Abstract
Accurate discrimination of the intensity and spatial location of nociceptive stimuli is essential to guide appropriate behaviour. The ability to discriminate the attributes of sensory stimuli is continuously refined by practice, even throughout adulthood - a phenomenon called perceptual learning. In the visual domain, perceptual learning to discriminate one of the features that define a visual stimulus (e.g., its orientation) can transfer to a different feature of the same stimulus (e.g., its contrast). Here, we performed two experiments on 48 volunteers to characterize perceptual learning in nociception, which has been rarely studied. We investigated whether learning to discriminate either the intensity or the location of nociceptive stimuli (1) occurs during practice and is subsequently maintained, (2) requires feedback on performance, and (3) transfers to the other, unpractised stimulus feature. First, we found clear evidence that perceptual learning in discriminating both the intensity and the location of nociceptive stimuli occurs, and is maintained for at least 3 hours after practice. Second, learning occurs only when feedback is provided during practice. Finally, learning is largely confined to the feature for which feedback was provided. We discuss these effects in a predictive coding framework, and consider implications for future studies.
Skin nociceptors are activated by intense mechanical, thermal and chemical stimuli, which signal potentially dangerous objects. Accurate discrimination of both the intensity and the spatial location of these stimuli is essential to determine appropriate motor behavior in everyday life.
The ability to discriminate the attributes of sensory stimuli is continuously refined through practice, even throughout adulthood. This phenomenon, often referred to as perceptual learning1, is important to adapt behaviour to the environment. Perceptual learning is typically obtained in an experimental context by providing feedback on discrimination performance, on a trial-by-trial basis1. Several mechanisms underlying perceptual learning have been proposed, from refined encoding of sensory information to improved decision making2,3,4,5. Interestingly, learning is not necessarily confined to a specific stimulus feature: learning to discriminate one of the features that defines a sensory stimulus (e.g., the orientation of a visual stimulus) sometimes transfers, or generalizes, to a different feature of the same stimulus (e.g., the contrast of the same visual stimulus)6,7.
Despite a rich tradition of work in the visual, auditory and tactile domains, perceptual learning in the nociceptive system has been rarely studied. It is therefore unknown whether and how learning refines our ability to discriminate the intensity and location of nociceptive input. Given the behavioural importance of appropriate discrimination of nociceptive stimuli, learning should also occur within the nociceptive system.
In this study, we performed two experiments on 48 healthy volunteers, to characterize perceptual learning in nociception. First, we tested whether learning to discriminate either the intensity or the location of nociceptive stimuli occurs following explicit trial-by-trial feedback on performance (supervised practice), and whether it also emerges through passive exposure to the stimulus feature to be discriminated (unsupervised practice). Second, we tested whether learning to discriminate either the intensity or the spatial location of nociceptive stimuli transfers to the other stimulus feature. The feature-specificity of learning is often interpreted as the hallmark that learning modulated early sensory representations that are specific to the practiced feature3. In contrast, the generalization of learning across different discrimination tasks is generally interpreted as learning modulated processes that are less or not at all feature-specific8.
Both experiments were performed in a blinded fashion: an experimenter unaware of the practice condition assessed discrimination in two sessions: 3 hours before practice (baseline), and 3 hours after practice (+3 h test). Nociceptive stimuli were radiant heat laser pulses (diameter: 6 mm), which selectively activate skin nociceptors9, and allow optimal control of both stimulus intensity and spatial location10,11.
With respect to the occurrence of learning and its dependence on practice effects (i.e. regardless of transfer), in both experiments we compared the effects of verbal feedback versus no feedback on discrimination performance. During supervised practice, participants performed either the intensity discrimination task or the spatial discrimination task while receiving trial-by-trial feedback on performance. Verbal feedback (“correct”, “error”) was only given in the supervised condition, by playing pre-recorded signals. We hypothesized that learning effects would be stronger in the supervised than the unsupervised condition.
With respect to learning transfer, in Experiment 1 we investigated whether learning in the intensity domain transferred to spatial discrimination, and in Experiment 2 we tested whether learning in the spatial domain transferred to intensity discrimination. Given that the transfer of perceptual learning can depend on the perceptual precision of the discrimination task12, we designed intensity and spatial discrimination tasks that required comparable levels of precision.
