 | −.057 | .001 | −.597** | .065 | .118* | .157** | −.464** | .002 | |||||
7. Sex | 1 | −.024 | −.097a | .010 | −.016 | −.039 | .058 | −.014 | ||||||
8. Race | 1 | −.136* | .090a | −.024 | −.154** | .064 | −.060 | |||||||
9. Age | 1 | −.088 | −.042 | −.090 | .561** | .005 | ||||||||
10. Temper Loss | 1 | .788** | .501** | −.045 | .137* | |||||||||
11. Aggression | 1 | .485** | −.132* | .169** | ||||||||||
12. Impulsivity | 1 | −.062 | .226** | |||||||||||
13. DAS | 1 | −.037 | ||||||||||||
14. Child-Directed Aggression | 1 |
p<.01,
p<.05,
p<.1;
LC=Low Concern; PI=Punishment Insensitivity; DAS=Differential Ability Scales
Analyses employed weights that account for both unequal probabilities of selection and differential non-response rates. Model estimation procedures accounted for the complex stratified sampling design wherever possible. For example, the survey design-based estimator (available in SAS) was employed via PROC SURVEYREG to account for the stratification and unequal probabilities of selection in the sample design.
Results
Correlations among study variables are presented in Table 3. Child-directed aggression was not significantly associated with accuracy or response latency. The patterns observed indicate increased response accuracy and faster latencies for shapes and faces with increases in child age. Further, the observed data indicate increased faces accuracy for girls, but not faster response latencies. Children’s accuracy and latency in the Shapes block were positively correlated with accuracy and latency in the Emotion blocks (r=.57 & .49 respectively, p<.01). Nonverbal reasoning, age, sex, Temper Loss, Aggression and Impulsivity (but not race/ ethnicity) were significantly correlated with accuracy and/or latency, supporting their inclusion as covariates in the models. As such, these variables were included as covariates in the model.
Response Accuracy
A repeated-measures ANCOVA was conducted to examine the influence on response accuracy to emotional facial expressions (fear relative to anger relative to happy) of PI (below 80th percentile, above 80th percentile) and LC (below 80th percentile, above 80th percentile) while accounting for variance due to the continuous covariates of age, child-directed aggression, Temper Loss, Aggression, Activity/Impulsivity and Shapes block response accuracy and a dichotomous covariate of sex. Significant main effects were observed for sex [F(1,325)=7.12, p=.008] and Shapes block response accuracy [F(1,325)=79.98, p<.001]. That is, Shapes block accuracy was associated with increased accuracy and females were more accurate than males.
A significant interaction effect was observed for the emotional facial expression-by-LC interaction [F(2,650)=7.84, GG Epsilon=.961, p<.001], but not for the emotional facial expression-by-PI interaction [F(2,650)=1.11, p=.328]. Review of the univariate ANCOVAs, which included the PI and LC groups, indicated that a significant difference between low and high LC groups was observed for fearful faces [difference=−.046, 95% CI=−.087 to −.005; F(1, 327)=4.95, p=.027], but not angry [difference=−.025, 95% CI=−.001 to .050; F(1, 327)=3.56, p=..060] or happy faces [difference=−.027, 95% CI=−.063 to .009; F(1, 327)=2.16, p=.143]. The high LC group was less accurate in identifying fearful faces than the low LC group, while accuracy rates for the high and low LC groups did not significantly differ for angry or happy faces (see Figure 3). Results of models that tested for interactions between LC and PI and child sex revealed no significant effect on accuracy, [Between Subjects F(1,324)=.00 to .65, p>.41; Within Subjects F(2,648)=1.19 to 1.39, GG Epsilon=.960 to .965, p>.24].
Response Latency
A parallel repeated-measures ANCOVA was conducted to examine the influence on response latency to emotional facial expressions of PI and LC while accounting for variance due to the continuous covariates of age, child-directed aggression, temper loss, aggression, impulsivity, nonverbal reasoning and Shapes block response latency and a dichotomous covariate of sex. Significant main effects were observed for age [F(1,325)=24.68, p<.001], temper loss [F(1,325)=12.19, p<.001] and Shapes block response latency [F(1,325)=35.30, p<.001]. Greater age was associated with quicker response latencies, whereas increased levels of Temper Loss and slower response latencies on Shapes block were associated with slower response latencies.
