Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2016 Dec 14;5:e20320. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20320

Genetic and environmental influences on adult human height across birth cohorts from 1886 to 1994

Aline Jelenkovic 1,2,*, Yoon-Mi Hur 3, Reijo Sund 1, Yoshie Yokoyama 4, Sisira H Siribaddana 5,6, Matthew Hotopf 7, Athula Sumathipala 5,8, Fruhling Rijsdijk 9, Qihua Tan 10, Dongfeng Zhang 11, Zengchang Pang 12, Sari Aaltonen 1,13, Kauko Heikkilä 13, Sevgi Y Öncel 14, Fazil Aliev 15,16,17, Esther Rebato 2, Adam D Tarnoki 18,19, David L Tarnoki 18,19, Kaare Christensen 20,21,22,23, Axel Skytthe 20,21, Kirsten O Kyvik 24,25, Judy L Silberg 26, Lindon J Eaves 26, Hermine H Maes 27, Tessa L Cutler 28, John L Hopper 28,29, Juan R Ordoñana 30,31, Juan F Sánchez-Romera 31,32, Lucia Colodro-Conde 30,33, Wendy Cozen 34,35, Amie E Hwang 34, Thomas M Mack 34,35, Joohon Sung 29,36, Yun-Mi Song 37, Sarah Yang 29,36, Kayoung Lee 38, Carol E Franz 39, William S Kremen 39,40, Michael J Lyons 41, Andreas Busjahn 42, Tracy L Nelson 43, Keith E Whitfield 44, Christian Kandler 45, Kerry L Jang 46, Margaret Gatz 47,48, David A Butler 49, Maria A Stazi 50, Corrado Fagnani 50, Cristina D'Ippolito 50, Glen E Duncan 51, Dedra Buchwald 52, Catherine A Derom 53,54, Robert F Vlietinck 53, Ruth JF Loos 55,56, Nicholas G Martin 57, Sarah E Medland 57, Grant W Montgomery 58, Hoe-Uk Jeong 59, Gary E Swan 60, Ruth Krasnow 61, Patrik KE Magnusson 48, Nancy L Pedersen 48, Anna K Dahl-Aslan 48,62, Tom A McAdams 9, Thalia C Eley 9, Alice M Gregory 63, Per Tynelius 64, Laura A Baker 47, Catherine Tuvblad 47,65, Gombojav Bayasgalan 66, Danshiitsoodol Narandalai 66,67, Paul Lichtenstein 48, Timothy D Spector 68, Massimo Mangino 68, Genevieve Lachance 68, Meike Bartels 69, Toos CEM van Beijsterveldt 69, Gonneke Willemsen 69, S Alexandra Burt 70, Kelly L Klump 70, Jennifer R Harris 71, Ingunn Brandt 71, Thomas Sevenius Nilsen 71, Robert F Krueger 72, Matt McGue 72, Shandell Pahlen 72, Robin P Corley 73, Jacob v B Hjelmborg 20,21, Jack H Goldberg 74, Yoshinori Iwatani 75, Mikio Watanabe 75, Chika Honda 75, Fujio Inui 75,76, Finn Rasmussen 64, Brooke M Huibregtse 73, Dorret I Boomsma 69, Thorkild I A Sørensen 77,78,79, Jaakko Kaprio 13,80, Karri Silventoinen 1,75
Editor: Eduardo Franco81
PMCID: PMC5156525  PMID: 27964777

Abstract

Human height variation is determined by genetic and environmental factors, but it remains unclear whether their influences differ across birth-year cohorts. We conducted an individual-based pooled analysis of 40 twin cohorts including 143,390 complete twin pairs born 1886–1994. Although genetic variance showed a generally increasing trend across the birth-year cohorts, heritability estimates (0.69-0.84 in men and 0.53-0.78 in women) did not present any clear pattern of secular changes. Comparing geographic-cultural regions (Europe, North America and Australia, and East Asia), total height variance was greatest in North America and Australia and lowest in East Asia, but no clear pattern in the heritability estimates across the birth-year cohorts emerged. Our findings do not support the hypothesis that heritability of height is lower in populations with low living standards than in affluent populations, nor that heritability of height will increase within a population as living standards improve.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.001

Research Organism: Human

Introduction

Height is a classic anthropometric quantitative trait in humans due to its ease of measurement, approximately normal distribution and relative stability in adulthood. Since the studies of height in the late 19th and early 20th centuries (Galton, 1886; Pearson and Lee, 1903; Fisher, 1919), twin, adoption and family studies have shown that height is one of the most heritable human quantitative phenotypes (Silventoinen, 2003). More recently, genetic linkage studies have helped to elucidate the location of genetic effects in the genome (Perola et al., 2007) and genome-wide association (GWA) studies allowed identification of loci consistently associated with height in populations of different ancestry (Cho et al., 2009; Hao et al., 2013; Lango Allen et al., 2010; N'Diaye et al., 2011; Wood et al., 2014). Besides the genetic factors, a multitude of environmental factors, such as nutrition and childhood diseases, operate during the growth period and can affect the final attained height. These and other proximate biological determinants of height are further associated with social and economic conditions, which in turn are associated with living standards (Bozzoli et al., 2009; Bogin, 2001; Eveleth and Tanner, 1990; Steckel, 2009). The secular trend of increasing height over the 20th century observed in many parts of the world, which has slowed or stopped in most northern European countries, probably reflects the continuous improvement in the standard of living (Eveleth and Tanner, 1990; Cole, 2003; Stulp and Barrett, 2016). A recent study showed that the height difference between the tallest and shortest populations a century ago (19–20 cm) has remained the same for women and increased for men (NCD Risk Factor Collaboration (NCD-RisC), 2016) .

Twin and family studies have consistently estimated that the proportion of variation in adult height explained by genetic differences between individuals, or heritability, in general populations is approximately 0.80 (Fisher, 1919Silventoinen et al., 2003Stunkard et al., 1986). There is a hypothesis that heritability is not constant and can differ in environments having different amount of environmental variation. Accordingly, it has been suggested that heritability of height is lower in populations with low living standards compared with affluent populations since poverty can lead to a lack of basic necessities important for human growth in part of the population (Steckel, 2009). However, there is little direct evidence on this issue. A study in Finnish twins born between 1900 and 1957 showed that the heritability of height increased across birth cohorts born in the first half of the century when the standard of living increased and leveled off after World War II thus supporting this hypothesis (Silventoinen et al., 2000). Because this result needs to be replicated, we conducted an individual-based analysis of 40 twin cohorts from 20 countries. We aimed to analyze (i) the genetic and environmental contribution to the variation of adult height across nine birth-year cohorts covering more than 100 years and (ii) to assess whether the pattern varies by geographic-cultural region (Europe, North America and Australia, and East Asia).

Results

In the pooled data (all twin cohorts together), mean height was greater in men than in women and increased over the birth-year cohorts in both sexes; the decrease ( > 1 cm) observed in the latest birth cohort mainly reflects differences in the distribution of different twin cohorts within each group (Table 1). Both means and variances were significantly different between twin cohorts in all birth-year and sex groups. Mean height was shorter in East Asia than in Europe and North America and Australia in all birth-year and sex groups. The increase in mean height over the birth cohorts (from 1940–1949 to 1980–1994) was substantially greater in East Asia than in the other two geographic-cultural regions. The variance of height was generally greater in men than in women, lowest in East Asia and greatest in North America and Australia, and showed a general trend to increase over the birth cohorts.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics of age and height by sex, birth year and geographic-cultural region. Names list of the participating twin cohorts in this study: two cohorts from Australia (Australian Twin Registry and Queensland Twin Register), six cohorts from East-Asia (Korean Twin-Family Register, Mongolian Twin Registry, Osaka University Aged Twin Registry, South Korea Twin Registry, Qingdao Twin Registry of Adults and West Japan Twins and Higher Order Multiple Births Registry), 18 cohorts from Europe (Adult Netherlands Twin Registry, Berlin Twin Register, Bielefeld Longitudinal Study of Adult Twins, Danish Twin Cohort, East Flanders Prospective Twin Survey, Finnish Older Twin Cohort, FinnTwin12, FinnTwin16, Genesis 12–19 Study, Hungarian Twin Registry, Italian Twin Registry, Murcia Twin Registry, Norwegian Twin Registry, Swedish Twin Cohorts, Swedish Young Male Twins Study of Adults, TCHAD-study, TwinsUK and Young Netherlands Twin Registry), two cohorts from South-Asia and Middle-East (Sri Lanka Twin Registry and Turkish Twin Study) and 12 cohorts from North-America (California Twin Program, Carolina African American Twin Study of Aging, Colorado Twin Registry, Michigan State University Twin Registry, Mid Atlantic Twin Registry, Minnesota Twin Registry, NAS-NRC Twin Registry, SRI-international, University of British Columbia Twin Project, University of Southern California Twin Study, University of Washington Twin Registry and Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging).