Results
Learning during practice
In Experiment 1 (n = 24), one group of participants (n = 12) received feedback while practicing intensity discrimination, whereas a second group (n = 12) did not receive any feedback on performance. In Experiment 2 (n = 24), one group of participants (n = 12) was supervised while practicing spatial discrimination, whereas a second group (n = 12) was unsupervised during practice. We calculated the percentage change of discrimination thresholds relative to baseline, both during practice and 3 hours after practice (Figs 1 and 2).
Experiment 1: Effect of intensity discrimination practice
We first tested whether intensity discrimination thresholds improved across the first three blocks of supervised and unsupervised intensity practice. A mixed-effect ANOVA with a within-subject factor ‘practice block’ (3 levels: 1–3) and a between-subject factor ‘practice type’ (2 levels: supervised, unsupervised) provided strong evidence for a difference between practice types (main effect of practice type: F1,21 = 12.404, p = 0.002, η2p = 0.371), no evidence for a difference across practice blocks (main effect of practice block: F2,42 = 1.664, p = 0.202, η2p = 0.073), and moderate evidence for a practice block x practice type interaction (F2,42 = 5.32, p = 0.009, η2p = 0.202). To further explore this interaction we performed three post-hoc tests (Bonferroni-corrected α = 0.017), comparing the change in discrimination performance between the two groups, at each time point. In the first block, there was no evidence for a difference between supervised and unsupervised groups (p = 0.304, d = −0.603). However, in the second and third block, we found strong evidence for better performance in the supervised group relative to the unsupervised group (block 2: p = 0.003, d = −1.523; block 3: p < 0.0001, d = −1.878). To obtain an overall estimate of learning during practice, we averaged discrimination thresholds across all practice blocks and compared them against 0, separately for each practice type, using a one-sample t-test (p-values were estimated using a bootstrap procedure with 1,000 iterations and 95% confidence interval). We found strong evidence of improvement in discrimination performance (i.e., learning) during practice for the group that received feedback (mean ±SE, −29.8% ± 6.89%; p = 0.006, Cohen’s d = −1.207), and moderate evidence of a worsening of discrimination performance in the group that did not receive feedback (+46.04% ± 16.52%; p = 0.026, d = 0.749).
Experiment 2: Effect of spatial discrimination practice
As for Experiment 1, we tested whether spatial discrimination thresholds improved across the first three blocks of supervised and unsupervised practice. A mixed ANOVA with a within-subject factor ‘practice block’ (3 levels: 1–3) and a between-subject factor ‘practice type’ (2 levels: supervised, unsupervised) did not show evidence for main effects of practice blocks (F < 1) and practice type (F1,22 = 3.344, p = 0.081, η2p = 0.132). There was, however, strong evidence for an interaction between practice block and practice type (F2,44 = 5.89, p = 0.005, η2p = 0.211). To further explore this interaction we performed post-hoc tests (Bonferroni-corrected α = 0.017), comparing the change in discrimination performance between the two groups, at each time point. In the first and second blocks, there was no evidence for a difference between supervised and unsupervised groups (block 1: p = 0.219, d = −0.539; block 2: p = 0.698, d = −0.167). However, in the third block, we found strong evidence for better performance in the supervised group relative to the unsupervised group (p = 0.003, d = −1.441). To obtain an overall estimate of learning during practice, we averaged discrimination thresholds across all practice blocks and compared them against 0, separately for the each practice type, using a one-sample t-test. We found weak evidence for learning in the group that received feedback (−18.41% ± 6.68%; p = 0.038, d = −0.750) and no evidence for learning in the group that did not receive feedback (30% ± 22.6%; p = 0.256, d = 0.366).
Altogether, these results indicate that, during practice, nociceptive learning occurs in both intensity and spatial discrimination. However, it only occurs when the practice is supervised.
Learning at +3 h test
Approximately three hours after practice, we tested both intensity and spatial discrimination thresholds. At this test stage, no feedback on performance was given to either group. This test session allowed us to assess whether learning to discriminate the practiced stimulus feature was still present 3 hours after practice, and, if so, whether it transferred to the unpractised task.
Experiment 1: Effect of intensity discrimination practice
We conducted a mixed-effects analysis of variance with a within-subject factor ‘stimulus feature’ (2 levels: practiced, unpractised) and a between-subject factor ‘practice type’ (2 levels: supervised, unsupervised) on the percentage change in intensity and spatial discrimination thresholds relative to baseline. There was no evidence for main effects of ‘stimulus feature’ (F < 1) or ‘practice type’ (F < 1), and weak evidence for an interaction between ‘stimulus feature’ and ‘practice type’ (F1,22 = 4.759, p = 0.040, η2p = 0.178). Post-hoc tests provided moderate evidence for a difference in discriminating the practiced and unpractised stimulus feature in the group that was supervised during practice (p = 0.021, d = −0.867), and, as expected, no evidence for a difference in discriminating the practiced and unpractised stimulus feature in the unsupervised group (p = 0.38, d = 0.254).