Significant interaction effects were observed for the emotional facial expression-by-age [F(2,650)=3.44, GG Epsilon=.976, p=.034] and emotional facial expression-by-nonverbal reasoning score interactions [F(2,650)=4.80, p=.009]. Significant effects for the emotional facial expression-by-PI or emotional facial expression-by-LC interactions were not observed [FPI(2,650)=2.17, p=.117; FLC(2,650)=1.64, p=.195]. These patterns held when tests for moderation by sex were evaluated in a second model, with no significant emotional facial expression-by-sex-by-PI or emotional-facial-expression-by-sex-by-LC interactions [Within Subjects F(2,648)<1.22, Greenhouse-Geisser Epsilon=.975, p>.298] and no significant sex-by-LC interactions [FBetween(1, 324)=.64, p=.425). There was a significant sex-by-PI interaction [FBetween(1, 324)=4.19, p=.042]; however, these differences did not hold up when examined within boys and girls separately [F(1,174=1.45, p=.231 and F(1,141)=2.91, p=.090].
The magnitude of the inverse relationship between age and response latency was observed to be greater for angry expressions [r=−.471] relative to happy expressions [r=−.404, Steiger’s Z=2.01, p=.04]. The relationship between age and response latency to fearful expressions [r= −.428] did not significantly differ from the relationship between age and response latency to happy [Steiger’s Z=0.68, p=.50] or angry expressions [Steiger’s Z=1.21, p=.23]. The magnitude of the inverse relationship between nonverbal reasoning scores and response latency was observed to be greater for fearful expressions [r=−.277] relative to happy expressions [r=−.179, Steiger’s Z=2.30, p=.02] and relative to angry expressions [r=−.186, Steiger’s Z=2.56, p=.01]. The magnitude of the relationship between nonverbal reasoning scores and response latency did not significantly for angry relative to happy faces [Steiger’s Z=.19, p=.85].
Discussion
The goal of the current study was to employ developmentally-sensitive methods to examine the extent to which a distinct pattern of fear recognition deficits associated with empathic features of CU traits in older individuals is also evident in early childhood. A fear-specific deficit in emotion recognition was observed in preschoolers with high LC scores, similar to older individuals with high CU traits. This relationship was significant when accounting for the contributions of child-directed aggression, sex, age, shape discrimination, nonverbal reasoning, and other behavioral correlates. No significant interactions between LC or PI and sex were observed.
The current data indicate that the fear-specific accuracy deficits in the identification of distress facial expressions associated with high levels of CU related empathic deficits in older youth (Dawel et al., 2012; Marsh & Blair, 2008) can be observed in preschoolers. This is a strong indication of the validity of the LC subscale of the MAP-DB as a measure of early expression of CU traits in preschoolers. A developmentally appropriate measure of CU traits will allow researchers to test theoretical assumptions about the development of CU traits and antisocial behavior in younger children than is currently possible (Wakschlag, Henry, et al., 2012).
One such theoretical assumption is that the neural deficits associated with CU traits observed in older children and adults are present in very young children. The fear-specific emotion recognition deficits associated with CU traits are argued to be the product of amygdala hypo-responsivity to fear/distress cues (Blair et al., 2014). Indeed, the level of dysfunction during emotion recognition (Blair et al., 2005) and during viewing of emotional expressions (Viding et al., 2012; White et al., 2012) has been related to level of CU traits. The common neurobehavioral deficits seen across development suggest that, consistent with theoretical positions (e.g., Blair et al., 2014), the amygdala dysfunction during emotion recognition observed in older youth and adults with high levels of CU traits are present early in development. This is consistent with the developmental processes theorized to be associated with deficits in the development of conscience. Specifically, multiple theorists (e.g., Blair, 2007; Kochanska & Aksan, 2006) have suggested that children with impaired conscience development and moral regulation have difficulty recognizing emotional distress in others and therefore have reduced responsiveness to socialization. Given that substrates of conscience can be measured as early as toddlerhood (Kochanska & Aksan, 2006), the present findings suggest that the early emergence of the atypicalities that mark callous patterns may be detectable (and preventable) well before severe antisocial behavior is developmentally possible.
Additionally, girls were more accurate, but not faster, than boys in identifying emotional expressions. Female adults (Hoffmann, Kessler, Eppel, Rukavina, & Traue, 2010) and youth (McClure, 2000) have consistently shown better expression recognition than males. Importantly, however, no significant interactions with sex were found in this sample. This is consistent with other work failing to find significant interactions between sex and CU traits (e.g., Dadds et al., 2014; Horan, Brown, Jones, & Aber, 2014; Stickle, Marini, & Thomas, 2012).