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.002

Age

Height

All cohorts

All cohorts

Europe

NA and Australia

East Asia

Birth year

Mean

SD

Range

N

Mean (F, p-value)*

SD (F, p-value)

N

Mean

SD

N

Mean

SD

N

Mean

SD

Men

1886–1909

67.0

7.5

53.5–99.2

3747

171.6 (15, < 0.001)

6.34 (2.5,0.019)

3569

171.5

6.27

178

174.6

6.88

1910–1919

52.2

16.2

20.0–95.8

9171

174.2 (23, < 0.001)

6.72 (5.0,<0.001)

4117

173.3

6.37

5052

174.9

6.91

1920–1929

51.6

16.1

20.0–90.9

23147

175.4 (62, < 0.001)

6.81 (5.7,<0.001)

6382

173.9

6.42

16714

176.0

6.82

1930–1939

57.5

10.5

33.5–83.2

12028

175.7 (413, < 0.001)

6.70 (2.9,<0.001)

9308

175.2

6.42

2658

178.1

6.78

1940–1949

49.3

10.6

23.5–73.9

22967

177.4 (72, < 0.001)

6.73 (2.5,<0.001)

16629

177.0

6.53

6235

178.4

6.95

68

164.8

6.57

1950–1959

41.4

10.0

19.5–65.0

24560

178.4 (120, < 0.001)

6.96 (6.5,<0.001)

15199

178.5

6.73

9124

178.7

7.04

161

167.1

4.79

1960–1969

35.5

7.1

19.5–54.0

13264

179.0 (99, < 0.001)

7.49 (2.3,<0.001)

6218

179.6

7.04

6574

179.2

7.22

298

168.1

6.24

1970–1979

28.7

5.4

19.5–44.0

14975

179.9 (121, < 0.001)

7.55 (5.5,<0.001)

10339

180.7

7.01

3906

179.7

7.51

456

170.1

5.68

1980–1994

23.1

3.2

19.5–34.4

9948

178.4 (70, < 0.001)

7.59 (4.9,<0.001)

5077

178.8

7.22

4066

179.4

7.49

329

173.1

6.37

Women

1886–1909

68.5

8.1

53.5–98.0

5423

160.2 (23, < 0.001)

6.14 (3.3,0.006)

5011

160.2

6.11

412

160.2

6.41

1910–1919

62.0

10.9

43.6–95.9

7169

161.1 (18, < 0.001)

5.93 (2.5,0.002)

5621

161.0

5.85

1548

161.2

6.20

1920–1929

59.7

11.4

37.5–91.7

10975

162.1 (65, < 0.001)

5.99 (3.8,<0.001)

7908

162.0

5.89

3052

162.4

6.16

1930–1939

57.9

10.0

33.5–83.0

14610

162.7 (249, < 0.001)

6.05 (5.8,<0.001)

11226

162.5

5.83

3344

163.2

6.49

1940–1949

49.9

10.2

23.5–74.0

28537

163.7 (175, < 0.001)

6.19 (10.3,<0.001)

20097

163.9

5.93

8285

163.5

6.57

100

153.6

5.33

1950–1959

41.3

9.5

19.5–64.0

31250

164.4 (146, < 0.001)

6.58 (13.6,<0.001)

18817

164.8

6.22

12080

164.1

6.78

225

155.1

5.10

1960–1969

35.8

6.9

19.5–54.3

20422

165.1 (163, < 0.001)

7.00 (8.6,<0.001)

9604

166.2

6.58

10182

164.6

6.87

438

156.8

5.17

1970–1979

29.3

5.4

19.5–44.3

19893

165.9 (180, < 0.001)

7.27 (11.5,<0.001)

11819

167.3

6.67

7034

165.0

7.22

718

158.5

5.58

1980–1994

23.4

3.3

19.5–34.3

14694

164.7 (118, < 0.001)

7.07 (6.2,<0.001)

7291

165.6

6.77

6274

164.9

6.96

633

159.8

5.74

*Welch ANOVA test for equality of means.

Levene’s test for equality of variances; SD: standard deviation.

The variance of adult height explained by additive genetic, shared environmental and unique environmental factors by birth-year cohorts is presented in Figure 1 (estimates with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are available in Supplementary file 1A). In men, there was a trend for an increasing total variance from birth cohort 1940–1949 onwards; genetic variance also increased during this period but especially in the two latest birth-year cohorts (1970–1979 and 1980–1994). Height variance due to the environment shared by co-twins was significant from birth cohorts 1920–1929 to 1970–1979, being greatest from 1950 to 1969. The effect of environmental factors unique to each twin individual including measurement error was more similar across birth-year cohorts. Heritability estimates ranged from 0.69 to 0.84 and were greatest in the two earliest and the two latest birth-year cohorts (Table 2). In women, although the total variance also started to increase from birth cohort 1940–1949, genetic variance showed an increasing trend from the earliest birth-year cohort. Both shared and unique environmental factors explained variation in height in all analyzed birth-year cohorts; whereas the shared environmental variance was somewhat greater in the latest cohorts (1970–1979 and 1980–1994) unique environmental variance was greatest in the earliest one. Although the variance components differed between sexes in all birth-year cohorts, the relative contribution of the genetic and environmental variance components did not differ by sex from 1930–1939 to 1960–1969 (Supplementary file1B). In contrast to the results in men, heritability estimates in women (0.53 to 0.78) were lowest in the earliest and latest cohorts, particularly in 1886–1909. When we studied the effect of birth year on the genetic variance by using gene-environment interaction models, modest but statistically significant increase was found. The interaction effect was 0.050 (95% CI 0.018–0.082) in men and 0.043 (95% CI 0.019–0.071) in women for the genetic path coefficient per 10 years. This turns to 1.37 (95% CI 0.50–2.27) increase of genetic variance in men and 1.07 (95% CI 0.46–1.79) increase of genetic variance in women per 25 years, i.e. approximately one human generation.

Figure 1. Additive genetic (grey), shared environmental (black) and unique environmental (white) variances of height across birth-year cohorts for the pooled data and by geographic-cultural region.

Figure 1.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.003

Table 2.

Proportion of the height variance explained by additive genetic, shared environmental and unique environmental factors by birth year, sex and geographic-cultural region.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.004