To check whether learning was still present three hours after supervised practice, we compared the improvement in discrimination performance against 0, using one-sample t-tests (i.e., no change relative to baseline). In the group that was supervised during practice, we found strong evidence of learning in the practiced intensity discrimination task (p = 0.002, d = −1.358), but not in the unpractised spatial discrimination task (p = 0.387, d = −0.255). In contrast, in the group that was not supervised during practice, we found no evidence of learning in intensity discrimination (p = 0.653, d = −0.133), and weak evidence of learning in spatial discrimination (p = 0.049, d = −0.635).
These findings show that supervised learning to discriminate nociceptive intensity is maintained for at least 3 hours, and does not transfer to the spatial discrimination of nociceptive stimuli.
Experiment 2: Effect of spatial discrimination practice
Similarly to what performed in Experiment 1, we conducted a mixed-effects analysis of variance with a within-subject factor ‘stimulus feature’ and a between-subject factor ‘practice type’ on the percentage change in intensity and spatial discrimination thresholds estimated 3 hours after spatial practice. There was strong evidence for a main effect of practice type (F1,22 = 8.717, p = 0.007, η2p = 0.284), and no evidence for a main effect of stimulus feature (F < 1). In contrast to what observed in Experiment 1, there was no interaction between practice type and stimulus feature (F < 1), possibly due to the higher inter-individual variability of discrimination performance (see Fig. 2B). The overall change in discrimination thresholds (i.e. in both spatial and intensity discrimination) was greater in the group that was supervised during practice, than in the group that was not supervised (difference: −45.31% ± 15.35%).
To test whether learning was still present three hours after practice, we compared the mean percentage change in discrimination thresholds against 0 (no change relative to baseline). In the group that was supervised during practice, we found strong evidence of learning in the practiced spatial discrimination task (p = 0.003, d = −1.02), but inconsistent evidence of an improvement in the unpractised intensity discrimination task (p = 0.084, bootstrap n/a; d = −0.548). In contrast, in the group that was unsupervised during practice, we found no evidence of learning in either intensity or spatial discrimination tasks (p > 0.12, d < 0.05).
These findings indicate that supervised learning to discriminate the spatial location of nociceptive stimuli through supervised feedback is maintained for at least 3 hours, and does not appear to transfer to the unpractised feature at group-level.
Comparison between supervised learning in intensity and spatial discrimination
We also tested whether supervised practice in spatial discrimination led to the same degree of learning as supervised practice in intensity discrimination (Fig. 1). We conducted an independent sample t-test to compare the average percentage change in discrimination thresholds estimated both during and after supervised practice in intensity discrimination (Experiment 1) vs. spatial discrimination (Experiment 2). We found no evidence for a difference in the learning achieved by the groups supervised in spatial discrimination and intensity discrimination (learning during practice: p = 0.289, d = 0.483; learning at + 3 h: p = 0.399, d = 0.372). Thus, learning achieved through supervised practice was comparable in the intensity and spatial domains.
Control 1: effect of exposure to the stimuli
To test whether the observed differences in learning between supervised and unsupervised practice conditions were due to different exposure to the stimuli1, we compared the number of trials that were presented, during practice, across the four practice conditions of Experiments 1 and 2 (Fig. 2B). We conducted a one-way ANOVA on the total number of trials used to estimate discrimination thresholds during practice, with a between-subject factor practice type (4 levels: supervised intensity, unsupervised intensity, supervised spatial, unsupervised spatial). This analysis did not reveal any effect of practice type (F < 1), thus ruling out that the observed differences in learning were due to different exposure to the stimuli.
