Abstract
Aggression by top predators can create a “landscape of fear” in which subordinate predators restrict their activity to low‐risk areas or times of day. At large spatial or temporal scales, this can result in the costly loss of access to resources. However, fine‐scale reactive avoidance may minimize the risk of aggressive encounters for subordinate predators while maintaining access to resources, thereby providing a mechanism for coexistence. We investigated fine‐scale spatiotemporal avoidance in a guild of African predators characterized by intense interference competition. Vulnerable to food stealing and direct killing, cheetahs are expected to avoid both larger predators; hyenas are expected to avoid lions. We deployed a grid of 225 camera traps across 1,125 km2 in Serengeti National Park, Tanzania, to evaluate concurrent patterns of habitat use by lions, hyenas, cheetahs, and their primary prey. We used hurdle models to evaluate whether smaller species avoided areas preferred by larger species, and we used time‐to‐event models to evaluate fine‐scale temporal avoidance in the hours immediately surrounding top predator activity. We found no evidence of long‐term displacement of subordinate species, even at fine spatial scales. Instead, hyenas and cheetahs were positively associated with lions except in areas with exceptionally high lion use. Hyenas and lions appeared to actively track each, while cheetahs appear to maintain long‐term access to sites with high lion use by actively avoiding those areas just in the hours immediately following lion activity. Our results suggest that cheetahs are able to use patches of preferred habitat by avoiding lions on a moment‐to‐moment basis. Such fine‐scale temporal avoidance is likely to be less costly than long‐term avoidance of preferred areas: This may help explain why cheetahs are able to coexist with lions despite high rates of lion‐inflicted mortality, and highlights reactive avoidance as a general mechanism for predator coexistence.
Keywords: Acinonyx jubatus, carnivore coexistence, Crocuta crocuta, interference competition, Panthera leo
1. Introduction
In ecosystems around the world, top predators kill, harass, and steal food from smaller predators (Caro & Stoner, 2003; Donadio & Buskirk, 2006; Linnell & Strand, 2000; Palomares & Caro, 1999). These direct, aggressive interactions, generally referred to as interference competition, are widespread and substantial, and can have profound consequences for the distributions and population dynamics of smaller predators (Prugh et al., 2009; Ritchie & Johnson, 2009). Top predators often suppress or exclude subordinate predators from the larger landscape (Kamler, Ballard, Gilliland, Lemons, & Mote, 2003; Newsome & Ripple, 2014; Swanson et al., 2014), and the loss of top predators can result in subordinate predator “release” or rapid population expansion (Prugh et al., 2009; Ritchie & Johnson, 2009); in turn, these dynamics can have widespread, cascading effects throughout the larger ecosystem (Estes et al., 2011; Ripple et al., 2014; Suraci, Clinchy, Dill, Roberts, & Zanette, 2016). However, these patterns of suppression and coexistence vary across systems and species (Elmhagen, Ludwig, Rushton, Helle, & Lindén, 2010; Elmhagen & Rushton, 2007; Swanson et al., 2014); thus, as top predators decline globally (Estes et al., 2011; Ripple et al., 2014), understanding the drivers of predator suppression and coexistence becomes increasingly important.
Spatiotemporal avoidance plays a critical role in minimizing the costs of interference competition and promoting predator–predator coexistence. Such avoidance takes many forms: Subordinate predators may avoid risky habitat types (Fedriani, Palomares, & Delibes, 1999; Mukherjee, Zelcer, & Kotler, 2008), hours of the day (Bischof, Ali, Kabir, Hameed, & Nawaz, 2014; Carothers & Jaksić, 1984; Hayward & Slotow, 2009), or known areas of apex predator activity (Kamler et al., 2003; Swanson et al., 2014). For example, foxes avoid habitats used by European lynx (Fedriani et al., 1999), and coyotes concentrate primarily at boundaries between wolf‐pack territories (Fuller & Keith, 1981). These avoidance strategies can be costly: By restricting their activity to “safe” areas or times of day in what is referred to as a “landscape of fear” (sensu Laundré, Hernández, & Altendorf, 2001), subordinate species can lose access to vital resources such as prey, water, or shelter (Lesmeister, Nielsen, Schauber, & Hellgren, 2015; Mukherjee et al., 2008; Ritchie & Johnson, 2009; Sergio & Hiraldo, 2008).
The demographic consequences of behavioral avoidance are well documented in small‐scale predator–prey systems (Preisser, Bolnick, & Benard, 2005; Schmitz, Beckerman, & O'Brien, 1997; Werner & Peacor, 2003) and can reasonably be expected to apply to predator–predator systems with interference competition. Indeed, large‐scale avoidance of top predator territories appears to be a primary driver of suppression of African wild dogs (Swanson et al., 2014) and swift foxes (Kamler et al., 2003). However, the strength of these costs depends on numerous factors such as predator hunting strategy (Schmitz, Krivan, & Ovadia, 2004) and habitat productivity (Bolnick & Preisser, 2005); considerable debate remains as to the relative importance of the landscape of fear in driving dynamics of large, wide‐ranging species (Beschta & Ripple, 2013; Kauffman, Brodie, & Jules, 2010).
Recent studies of large African predators have suggested that fine‐scale reactive avoidance strategies may promote coexistence by minimizing the risks of aggressive encounters but allowing subordinate species to maintain access to resources (Broekhuis, Cozzi, Valeix, McNutt, & Macdonald, 2013; Swanson et al., 2014). Instead of preemptively avoiding large portions of the landscape or preferred habitat types and losing access to the resources within, subordinate predator species may reactively alter their habitat use on a moment‐to‐moment basis. Fine‐scale avoidance may explain contrasting patterns of predator suppression documented in African carnivores (Swanson et al., 2014), highlighting a need to better understand how large predators share the landscape at fine spatial and temporal scales.