Men

Women

Additive genetics

Shared environment

Unique environment

Additive genetics

Shared environment

Unique environment

Birth year

A

95% CIs

C

95% CIs

E

95% CIs

A

95% CIs

C

95% CIs

E

95% CIs

All cohorts

1886–1909

0.78

0.69

0.80

0.00

0.00

0.08

0.22

0.20

0.25

0.53

0.43

0.62

0.13

0.05

0.21

0.34

0.31

0.37

1910–1919

0.82

0.76

0.87

0.04

0.00

0.10

0.14

0.13

0.15

0.67

0.60

0.74

0.11

0.04

0.18

0.22

0.20

0.24

1920–1929

0.72

0.69

0.76

0.14

0.10

0.17

0.14

0.13

0.15

0.70

0.64

0.76

0.12

0.07

0.18

0.18

0.17

0.19

1930–1939

0.73

0.68

0.79

0.10

0.04

0.16

0.16

0.15

0.18

0.74

0.68

0.79

0.11

0.06

0.16

0.15

0.14

0.16

1940–1949

0.74

0.70

0.78

0.13

0.09

0.17

0.13

0.12

0.13

0.78

0.75

0.82

0.09

0.05

0.13

0.13

0.12

0.13

1950–1959

0.72

0.69

0.76

0.16

0.12

0.20

0.12

0.11

0.12

0.73

0.69

0.76

0.15

0.11

0.18

0.12

0.12

0.13

1960–1969

0.69

0.63

0.76

0.16

0.09

0.21

0.15

0.14

0.16

0.70

0.66

0.75

0.14

0.09

0.18

0.16

0.15

0.17

1970–1979

0.77

0.72

0.83

0.11

0.06

0.17

0.11

0.10

0.12

0.68

0.64

0.73

0.19

0.14

0.23

0.13

0.12

0.13

1980–1994

0.84

0.77

0.90

0.05

0.00

0.13

0.11

0.10

0.12

0.66

0.61

0.72

0.21

0.16

0.27

0.13

0.12

0.13

Europe

1886–1909

0.78

0.69

0.80

0.00

0.00

0.08

0.22

0.20

0.25

0.50

0.40

0.60

0.14

0.06

0.23

0.35

0.32

0.39

1910–1919

0.85

0.79

0.87

0.00

0.00

0.07

0.15

0.13

0.16

0.66

0.58

0.74

0.10

0.02

0.17

0.24

0.22

0.26

1920–1929

0.69

0.62

0.76

0.14

0.07

0.20

0.17

0.16

0.19

0.72

0.65

0.79

0.09

0.03

0.16

0.19

0.17

0.21

1930–1939

0.75

0.69

0.81

0.11

0.05

0.17

0.14

0.13

0.16

0.76

0.70

0.82

0.09

0.03

0.14

0.16

0.15

0.17

1940–1949

0.77

0.72

0.82

0.10

0.06

0.15

0.13

0.12

0.13

0.79

0.75

0.83

0.08

0.04

0.13

0.13

0.12

0.13

1950–1959

0.72

0.68

0.77

0.16

0.11

0.20

0.12

0.11

0.12

0.79

0.75

0.83

0.09

0.05

0.13

0.12

0.11

0.13

1960–1969

0.74

0.66

0.83

0.15

0.06

0.23

0.11

0.10

0.12

0.78

0.72

0.85

0.08

0.02

0.15

0.13

0.12

0.14

1970–1979

0.81

0.74

0.88

0.09

0.02

0.16

0.10

0.09

0.10

0.74

0.69

0.81

0.15

0.09

0.21

0.11

0.10

0.11

1980–1994

0.87

0.77

0.92

0.04

0.00

0.14

0.09

0.08

0.10

0.64

0.57

0.72

0.26

0.18

0.32

0.10

0.09

0.11

North America and Australia

1886–1909

0.83

0.33

0.90

0.01

0.00

0.49

0.16

0.10

0.26

0.76

0.43

0.82

0.00

0.00

0.31

0.24

0.18

0.32

1910–1919

0.78

0.70

0.87

0.09

0.00

0.17

0.13

0.12

0.15

0.60

0.44

0.78

0.24

0.06

0.39

0.16

0.14

0.19

1920–1929

0.73

0.69

0.77

0.14

0.10

0.18

0.13

0.12

0.14

0.63

0.52

0.76

0.22

0.09

0.33

0.15

0.14

0.17

1930–1939

0.81

0.66

0.83

0.00

0.00

0.14

0.19

0.17

0.22

0.70

0.59

0.82

0.17

0.04

0.28

0.13

0.12

0.15

1940–1949

0.69

0.61

0.77

0.19

0.10

0.27

0.13

0.12

0.14

0.80

0.72

0.87

0.08

0.00

0.15

0.13

0.12

0.14

1950–1959

0.75

0.68

0.82

0.14

0.07

0.21

0.11

0.10

0.12

0.67

0.61

0.73

0.21

0.15

0.26

0.13

0.12

0.13

1960–1969

0.66

0.58

0.76

0.16

0.06

0.24

0.18

0.17

0.20

0.63

0.57

0.70

0.18

0.11

0.24

0.18

0.17

0.20

1970–1979

0.68

0.57

0.81

0.18

0.05

0.29

0.14

0.13

0.16

0.60

0.53

0.67

0.25

0.18

0.32

0.15

0.14

0.16

1980–1994

0.83

0.72

0.89

0.04

0.00

0.16

0.12

0.11

0.14

0.71

0.62

0.81

0.14

0.04

0.23

0.15

0.14

0.16

East Asia

1940–1949

0.83

0.33

0.97

0.12

0.00

0.61

0.05

0.03

0.12

0.71

0.17

0.94

0.19

0.00

0.73

0.10

0.06

0.18

1950–1959

0.64

0.24

0.91

0.23

0.00

0.63

0.13

0.08

0.20

0.42

0.14

0.92

0.48

0.00

0.75

0.10

0.07

0.15

1960–1969

0.67

0.36

0.94

0.24

0.00

0.56

0.08

0.06

0.12

0.92

0.67

0.94

0.00

0.00

0.25

0.08

0.06

0.10

1970–1979

0.85

0.51

0.95

0.08

0.00

0.43

0.07

0.05

0.09

0.79

0.52

0.96

0.17

0.00

0.43

0.05

0.04

0.06

1980–1994

0.88

0.51

0.91

0.00

0.00

0.37

0.12

0.09

0.17

0.58

0.34

0.90

0.31

0.00

0.55

0.11

0.09

0.14

Univariate quantitative genetic models for height were then conducted separately in the three geographic-cultural regions (Figure 1 and Supplementary file 1A). The pattern in Europe was practically the same as that observed for the pooled data because it represents a large proportion of the total sample. In North America and Australia, the total variance of height was greater than in Europe, but the pattern of genetic and environmental variances was less consistent across birth-year cohorts. In East Asia, because of the smaller sample size, the magnitude of the variance components between the birth-year cohorts fluctuated more than in the other two geographic-cultural regions. Genetic variance was generally greater in men than in women in the three geographic-cultural regions. Variance components of height (both raw and relative proportion) showed a similar pattern across birth-year cohorts when analyses were performed for men and women together (Supplementary file 1C).

Discussion

This very large twin study showed no clear pattern in the heritability of height across birth-year cohorts and thus does not support the hypothesis that the heritability of height is lower in populations with low living standards compared with affluent populations, nor that the heritability of height will increase within a population as living standards improve. Since infant mortality rates are higher in men than in women, both in singletons (Drevenstedt et al., 2008) and twins (Pongou, 2013), the higher heritability observed for men in the earliest cohorts could be explained by selection effects since those who survived were the genetically more advantaged and thus less vulnerable to environmental conditions. The greater relative environmental effect on height variation in women than in men, although unexpected because women’s growth is considered to be more resistant to environmental influences, is in agreement with the findings in Finnish twins born prior to 1958 (Silventoinen et al., 2000). This might indicate differential access to food and medical care (Eveleth and Tanner, 1990). Women are also more likely to develop osteoporosis leading to shrinking in old age (National Institute of Arthritis and musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases, 2014), which may affect the greater influence of unique environmental factors in women born in 1886–1910. This idea is supported by results showing that although genetic factors play an important role in bone loss in early postmenopausal women, their effect weakens with age and completely disappears with advanced aging (Moayyeri et al., 2012).

Total and genetic variance of height generally increased across birth-year cohorts; gene-birth year interaction analysis showed that the genetic variance increase was only modest even when it was statistically significant in this very large twin cohort. However, part of the increase in total variance in some birth-year cohorts was also due to the increase in shared environmental variance. This suggests that both greater ethnic diversity and variation in living standards have contributed to the secular increase in height variation. The greatest total height variation in North America and Australia was due to both genetic and environmental factors and the pattern of variance components across the birth cohorts was less consistent than in Europe. A recent study across 14 European countries found that many independent loci contribute to population genetic differences in height and estimated that these differences account for 24% of the captured additive genetic variance (Robinson et al., 2015). Therefore, it may be that both allelic frequencies and the effects of genes affecting height vary between the geographic-cultural regions. It has been previously shown that even when the total variance of height was greater in Western populations than in East Asian populations, heritability estimates were largely similar in adolescence (Hur et al., 2008) and from 1 to 19 years of age (Jelenkovic et al., 2016); however, the limited statistical power in the data from East Asia does not allow for comparisons across birth cohorts.

The main strength of the present study is the very large sample size of our multinational database of twin cohorts, with adult height data from individuals born between year 1886 and 1994, allowing a more detailed investigation of the genetic and environmental contributions to individual differences in height across birth cohorts than in the previous studies. Important advantages of individual-based data are improved opportunities for statistical modeling and lack of publication bias. This type of analysis is difficult to perform by using literature-based meta-analyses because most of the published studies do not provide the needed statistics by birth-year cohorts. However, our study also has limitations. Countries and/or ethnic-cultural regions are not equally represented and the database is heavily weighted toward populations following Westernized lifestyles. In the classical twin design, parental phenotypic assortment increases dizygotic correlations and thus inflates the shared environmental component when not accounted for in the modeling. In our database, we do not have information on parental height and thus could not take into account assortative mating, which may thus explain part of the shared environmental variation. In addition, most of the height measures were self-reported (Silventoinen et al., 2015), which may bias our analyses toward higher estimates of unique environmental effects due to increased measurement error. However, these sources of bias are unlikely to explain our main result, i.e., relatively similar heritability estimates of adult height over birth cohorts. Finally, since we previously showed that there was no zygosity difference in height variance (Jelenkovic et al., 2015), variance components estimates should not be affected by changes in the proportion of MZ to DZ twins across birth-year cohorts.

In conclusion, although the genetic variance of height showed a slightly increasing trend with birth year, heritability estimates did not present any clear pattern of secular changes across birth-year cohorts from 1886 to 1994. Thus, our findings do not support the hypothesis that the heritability of height increases along with increasing living standards and diminishing rate of absolute poverty within populations.

Materials and methods

Sample

This study is based on the data from the Collaborative project of Development of Anthropometrical measures in Twins (CODATwins), which was intended to pool data from all twin projects in the world with information on height and weight measurements for MZ and DZ twins (Silventoinen et al., 2015). For the present analyses, we selected height measurements at ages 19.5–99.5 years. After excluding four cohorts having less than 50 twin individuals in the final database, we had data from 40 cohorts in 20 countries. The participating twin cohorts are identified in Table 1 (footnote) and were previously described in detail (Silventoinen et al., 2015).

From the initial 558,672 height measurements, we excluded those <145 or>210 cm in men and <135 or >195 cm in women (<0.1% of the measurements). Since individuals in longitudinal studies have more than one measurement over time, analyses were restricted to one observation per individual resulting in 323,491 individuals. After excluding unmatched pairs (without data on their co-twins), we had 286,780 twin individuals (143,390 complete twin pairs) born between year 1886 and 1994 (40% monozygotic (MZ), 41% same- sex dizygotic (SSDZ) and 19% opposite-sex dizygotic (OSDZ) twin pairs). The smaller proportion of OSDZ compared to SSDZ twins in this study is explained by the fact that some of the twin cohorts in our database have collected, by design, only SSDZ twins and thus do not have data on OSDZ twins. These individuals were categorized into nine consecutive birth year groups described in Table 1. In order to analyze possible ethnic-cultural differences in the genetic and environmental contribution on height, cohorts were grouped in three geographical-cultural regions: Europe (18 cohorts), North America and Australia (14 cohorts) and East Asia (six cohorts) with 87,116, 53,359 and 1793 twin pairs, respectively. One cohort from the Middle-East and the one from South-Asia were not included in these sub-analyses by geographic-cultural region because the data were too sparse to study these two areas separately.

Statistical analyses

We first tested whether the means and variances of height differed between twin cohorts within each sex and birth-year group (Table 1). Since the Levene´s test for homogeneity indicated that variances were not homogeneous, a Welch’s ANOVA was performed showing that means were significantly different between twin cohorts in all sex and birth-year groups.

To analyze genetic and environmental influences on the variation of height, we used classic twin modeling based on linear structural equations (Neale and Cardon, 1992). MZ twins share the same genomic sequence, whereas DZ twins share, on average, 50% of their genes identical-by-descent. On this basis, it is possible to divide the total variance of height into variance due to additive genetic effects (A: correlated 1.0 for MZ and 0.5 for DZ pairs), dominance genetic effects (D: 1.0 for MZ and 0.25 for DZ pairs), common (shared) environmental effects (C: by definition, correlated 1.0 for MZ and DZ pairs) and unique (non-shared) environmental effects (E: by definition, uncorrelated in MZ and DZ pairs). However, since our data included only twins reared together, we cannot simultaneously estimate shared environmental and dominance genetic effects. All genetic models were fitted by the OpenMx package (version 2.0.1) in the R statistical platform (Boker et al., 2011) using the maximum likelihood method.