Control 2: effect of baseline discrimination performance
To assess whether the differences in learning to discriminate the intensity and spatial location of the stimulus through supervised versus unsupervised practice were due to a difference in baseline discrimination performance, we pooled baseline discrimination thresholds from Experiments 1 and 2, separately for the supervised and unsupervised features, and performed independent sample t-tests to compare these baseline thresholds across the group supervised in intensity discrimination, and across the group supervised in spatial discrimination, separately for each task. We did not find evidence for a difference of baseline discrimination thresholds across supervised groups, in either the intensity discrimination task (p = 0.42, bootstrap n/a; d = 0.351) or the spatial discrimination task (p = 0.51, bootstrap n/a; d = 0.286; Fig. 2A). Similarly, we found no evidence for a difference of baseline discrimination thresholds across unsupervised groups, in either task (intensity: p = 0.116, bootstrap n/a; d = 0.697; space: p = 0.853, bootstrap n/a; d = 0.08).
Control 3: effect of task precision
Given that the transfer of perceptual learning can depend on the precision of the task12, we designed intensity and spatial discrimination tasks that required comparable levels of precision. We quantified the precision of the task as the number of trials required to estimate the discrimination threshold with the method of limits, at baseline. We therefore pooled the average number of these trials across practice types, for each participant. Similarly, we pooled the average number of trials used to estimate spatial discrimination thresholds, at baseline, across practice types. The resulting values are shown in Fig. 3A. The average number of trials used to estimate intensity and spatial discrimination thresholds were comparable (p = 0.987, d = 0.004), suggesting that the precision of intensity and spatial discrimination tasks was balanced.
Control 4: effect of learning on detection thresholds
Finally, we checked whether detection thresholds of pinprick pain were different at the +3 h testing session, with respect to baseline. We compared the percentage change of pinprick detection threshold at +3 h against 0 (i.e., against no change between baseline and +3 h), separately for each practice type and experiment. There was no evidence for changes in detection thresholds at +3 h in both practice types (Experiment 1, supervised intensity: p = 0.955, d = −0.017; Experiment 1, unsupervised intensity: p = 0.193, d = 0.416; Experiment 2, supervised spatial location: p = 0.932, d = 0.025; Experiment 2, unsupervised spatial location: p = 0.465, d = −0.229).
Discussion
This study provides the first comprehensive investigation into nociceptive perceptual learning in humans. We obtained three main results. First, the discrimination of the intensity and spatial location of a nociceptive stimulus is improved through perceptual learning. This improvement is maintained for at least 3 h after practice. Second, learning occurs only when feedback is provided during practice. Finally, nociceptive learning in the intensity and spatial domains is largely limited to the practiced feature.
Learning to discriminate nociceptive stimuli depends on feedback during practice
Although participants were exposed to a comparable number of trials in supervised and unsupervised practice conditions (Fig. 3), nociceptive learning occurred only through supervised practice, both in the intensity and spatial domains (Fig. 1). These results highlight the importance of feedback in learning to discriminate features of sensory stimuli13. A few studies have reported that visual learning in a spatial discrimination task can occur in absence of feedback, but of a much smaller degree than in presence of feedback14. Importantly, several other studies did not demonstrate consistent visual learning without feedback, even when using the same paradigm8,15. We cannot exclude that learning could emerge from prolonged unsupervised practice over multiple testing sessions, but we note that we did not observe any evidence of learning in a single unsupervised practice session. Instead of learning, we found moderate evidence of a worsening of discrimination thresholds during unsupervised practice in intensity discrimination, perhaps due to reduced attention in absence of feedback or due to the unpleasant nature of the stimulation.
Nociceptive learning is largely confined to the practiced stimulus feature
Perceptual learning optimizes processes that are exploited by the practiced discrimination task3,8,16,17,18. Studies on perceptual learning have shown that transfer of learning is possible, but it is more the exception rather than the rule3: the transfer of learning depends on many factors, such as the precision of the tasks12, the amount of practice19, and the architecture of the neural systems activated by the tasks2,8. In our experiments, we balanced task precision and amount of practice, and observed that learning to discriminate the intensity and spatial location of nociceptive input is largely confined to the trained feature: it follows that learning is likely to have modulated processes specific to the practiced discrimination tasks.
The information carried by the nociceptive afferent volley is encoded and processed at multiple levels, which include the dorsal horn, the thalamus, the primary and secondary somatosensory cortices (SI and SII) and the dorso-posterior insula20,21,22. All these regions contain somatotopic representations of nociceptive input to the skin23,24,25,26,27,28. However, SI is the only known region containing a fine-grained spatial representation of nociceptive input23, which could support the ability to discriminate small changes in the spatial location of nociceptive stimuli29. The sensitivity of SI neurons (wide dynamic range neurons) also improves to a point that allows discriminating temperature differences as small as 0.2 °C, in awake monkeys trained to discriminate the intensity of nociceptive stimuli30. Thus, it is reasonable to suggest that nociceptive coding in SI is refined through supervised practice in intensity and spatial discrimination. However, it is also possible that representations of nociceptive input in other areas, such as the insula and secondary somatosensory cortices, are also sharpened by supervised discrimination practice.