Here, we evaluate patterns of spatial and temporal avoidance among lions (Panthera leo), spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta), and cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) in Serengeti National Park, Tanzania. Despite dissimilar hunting strategies (Kruuk, 1972; Schaller, 1972), lions and hyenas show considerable dietary overlap (Hayward, 2006) and reciprocally harass and steal food from each other (Höner, Wachter, East, & Hofer, 2002; Kissui & Packer, 2004; Périquet, Fritz, & Revilla, 2015). Lions tend to dominate aggressive interactions and can suppress hyena populations through extensive food stealing (Watts & Holekamp, 2008). In large groups, hyenas can reciprocally displace lions from a carcass (Cooper, 1991; Höner et al., 2002) and directly benefit from these interactions (Watts & Holekamp, 2008). However, this does not appear to inflict a measurable cost to lions (Kissui & Packer, 2004), and lion and hyena population densities are positively correlated across African reserves (Creel & Creel, 1996). In contrast, lions and hyenas are both believed to suppress cheetah populations through high rates of direct killing (Laurenson, 1995) and cheetahs are often described as fugitive species, ranging widely and persisting only in marginal areas with low lion and hyena densities (e.g., Caro & Stoner, 2003; Chauvenet, Durant, Hilborn, & Pettorelli, 2011; Durant, 1998, 2000; Saleni, 2007). However, recent studies show that lion‐inflicted mortality is lower than previously assumed (Mills & Mills, 2014) and that cheetah populations are not suppressed by high lion densities (Swanson et al., 2014). Thus, while cheetahs actively avoid lions (Durant, 2000), considerable debate remains as to whether this response translates into long‐term spatial displacement that could threaten cheetah populations and conservation efforts (cf. Broekhuis et al., 2013 and Swanson et al., 2014 with Durant, 1998 and Vanak et al., 2013).
We use camera traps to evaluate fine‐scale spatial avoidance, characterized by long‐term (preemptive) avoidance of camera sites, and fine‐scale temporal avoidance, characterized by short‐term (reactive) avoidance of camera sites in the hours immediately surrounding top predator activity. Fine‐scale avoidance behavior has primarily been evaluated via extensive use of GPS collars (e.g., Broekhuis et al., 2013; Courbin et al., 2015; Vanak et al., 2013), but this can be prohibitively expensive. Camera traps provide an affordable alternative to simultaneously assess fine‐scale avoidance across multiple species, and we apply new methodological approaches to analyze camera‐trap data at finer temporal scales than allowed by standard multispecies occupancy models (MacKenzie et al., 2006; Richmond, Hines, & Beissinger, 2010; Sollmann et al., 2012).
We frame our analyses according to the respective competitive abilities of each species. As the dominant competitor, lions are expected to gain access to their preferred landscape characteristics, regardless of hyena or cheetah activity. Hyenas should be sensitive to lions, either avoiding lions because lions dominate aggressive interactions or following lions for scavenging opportunities. Because the outcomes of lion–hyena interactions depend on group size, this relationship may be complex. We expect cheetahs to avoid both lions and hyenas according to their “fugitive species” status.
2. Methods
2.1. Study system and field survey
Our 1,125‐km2 study area (bounded in the northwest at lat/long of −2.363589, 34.72594; in the southeast at −2.660651, 35.18051) was located at the center of Serengeti National Park, Tanzania, at the intersection of open plains and savanna woodlands (Figure 1a). Rainfall and vegetation follow a northwest–southeast gradient: wetter, denser woodlands in the northwest to drier, short‐grass plains in the southeast (Sinclair, 1995). The ecosystem holds some of the highest concentrations of large predators in the world (Sinclair, 1995) and is dominated by the annual migration of 1.6 million wildebeest (Connochaetes taurinus), zebra (Equus quagga), and gazelle (Eudorcas thomsonii) that follow the seasonal rains from Kenya's Masai Mara Reserve onto the nutrient‐rich plains of the Ngorongoro Conservation Area and Serengeti National Park (Holdo, Holt, & Fryxell, 2009).
The Serengeti Lion Project has monitored lion ranging and demography since the 1960s (Packer et al., 2005; Swanson et al., 2014). The camera survey is located within the long‐term lion study area, where all prides are monitored by radiotelemetry of one VHF‐collared female per pride.
We established 225 camera traps across a grid layout such that each camera was near the center of a 5‐km2 grid cell (Figure 1b). Cameras were set ~50 cm above ground level and covered a detection zone of ~15 m distance and 50° angle (Cusack et al., 2015). Operating continuously (aside from camera failure), the survey recorded 99,241 camera‐trap days between June 2010 and May 2013. Species, number of individuals, and behaviors in each image were identified by citizen scientists via Snapshot Serengeti (www.snapshotserengeti.org). Swanson et al. (2015) provide details on the camera survey and data processing. Although validation of volunteer classifications against an expert‐classified dataset demonstrates that final volunteer species classifications agree with experts 97% of the time (Swanson et al., 2015), we limited all analyses in this study to images with at least 75% agreement among raw classifications, guaranteeing 99.7% species identification accuracy (see Swanson, Kosmala, Lintott, and Packer (2016) for additional details).
2.2. Analytical approaches
2.2.1. Spatial avoidance
We evaluated patterns of fine‐scale spatial avoidance among lions, hyenas, and cheetahs by comparing species‐specific capture rates at each site. Although raw camera‐trap capture rates do not reflect larger‐scale lion densities, likely due to lions’ attraction to isolated trees, comparison of camera‐trap captures to VHF‐collar lion locations indicates that cameras accurately reflect localized lion presence (Swanson, 2014). To ensure independence across captures, we limited analyses to no more than one sighting per day for a given species at a given site.
Because the camera‐trap data were zero‐inflated and overdispersed, we used hurdle models [also known as “zero altered Poisson/negative binomial” or ZAP/ZINB models (Zuur, Ieno, Walker, Saveliev, & Smith, 2009)] to specify two different underlying processes: a binomial process (species was detected or not) and a truncated count process [how many were seen (1 or more), given that the species was detected]. Because camera traps sample in only one direction and additional animals could have passed behind the camera, absence can imply true absence or lack of detection, and abundance represents minimum abundance.
For each subordinate species (hyenas and cheetahs), we evaluated a full model that included localized habitat characteristics, localized prey availability, and the localized presence/abundance of lions and the other subordinate species as predictors. For completeness and ability to compare joint habitat selection, we evaluated the same model for lions. We evaluated the relative importance of intraguild predators using analysis of deviance (ANODEV; Harris et al., 2005) and comparing models with and without covariates indicating the presence and abundance of intraguild predators. ANODEV was calculated as the difference in model deviance (−2logL) between the null model (no predictors) and the current model, divided by the difference between the null model and a full model that included all potential predictor variables. ANODEV reflects the fraction of model variation captured by the predictors in the current model, relative to the null model (which has a value of 0) and the full model (which has a value of 1).