Prior to conducting the modeling, height values were adjusted for the year of birth and twin cohort within each birth year and sex groups using linear regressions, and the resulting residuals were used as input phenotypes. The ACE sex-limitation model was selected as a starting point of the univariate modeling based on the following criteria: (i) MZ within-pair correlations were clearly higher than DZ correlations consistent with the influence of genetic effects, (ii) the magnitude of the difference between MZ and DZ correlations (rDZ > 1/2 rMZ) indicated the presence of common environmental effects and (iii) the lower within-pair correlations for OSDZ than for SSDZ twins observed for most birth-year groups suggested the presence of sex-specific genetic effects (results not shown). Previous findings from this international database showed that both male and female DZ twins are slightly taller than MZ twins in these age groups (Jelenkovic et al., 2015), and thus different means for MZ and DZ twins were allowed. The fit of the univariate models for height at each birth-year group is shown in Supplementary file 1B. In the present study, the equal-environment assumption was tested by comparing the ACE model to the saturated model. The fit of the models after Bonferroni correction of multiple testing did not worsen for most birth-year groups, which suggested that the assumption of equality of variances between MZ and DZ twins was not violated. When fixing A, C and E parameters to be the same in men and women, the fit of the model was poorer in all birth-year groups (p<0.0001), suggesting that these variance components differ between sexes. We additionally fitted a scale model allowing for different sizes of variance components but fixing the relative size of these components to be equal. Since this model also showed statistically significant differences (p<0.0001) in some birth-year cohorts, we decided to present the results separately for men and women. Sex-specific genetic effects were significant for some birth-year cohorts, and thus all modeling results are presented in sex-limited form for consistency. Comparative model fitting revealed that the C parameter could be not excluded from the model without a significant deterioration in fit. In order to study how birth year modifies the genetic and environmental variances of height, we additionally conducted gene-environment interaction modeling using birth year as an environmental modification factor (Purcell, 2002). This modeling offers intercept and interaction term describing the change per birth year which then need to be squared to get raw genetic and environmental variances. To make the results easier to understand, we calculated expected variance change with 95% CI per 25 years, i.e. approximately one human generation.

Acknowledgements

Support for collaborating projects: The Australian Twin Registry is supported by a Centre of Research Excellence (grant ID 1079102) from the National Health and Medical Research Council administered by the University of Melbourne. The California Twin Program was supported by The California Tobacco-Related Disease Research Program (7RT-0134H, 8RT-0107H, 6RT-0354H) and the National Institutes of Health (1R01ESO15150-01). The Carolina African American Twin Study of Aging (CAATSA) was funded by a grant from the National Institute on Aging (grant 1RO1-AG13662-01A2) to K. E. Whitfield. Colorado Twin Registry is funded by NIDA funded center grant DA011015, and Longitudinal Twin Study HD10333; Author Huibregtse is supported by 5T32DA017637-11. Danish Twin Registry is supported by the National Program for Research Infrastructure 2007 from the Danish Agency for Science, Technology and Innovation, The Research Council for Health and Disease, the Velux Foundation and the US National Institute of Health (P01 AG08761). Since its origin the East Flanders Prospective Survey has been partly supported by grants from the Fund of Scientific Research, Flanders and Twins, a non-profit Association for Scientific Research in Multiple Births (Belgium). Data collection and analyses in Finnish twin cohorts have been supported by ENGAGE – European Network for Genetic and Genomic Epidemiology, FP7-HEALTH-F4-2007, grant agreement number 201413, National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (grants AA-12502, AA-00145, and AA-09203 to R J Rose, the Academy of Finland Center of Excellence in Complex Disease Genetics (grant numbers: 213506, 129680), and the Academy of Finland (grants 100499, 205585, 118555, 141054, 265240, 263278 and 264146 to J Kaprio). K Silventoinen is supported by Osaka University's International Joint Research Promotion Program. Waves 1–3 of Genesis 12–19 were funded by the W T Grant Foundation, the University of London Central Research fund and a Medical Research Council Training Fellowship (G81/343) and Career Development Award (G120/635) to Thalia C. Eley. Wave four was supported by grants from the Economic and Social Research Council (RES-000-22–2206) and the Institute of Social Psychiatry (06/07–11) to Alice M. Gregory who was also supported at that time by a Leverhulme Research Fellowship (RF/2/RFG/2008/0145). Wave five was supported by funding to Alice M. Gregory from Goldsmiths, University of London. Anthropometric measurements of the Hungarian twins were supported by Medexpert Ltd., Budapest, Hungary. Korean Twin-Family Register was supported by the Global Research Network Program of the National Research Foundation (NRF 2011–220-E00006). The Michigan State University Twin Registry has been supported by Michigan State University, as well as grants R01-MH081813, R01-MH0820-54, R01-MH092377-02, R21-MH070542-01, R03-MH63851-01 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), R01-HD066040 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), and 11-SPG-2518 from the MSU Foundation. The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH, the NICHD, or the National Institutes of Health. The Murcia Twin Registry is supported by Fundación Séneca, Regional Agency for Science and Technology, Murcia, Spain (08633/PHCS/08, 15302/PHCS/10 and 19479/PI/14) and Ministry of Science and Innovation, Spain (PSI2009-11560 and PSI2014-56680-R). Data collection and research stemming from the Norwegian Twin Registry is supported, in part, from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programmes ENGAGE Consortium (grant agreement HEALTH-F4-2007–201413, and BioSHaRE EU (grant agreement HEALTH-F4-2010–261433). The NAS-NRC Twin Registry acknowledges financial support from the National Institutes of Health grant number R21 AG039572. Netherlands Twin Register acknowledges the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) and MagW/ZonMW grants 904-61–090, 985-10–002, 912-10–020, 904-61–193,480-04–004, 463-06–001, 451-04–034, 400-05–717, Addiction-31160008, Middelgroot-911-09–032, Spinozapremie 56-464–14192; VU University’s Institute for Health and Care Research (EMGO+); the European Research Council (ERC - 230374), the Avera Institute, Sioux Falls, South Dakota (USA). South Korea Twin Registry is supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-371-2011–1 B00047). S.Y. Öncel and F. Aliev are supported by Kırıkkale University Research Grant: KKU, 2009/43 and TUBITAK grant 114C117. TwinsUK was funded by the Wellcome Trust; European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013). The study also receives support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) BioResource Clinical Research Facility and Biomedical Research Centre based at Guy's and St Thomas' NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. The University of Southern California Twin Study is funded by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH58354). Washington State Twin Registry (formerly the University of Washington Twin Registry) was supported in part by grant NIH RC2 HL103416 (D. Buchwald, PI). Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging was supported by the National Institute of Health grants NIA R01 AG018384, R01 AG018386, R01 AG022381, and R01 AG022982, and, in part, with resources of the VA San Diego Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health. The Cooperative Studies Program of the Office of Research and Development of the United States Department of Veterans Affairs has provided financial support for the development and maintenance of the Vietnam Era Twin (VET) Registry. The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIA/NIH, or the VA. The West Japan Twins and Higher Order Multiple Births Registry was supported by Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (B) (grant number 15H05105) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grant:

  • Suomen Akatemia 266592 to Karri Silventoinen.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Author contributions

AJ, In charge of data management, Conducted the analyses, Wrote the first draft of the manuscript and has primary responsibility of the final content.

Y-MH, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

RS, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

YY, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

SHS, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

ASu, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

FRi, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

QT, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

DZ, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

ZP, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

SA, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

SYÖ, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

FA, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

ER, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

ADT, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

DLT, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KC, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

ASk, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KOK, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JLS, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

LJE, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

HHM, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TLC, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JLH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JRO, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JFS-R, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

LC-C, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

WC, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

AEH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TMM, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JS, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Y-MS, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

SY, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KL, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CEF, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

WSK, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MJL, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

AB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TLN, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KEW, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CK, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KLJ, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MG, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

DAB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MAS, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CF, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CD, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

GED, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

DB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CAD, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

RFV, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

RJFL, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

NGM, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

SEM, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

GWM, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

H-UJ, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

GES, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

RK, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

PKEM, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

NLP, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

AKD-A, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TAM, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TCE, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

AMG, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

PT, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

LAB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CT, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

GB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

DN, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

PL, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TDS, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MMa, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

GL, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TCEMvB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

GW, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

SAB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KLK, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JRH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

IB, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TSN, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

RFK, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MMcGu, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

SP, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

RPC, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JvBH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JHG, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

YI, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

MW, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

CH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

FI, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

FRa, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

BMH, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

DIB, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

TIAS, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

JK, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

KS, Planned the study design of the CODATwins project, Collected the data used in this study, Commented the manuscript, Read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Ethics

Human subjects: All participants were volunteers and gave their informed consent when participating in their original study. Only a limited set of observational variables and anonymized data were delivered to the data management center at University of Helsinki. The pooled analysis was approved by the ethical committee of Department of Public Health, University of Helsinki.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Supplementary tables.

(A) Height variance explained by additive genetic, shared environmental and unique environmental factors by birth year, sex and geographic-cultural region. (B) Model fit statistics for adult height by birth-year cohorts (all twin cohorts together). (C) Height variance and proportion of height variance explained by additive genetic, shared environmental and unique environmental factors by birth year, sex and geographic-cultural region.