Functional neuroimaging studies in humans have shown that other higher-order regions, such as the prefrontal, cingulate and parietal areas, are also active during the discrimination of the intensity and spatial location of nociceptive input31,32,33,34. Given that learning can occur at multiple processing levels and modulate the inter-regional connectivity, all these areas are potential candidates for experience-dependent plasticity that mediates improvements in perceptual discrimination. We hope that future studies will shed light on the neural correlates of nociceptive learning, which remain largely unknown.
Effect of exposure to untrained features on learning transfer
Several studies of perceptual learning in the visual domain have reported that exposure to a task-irrelevant sensory feature, during supervised practice on a task-relevant feature, can cause learning to transfer to the task-irrelevant feature. However, learning rarely occurs during unsupervised practice13,35.
We aimed to minimise the possibility of transfer due to changes in the task-irrelevant feature by selectively manipulating only one sensory feature during the practiced blocks. Indeed, in the intensity discrimination task there was no change in stimulus location: the two successive stimuli in each trial were delivered to the same skin location. Similarly, in the spatial discrimination task, the two stimuli had different locations, but the same intensity. Although we did not observe any consistent transfer of supervised learning at the group level, the inter-individual variability of performance seemed higher in the unpractised (transfer) tasks than in the practiced tasks (Fig. 2B). Our design and sample size do not allow us quantitatively testing inter-individual differences. However, in the interest of future studies, we note that some participants improved in the unpractised task after supervised practice in intensity and spatial discrimination (Fig. 2B).
It is possible that the variability of performance in the unpractised task is due to exposure to small unavoidable changes in the untrained stimulus feature. Indeed, task-irrelevant visual learning occurs only when the discriminability of the task-irrelevant feature is just below discrimination threshold, and only during supervised practice13,35. In the spatial discrimination task, testing two different skin locations inevitably entailed stimulating two different sets of nociceptive afferents, thus introducing an unavoidable variation in the magnitude of the afferent input. Such unintended exposure to changes in task-irrelevant features might account for the inter-individual variability of learning transfer, particularly from spatial discrimination to intensity discrimination (Fig. 2B).
Future directions: Interpreting learning in a predictive coding framework
Learning has been interpreted in different computational frameworks, with different emphases on early versus late loci of learning. The predictive coding framework may account for learning at any processing stage, as well as transfer learning. This framework postulates that the brain actively constructs perceptual experiences based on the comparison between predictions of sensory input and the actual sensory input. The idea that the brain tries to infer the causes of its sensations through generative models dates back to the Empiricist tradition, and was first articulated formally by Helmholtz36. Hierarchical representations of the hidden or latent causes of sensory input provide descending predictions of the incoming afferent information. These comparisons result in mismatch (or prediction error) signals, which are used to update the representations generating predictions37,38,39,40,41.
In the predictive coding framework, learning is equivalent to Hebbian plasticity, which minimises prediction error. This applies to perceptual inference or discrimination. Large prediction errors result in greater adjustments to the parameters of models generating sensory predictions. This enables subsequent predictions to minimize prediction errors more efficiently37,42. When prediction errors are minimal, learning is achieved. Perceptual learning is likely to involve fine-grained representations of the intensity and spatial location of nociceptive stimuli. In this framework, learning can be considered an adjustment of between-areas connectivity and lateral interactions to ensure a more precise representation of sensory input (c.f., representational sharpening) and thereby of the generated sensory impressions. It is likely that the feedback provided during training enhances the effects of prediction errors pertaining to that feature through a mechanism of attentional gain43. This hypothesis could be tested by coupling the paradigm described in this study with dynamic causal modelling44 of the sources of the electromagnetic responses elicited by nociceptive stimuli during learning.