The localized influence of each predator and prey species was included as presence in the binomial models (i.e., 1 if ever detected at that site; 0 if never detected at that site) and abundance in count models (i.e., number of independent captures at that site). The prey species included buffalo (Syncerus caffer), wildebeest (Connochaetes taurinus), and Thomson's gazelle (Eudorcas thomsonii) as the three main prey species of Serengeti lions, hyenas, and cheetahs, respectively (Schaller, 1972; Sinclair & Norton‐Griffiths, 1979). Habitat characteristics were as follows: Distance to river (m), Distance to kopje (m), Grass height (visually estimated on a scale of 1–5), Percent Tree Cover (<1 km, characterized from Landsat imagery by Sexton et al., 2013), Tree isolation (measured by the average distance (m) to the 10 nearest trees), Habitat (plains vs. woodlands), Shade (visually estimated on a scale of 0–4), and an interaction term for Tree isolation * Shade to reflect the potential attractiveness of an isolated shade tree. All nonbinary predictors were standardized by z‐score to facilitate comparing effect sizes across predictors with widely varying ranges and units (mean and standard deviations provided in Table S1). There was no significant colinearity among the predictor variables (r < 0.7 for all pairwise comparisons).
We evaluated patterns of partitioning aggregated across all years and controlled for site‐specific search effort by specifying an offset of log(# Camera‐trap days). To verify that patterns were robust within and among years, we aggregated all diurnal versus nocturnal sightings and all seasonal data (wet vs. dry season), as well as separately for each individual wet and dry season. In all cases, results were qualitatively similar to the overall aggregated analysis and are not reported here.
2.2.2. Temporal avoidance
We summarized hourly detections of predator and prey species and interpreted statistically significant (p < .05) departures from an expected hourly frequency of 0.042 (1/24) as evidence of nonrandom diel activity patterns.
We evaluated temporal avoidance or attraction by calculating time since last prey (gazelle, wildebeest, or buffalo) and time since last predator (lion, hyena, or cheetah) for all independent predator sightings. Because many species will spend multiple hours at a site, we calculated all “time since” measures as the time difference between when the first species left and the second species arrived. We aggregated “time since” observations into thirteen 12‐hr bins (e.g., 0–12, 12–24, …, 132–144, or >144 hr since the last species) and assessed short‐term temporal attraction or avoidance by comparing observed versus expected number of observations during the first 0–12 and 12–24 hr following detection of another species. We calculated expected values for number of observations during each 12‐hr bin using a discrete time‐to‐event model that assumed exponential decay in the probability that the species of interest had not been seen yet, with expected encounter probabilities calculated using pooled information on detection probability throughout the first 6 days, with an adjustment based on probability of activity during each 12‐hr bin based on diel activity patterns for the second observed species. This assured that observed patterns were not due to similar or contrasting diel patterns (e.g., a strictly nocturnal species not observed in the 12 hr following an early morning observation of a mostly diurnal species might reflect temporal niche partitioning rather than active avoidance; see Swanson, 2014).
3. Results
3.1. Habitat use and spatial avoidance
Cheetah presence was highest at sites with shaded, isolated trees in relatively open habitat; of those sites where cheetahs were present, cheetah sightings were highest at shaded sites away from river confluences but closer to kopjes (Table 1a). Cheetah sightings also increased with lion sightings, reaching a maximum at approximately 0.06 lion sightings per day; although cheetah sightings declined slightly at sites with the highest lion numbers, confidence intervals overlapped broadly over a range of 0.025–0.1 lions per day (Figure 3). Although a significant predictor, lion abundance explained relatively little additional variation (8.6%) in cheetah presence and abundance after considering habitat and prey, which explained 88.0% of the total deviance (Table 2). Cheetah presence was not significantly related to hyena presence, but cheetah abundance exhibited a nonlinear relationship with hyena abundance (p = .006), declining when hyena abundance exceeded ~1.1 SD above average) (Table 1a; Figures 2 and 3). However, this effect was weak, and including hyenas in the spatial model explained only 3.4% more deviance (Table 2).
Table 1.
Estimate | SE | p | |
---|---|---|---|
(A) Full model: Cheetahs ~ Habitat, Prey, Lions, Hyenas | |||
Presence model: binomial with logit link | |||
(Intercept) | −3.991 | 1.336 | 0.0028** |
Shade | 0.881 | 0.249 | <0.0001*** |
Tree isolation | 1.118 | 0.303 | <0.0001*** |
Percent Cover | −0.56 | 0.251 | 0.0256* |
Distance to river | −0.028 | 0.258 | 0.9131 |
Distance to confluence | −0.247 | 0.278 | 0.375 |
Distance to kopje | 0.077 | 0.189 | 0.6852 |
Habitat | −0.827 | 0.223 | <0.0001*** |
Buffalo | 0.327 | 0.529 | 0.5361 |
Wildebeest | 2.256 | 1.203 | 0.0607 |
T. gazelle | 0.181 | 0.829 | 0.827 |
Lion | 0.593 | 0.421 | 0.1585 |
Hyena | 1.087 | 0.929 | 0.2423 |
Shade*Tree isolation | 0.688 | 0.276 | 0.0127* |
Count model: truncated Poisson with log link | |||
(Intercept) | −5.342 | 0.145 | <0.0001*** |