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.005

elife-20320-supp1.doc (272KB, doc)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.20320.005

References

  1. Bogin B. The Growth of Humanity. New York: Wiley-Liss; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  2. Boker S, Neale M, Maes H, Wilde M, Spiegel M, Brick T, Spies J, Estabrook R, Kenny S, Bates T, Mehta P, Fox J. OpenMx: An Open Source Extended Structural Equation Modeling Framework. Psychometrika. 2011;76:306–317. doi: 10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bozzoli C, Deaton A, Quintana-Domeque C. Adult height and childhood disease. Demography. 2009;46:647–669. doi: 10.1353/dem.0.0079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cho YS, Go MJ, Kim YJ, Heo JY, Oh JH, Ban HJ, Yoon D, Lee MH, Kim DJ, Park M, Cha SH, Kim JW, Han BG, Min H, Ahn Y, Park MS, Han HR, Jang HY, Cho EY, Lee JE, Cho NH, Shin C, Park T, Park JW, Lee JK, Cardon L, Clarke G, McCarthy MI, Lee JY, Lee JK, Oh B, Kim HL. A large-scale genome-wide association study of asian populations uncovers genetic factors influencing eight quantitative traits. Nature Genetics. 2009;41:527–534. doi: 10.1038/ng.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cole TJ. The secular trend in human physical growth: a biological view. Economics & Human Biology. 2003;1:161–168. doi: 10.1016/S1570-677X(02)00033-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Drevenstedt GL, Crimmins EM, Vasunilashorn S, Finch CE. The rise and fall of excess male infant mortality. PNAS. 2008;105:5016–5021. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800221105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Eveleth PB. Tanner JM. Worldwide Variation in Human Growth. 2nd edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  8. Fisher RA. XV.—The correlation between relatives on the supposition of mendelian Inheritance. Transactions of the Royal Society of Edinburgh. 1919;52:399–433. doi: 10.1017/S0080456800012163. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Galton F. Regression towards mediocrity in heriditary stature. Journal of the Anthropological Institute. 1886;15:246–262. doi: 10.2307/2841583. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Hao Y, Liu X, Lu X, Yang X, Wang L, Chen S, Li H, Li J, Cao J, Chen J, Li Y, Zhao L, Shi Y, Shen C, Yan W, He J, Huang J, Gu D. Genome-wide association study in Han Chinese identifies three novel loci for human height. Human Genetics. 2013;132:681–689. doi: 10.1007/s00439-013-1280-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hur YM, Kaprio J, Iacono WG, Boomsma DI, McGue M, Silventoinen K, Martin NG, Luciano M, Visscher PM, Rose RJ, He M, Ando J, Ooki S, Nonaka K, Lin CC, Lajunen HR, Cornes BK, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt CE, Cherny SS, Mitchell K. Genetic influences on the difference in variability of height, weight and body mass index between caucasian and east asian adolescent twins. International Journal of Obesity. 2008;32:1455–1467. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jelenkovic A, Sund R, Hur YM, Yokoyama Y, Hjelmborg JB, Möller S. Heritability of height from infancy to early adulthood in pooled data of 45 twin cohorts. Scientific Reports. 2016;6:28496. doi: 10.1038/srep28496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Jelenkovic A, Yokoyama Y, Sund R, Honda C, Bogl LH, Aaltonen S, Ji F, Ning F, Pang Z, Ordoñana JR, Sánchez-Romera JF, Colodro-Conde L, Burt SA, Klump KL, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Kandler C, McAdams TA, Eley TC, Gregory AM, Saudino KJ, Dubois L, Boivin M, Tarnoki AD, Tarnoki DL, Haworth CM, Plomin R, Öncel SY, Aliev F, Stazi MA, Fagnani C, D'Ippolito C, Craig JM, Saffery R, Siribaddana SH, Hotopf M, Sumathipala A, Rijsdijk F, Spector T, Mangino M, Lachance G, Gatz M, Butler DA, Bayasgalan G, Narandalai D, Freitas DL, Maia JA, Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM, Kim B, Chong Y, Hong C, Shin HJ, Christensen K, Skytthe A, Kyvik KO, Derom CA, Vlietinck RF, Loos RJ, Cozen W, Hwang AE, Mack TM, He M, Ding X, Chang B, Silberg JL, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Cutler TL, Hopper JL, Aujard K, Magnusson PK, Pedersen NL, Aslan AK, Song YM, Yang S, Lee K, Baker LA, Tuvblad C, Bjerregaard-Andersen M, Beck-Nielsen H, Sodemann M, Heikkilä K, Tan Q, Zhang D, Swan GE, Krasnow R, Jang KL, Knafo-Noam A, Mankuta D, Abramson L, Lichtenstein P, Krueger RF, McGue M, Pahlen S, Tynelius P, Duncan GE, Buchwald D, Corley RP, Huibregtse BM, Nelson TL, Whitfield KE, Kremen WS, Lyons MJ, Ooki S, Brandt I, Nilsen TS, Inui F, Watanabe M, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt TC, Wardle J, Llewellyn CH, Fisher A, Rebato E, Martin NG, Iwatani Y, Hayakawa K, Sung J, Harris JR, Willemsen G, Busjahn A, Goldberg JH, Rasmussen F, Hur YM, Boomsma DI, Sørensen TI, Kaprio J, Silventoinen K. Zygosity differences in height and body mass index of twins from infancy to old age: A study of the CODATwins project. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2015;18:557–570. doi: 10.1017/thg.2015.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lango Allen H, Estrada K, Lettre G, Berndt SI, Weedon MN, Rivadeneira F, Willer CJ, Jackson AU, Vedantam S, Raychaudhuri S, Ferreira T, Wood AR, Weyant RJ, Segrè AV, Speliotes EK, Wheeler E, Soranzo N, Park JH, Yang J, Gudbjartsson D, Heard-Costa NL, Randall JC, Qi L, Vernon Smith A, Mägi R, Pastinen T, Liang L, Heid IM, Luan J, Thorleifsson G, Winkler TW, Goddard ME, Sin Lo K, Palmer C, Workalemahu T, Aulchenko YS, Johansson A, Zillikens MC, Feitosa MF, Esko T, Johnson T, Ketkar S, Kraft P, Mangino M, Prokopenko I, Absher D, Albrecht E, Ernst F, Glazer NL, Hayward C, Hottenga JJ, Jacobs KB, Knowles JW, Kutalik Z, Monda KL, Polasek O, Preuss M, Rayner NW, Robertson NR, Steinthorsdottir V, Tyrer JP, Voight BF, Wiklund F, Xu J, Zhao JH, Nyholt DR, Pellikka N, Perola M, Perry JR, Surakka I, Tammesoo ML, Altmaier EL, Amin N, Aspelund T, Bhangale T, Boucher G, Chasman DI, Chen C, Coin L, Cooper MN, Dixon AL, Gibson Q, Grundberg E, Hao K, Juhani Junttila M, Kaplan LM, Kettunen J, König IR, Kwan T, Lawrence RW, Levinson DF, Lorentzon M, McKnight B, Morris AP, Müller M, Suh Ngwa J, Purcell S, Rafelt S, Salem RM, Salvi E, Sanna S, Shi J, Sovio U, Thompson JR, Turchin MC, Vandenput L, Verlaan DJ, Vitart V, White CC, Ziegler A, Almgren P, Balmforth AJ, Campbell H, Citterio L, De Grandi A, Dominiczak A, Duan J, Elliott P, Elosua R, Eriksson JG, Freimer NB, Geus EJ, Glorioso N, Haiqing S, Hartikainen AL, Havulinna AS, Hicks AA, Hui J, Igl W, Illig T, Jula A, Kajantie E, Kilpeläinen TO, Koiranen M, Kolcic I, Koskinen S, Kovacs P, Laitinen J, Liu J, Lokki ML, Marusic A, Maschio A, Meitinger T, Mulas A, Paré G, Parker AN, Peden JF, Petersmann A, Pichler I, Pietiläinen KH, Pouta A, Ridderstråle M, Rotter JI, Sambrook JG, Sanders AR, Schmidt CO, Sinisalo J, Smit JH, Stringham HM, Bragi Walters G, Widen E, Wild SH, Willemsen G, Zagato L, Zgaga L, Zitting P, Alavere H, Farrall M, McArdle WL, Nelis M, Peters MJ, Ripatti S, van Meurs JB, Aben KK, Ardlie KG, Beckmann JS, Beilby JP, Bergman RN, Bergmann S, Collins FS, Cusi D, den Heijer M, Eiriksdottir G, Gejman PV, Hall AS, Hamsten A, Huikuri HV, Iribarren C, Kähönen M, Kaprio J, Kathiresan S, Kiemeney L, Kocher T, Launer LJ, Lehtimäki T, Melander O, Mosley TH, Musk AW, Nieminen MS, O'Donnell CJ, Ohlsson C, Oostra B, Palmer LJ, Raitakari O, Ridker PM, Rioux JD, Rissanen A, Rivolta C, Schunkert H, Shuldiner AR, Siscovick DS, Stumvoll M, Tönjes A, Tuomilehto J, van Ommen GJ, Viikari J, Heath AC, Martin NG, Montgomery GW, Province MA, Kayser M, Arnold AM, Atwood LD, Boerwinkle E, Chanock SJ, Deloukas P, Gieger C, Grönberg H, Hall P, Hattersley AT, Hengstenberg C, Hoffman W, Lathrop GM, Salomaa V, Schreiber S, Uda M, Waterworth D, Wright AF, Assimes TL, Barroso I, Hofman A, Mohlke KL, Boomsma DI, Caulfield MJ, Cupples LA, Erdmann J, Fox CS, Gudnason V, Gyllensten U, Harris TB, Hayes RB, Jarvelin MR, Mooser V, Munroe PB, Ouwehand WH, Penninx BW, Pramstaller PP, Quertermous T, Rudan I, Samani NJ, Spector TD, Völzke H, Watkins H, Wilson JF, Groop LC, Haritunians T, Hu FB, Kaplan RC, Metspalu A, North KE, Schlessinger D, Wareham NJ, Hunter DJ, O'Connell JR, Strachan DP, Wichmann HE, Borecki IB, van Duijn CM, Schadt EE, Thorsteinsdottir U, Peltonen L, Uitterlinden AG, Visscher PM, Chatterjee N, Loos RJ, Boehnke M, McCarthy MI, Ingelsson E, Lindgren CM, Abecasis GR, Stefansson K, Frayling TM, Hirschhorn JN. Hundreds of variants clustered in genomic loci and biological pathways affect human height. Nature. 2010;467:832–838. doi: 10.1038/nature09410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Moayyeri A, Hammond CJ, Hart DJ, Spector TD. Effects of age on genetic influence on bone loss over 17 years in women: the healthy ageing Twin Study (HATS) Journal of Bone and Mineral Research. 2012;27:2170–2178. doi: 10.1002/jbmr.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. N'Diaye A, Chen GK, Palmer CD, Ge B, Tayo B, Mathias RA, Ding J, Nalls MA, Adeyemo A, Adoue V, Ambrosone CB, Atwood L, Bandera EV, Becker LC, Berndt SI, Bernstein L, Blot WJ, Boerwinkle E, Britton A, Casey G, Chanock SJ, Demerath E, Deming SL, Diver WR, Fox C, Harris TB, Hernandez DG, Hu JJ, Ingles SA, John EM, Johnson C, Keating B, Kittles RA, Kolonel LN, Kritchevsky SB, Le Marchand L, Lohman K, Liu J, Millikan RC, Murphy A, Musani S, Neslund-Dudas C, North KE, Nyante S, Ogunniyi A, Ostrander EA, Papanicolaou G, Patel S, Pettaway CA, Press MF, Redline S, Rodriguez-Gil JL, Rotimi C, Rybicki BA, Salako B, Schreiner PJ, Signorello LB, Singleton AB, Stanford JL, Stram AH, Stram DO, Strom SS, Suktitipat B, Thun MJ, Witte JS, Yanek LR, Ziegler RG, Zheng W, Zhu X, Zmuda JM, Zonderman AB, Evans MK, Liu Y, Becker DM, Cooper RS, Pastinen T, Henderson BE, Hirschhorn JN, Lettre G, Haiman CA. Identification, replication, and fine-mapping of Loci associated with adult height in individuals of african ancestry. PLoS Genetics. 2011;7:e1002298. doi: 10.1371/annotation/58c67154-3f10-4155-9085-dcd6e3689008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. National Institute of Arthritis and musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases [Accessed April, 2016];Handout on Health: Osteoporosis. 2014 http://www.niams.nih.gov/health_info/Osteoporosis/default.asp