Methods
Participants
Forty-eight healthy volunteers participated in the study (Experiment 1: n = 24, mean age ±SD: 23.08 ± 3.41 years old, 6 males; Experiment 2: n = 24, mean age ±SD: 22.71 ± 3.32 years old, 8 males). All participants gave written informed consent and received payment for their participation. The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee and was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Stimuli
Nociceptive stimuli were radiant heat pulses that selectively activate Aδ and C skin nociceptors, without co-activating Aβ mechanoreceptors45,46. Radiant heat stimuli were generated using a CO2 laser (10.6 nm, beam diameter 6 mm), whose power is continuously regulated using a feedback based on an online measurement of skin temperature at the site of stimulation (LSD, SIFEC, Belgium)47. This device allows delivering two consecutive stimuli to the same skin location. This characteristic makes it optimal for testing the intensity discrimination: delivering two consecutive stimuli at the same location is important to avoid unwanted exposure to changes in stimulus location during practice in intensity discrimination. Indeed, exposure to changes in a feature other than the feature for which feedback is provided can result in implicit learning13. The laser spot size was 6 mm in all tasks, and the stimuli were delivered to a defined 8 × 4 cm skin region in the low back. The decision to stimulate the low back was driven by the observations from a series of pilot experiments, aimed to find a body region where both intensity and spatial discrimination were similarly poor, thus allowing to observe an improvement consequent to practice.
Procedure
In each experiment, we manipulated the type of practice, in two separate groups of randomly-assigned participants. In Experiment 1, one group received feedback while practicing intensity discrimination (supervised intensity practice), whereas a second group did not receive any feedback on performance (unsupervised intensity practice). In Experiment 2, one group received feedback while practicing spatial discrimination (supervised spatial practice), whereas a second group did not receive any feedback on performance (unsupervised spatial practice).
Each experiment was comprised of three sessions: one baseline session, one practice session (3 hours after baseline), and one testing session (3 hours after practice). In all groups, practice sessions lasted approximately 50 min. During supervised practice, participants performed either the intensity or the spatial discrimination task while receiving trial-by-trial feedback on performance (“correct”, “error”) by playing a pre-recoded robotic voice. Given that thresholds were estimated with the method of limits (see below), neither the number of trials within each practice block nor the number of practice blocks was fixed a priori.
A second experimenter blinded to the practice condition assessed the discrimination abilities of all participants at baseline and 3 hours after practice (+3 h test), each session lasting for approximately 1 hour. In each session, the blinded experimenter (K.D.) first tested detection thresholds for pinprick sensations, to ensure that thermal sensitivity was stable over the course of the day. Then, the blinded experimenter estimated two intensity discrimination thresholds, and two spatial discrimination thresholds, using the method of limits10,48. The order of presentation of intensity and spatial discrimination tasks was randomized across participants. During all sessions, subjects were blindfolded, and lay face down on a comfortable mattress.
To estimate the detectionthresholds for pinprick pain, we used the method of ascending staircases. In all experiments, we delivered slow-rising CO2 laser stimuli of increasing temperature (1 °C/sec). Participants were asked to press a button as soon as they felt a pinprick sensation, reflecting the activation of Aδ afferents49. The temperature at which the button was pressed defined the pinprick detection threshold. Two detection thresholds were measured in each subject, and were subsequently averaged.
To estimate the intensity discrimination thresholds, we delivered two consecutive stimuli with an inter-stimulus interval of 3 s. The two stimuli had different intensities but the same spatial location. The first stimulus was either more or less intense than the second stimulus, with equal probability of occurrence. The intensity of one of the two stimuli was fixed, and clearly detectable (47 °C; stimulus duration: 500 ms). To minimize effects of response bias, half of participants were asked to judge which stimulus in a pair was more intense, while the other half were asked to judge which stimulus was less intense. To avoid nociceptor fatigue, the laser beam was randomly shifted after each trial, within the defined 8 × 4 cm skin region in the low back. Thresholds were estimated using the method of limits. Blocks of ascending and descending staircases were alternated, and the first staircase type was balanced across participants. The initial intensity difference was 0.5 °C in ascending staircases, and 3 °C in descending staircases. The difference between the two stimuli was progressively adjusted, and the smallest stimulus difference was 0.5 °C. Intensity discrimination thresholds were defined as the minimal intensity difference that was correctly discriminated on three consecutive trials.