Shade | 0.401 | 0.114 | 0.0004*** |
Tree isolation | 0.15 | 0.096 | 0.1174 |
Percent Cover | −0.083 | 0.112 | 0.4556 |
Distance to river | −0.047 | 0.073 | 0.522 |
Distance to confluence | 0.295 | 0.075 | <0.0001*** |
Distance to kopje | −0.394 | 0.076 | <0.0001*** |
Habitat | −0.225 | 0.15 | 0.1345 |
Buffalo/day | −0.13 | 0.104 | 0.2097 |
Wildebeest/day | −0.179 | 0.096 | 0.0636 |
T. gazelle/day | −0.162 | 0.116 | 0.1612 |
Lions/day | 0.458 | 0.093 | <0.0001*** |
Lions/day2 | −0.055 | 0.014 | <0.0001*** |
Hyenas/day | 0.363 | 0.198 | 0.0661 |
Hyenas/day2 | −0.331 | 0.12 | 0.0057** |
Shade*Tree isolation | −0.196 | 0.104 | 0.0587 |
(B) Full model: Hyenas ~ habitat, prey, lions, cheetahs | |||
Presence model: binomial with logit link | |||
(Intercept) | −0.91 | 1.13 | 0.421 |
Shade | −0.153 | 0.412 | 0.711 |
Tree isolation | 0.128 | 0.536 | 0.812 |
Percent Cover | −0.055 | 0.365 | 0.881 |
Distance to river | −0.55 | 0.399 | 0.168 |
Distance to confluence | 0.255 | 0.434 | 0.556 |
Distance to kopje | 0.057 | 0.329 | 0.864 |
Habitat | 0.058 | 0.411 | 0.888 |
Buffalo | −0.201 | 0.987 | 0.838 |
Wildebeest | 1.485 | 1.265 | 0.24 |
T. gazelle | 2.066 | 0.822 | 0.012* |
Lion | 1.141 | 0.696 | 0.101 |
Cheetah | 0.932 | 0.869 | 0.284 |
Shade*Tree isolation | 0.238 | 0.431 | 0.582 |
Count model: truncated Poisson with log link | |||
(Intercept) | −3.626 | 0.025 | <0.0001*** |
Shade | −0.138 | 0.027 | <0.0001*** |
Tree isolation | 0.02 | 0.034 | 0.56591 |
Percent Cover | 0.207 | 0.022 | <0.0001*** |
Distance to river | 0.065 | 0.033 | 0.0529 |
Distance to confluence | −0.012 | 0.034 | 0.7358 |
Distance to kopje | −0.071 | 0.023 | 0.0025** |
Habitat | −0.02 | 0.028 | 0.47593 |
Buffalo/day | 0.069 | 0.022 | 0.00163** |
Wildebeest/day | 0.136 | 0.021 | <.0001*** |
T. gazelle/day | 0.331 | 0.022 | <0.0001*** |
Lions/day | 0.485 | 0.04 | <0.0001*** |
Lions/day2 | −0.059 | 0.008 | <0.0001*** |
Cheetahs/day | −0.01 | 0.057 | 0.86247 |
Cheetahs/day2 | 0.011 | 0.015 | 0.4671 |
Shade*Tree isolation | −0.013 | 0.028 | 0.63756 |
(C) Full model: Lions ~ habitat, prey, hyenas, cheetahs | |||
Presence model: binomial with logit link | |||
(Intercept) | −2.001 | 1.014 | 0.04839* |
Shade | 0.159 | 0.231 | 0.4916 |
Tree isolation | 0.253 | 0.275 | 0.35792 |
Percent Cover | −0.261 | 0.199 | 0.18974 |
Distance to river | −0.459 | 0.249 | 0.06523 |
Distance to confluence | 0.624 | 0.263 | 0.01791* |
Distance to kopje | −0.26 | 0.173 | 0.13265 |
Habitat | 0.2 | 0.228 | 0.38122 |
Buffalo | 1.427 | 0.508 | 0.00496** |
Wildebeest | 0.13 | 0.989 | 0.89519 |
T. gazelle | 0.629 | 0.613 | 0.30458 |
Hyena | 1.229 | 0.699 | 0.07849. |
Cheetah | 0.579 | 0.41 | 0.15745 |
Shade*Tree isolation | 0.242 | 0.231 | 0.29481 |
Count model: truncated Poisson with log link | |||
(Intercept) | −5.039 | 0.059 | <0.0001*** |
Shade | −0.089 | 0.061 | 0.14963 |
Tree isolation | 0.372 | 0.066 | <0.0001*** |
Percent Cover | −0.15 | 0.057 | 0.00911** |
Distance to river | 0.015 | 0.062 | 0.80262 |
Distance to confluence | −0.199 | 0.069 | 0.00384** |
Distance to kopje | 0.068 | 0.053 | 0.19691 |
Habitat | −0.071 | 0.066 | 0.28409 |
Buffalo/day | 0.344 | 0.028 | <0.0001*** |
Wildebeest/day | 0.114 | 0.053 | 0.03145* |
T. gazelle/day | −0.433 | 0.098 | <.0001*** |
Cheetahs/day | 0.314 | 0.109 | 0.00380** |
Cheetahs/day2 | −0.004 | 0.029 | 0.88546 |
Hyenas/day | 0.587 | 0.07 | <0.0001*** |
Hyenas/day2 | −0.032 | 0.012 | 0.00803*** |
Shade*Tree isolation | 0.19 | 0.059 | 0.00122** |
* indicates <0.05, ** indicates <0.01, and *** indicates <0.001
Table 2.
Models | ANODEV (%) | Incremental deviance |
---|---|---|
Cheetahs | ||
Null model | 0.0 | – |
Habitat, prey | 88.0 | – |
Habitat, prey, hyenas | 91.4 | 3.4 |
Habitat, prey, lions | 96.6 | 8.6 |
Habitat, prey, hyenas, lions | 100.0 | 12.0 |
Hyenas | ||
Null model | 0.0 | – |
Habitat, prey | 78.2 | – |
Habitat, prey, cheetahs | 81.1 | 2.9 |
Habitat, prey, lions | 99.7 | 21.5 |
Habitat, prey, cheetahs, lions | 100.0 | 21.8 |
Hyena presence and abundance were highest at sites with high localized prey availability (Table 1b), and abundance was higher at sites with increased tree cover and lower shade that were farther from kopjes. Hyena sightings increased with lion numbers up to a threshold of about 0.06 lion sightings per day, declining slightly at sites with highest lion numbers (Figure 3). Including lions as a predictor explained an incremental 21.5% of deviance in the hyena spatial models (Table 2).
3.2. Temporal activity patterns
All species except spotted hyenas were detected primarily during the day (Figure 4), and hourly activity patterns were positively correlated (r = 0.46–0.87) for all species pairs except those involving hyenas (r = −0.90 to −0.40). Lion and cheetah sightings occurred most frequently during midday, reflecting their use of shaded sites as daytime resting spots (Swanson, 2014); however, cheetahs were less active at night but exhibited activity spikes at dawn and dusk, whereas lions remained moderately active at all hours (Figure 4). In contrast, hyenas were detected primarily at night, and were most often observed moving, although they sometimes utilized trees or kopjes as daytime resting sites. All prey species were primarily diurnal, although buffalo were more likely to remain active into early nocturnal hours and gazelles demonstrated multiple activity peaks at dawn, midday, and dusk and a smaller peak near midnight (Figure 4). Thus, camera traps appear to reflect fine‐scale selection of shaded resting spots for lions and cheetahs during midday, but capture more general activity patterns throughout the remainder of the day.