  18. NCD Risk Factor Collaboration (NCD-RisC) A century of trends in adult human height. eLife. 2016;5:e13410. doi: 10.7554/eLife.13410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Neale MC, Cardon LR. Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  20. Pearson K, Lee A. On the laws on inheritance in man. Biometrika. 1903;2:356–462. doi: 10.1093/biomet/2.4.357. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Perola M, Sammalisto S, Hiekkalinna T, Martin NG, Visscher PM, Montgomery GW, Benyamin B, Harris JR, Boomsma D, Willemsen G, Hottenga JJ, Christensen K, Kyvik KO, Sørensen TI, Pedersen NL, Magnusson PK, Spector TD, Widen E, Silventoinen K, Kaprio J, Palotie A, Peltonen L, GenomEUtwin Project Combined genome scans for body stature in 6,602 European twins: evidence for common Caucasian loci. PLoS Genetics. 2007;3:e97. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0030097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Pongou R. Why is infant mortality higher in boys than in girls? A new hypothesis based on preconception environment and evidence from a large sample of twins. Demography. 2013;50:421–444. doi: 10.1007/s13524-012-0161-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Purcell S. Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in twin analysis. Twin Research. 2002;5:554–571. doi: 10.1375/136905202762342026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Robinson MR, Hemani G, Medina-Gomez C, Mezzavilla M, Esko T, Shakhbazov K, Powell JE, Vinkhuyzen A, Berndt SI, Gustafsson S, Justice AE, Kahali B, Locke AE, Pers TH, Vedantam S, Wood AR, van Rheenen W, Andreassen OA, Gasparini P, Metspalu A, Berg LH, Veldink JH, Rivadeneira F, Werge TM, Abecasis GR, Boomsma DI, Chasman DI, de Geus EJ, Frayling TM, Hirschhorn JN, Hottenga JJ, Ingelsson E, Loos RJ, Magnusson PK, Martin NG, Montgomery GW, North KE, Pedersen NL, Spector TD, Speliotes EK, Goddard ME, Yang J, Visscher PM. Population genetic differentiation of height and body mass index across Europe. Nature Genetics. 2015;47:1357–1362. doi: 10.1038/ng.3401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Silventoinen K, Jelenkovic A, Sund R, Honda C, Aaltonen S, Yokoyama Y, Tarnoki AD, Tarnoki DL, Ning F, Ji F, Pang Z, Ordoñana JR, Sánchez-Romera JF, Colodro-Conde L, Burt SA, Klump KL, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Kandler C, McAdams TA, Eley TC, Gregory AM, Saudino KJ, Dubois L, Boivin M, Haworth CM, Plomin R, Öncel SY, Aliev F, Stazi MA, Fagnani C, D'Ippolito C, Craig JM, Saffery R, Siribaddana SH, Hotopf M, Sumathipala A, Spector T, Mangino M, Lachance G, Gatz M, Butler DA, Bayasgalan G, Narandalai D, Freitas DL, Maia JA, Harden KP, Tucker-Drob EM, Christensen K, Skytthe A, Kyvik KO, Hong C, Chong Y, Derom CA, Vlietinck RF, Loos RJ, Cozen W, Hwang AE, Mack TM, He M, Ding X, Chang B, Silberg JL, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Cutler TL, Hopper JL, Aujard K, Magnusson PK, Pedersen NL, Aslan AK, Song YM, Yang S, Lee K, Baker LA, Tuvblad C, Bjerregaard-Andersen M, Beck-Nielsen H, Sodemann M, Heikkilä K, Tan Q, Zhang D, Swan GE, Krasnow R, Jang KL, Knafo-Noam A, Mankuta D, Abramson L, Lichtenstein P, Krueger RF, McGue M, Pahlen S, Tynelius P, Duncan GE, Buchwald D, Corley RP, Huibregtse BM, Nelson TL, Whitfield KE, Franz CE, Kremen WS, Lyons MJ, Ooki S, Brandt I, Nilsen TS, Inui F, Watanabe M, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt TC, Wardle J, Llewellyn CH, Fisher A, Rebato E, Martin NG, Iwatani Y, Hayakawa K, Rasmussen F, Sung J, Harris JR, Willemsen G, Busjahn A, Goldberg JH, Boomsma DI, Hur YM, Sørensen TI, Kaprio J. The CODATwins project: The cohort description of collaborative project of development of Anthropometrical measures in twins to study Macro-environmental variation in genetic and environmental effects on Anthropometric traits. Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2015;18:348–360. doi: 10.1017/thg.2015.29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Silventoinen K, Kaprio J, Lahelma E, Koskenvuo M. Relative effect of genetic and environmental factors on body height: differences across birth cohorts among Finnish men and women. American Journal of Public Health. 2000;90:627–630. doi: 10.2105/ajph.90.4.627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Silventoinen K, Sammalisto S, Perola M, Boomsma DI, Cornes BK, Davis C, Dunkel L, De Lange M, Harris JR, Hjelmborg JV, Luciano M, Martin NG, Mortensen J, Nisticò L, Pedersen NL, Skytthe A, Spector TD, Stazi MA, Willemsen G, Kaprio J. Heritability of adult body height: a comparative study of twin cohorts in eight countries. Twin Research. 2003;6:399–408. doi: 10.1375/136905203770326402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Silventoinen K. Determinants of variation in adult body height. Journal of Biosocial Science. 2003;35:263–285. doi: 10.1017/s0021932003002633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Steckel RH. Heights and human welfare: recent developments and new directions. Explorations in Economic History. 2009;46:1, 1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.eeh.2008.12.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Stulp G, Barrett L. Evolutionary perspectives on human height variation. Biological Reviews. 2016;91:206–234. doi: 10.1111/brv.12165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Stunkard AJ, Foch TT, Hrubec Z. A twin study of human obesity. JAMA. 1986;256:51–54. doi: 10.1001/jama.256.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Wood AR, Esko T, Yang J, Vedantam S, Pers TH, Gustafsson S, Chu AY, Estrada K, Luan J, Kutalik Z, Amin N, Buchkovich ML, Croteau-Chonka DC, Day FR, Duan Y, Fall T, Fehrmann R, Ferreira T, Jackson AU, Karjalainen J, Lo KS, Locke AE, Mägi R, Mihailov E, Porcu E, Randall JC, Scherag A, Vinkhuyzen AA, Westra HJ, Winkler TW, Workalemahu T, Zhao JH, Absher D, Albrecht E, Anderson D, Baron J, Beekman M, Demirkan A, Ehret GB, Feenstra B, Feitosa MF, Fischer K, Fraser RM, Goel A, Gong J, Justice AE, Kanoni S, Kleber ME, Kristiansson K, Lim U, Lotay V, Lui JC, Mangino M, Mateo Leach I, Medina-Gomez C, Nalls MA, Nyholt DR, Palmer CD, Pasko D, Pechlivanis S, Prokopenko I, Ried JS, Ripke S, Shungin D, Stancáková A, Strawbridge RJ, Sung YJ, Tanaka T, Teumer A, Trompet S, van der Laan SW, van Setten J, Van Vliet-Ostaptchouk JV, Wang Z, Yengo L, Zhang W, Afzal U, Arnlöv J, Arscott GM, Bandinelli S, Barrett A, Bellis C, Bennett AJ, Berne C, Blüher M, Bolton JL, Böttcher Y, Boyd HA, Bruinenberg M, Buckley BM, Buyske S, Caspersen IH, Chines PS, Clarke R, Claudi-Boehm S, Cooper M, Daw EW, De Jong PA, Deelen J, Delgado G, Denny JC, Dhonukshe-Rutten R, Dimitriou M, Doney AS, Dörr M, Eklund N, Eury E, Folkersen L, Garcia ME, Geller F, Giedraitis V, Go AS, Grallert H, Grammer TB, Gräßler J, Grönberg H, de Groot LC, Groves CJ, Haessler J, Hall P, Haller T, Hallmans G, Hannemann A, Hartman CA, Hassinen M, Hayward C, Heard-Costa NL, Helmer Q, Hemani G, Henders AK, Hillege HL, Hlatky MA, Hoffmann W, Hoffmann P, Holmen O, Houwing-Duistermaat JJ, Illig T, Isaacs A, James AL, Jeff J, Johansen B, Johansson Å, Jolley J, Juliusdottir T, Junttila J, Kho AN, Kinnunen L, Klopp N, Kocher T, Kratzer W, Lichtner P, Lind L, Lindström J, Lobbens S, Lorentzon M, Lu Y, Lyssenko V, Magnusson PK, Mahajan A, Maillard M, McArdle WL, McKenzie CA, McLachlan S, McLaren PJ, Menni C, Merger S, Milani L, Moayyeri A, Monda KL, Morken MA, Müller G, Müller-Nurasyid M, Musk AW, Narisu N, Nauck M, Nolte IM, Nöthen MM, Oozageer L, Pilz S, Rayner NW, Renstrom F, Robertson NR, Rose LM, Roussel R, Sanna S, Scharnagl H, Scholtens S, Schumacher FR, Schunkert H, Scott RA, Sehmi J, Seufferlein T, Shi J, Silventoinen K, Smit JH, Smith AV, Smolonska J, Stanton AV, Stirrups K, Stott DJ, Stringham HM, Sundström J, Swertz MA, Syvänen AC, Tayo BO, Thorleifsson G, Tyrer JP, van Dijk S, van Schoor NM, van der Velde N, van Heemst D, van Oort FV, Vermeulen SH, Verweij N, Vonk JM, Waite LL, Waldenberger M, Wennauer R, Wilkens LR, Willenborg C, Wilsgaard T, Wojczynski MK, Wong A, Wright AF, Zhang Q, Arveiler D, Bakker SJ, Beilby J, Bergman RN, Bergmann S, Biffar R, Blangero J, Boomsma DI, Bornstein SR, Bovet P, Brambilla P, Brown MJ, Campbell H, Caulfield MJ, Chakravarti A, Collins R, Collins FS, Crawford DC, Cupples LA, Danesh J, de Faire U, den Ruijter HM, Erbel R, Erdmann J, Eriksson JG, Farrall M, Ferrannini E, Ferrières J, Ford I, Forouhi NG, Forrester T, Gansevoort RT, Gejman PV, Gieger C, Golay A, Gottesman O, Gudnason V, Gyllensten U, Haas DW, Hall AS, Harris TB, Hattersley AT, Heath AC, Hengstenberg C, Hicks AA, Hindorff LA, Hingorani AD, Hofman A, Hovingh GK, Humphries SE, Hunt SC, Hypponen E, Jacobs KB, Jarvelin MR, Jousilahti P, Jula AM, Kaprio J, Kastelein JJ, Kayser M, Kee F, Keinanen-Kiukaanniemi SM, Kiemeney LA, Kooner JS, Kooperberg C, Koskinen S, Kovacs P, Kraja AT, Kumari M, Kuusisto J, Lakka TA, Langenberg C, Le Marchand L, Lehtimäki T, Lupoli S, Madden PA, Männistö S, Manunta P, Marette A, Matise TC, McKnight B, Meitinger T, Moll FL, Montgomery GW, Morris AD, Morris AP, Murray JC, Nelis M, Ohlsson C, Oldehinkel AJ, Ong KK, Ouwehand WH, Pasterkamp G, Peters A, Pramstaller PP, Price JF, Qi L, Raitakari OT, Rankinen T, Rao DC, Rice TK, Ritchie M, Rudan I, Salomaa V, Samani NJ, Saramies J, Sarzynski MA, Schwarz PE, Sebert S, Sever P, Shuldiner AR, Sinisalo J, Steinthorsdottir V, Stolk RP, Tardif JC, Tönjes A, Tremblay A, Tremoli E, Virtamo J, Vohl MC, Amouyel P, Asselbergs FW, Assimes TL, Bochud M, Boehm BO, Boerwinkle E, Bottinger EP, Bouchard C, Cauchi S, Chambers JC, Chanock SJ, Cooper RS, de Bakker PI, Dedoussis G, Ferrucci L, Franks PW, Froguel P, Groop LC, Haiman CA, Hamsten A, Hayes MG, Hui J, Hunter DJ, Hveem K, Jukema JW, Kaplan RC, Kivimaki M, Kuh D, Laakso M, Liu Y, Martin NG, März W, Melbye M, Moebus S, Munroe PB, Njølstad I, Oostra BA, Palmer CN, Pedersen NL, Perola M, Pérusse L, Peters U, Powell JE, Power C, Quertermous T, Rauramaa R, Reinmaa E, Ridker PM, Rivadeneira F, Rotter JI, Saaristo TE, Saleheen D, Schlessinger D, Slagboom PE, Snieder H, Spector TD, Strauch K, Stumvoll M, Tuomilehto J, Uusitupa M, van der Harst P, Völzke H, Walker M, Wareham NJ, Watkins H, Wichmann HE, Wilson JF, Zanen P, Deloukas P, Heid IM, Lindgren CM, Mohlke KL, Speliotes EK, Thorsteinsdottir U, Barroso I, Fox CS, North KE, Strachan DP, Beckmann JS, Berndt SI, Boehnke M, Borecki IB, McCarthy MI, Metspalu A, Stefansson K, Uitterlinden AG, van Duijn CM, Franke L, Willer CJ, Price AL, Lettre G, Loos RJ, Weedon MN, Ingelsson E, O'Connell JR, Abecasis GR, Chasman DI, Goddard ME, Visscher PM, Hirschhorn JN, Frayling TM, Electronic Medical Records and Genomics (eMEMERGEGE) Consortium. MIGen Consortium. PAGEGE Consortium. LifeLines Cohort Study Defining the role of common variation in the genomic and biological architecture of adult human height. Nature Genetics. 2014;46:1173–1186. doi: 10.1038/ng.3097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
eLife. 2016 Dec 14;5:e20320. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20320.006