To estimate the spatial discriminationthresholds, we delivered two consecutive stimuli with an inter-stimulus interval of 3 s. The two stimuli had identical intensities (47 °C), but different spatial locations. The first stimulus was either proximal or distal relative to the second stimulus, with equal probability of occurrence. To minimize effects of response bias, half of the participants of each group were asked to judge which stimulus in the pair was proximal, while the other half were asked to judge which stimulus was distal. To avoid nociceptor fatigue, the laser beam was randomly shifted after each trial, within the defined 8 × 4 cm skin region in the low back. Thresholds were estimated with the method of limits, following the same procedure described above and used in previous studies10. In ascending staircases, the initial distance between the two stimuli was 0.7 cm. In descending staircases, the initial distance was 5.6 cm. The distance between the two stimuli was progressively adjusted. The smallest distance between the two stimuli was 0.7 cm. Spatial discrimination thresholds were defined as the minimal distance at which the relative location of the two stimuli was correctly discriminated on three consecutive trials.
Analyses
All t-tests were two-tailed, and p-values were estimated using a bootstrap procedure with 1,000 samples and 95% CI level. When the boostrapping procedure failed to estimate non significant p-values, this was indicated in the text as “bootstrap n/a”. We took into account the false discovery rate when interpreting p-values50.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Mancini, F. et al. Perceptual learning to discriminate the intensity and spatial location of nociceptive stimuli. Sci. Rep. 6, 39104; doi: 10.1038/srep39104 (2016).
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Acknowledgments
FM and GDI were supported by a Wellcome Trust strategic award (COLL JLARAXR). FM was additionally supported by an EFIC-Grunenthal grant, and GDI by the ERC Consolidator grant PAINSTRAT. We are grateful to Massieh Moayedi, Lewis Griffin, and Peter Neri for helpful discussions about this study.
Footnotes
Author Contributions F.M. and G.D.I. designed the study. F.M., K.D., J.S. conducted the experiments and analysed the data. F.M., K.F., P.H. and G.D.I. wrote the article.
References
- Gibson E. J. Improvement in perceptual judgments as a function of controlled practice or training. Psychol Bull 50, 401–431 (1953). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dosher B. A., Jeter P., Liu J. & Lu Z. L. An integrated reweighting theory of perceptual learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110, 13678–13683 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert C. D. & Li W. Adult visual cortical plasticity. Neuron 75, 250–264 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang R. et al. Perceptual Learning at a Conceptual Level. J Neurosci 36, 2238–2246 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuai S. G., Levi D. & Kourtzi Z. Learning optimizes decision templates in the human visual cortex. Curr Biol 23, 1799–1804 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hochstein S. & Ahissar M. View from the top: hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron 36, 791–804 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao L. Q. et al. Complete transfer of perceptual learning across retinal locations enabled by double training. Curr Biol 18, 1922–1926 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang D. H., Kourtzi Z. & Welchman A. E. Mechanisms for extracting a signal from noise as revealed through the specificity and generality of task training. J Neurosci 33, 10962–10971 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bromm B., Jahnke M. T. & Treede R. D. Responses of human cutaneous afferents to CO2 laser stimuli causing pain. Exp Brain Res 55, 158–166 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mancini F. et al. Whole-body mapping of spatial acuity for pain and touch. Ann Neurol 75, 917–924 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iannetti G. D. et al. Adelta nociceptor response to laser stimuli: selective effect of stimulus duration on skin temperature, brain potentials and pain perception. Clin Neurophysiol 115, 2629–2637 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeter P. E., Dosher B. A., Petrov A. & Lu Z. L. Task precision at transfer determines specificity of perceptual learning. J Vis 9, 11–13 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasaki Y., Nanez J. E. & Watanabe T. Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 53–60 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahle M. & Edelman S. Long-term learning in vernier acuity: effects of stimulus orientation, range and of feedback. Vision Res 33, 397–412 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herzog M. H. & Fahle M. The role of feedback in learning a vernier discrimination task. Vision Res 37, 2133–2141 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike E. L. & Woodworth R. S. The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. III. Functions involving attention, observation and discrimination. Psychological Review 8, 553–564 (1901). [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike E. L. & Woodworth R. S. The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. II. The estimation of magnitudes. Psychological Review 8, 384–395 (1901). [Google Scholar]
- Thorndike E. L. & Woodworth R. S. The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. (I). Psychological Review 8, 247–261 (1901). [Google Scholar]