It is worth noting that high levels of PI did not significantly predict the fear-specific emotion deficits in the current study despite the fact that PI and LC were correlated. In previous work using the current sample, PI was associated with reinforcement-learning deficits, although LC was not (Briggs-Gowan et al., 2014). This differentiated pattern of LC to fear identification deficits reported here and our prior reports of PI’s association to decision-making deficits in early childhood (Briggs-Gowan et al., 2014) is strikingly similar to differentiated patterns found in older youth (Dawel et al., 2012; Marsh & Blair, 2008; Viding et al., 2012; White, Clanton, et al., 2014; White, Fowler, et al., 2014; White et al., 2013).
It is also worth noting that the environmental variable, child-directed aggression, did not show a relationship with task performance. Clearly, environmental variables have a significant role in developmental pathways for externalizing behavior (Moffitt, 2003). Adverse environmental experiences, such as child-directed aggression, increase the risk for externalizing behavior (Wilson, Stover, & Berkowitz, 2009). However, this appears to particularly occur through increasing threat sensitivity (Dodge, 1980; Dodge et al., 2015). This has been indexed as increased sensitivity to angry expressions (Pollak & Tolley-Schell, 2003; Pollak et al., 2005; Shackman & Pollak, 2014) rather than decreased sensitivity to fearful expressions that here related to LC and in work with older youth has related to callous-unemotional traits (Dawel et al., 2012; Marsh & Blair, 2008). Of course, there may be other environmental variables that are associated with decreased sensitivity to the fearful expressions of others (Blair, Leibenluft, & Pine, 2014). While beyond the scope of the current paper, a more thorough investigation of the role of environmental processes in this and other developmental pathways for externalizing behavior will be addressed in other analyses involving this sample.
Although our hypotheses regarding response accuracy were supported, it is notable that our hypotheses with respect to response latencies were not supported. High LC/PI scores were unrelated to fear-specific increases in response latency. There are several possible explanations for this null finding. This finding may reflect type II error. Alternatively, the wide range of response latencies associated with age within the sample may have obscured any response latency findings. However, it is also possible that the relationship between LC and emotion recognition is limited to accuracy.
These data have potentially important clinical implications. Preventing or intervening early in cases of disruptive behavior is more effective during this period of heightened plasticity rather than when the disruptive behaviors have become entrenched (Dodge, Bierman, et al., 2015). By demonstrating that the neurocognitive substrates of developmentally-defined deficits in concern for others parallel those found in older youth with CU traits, these findings suggest that low concern for others is an early manifestation of CU traits. Combined with a burgeoning body of evidence demonstrating callous patterns in early childhood using developmentally-based methods (Nichols et al., 2014; Waller et al., 2015), the current data suggest that early identification of young children predisposed to this severe developmental pathway may be efficacious. Importantly, a promising intervention targeting empathic emotion deficits in young children with callous traits is currently being tested (Hawes, Price, & Dadds, 2014).
Linking developmental expressions of early callousness and specific neurobehavioral deficits in very young children when using developmentally sensitive methods further supports the notion that developmental specification may elucidate prodromal patterns well in advance of serious antisocial behavior. Of course, longitudinal prediction to callous behavior at ages where the validity of the construct of CU traits is well established and examination of the continuity of these patterns of behavior across developmental periods (as well as factors that buffer against CU risk in young children) will be important next steps. We are currently conducting a longitudinal follow-up of the MAPS sample for this purpose. It is our hope that these next steps will provide insights critical to neuroscience-based targeted early intervention and prevention of severe and chronic antisocial syndromes.
Acknowledgments
This study would not have been possible without the outstanding efforts of Jacqueline Kestler, MPH, Elvira Zobel, Dipl, and the MAPS Study dedicated research staff, and the generous participation of MAPS study families. We are grateful to Vanessa LoBue, Ph.D., for sharing her search task with this study. We thank the pediatric clinics and participants from Rush University, the University of Illinois at Chicago, NorthShore University Health System, North Suburban Pediatrics and the following pediatric research group practices for their participation: Healthlinc in Valparaiso, IN, Healthlinc in Michigan City, IN, and Associated Pediatricians in Valparaiso, IN.
Drs. White and Blair were supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health (1-ZIA-MH002860), Dr. Blair principal investigator. Drs. Wakschlag, Briggs-Gowan, Voss and Ms. Kimberly McCarthy were supported by National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grants R01MH082830, 2U01MH082830 and U01MH090301. Dr. Wakschlag was also supported by the Walden & Jean Young Shaw Foundation.
Footnotes
The authors have no financial interest or benefit arising from the direct applications of this research.