3.3. Time since prey
Lion and hyena activity was 1.5‐ to 3.5‐fold greater within the first 12–24 hr after prey sightings (Table 3; Figure 5); cheetahs exhibited a similar boost in activity during the first 12 hr after prey sightings, but relationships were insignificant, presumably due to smaller sample sizes. For all three predators, prey relationships were strongest for wildebeest during the first 12 hr, and then declined rapidly over the next 24 hr, suggesting that all three predators actively tracked wildebeest. Hyena exhibited a similar pattern with gazelle, whereas lion and cheetah activity declined more slowly following gazelle activity. Activity patterns for all three predators were higher for up to 36 hr following buffalo activity, but relationships were generally weaker than for other prey (Table 3).
Table 3.
Species seen | Since | 0–12 hr | 12–24 hr | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
O/E | χ2 | O/E | χ2 | ||
Lion | Gazelle | 1.72 | 11.63 | 1.97 | 17.10 |
Wildebeest | 2.45 | 29.25 | 1.96 | 10.90 | |
Buffalo | 1.52 | 3.21 | 1.78 | 7.11 | |
Hyena | 2.20 | 33.38 | 1.53 | 8.93 | |
Cheetah | 0.89 | 0.09 | 1.84 | 4.22 | |
Hyena | Gazelle | 2.44 | 229.64 | 1.97 | 82.22 |
Wildebeest | 3.23 | 356.16 | 1.54 | 17.36 | |
Buffalo | 1.91 | 34.10 | 1.78 | 11.31 | |
Lion | 2.29 | 45.09 | 1.39 | 3.66 | |
Cheetah | 2.10 | 9.20 | 0.59 | 1.15 | |
Cheetah | Gazelle | 1.29 | 0.64 | 1.64 | 2.49 |
Wildebeest | 1.55 | 1.56 | 1.18 | 0.14 | |
Buffalo | 2.15 | 1.23 | 1.06 | 0.00 | |
Lion | 0.00 | 5.34 | 1.09 | 0.04 | |
Hyena | 1.60 | 2.03 | 0.60 | 1.33 |
3.4. Time since predator
Cheetahs never appeared at sites within 12 hr of a lion sighting (Figure 6; Table 3), but showed expected levels of activity 12–36 hr after lion sightings and heightened activity 36–48 hr after a lion sighting (Figure 6; χ2 = 5.66, p = .017). By contrast, lions neither followed nor avoided cheetahs during the first 12 hr after a cheetah sighting (Table 3). Although cheetahs did not show any patterns of attraction or avoidance to hyenas, hyenas appeared at sites approximately 2.2 times more often than expected by chance in the first 12 hr after cheetah sightings (Table 3).
Lions and hyenas often appeared in the first hours after each other (Figure 6), and hyenas sometimes appeared while lions are still at the camera (three cases, all of which involved lions making and remaining on a kill). However, whereas lion sightings remained significantly higher for 24 hr following hyena sightings, hyena sightings declined sharply following 12 hr after a lion sighting (Table 3; Figure 6).
4. Discussion
Although subordinate competitors are expected to seek out “competition refuges” by selecting marginal habitats (Durant, 1998; Linnell & Strand, 2000), we found that interference competition among lions, hyenas, and cheetahs did not translate into long‐term displacement by subordinate species, even at fine spatial scales. In fact, hyenas and cheetahs were positively associated with lions except perhaps in areas with exceptionally high lion use; similarly, cheetahs showed no evidence of avoiding hyenas except in areas of extremely high hyena use. Fine‐scale temporal analyses further indicated that hyenas and lions actively tracked each other, whereas cheetahs actively avoided lion‐occupied areas for at least 12 hr. These contrasting patterns suggest that while cheetahs perceive lions as a threat, they are able to avoid them behaviorally, thus minimizing the need for long‐term spatial avoidance and the subsequent loss of access to resources.
4.1. Cheetahs
Our results challenge the long‐standing perception of cheetahs as “refuge species” that are only able to persist in marginal areas with low lion, hyena, and prey densities (e.g., Caro & Stoner, 2003; Chauvenet et al., 2011; Durant, 1998, 2000; Laurenson, 1994, 1995; Saleni, 2007). Lions and cheetahs were both attracted to shady trees on the open plains (Table 1a, c), and cheetahs continued to use these preferred habitat patches despite moderately high levels of lion use. Instead of generally avoiding areas of known lion use, cheetahs reduced the chance of encountering a lion by avoiding those areas only when lions were present (Figure 6). Such active temporal avoidance did not appear to reduce access by cheetahs to their primary prey (Figure 5), Thomson's gazelle. In contrast to their temporal avoidance of lions, cheetahs show no temporal avoidance of hyenas (Tables 1a and 2; Figure 6) perhaps because hyenas are less of a threat than lions (Laurenson, 1994, 1995) or because cursorial predators are expected to trigger smaller avoidance responses than ambush predators (Preisser, Orrock, & Schmitz, 2007; Schmitz et al., 2004).
By responding reactively, and only avoiding preferred habitats immediately following lion presence, cheetahs may minimize their risk of encountering lions while still maintaining access to vital resources. Indeed, Rostro‐García, Kamler, and Hunter (2015) found that cheetahs successfully utilized and hunted in a wider array of habitats than previously thought, and recent work by Broekhuis et al. (2013) and Vanak et al. (2013) found that cheetahs distributed themselves primarily with respect to their prey and only secondarily maintained a safe distance from the nearest lion. Our results confirm that this behavior does not translate into spatial displacement but is instead achieved by fleeting temporal avoidance at a given location (i.e., within 12 hr of a lion sighting). Such fine‐scale active avoidance may be key to cheetah persistence in the face of interference competition by lions: Lion density has no significant impact on cheetah numbers through time or across reserves (Mills & Mills, 2014; Swanson et al., 2014).