- Jeter P. E., Dosher B. A., Liu S. H. & Lu Z. L. Specificity of perceptual learning increases with increased training. Vision Res 50, 1928–1940 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y. et al. Ensemble encoding of nociceptive stimulus intensity in the rat medial and lateral pain systems. Mol Pain 7, 64 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apkarian A. V. & Shi T. Squirrel monkey lateral thalamus. I. Somatic nociresponsive neurons and their relation to spinothalamic terminals. J Neurosci 14, 6779–6795 (1994). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenshalo D. R., Iwata K., Sholas M. & Thomas D. A. Response properties and organization of nociceptive neurons in area 1 of monkey primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 84, 719–729 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mancini F., Haggard P., Iannetti G. D., Longo M. R. & Sereno M. I. Fine-grained nociceptive maps in primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci 32, 17155–17162 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bingel U. et al. Somatotopic organization of human somatosensory cortices for pain: a single trial fMRI study. Neuroimage 23, 224–232 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumgartner U. et al. Multiple somatotopic representations of heat and mechanical pain in the operculo-insular cortex: a high-resolution fMRI study. J Neurophysiol 104, 2863–2872 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzola L., Isnard J., Peyron R., Guenot M. & Mauguiere F. Somatotopic organization of pain responses to direct electrical stimulation of the human insular cortex. PAIN 146, 99–104 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks J. C., Zambreanu L., Godinez A., Craig A. D. & Tracey I. Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging. Neuroimage 27, 201–209 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson J. L. et al. Somatotopic organization along the central sulcus, for pain localization in humans, as revealed by positron emission tomography. Exp Brain Res 117, 192–199 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mancini F. et al. A fovea for pain at the fingertips. Curr Biol 23, 496–500 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenshalo D. R. Jr., Chudler E. H., Anton F. & Dubner & SI nociceptive neurons participate in the encoding process by which monkeys perceive the intensity of noxious thermal stimulation. Brain Res 454, 378–382 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oshiro Y., Quevedo A. S., McHaffie J. G., Kraft R. A. & Coghill R. C. Brain mechanisms supporting spatial discrimination of pain. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 27, 3388–3394 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kulkarni B. et al. Attention to pain localization and unpleasantness discriminates the functions of the medial and lateral pain systems. Eur J Neurosci 21, 3133–3142 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobanov O. V., Quevedo A. S., Hadsel M. S., Kraft R. A. & Coghill R. C. Frontoparietal mechanisms supporting attention to location and intensity of painful stimuli. PAIN 154, 1758–1768 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oshiro Y., Quevedo A. S., McHaffie J. G., Kraft R. A. & Coghill R. C. Brain Mechanisms Supporting Discrimination of Sensory Features of Pain: A New Model. The Journal of Neuroscience 29, 14924–14931 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe T. & Sasaki Y. Perceptual learning: toward a comprehensive theory. Annu Rev Psychol 66, 197–221 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helmholtz H. F. Treatise on Physiological Optics. Vol. III (Dover, 1866). [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 127–138 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao R. P. & Ballard D. H. Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nat Neurosci 2, 79–87 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mumford D. On the computational architecture of the neocortex. II. The role of cortico-cortical loops. Biol Cybern 66, 241–251 (1992). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P., Hinton G. E., Neal R. M. & Zemel R. S. The Helmholtz machine. Neural Comput 7, 889–904 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory R. L. Perceptual illusions and brain models. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 171, 279–296 (1968). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K. Learning and inference in the brain. Neural Netw 16, 1325–1352 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanai R., Komura Y., Shipp S. & Friston K. Cerebral hierarchies: predictive processing, precision and the pulvinar. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 370 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiebel S. J., Garrido M. I., Moran R. J. & Friston K. J. Dynamic causal modelling for EEG and MEG. Cognitive neurodynamics 2, 121–136 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bromm B. & Treede R. D. Nerve fibre discharges, cerebral potentials and sensations induced by CO2 laser stimulation. Hum Neurobiol 3, 33–40 (1984). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumgartner U., Cruccu G., Iannetti G. D. & Treede R. D. Laser guns and hot plates. PAIN 116, 1–3 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Churyukanov M., Plaghki L., Legrain V. & Mouraux A. Thermal detection thresholds of Adelta- and C-fibre afferents activated by brief CO2 laser pulses applied onto the human hairy skin. PLoS One 7, e35817 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornsweet T. N. The staircrase-method in psychophysics. Am J Psychol 75, 485–491 (1962). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beissner F. et al. Quick discrimination of A(delta) and C fiber mediated pain based on three verbal descriptors. PLoS One 5, e12944 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colquhoun D. An investigation of the false discovery rate and the misinterpretation of p-values. R Soc Open Sci 1, 140216 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]