References
- APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Blair RJ. The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(11):786–799. doi: 10.1038/nrn3577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair RJ, Budhani S, Colledge E, Scott S. Deafness to fear in boys with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2005;46(3):327–336. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00356.x. doi: JCPP356 [pii] 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00356.x [doi] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair RJ, Jones L, Clark F, Smith M. The psychopathic individual: a lack of responsiveness to distress cues? Psychophysiology. 1997;34(2):192–198. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02131.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair RJ, Leibenluft E, Pine DS. Conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits in youth. N Engl J Med. 2014;371(23):2207–2216. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1315612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair RJR. The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007;11(9):387–392. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briggs-Gowan MJ, Nichols SR, Voss J, Zobel E, Carter AS, McCarthy KJ, Wakschlag LS. Punishment insensitivity and impaired reinforcement learning in preschoolers. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2014;55(2):154–161. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ. ITSEA Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment Examiner’s Manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Dadds MR, Moul C, Cauchi A, Dobson-Stone C, Hawes DJ, Brennan J, Ebstein RE. Polymorphisms in the oxytocin receptor gene are associated with the development of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol. 2014;26(1):21–31. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawel A, O'Kearney R, McKone E, Palermo R. Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(10):2288–2304. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA. Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child Dev. 1980;51(1):162–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA, Bierman KL, Coie JD, Greenberg MT, Lochman JE, McMahon RJ, Pinderhughes EE. Impact of early intervention on psychopathology, crime, and well-being at age 25. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(1):59–70. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13060786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge KA, Malone PS, Lansford JE, Sorbring E, Skinner AT, Tapanya S, Pastorelli C. Hostile attributional bias and aggressive behavior in global context. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(30):9310–9315. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1418572112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott CD. Differential Abilities Scales: Introductory and technical handbook. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Ezpeleta L, de la Osa N, Granero R, Penelo E, Domenech JM. Inventory of callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of preschoolers. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2013;42(1):91–105. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.734221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychological bulletin. 2014;140(1):1–57. doi: 10.1037/a0033076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, Viding E. Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol. 2009;21(4):1111–1131. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409990071. doi: S0954579409990071 [pii] 10.1017/S0954579409990071 [doi] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, White SF. Research review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2008;49(4):359–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawes DJ, Price MJ, Dadds MR. Callous-unemotional traits and the treatment of conduct problems in childhood and adolescence: A comprehensive review. Clincal child and family psychology review. 2014;17(3):248–267. doi: 10.1007/s10567-014-0167-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann H, Kessler H, Eppel T, Rukavina S, Traue HC. Expression intensity, gender and facial emotion recognition: Women recognize only subtle facial emotions better than men. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010;135(3):278–283. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horan JM, Brown JL, Jones SM, Aber JL. Assessing Invariance Across Sex and Race/Ethnicity in Measures of Youth Psychopathic Characteristics. Psychological assessment. 2014 doi: 10.1037/pas0000043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyde LW, Shaw DS, Gardner F, Cheong J, Dishion TJ, Wilson M. Dimensions of callousness in early childhood: Links to problem behavior and family intervention effectiveness. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(2):347–363. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412001101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimonis ER, Frick PJ, Boris NW, Smyke AT, Cornell AH, Farrell JM, Zeanah CH. Callous-Unemotional Features, Behavioral Inhibition, and Parenting: Independent Predictors of Aggression in a High-Risk Preschool Sample. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2006;15(6):745–756. [Google Scholar]
- Kochanska G, Aksan N. Children's conscience and self-regulation. J Pers. 2006;74(6):1587–1617. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00421.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotler JS, McMahon RJ. Child psychopathy: theories, measurement, and relations with the development and persistence of conduct problems. Clinical child and family psychology review. 2005;8(4):291–325. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-8810-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LoBue V. Measuring attentional biases for threat in children and adults. J Vis Exp. 2014;(92):e52190. doi: 10.3791/52190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobue V, DeLoache JS. Detecting the snake in the grass: attention to fear-relevant stimuli by adults and young children. Psychol Sci. 2008;19(3):284–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02081.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh AA, Blair RJ. Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(3):454–465. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003. doi: S0149-7634(07)00107-8 [pii] 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003 [doi] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh AA, Finger EC, Mitchell DG, Reid ME, Sims C, Kosson DS, Blair RJ. Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(6):712–720. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071145. doi: appi.ajp.2007.07071145 [pii] 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07071145 [doi] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClure EB. A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychological bulletin. 2000;126(3):424–453. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE. Life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review and a research agenda. In: Lahey BB, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, editors. Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press; 2003. pp. 49–75. [Google Scholar]