4.2. Hyenas
Despite dissimilar habitat preferences, lions and hyenas appear at the same camera‐trap sites (Figure 3) on the same days (Figure 6). These patterns of attraction could reflect active attraction between predator species: Lions and hyenas actively scavenge from each other (Kissui & Packer, 2004; Périquet et al., 2015), although interference outcomes are dependent on group size and population densities (Cooper, 1991; Höner et al., 2002; Watts & Holekamp, 2008). Although the quadratic relationship between lions and hyenas across camera sites (Figure 3) was significant (Table 1) and may reflect an attraction of hyenas to lions until some threshold lion density is reached, the effect size at high lion densities is small and driven exclusively by observations at three sites (out of 210 sites occupied hyenas).
Alternatively, the apparent attraction between lions and hyenas may be driven by mutual attraction to shared prey. Lions and hyenas both prey upon wildebeest (Hayward, 2006; Kruuk, 1972; Schaller, 1972), appear more often at sites with higher localized prey densities (Table 1b, c), and appear to actively follow these prey (Figure 5). Lion and hyena peak more sharply and decline more quickly following wildebeest sightings than sightings of each other (Table 3), but further investigation and additional data are needed to determine whether lions and hyenas are actively attracted to each other or simply tracking the same prey.
5. Conclusions
Aggressive interactions among predators are widespread and substantial (Arim & Marquet, 2004), triggering active avoidance by subordinate species to minimize these encounters (Prugh et al., 2009; Ritchie & Johnson, 2009). This avoidance is often assumed to result in the costly displacement from preferred habitats or reduced access to prey and result in population suppression (e.g., Durant, 1998, 2000). However, while these costs are documented in a range of systems (Schmitz et al., 1997; Swanson et al., 2014), our results suggest that active avoidance does not universally translate into costly large‐scale displacement associated with population suppression.
Specifically, we found that despite actively avoiding lions (Figure 6; see also Durant, 2000), cheetahs are displaced from neither preferred habitat patches nor larger areas across the landscape (Broekhuis et al., 2013; Swanson et al., 2014; Vanak et al., 2013), nor do they lose access to their prey (Figure 5) or show any signs of population‐level suppression by lions (Swanson et al., 2014). In sharp contrast, African wild dogs actively avoid lions (Webster, McNutt, & McComb, 2012), and this avoidance translates into large‐scale displacement that carries heavy demographic costs: Lions appear to directly suppress wild dog populations largely through exclusion from large areas of the landscape (Creel & Creel, 1996; Swanson et al., 2014).
Thus, the relative importance of this “landscape of fear” varies across systems and species and is likely dependent on a number of factors including hunting strategy and habitat complexity. For example, ambush predators trigger more severe avoidance responses than those with more cursorial approaches (Preisser et al., 2007; Schmitz et al., 2004), and avoidance costs are low for non‐habitat‐specialists (Preisser et al., 2007; Schmitz et al., 2004). Habitat complexity (Janssen, Sabelis, Magalhães, Montserrat, & Van der Hammen, 2007) and high ecosystem productivity (Elmhagen & Rushton, 2007; Elmhagen et al., 2010) are further expected to minimize displacement and suppression. Ultimately, the landscape of fear created by top predators is complex and species‐specific and does not always translate into population‐level suppression. The different patterns of spatial and temporal avoidance by subordinate species may help explain the diverse patterns of suppression and coexistence within predator guilds.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interests for this work.
Data Accessibility
All data used in this study are available at datadryad.org/resource/doi:10.5061/dryad.5pt92.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
Research clearance was provided by the Tanzania Wildlife Research Institute and Tanzania National Parks. We thank members of the Serengeti Lion Project and the 28,040 volunteers who contributed to Snapshot Serengeti classifications (complete list at www.snapshotserengeti.org/#/authors). This work was supported by National Science Foundation grant DEB‐1020479 to CP for maintenance of the long‐term Lion Project, the University of Minnesota Supercomputing Institute, private donations raised during the Serengeti Live and Save Snapshot Serengeti crowd‐funding campaigns, and by grants to AS from Explorer's Club, UMN Thesis Research Grants, UMN Office of International Programs, American Society of Mammalogists, and Minnesota Zoo Ulysses S. Seal Conservation fund. Snapshot Serengeti website development was funded by awards to the Zooniverse from the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation.
Swanson, A. , Arnold, T. , Kosmala, M. , Forester, J. and Packer, C. (2016), In the absence of a “landscape of fear”: How lions, hyenas, and cheetahs coexist. Ecology and Evolution, 6: 8534–8545. doi: 10.1002/ece3.2569
References
- Arim, M. , & Marquet, P. A. (2004). Intraguild predation: A widespread interaction related to species biology. Ecology Letters, 7, 557–564. [Google Scholar]
- Beschta, R. L. , & Ripple, W. J. (2013). Are wolves saving Yellowstone's aspen? A landscape‐level test of a behaviorally mediated trophic cascade: Comment. Ecology, 94, 1420–1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bischof, R. , Ali, H. , Kabir, M. , Hameed, S. , & Nawaz, M. A. (2014). Being the underdog: An elusive small carnivore uses space with prey and time without enemies: Space and time use by Altai mountain weasel. Journal of Zoology, 293, 40–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bolnick, D. I. , & Preisser, E. L. (2005). Resource competition modifies the strength of trait‐mediated predator–prey interactions: A meta‐analysis. Ecology, 86, 2771–2779. [Google Scholar]
- Broekhuis, F. , Cozzi, G. , Valeix, M. , McNutt, J. W. , & Macdonald, D. W. (2013). Risk avoidance in sympatric large carnivores: Reactive or predictive?. Journal of Animal Ecology, 82, 1098–1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caro, T. M. , & Stoner, C. J. (2003). The potential for interspecific competition among African carnivores. Biological Conservation, 110, 67–75. [Google Scholar]
- Carothers, J. H. , & Jaksić, F. M. (1984). Time as a Niche difference: The role of interference competition. Oikos, 42, 403–406. [Google Scholar]