Decision letter

Editor: Eduardo Franco1

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "Genetic and environmental influences on adult human height across birth cohorts from 1886 to 1994" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been favorably evaluated by Prabhat Jha (Senior Editor) and four reviewers, one of whom, Eduardo Franco, is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Timothy Frayling.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

This study tested the plausible hypothesis – grounded on economic theory – that the genetic influences on adult height may have increased over the last several decades as living standards improved, which would have decreased the environmental constraints from socioeconomic deprivation on attained height. The investigators could only test this hypothesis because of the opportunity to pool together via a massive consortium many twin cohorts across multiple continents. They concluded that the hypothesis could not be confirmed, as the heritability of height did not follow any clear secular trends, irrespective of population. This study addresses an interesting question using the power of almost all, if not all, the twin studies in the world – how has the genetic and environmental component to height altered over the last 100 years? Given all the studies included this can be regarded as as close to the definitive study as one can get.

Essential revisions:

The following are specific points summarized from the reviewers' critiques. They require your attention for us to consider a revised version of your paper.

1) Could the relative proportion of MZ to DZ twins have changed over time and this affected the conclusions of this study? The entire set included 40% MZ, 41% same-sex DZ, and 19% opposite-sex DZ. Greater than average height and weight increases the likelihood of a pregnancy resulting in DZ twins. Age over 40 and prior history does the same. Average age at first pregnancy and numbers thereof are likely to have changed over time and represent variables that could be sensitive to era effects related to war and other determinants of deprivation. On the other hand, there are no known genetic correlates of MZ pregnancies and the rate is constant across populations. Another potential confounder related to the above is the improvements in obstetric care over time, which would have increased the survivability of MZ twins.

2) As a sidebar to the above question: Shouldn't the proportion of opposite-sex DZ be higher than what the study found?

3) I am intrigued by the apparent secular decreases in the proportion of variation due to the unique environment component. This happened for both men and women and were more noticeable for the European cohorts. It seems to me that this suggests that the original declining-deprivation hypothesis has some merit. The authors focused on the additive genetic component but did not discuss much what happened to the other components.

4) It is worth considering presenting the results of both sexes together as well as split by sex. Whilst I can think of reasons why changes in the heritability of height may differ by sex, it is not clear why the authors have stratified their primary analysis by sex. If the main hypothesis is that secular changes will change the heritability of height, one would expect these to operate in the childhood growth of boys and girls. Perhaps girls will be less susceptible because they grow for a shorter period of their lives. But why reduce the power of the study by half? (Especially when there are wider confidence intervals around estimates from before 1940s.)

5) Are the differences between men and women in the earliest time points significant? It is not clear. The authors speculate that there may have been stronger survival effects in men, but this will be unnecessary speculation if there is no evidence of a difference between sexes.

6) It is not possible to prove the negative. Instead can the authors place some bounds on their conclusions? E.g., "we could exclude an increase of x% genetic variance per generation (25 years) with 95% confidence"? On a related note, there are no p values or effect sizes anywhere in the main text. This makes the reader take the results on faith (e.g. It is not clear whether "trend" means a statistically robust trend or just a hint). I realise these appear in the supplementary information, but I think it would help the reader to see the genetic variances across time with 95% CIs at least (and combined sexes would be most powerful).