- Nichols SR, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Estabrook R, Burns JL, Kestler J, Berman G, Wakschlag LS. Punishment Insensitivity in Early Childhood: A Developmental, Dimensional Approach. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2014 doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9950-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak SD, Tolley-Schell SA. Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003;112(3):323–338. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.112.3.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak SD, Vardi S, Putzer Bechner AM, Curtin JJ. Physically abused children's regulation of attention in response to hostility. Child Dev. 2005;76(5):968–977. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00890.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shackman JE, Pollak SD. Impact of physical maltreatment on the regulation of negative affect and aggression. Dev Psychopathol. 2014;26(4 Pt 1):1021–1033. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens D, Charman T, Blair RJ. Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Genet Psychol. 2001;162(2):201–211. doi: 10.1080/00221320109597961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stickle TR, Marini VA, Thomas JN. Gender differences in psychopathic traits, types, and correlates of aggression among adjudicated youth. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2012;40(4):513–525. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9588-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straus MA, Hamby SL, Finkelhor D, Moore DW, Runyan D. "Identification of child maltreatment with the Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents": Erratum. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1998;22(11):1177. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(97)00174-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, Tanaka JW, Leon AC, McCarry T, Nurse M, Hare TA, Nelson C. The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry research. 2009;168(3):242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaidyanathan U, Hall JR, Patrick CJ, Bernat EM. Clarifying the role of defensive reactivity deficits in psychopathy and antisocial personality using startle reflex methodology. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120(1):253–258. doi: 10.1037/a0021224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viding E, Sebastian CL, Dadds MR, Lockwood PL, Cecil CA, De Brito SA, McCrory EJ. Amygdala response to preattentive masked fear in children with conduct problems: the role of callous-unemotional traits. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(10):1109–1116. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakschlag LS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Choi SW, Nichols SR, Kestler J, Burns JL, Henry D. Advancing a multidimensional, developmental spectrum approach to preschool disruptive behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2014;53(1):82.e83–96.e83. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.10.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakschlag LS, Choi SW, Carter AS, Hullsiek H, Burns J, McCarthy K, Briggs-Gowan MJ. Defining the developmental parameters of temper loss in early childhood: implications for developmental psychopathology. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2012;53(11):1099–1108. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02595.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakschlag LS, Estabrook R, Petitclerc A, Henry D, Burns JL, Perlman SB, Briggs-Gowan ML. Clinical Implications of a Dimensional Approach: The Normal:Abnormal Spectrum of Early Irritability. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;54(8):626–634. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakschlag LS, Henry DB, Tolan PH, Carter AS, Burns JL, Briggs-Gowan MJ. Putting theory to the test: modeling a multidimensional, developmentally-based approach to preschool disruptive behavior. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2012;51(6):593.e594–604.e594. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wakschlag LS, Tolan PH, Leventhal BL. Research Review: 'Ain't misbehavin': Towards a developmentally-specified nosology for preschool disruptive behavior. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2010;51(1):3–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02184.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waller R, Hyde LW, Grabell AS, Alves ML, Olson SL. Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2015;56(6):657–666. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White SF, Clanton R, Brislin SJ, Meffert H, Hwang S, Sinclair S, Blair RJ. Reward: empirical contribution. Temporal discounting and conduct disorder in adolescents. Journal of personality disorders. 2014;28(1):5–18. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2014.28.1.5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White SF, Fowler KA, Sinclair S, Schechter JC, Majestic CM, Pine DS, Blair RJ. Disrupted expected value signaling in youth with disruptive behavior disorders to environmental reinforcers. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2014;53(5):579 e579–588 e579. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White SF, Marsh AA, Fowler KA, Schechter JC, Adalio C, Pope K, Blair RJ. Reduced amygdala response in youths with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits: decreased emotional response versus increased top-down attention to nonemotional features. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(7):750–758. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11081270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White SF, Pope K, Sinclair S, Fowler KA, Brislin SJ, Williams WC, Blair RJ. Disrupted expected value and prediction error signaling in youths with disruptive behavior disorders during a passive avoidance task. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(3):315–323. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12060840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willoughby MT, Waschbusch DA, Moore GA, Propper CB. Using the ASEBA to screen for callous unemotional traits in early childhood: Factor structure, temporal stability, and utility. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2011;33(1):19–30. doi: 10.1007/s10862-010-9195-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HW, Stover CS, Berkowitz SJ. Research review: The relationship between childhood violence exposure and juvenile antisocial behavior: a meta-analytic review. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2009;50(7):769–779. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01974.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]