- Chauvenet, A. L. M. , Durant, S. M. , Hilborn, R. , & Pettorelli, N. (2011). Unintended consequences of conservation actions: Managing disease in complex ecosystems. PLoS ONE, 6: e28671. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0028671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, S. M. (1991). Optimal hunting group size: The need for lions to defend their kills against loss to spotted hyaenas. African Journal of Ecology, 29, 130–136. [Google Scholar]
- Courbin, N. , Loveridge, A. J. , Macdonald, D. W. , Fritz, H. , Valeix, M. , Makuwe, E. T. , & Chamaillé‐Jammes, S. (2015). Reactive responses of zebras to lion encounters shape their predator–prey space game at large scale. Oikos, 125, 829–838. [Google Scholar]
- Creel, S. , & Creel, N. M. (1996). Limitation of African wild dogs by competition with larger carnivores. Conservation Biology, 10, 526–538. [Google Scholar]
- Cusack, J. J. , Swanson, A. , Coulson, T. , Packer, C. , Carbone, C. , Dickman, A. J. , … Rowcliffe, J. M. (2015). Applying a random encounter model to estimate lion density from camera traps in Serengeti National Park, Tanzania. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 79, 1014–1021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donadio, E. , & Buskirk, S. W. (2006). Diet, morphology, and interspecific killing in Carnivora. The American Naturalist, 167, 524–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durant, S. M. (1998). Competition refuges and coexistence: An example from Serengeti carnivores . Journal of Animal Ecology, 67, 370–386. [Google Scholar]
- Durant, S. M. (2000). Living with the enemy: Avoidance of hyenas and lions by cheetahs in the Serengeti. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 624–632. [Google Scholar]
- Elmhagen, B. , Ludwig, G. , Rushton, S. P. , Helle, P. , & Lindén, H. (2010). Top predators, mesopredators and their prey: Interference ecosystems along bioclimatic productivity gradients. Journal of Animal Ecology, 79, 785–794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elmhagen, B. , & Rushton, S. P. (2007). Trophic control of mesopredators in terrestrial ecosystems: Top‐down or bottom‐up? Ecology Letters, 10, 197–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estes, J. A. , Terborgh, J. , Brashares, J. S. , Power, M. E. , Berger, J. , Bond, W. J. , … Jackson, J. B. (2011). Trophic downgrading of planet earth. Science, 333, 301–306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fedriani, J. M. , Palomares, F. , & Delibes, M. (1999). Niche relations among three sympatric Mediterranean carnivores. Oecologia, 121, 138–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuller, T. K. , & Keith, L. B. (1981). Non‐overlapping ranges of coyotes and wolves in Northeastern Alberta. Journal of Mammalogy, 62, 403–405. [Google Scholar]
- Harris, M. P. , Anker‐Nilssen, T. , McCleery, R. H. , Erikstad, K. E. , Shaw, D. N. , & Grosbois, V. (2005). Effect of wintering area and climate on the survival of adult Atlantic puffins Fratercula arctica in the eastern Atlantic. Marine Ecology Progress Series, 297, 283–296. [Google Scholar]
- Hayward, M. W. (2006). Prey preferences of the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta) and degree of dietary overlap with the lion (Panthera leo). Journal of Zoology, 270, 606–614. [Google Scholar]
- Hayward, M. W. , & Slotow, R. (2009). Temporal Partitioning of activity in large African carnivores: Tests of multiple hypotheses. South African Journal of Wildlife Research, 39, 109–125. [Google Scholar]
- Holdo, R. M. , Holt, R. D. , & Fryxell, J. M. (2009). Opposing rainfall and plant nutritional gradients best explain the wildebeest migration in the Serengeti. The American Naturalist, 173, 431–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Höner, O. P. , Wachter, B. , East, M. L. , & Hofer, H. (2002). The response of spotted hyaenas to long‐term changes in prey populations: Functional response and interspecific kleptoparasitism. Journal of Animal Ecology, 71, 236–246. [Google Scholar]
- Janssen, A. , Sabelis, M. W. , Magalhães, S. , Montserrat, M. , & Van der Hammen, T. (2007). Habitat structure affects intraguild predation. Ecology, 88, 2713–2719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamler, J. F. , Ballard, W. B. , Gilliland, R. L. II , Lemons, P. R. , & Mote, K. (2003). Impacts of coyotes on swift foxes in Northwestern Texas. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 67, 317–323. [Google Scholar]
- Kauffman, M. J. , Brodie, J. F. , & Jules, E. S. (2010). Are wolves saving Yellowstone's aspen? A landscape‐level test of a behaviorally mediated trophic cascade. Ecology, 91, 2742–2755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kissui, B. M. , & Packer, C. (2004). Top–down population regulation of a top predator: Lions in the Ngorongoro Crater. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, 1867–1874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruuk, H. (1972). The Spotted Hyena: A Study of Predation and Social Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Laundré, J. W. , Hernández, L. , & Altendorf, K. B. (2001). Wolves, elk, and bison: Reestablishing the “landscape of fear” in Yellowstone National Park, USA. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 79, 1401–1409. [Google Scholar]
- Laurenson, M. K. (1994). High juvenile mortality in cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) and its consequences for maternal care. Journal of Zoology, 234, 387–408. [Google Scholar]
- Laurenson, M. K. (1995). Implications of high offspring mortality for cheetah population dynamics In Sinclair A. R. E., & Arcese P. (Eds.), Serengeti II: Dynamics, management, and conservation of an ecosystem (pp. 385–399). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lesmeister, D. B. , Nielsen, C. K. , Schauber, E. M. , & Hellgren, E. C. (2015). Spatial and temporal structure of a mesocarnivore guild in midwestern north America. Wildlife Monographs, 191, 1–61. [Google Scholar]
- Linnell, J. D. C. , & Strand, O. (2000). Interference interactions, co‐existence and conservation of mammalian carnivores. Diversity and Distributions, 6, 169–176. [Google Scholar]
- MacKenzie, D. I. , Nichols, J. D. , Royle, J. A. , Pollock, K. H. , Bailey, L. L. , & Hines, J. E. (2006). Occupancy estimation and modeling: Inferring patterns and dynamics of species. Amsterdam, Boston: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- Mills, M. G. L. , & Mills, M. E. J. (2014). Cheetah cub survival revisited: A re‐evaluation of the role of predation, especially by lions, and implications for conservation. Journal of Zoology, 292, 136–141. [Google Scholar]
- Mukherjee, S. , Zelcer, M. , & Kotler, B. P. (2008). Patch use in time and space for a meso‐predator in a risky world. Oecologia, 159, 661–668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newsome, T. M. , & Ripple, W. J. (2014). A continental scale trophic cascade from wolves through coyotes to foxes. Journal of Animal Ecology, 84, 49–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packer, C. , Hilborn, R. , Mosser, A. , Kissui, B. , Borner, M. , Hopcraft, G. , … Sinclair, A. R. E. (2005). Ecological change, group territoriality, and population dynamics in Serengeti lions. Science, 307, 390–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palomares, F. , & Caro, T. M. (1999). Interspecific killing among mammalian carnivores. The American Naturalist, 153, 492–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Périquet, S. , Fritz, H. , & Revilla, E. (2015). The lion king and the hyaena queen: Large carnivore interactions and coexistence. Biological Reviews, 90, 1197–1214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preisser, E. L. , Bolnick, D. I. , & Benard, M. F. (2005). Scared to death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator–prey interactions. Ecology, 86, 501–509. [Google Scholar]
- Preisser, E. L. , Orrock, J. L. , & Schmitz, O. J. (2007). Predator hunting mode and habitat domain alter nonconsumptive effects in predator–prey interactions. Ecology, 88, 2744–2751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prugh, L. R. , Stoner, C. J. , Epps, C. W. , Bean, W. T. , Ripple, W. J. , Laliberte, A. S. , & Brashares, J. S. (2009). The rise of the Mesopredator. BioScience, 59, 779–791. [Google Scholar]
- Richmond, O. M. W. , Hines, J. E. , & Beissinger, S. R. (2010). Two‐species occupancy models: A new parameterization applied to co‐occurrence of secretive rails. Ecological Applications, 20, 2036–2046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ripple, W. J. , Estes, J. A. , Beschta, R. L. , Wilmers, C. C. , Ritchie, E. G. , Hebblewhite, M. , … Wirsing, A. J. (2014). Status and ecological effects of the world's largest carnivores. Science, 343, 1241484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ritchie, E. G. , & Johnson, C. N. (2009). Predator interactions, mesopredator release and biodiversity conservation. Ecology Letters, 12, 982–998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rostro‐García, S. , Kamler, J. F. , & Hunter, L. T. B. (2015). To kill, stay or flee: The effects of lions and landscape factors on habitat and kill site selection of cheetahs in South Africa. PLoS ONE, 10, e0117743. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0117743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleni, P. (2007). Refuges in time, temporal avoidance of interference competition in endangered wild dogs.pdf.
- Schaller, G. B. (1972). The Serengeti lion: A study of predator–prey relations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz, O. J. , Beckerman, A. P. , & O'Brien, K. M. (1997). Behaviorally mediated trophic cascades: Effects of predation risk on food web interactions. Ecology, 78, 1388–1399. [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz, O. J. , Krivan, V. , & Ovadia, O. (2004). Trophic cascades: The primacy of trait‐mediated indirect interactions. Ecology Letters, 7, 153–163. [Google Scholar]
- Sergio, F. , & Hiraldo, F. (2008). Intraguild predation in raptor assemblages: A review. Ibis, 150, 132–145. [Google Scholar]
- Sexton, J. O. , Song, X.‐P. , Feng, M. , Noojipady, P. , Anand, A. , Huang, C. , … Townshend, J. R. (2013). Global, 30‐m resolution continuous fields of tree cover: Landsat‐based rescaling of MODIS vegetation continuous fields with lidar‐based estimates of error. International Journal of Digital Earth, 6, 427–448. [Google Scholar]
- Sinclair, A. R. E. (1995). Serengeti past and present In Sinclair A. R. E., & Arcese P. (Eds.), Serengeti II: Dynamics, management, and conservation of an ecosystem (pp. 3–30). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sinclair, A. R. E. , & Norton‐Griffiths, M. (1979). Serengeti: Dynamics of an ecosystem. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sollmann, R. , Furtado, M. M. , Hofer, H. , Jácomo, A. T. A. , Tôrres, N. M. , & Silveira, L. (2012). Using occupancy models to investigate space partitioning between two sympatric large predators, the jaguar and puma in central Brazil. Mammalian Biology ‐ Zeitschrift für Säugetierkunde, 77, 41–46. [Google Scholar]
- Suraci, J. P. , Clinchy, M. , Dill, L. M. , Roberts, D. , & Zanette, L. Y. (2016). Fear of large carnivores causes a trophic cascade. Nature Communications, 7, 10698. doi:10.1038/ncomms10698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson, A. B. (2014). Living with Lions: Spatiotemporal aspects of coexistence in Savanna Carnivores. Ph.D., University of Minnesota, United States – Minnesota.
- Swanson, A. , Caro, T. , Davies‐Mostert, H. , Mills, M. G. L. , Macdonald, D. W. , Borner, M. , … Packer, C. (2014). Cheetahs and wild dogs show contrasting patterns of suppression by lions. Journal of Animal Ecology, 83, 1418–1427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson, A. , Kosmala, M. , Lintott, C. , & Packer, C. (2016). A generalized approach for producing, quantifying, and validating citizen science data from wildlife images. Conservation Biology, 30, 520–531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson, A. , Kosmala, M. , Lintott, C. , Simpson, R. , Smith, A. , & Packer, C. (2015). Snapshot Serengeti, high‐frequency annotated camera trap images of 40 mammalian species in an African savanna. Scientific Data, 2, 150026. doi:10.1038/sdata.2015.26 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanak, A. T. , Fortin, D. , Thaker, M. , Ogden, M. , Owen, C. , Greatwood, S. , & Slotow, R. (2013). Moving to stay in place: Behavioral mechanisms for coexistence of African large carnivores. Ecology, 94, 2619–2631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watts, H. E. , & Holekamp, K. E. (2008). Interspecific competition influences reproduction in spotted hyenas. Journal of Zoology, 276, 402–410. [Google Scholar]
- Webster, H. , McNutt, J. W. , & McComb, K. (2012). African wild dogs as a fugitive species: Playback experiments investigate how wild dogs respond to their major competitors. Ethology, 118, 147–156. [Google Scholar]
- Werner, E. E. , & Peacor, S. D. (2003). A review of trait‐mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology, 84, 1083–1100. [Google Scholar]
- Zuur, A. , Ieno, E. N. , Walker, N. , Saveliev, A. A. , & Smith, G. M. (2009). Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data used in this study are available at datadryad.org/resource/doi:10.5061/dryad.5pt92.