7) Can the authors comment more on the overall increase in variance observed? It is worth noting that the genetic variance appears to go up in line with the overall variance. The reasons for this are not testable I imagine, but presumably could be due to increased ethnic diversity, and greater variation in living standards, as the average increases. It is clear in Figure 1 but not in the text.

8) Supplementary file 1: Tables 1 and 2 should be part of the main article. It would help the reader to see some stats in the main section of the paper.

eLife. 2016 Dec 14;5:e20320. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20320.007

Author response


[…]

Essential revisions:

The following are specific points summarized from the reviewers' critiques. They require your attention for us to consider a revised version of your paper.

1) Could the relative proportion of MZ to DZ twins have changed over time and this affected the conclusions of this study? The entire set included 40% MZ, 41% same-sex DZ, and 19% opposite-sex DZ. Greater than average height and weight increases the likelihood of a pregnancy resulting in DZ twins. Age over 40 and prior history does the same. Average age at first pregnancy and numbers thereof are likely to have changed over time and represent variables that could be sensitive to era effects related to war and other determinants of deprivation. On the other hand, there are no known genetic correlates of MZ pregnancies and the rate is constant across populations. Another potential confounder related to the above is the improvements in obstetric care over time, which would have increased the survivability of MZ twins.

As mentioned by the reviewer, changes in twinning rates are largely attributable to dizygotic (DZ) twinning. Monozygotic (MZ) twinning is considered an essentially random event with fairly constant rates worldwide, but a significant increase from 1960 has been reported for some countries (Imaizumi et al., 2003). This increasing MZ twinning rate could be explained by the improvements in obstetric care over time increasing the survivability of MZ twins but it has also been associated with increasing use of oral contraceptives (Imaizumi et al., 2003). in vitro fertilization (IVF) also causes MZ twinning occasionally (Aston et al., 2008). Changes in DZ twinning rates are influenced by maternal age, ethnicity, family history, and height and weight. The higher DZ twinning rate since the 1980s have been attributed to the widespread use of IVF and other fertility treatments in most industrialized countries (Imaizumi et al., 2003; Blickstein et al., 2005). Therefore, after the introduction of fertility drugs and IVF, variations in the DZ twinning were not only due to biological factors, but also depended on the popularity of fertility drugs and IVF in each country.

In this sample, the proportion of MZ to DZ twins across the studied birth-year cohorts (from 1886-1909 to 1980-1994) is as follows: 37%, 39%, 41%, 35%, 33%, 38%, 43%, 48%, 50%. This shows that the proportion of MZ to DZ twins is quite similar from 1886-1909 to 1950-1959 (33-41%) and starts to increase from 1960, which does not reflect the rise in DZ twins seen in developed countries during the past three decades. In a previous study on this database, we showed that there was no zygosity difference in height variance, neither in childhood nor in adulthood (Jelenkovic et al., 2015). Therefore, there is no reason to think that changes in the proportion of MZ to DZ twins would affect variance components estimates. This has now been discussed in limitations.

2) As a sidebar to the above question: Shouldn't the proportion of opposite-sex DZ be higher than what the study found?

The reviewer is right in that the proportion of same sex (SSDZ) and opposite sex (OSDZ) dizygotic twins should be the same. The considerably smaller proportion of OSDZ compared to SSDZ twins in this study is explained by the fact that some of the twin cohorts in our database have collected, by design, only SSDZ twins and thus do not have data on OSDZ twins. This has now been mentioned in the manuscript.

3) I am intrigued by the apparent secular decreases in the proportion of variation due to the unique environment component. This happened for both men and women and were more noticeable for the European cohorts. It seems to me that this suggests that the original declining-deprivation hypothesis has some merit. The authors focused on the additive genetic component but did not discuss much what happened to the other components.

A decreasing trend in the proportion of variation due to unique environmental factors (E) across birth-year cohorts was observed only for the four earliest birth cohorts, and was more noticeable in Europe and in women. That is, this trend was not observed in East Asia or North America and Australia (except for the slightly greater relative E variance in 1886-1909 for women in North America and Australia), nor in Europe from 1940 onwards. Moreover, in men, the decrease in relative E variance was not associated with a parallel increase in relative A variance (because relative C variance increased), which does not support the declining-deprivation hypothesis. That is, since height is influenced by environmental factors during the whole growth period (particularly in infancy and puberty), we expect that some of these environmental factors are shared by co-twins; therefore, and according to the hypothesis, this should have been seen as a decrease in C variance, which was not observed. In fact, in several cases a decrease in relative E variance was associated with an increase in relative C variance. If we look at the raw variances, the decreasing trend in E variance in the earliest birth cohorts is noticeable for women but not clear for men.

In summary, the parallel decrease in relative E variance and increase in relative A variance was observed only in European women for the four earliest birth-year cohorts; in fact, the heritability estimate decreased again in the two latest birth cohorts. Therefore, we alternatively speculated that the greater influence of unique environmental factors in the earliest birth cohorts in womenmight be explained byshrinkage in old age. Finally, since shared environmental factors did not show any pattern across birth-years cohorts, we described the results but not discussed them in the Discussion.

4) It is worth considering presenting the results of both sexes together as well as split by sex. Whilst I can think of reasons why changes in the heritability of height may differ by sex, it is not clear why the authors have stratified their primary analysis by sex. If the main hypothesis is that secular changes will change the heritability of height, one would expect these to operate in the childhood growth of boys and girls. Perhaps girls will be less susceptible because they grow for a shorter period of their lives. But why reduce the power of the study by half? (Especially when there are wider confidence intervals around estimates from before 1940s.)

We presented the results separately in men and women because the model fit statistics showed that the variance components differed between sexes in all birth-year cohorts, and the relative contribution of the genetic and environmental variance components differ in the three earliest and two latest birth-year cohorts (Table 2 in Supplementary file 1).

As suggested by the reviewer, we have now estimated both raw and relative genetic and environmental variances for men and women together. However, we decided to present these combined results as a supplementary table (Table 3 in Supplementary file 1) because 1) they did not provide any additional information on the trend across birth-year cohorts compared to the results for men and women separately and 2) since variance components differed between sexes, we think it is more appropriate to estimate them separately in men and women. This has now been mentioned in the text.

5) Are the differences between men and women in the earliest time points significant? It is not clear. The authors speculate that there may have been stronger survival effects in men, but this will be unnecessary speculation if there is no evidence of a difference between sexes.

The variance components differed between sexes in all birth-year cohorts, and the relative contribution of the genetic and environmental variance components differed in the three earliest and two latest birth-year cohorts (Table 2 in Supplementary file 1). We have now mentioned in Methods section that these differences were statistically significant at p<0.0001. Based on these results, we think that it is worth to speculate that there may have been stronger survival effects in men.

6) It is not possible to prove the negative. Instead can the authors place some bounds on their conclusions? E.g., "we could exclude an increase of x% genetic variance per generation (25 years) with 95% confidence"? On a related note, there are no p values or effect sizes anywhere in the main text. This makes the reader take the results on faith (e.g. It is not clear whether "trend" means a statistically robust trend or just a hint). I realise these appear in the supplementary information, but I think it would help the reader to see the genetic variances across time with 95% CIs at least (and combined sexes would be most powerful).

As suggested by the reviewer, we have quantified the increase in genetic variance per generation by using G-E interaction analyses. The results showed that genetic variance increased 1.37 (95% CI 0.50-2.27) and 1.07 (95% CI 0.46-1.79) per 25 years in men and women, respectively, which information is now given in the main text. The 95% CIs thus shows that the increase of genetic variance is statistically significant but the increasing effect in variance is still quite modest. As suggested by the reviewer, we have now also included in the main text the table showing the proportion of height variance explained by A, C and E factors. As previously explained in comment 4, we finally decided to present in the main text the results separately in men and women (and combined results in supplementary table) because variance components differed between sexes and thus we think that, even if less powerful, results are more correct.

7) Can the authors comment more on the overall increase in variance observed? It is worth noting that the genetic variance appears to go up in line with the overall variance. The reasons for this are not testable I imagine, but presumably could be due to increased ethnic diversity, and greater variation in living standards, as the average increases. It is clear in Figure 1 but not in the text.

Although there is a general trend to increasing total and genetic variance across birth cohorts, genetic variance does not always go up with total variance. For example, in men, the greatest increase in total variance was observed from birth cohort 1940-1949 to 1960-1969 and although genetic variance also increased during this period it increased especially in the two latest birth-year cohorts (1970-1979 and 1980-1994). In women, although total variance also started to increase from birth cohort 1940-1949, genetic variance showed the greatest increase from 1886-1900 to 1940-1949. As can be seen in Figure 1, part of the increase in total variance is due to the increase in shared environmental variance. Therefore, and as suggested by the reviewer, the increase in total height variation could be due to both increased ethnic diversity and greater variation in living standards. This has now been discussed in the text.

8) Supplementary file 1: Tables 1 and 2 should be part of the main article. It would help the reader to see some stats in the main section of the paper.

Supplementary Table 2 is now part the main text as Table 2: however, we have not included Supplementary Table 1 because the results are already provided in Figure 1 (without CIs) and we think it would provide repeated information.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Supplementary tables.

    (A) Height variance explained by additive genetic, shared environmental and unique environmental factors by birth year, sex and geographic-cultural region. (B) Model fit statistics for adult height by birth-year cohorts (all twin cohorts together). (C) Height variance and proportion of height variance explained by additive genetic, shared environmental and unique environmental factors by birth year, sex and geographic-cultural region.

    DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.20320.005

    elife-20320-supp1.doc (272KB, doc)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.20320.005

    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES