Abstract
This article reviews research on the development of motor behavior from a developmental systems perspective. We focus on infancy when basic action systems are acquired. Posture provides a stable base for locomotion, manual actions, and facial actions. Experience facilitates improvements in motor behavior and infants accumulate immense amounts of experience with all of their basic action systems. At every point in development, perception guides motor behavior by providing feedback about the results of just prior movements and information about what to do next. Reciprocally, the development of motor behavior provides fodder for perception. More generally, motor development brings about new opportunities for acquiring knowledge about the world, and burgeoning motor skills can instigate cascades of developmental changes in perceptual, cognitive, and social domains.
Keywords: Motor Action, Exploration, Posture, Locomotion, Reaching, Infants, Fetal
Introduction
Motor behavior includes every kind of movement from involuntary twitches to goal-directed actions, in every part of the body from head to toe, in every physical and social context from solitary play to group interactions. The development of motor behavior bridges the entire lifespan from the first fetal movement to the last dying breath.
Although movements fundamentally depend on generating, controlling, and exploiting physical forces, managing forces requires more than muscles and biomechanics. At every point in development, adaptive control of movement relies on core psychological functions1, 2. Perception and cognition are required to plan and guide actions3. Social and cultural factors spur and constrain motor behaviors4. Motor behaviors, in turn, provide the raw material for perception, cognition, and social interaction5, 6. Movements generate perceptual information, provide the means for acquiring knowledge about the world, and make social interactions possible.
According to a developmental systems view, motor behaviors cannot be understood in isolation, divorced from the bodily, environmental, and social/cultural context in which they occur7. Movements are inextricably nested in a body-environment system. The body and the environment develop in tandem. New or improved motor skills bring new parts of the environment into play and thereby provide new or enhanced opportunities for learning and doing. Caregiving practices facilitate and constrain motor development. As a consequence, differences in the way caregivers structure the environment and interact with their children affect the form of new skills, the ages when they first appear, and the shape of their developmental trajectory.
New motor behaviors can emerge from a mix of interacting factors, some so pervasive that we mistakenly take them for granted, and some so subtle or non-obvious that we fail to recognize the link. Developmental changes in one domain can have cascading effects on development in other domains, sometimes far afield from the original accomplishment8, 9. Moreover, the context in which behavior develops can be very different for individual children, resulting in developmental pathways that sometimes converge at the same outcome and sometimes veer off in unique directions.
This article is organized around four basic action systems—posture, locomotion, manual actions, and movements in the face and head. We focus primarily on the infancy period, when basic action systems are acquired.
Posture
Posture is the most fundamental of motor actions. It is the foundation upon which other actions are built10. The instant that any part of the body breaks from the support surface—merely raising an arm while supine or lifting the head while prone—torque acting on the body part creates disequilibrium. This is why novice sitting and standing infants lose balance just from turning their heads or lifting their arms. Posture must be sufficiently stable to allow movements of the extremities, and maintaining a stable posture sets up the necessary conditions for looking around, handling objects, holding conversations, or going somewhere. As such, the emergence of most skills—including those not obviously related to posture—must await the development of sufficient postural control. Like every action, posture is perceptually guided and maintained.
Overcoming Gravity
Gravity and the surrounding media (e.g., air, water, the ground beneath the feet) are so quietly pervasive, so hidden in plain sight, that these important factors are often overlooked as causal forces in development. But they are central for motor development. Before birth, the buoyant uterine environment supports a variety of postures. Large body movements—whole body flexion and extension, stretching and writhing, and vigorous leg kicks that somersault the fetus through the amniotic fluid—peak at 14 to 16 weeks gestation11, 12. As the growing fetus occupies increasingly more space in the uterus, the propensity for movement is masked until the fetus can no longer extend its limbs or turn its head. Many of the movements practiced by the fetus are present in the repertoire of the neonate12, but after birth begins the real struggle against gravity.
Postural development is the attainment of increasingly erect postures poised over an increasingly small base of support. Think of a newborn struggling to lift its head, a toddler’s wide walking stance, and an older child dancing on pointe. Indeed, the most common images of motor development are milestone charts of postural development (Figure 1). The milestone charts suggest an orderly, age-related march through a series of stages, but developmental pathways can differ and individual infants do not strictly adhere to the normative sequence derived from average onset ages. Infants can acquire skills in various orders, skip stages, and revert to earlier forms13–15. Moreover, the skills highlighted on the milestone charts reflect the cultural biases of the initial researchers and samples4. In some cultures, for example, many infants do not crawl, or they do so after they learn to walk16.
Figure 1.
Typical example of milestone chart illustrating age-related changes in postural development. Adapted from4.
Generally speaking, infants’ gradual triumph over gravity precedes top down from head to feet. The top down progression is especially striking in the development of sitting. At first, head and trunk control is so poor that unsupported infants fold in half, falling chest to legs. Increasing control moves slowly down the spine—neck, shoulders, waist, hips17, 18. Infants eventually “tripod sit” by stabilizing their torso with arms propped between their outstretched legs. Finally, around 6 months of age, infants sit independently with hands freed from a supporting role19, and over the ensuing weeks gain sufficient stability to manage the destabilizing forces caused by turning the head, twisting the torso, and moving the arms. The skill progression is not locked to a strict maturational timetable. Differences in childrearing practices affect the timing and trajectory of sitting4, 20. In cultures where caregivers routinely exercise and massage their infants, the babies sit independently before 5 months of age, and they do so with such assured stability that their mothers regularly perch them on high furniture and leave the room to do chores.
Like sitting, standing typically begins with manual support of balance. Infants pull to stand and hold themselves upright by gripping furniture for support15. Toward the end of the first year, they stand freely and cruise holding furniture for support19. Locomotion in prone, sitting, crawling, and upright postures appears only after infants can keep balance in one place, and transitions between postures (shifting from prone to sitting, sitting to standing, and so on) typically emerge last.
Basis for Action
A stable postural base opens up new possibilities for acquiring knowledge and acting on the world. The ability to maintain head position while held in caregivers’ arms allows infants to look around21 and maintain gaze with caregivers22. The ability to sit and stand upright provides new vantage points for visual exploration23. Stability in a sitting posture frees the arms for reaching and the hands for manual exploration17, 24–26.
Indeed, reaching and manual exploration have different developmental trajectories for prone, supine, and sitting postures25, 27. While prone, bimanual exploration is difficult because one arm is occupied in holding the chest off the floor. While supine, infants struggle to raise objects against gravity and have difficulty exploring them visually. But while sitting, more sophisticated bimanual object exploration is possible because head, arms, and hands are more free to move.
Developmental changes in postural control instigate a cascade of far-flung changes: Independent sitting facilitates more sophisticated bimanual object exploration such as fingering, transferring, and rotating, which in turn facilitate learning about the three-dimensionality of objects28. Improvements in manual skills are also linked with shifts in infants’ attention to changes in object appearance29, object size30, multimodal information about objects31, and other people’s intentions to grasp objects32, 33. The path from posture to prehension to perceptual learning is not immediately obvious, but it is there nonetheless.
Dynamic Postural Control
Movement is ubiquitous in every posture. Even while lying down, the body is in motion. Similarly, sitting, crawling, and standing postures may appear stationary to casual observation, but they are not. Rather, the body gently sways back and forth within the base of support18, 34. A torque-induced sway in one direction must be met by a muscle-induced compensatory sway in the opposite direction. Standing infants are sensitive to perceptual information for body sway and can control swaying movements with merely a light touch of the hand on a support surface35, 36.
Visual information for body sway is extremely powerful. Slight movement of the walls around a sitting or standing infant in a “moving room” creates the illusion of postural sway (Figure 2), and infants compensate for the visual information for body position by leaning in the opposite direction37–39. However, infants do not use visual information for postural control as efficiently as older children and adults. Whereas adults rock gently back and forth like puppets in tune with the wall oscillations, infants’ compensatory sways are excessive and they often stagger and fall.
Figure 2.
Toddler losing balance in a “moving room.” Child stands on a solid floor surrounded by walls that move back and forth along a track. Here, the walls move toward the child creating the visual illusion of the body swaying forward; the child compensates by swaying backward. Adapted from39.
Summary: Posture
Posture is the core ingredient of motor skill. With no postural control, most motor behaviors are impossible. The development of postural control instigates a cascade of new skills and opens up new possibilities for looking, social interactions, manual actions, and locomotion. Postural development is partly a perceptual accomplishment because even while sitting and standing, the body is always slightly swaying and perception plays a key role in keeping the body inside the base of support. Postural control emerges from the interaction of a growing body dealing with the constraints of the physical environment—gravity, air, the properties of the support surface, and so on. Caregiving practices can speed up or delay postural control and the cascade of new skills that follow.
Locomotion
Precursory locomotor movements are exhibited during fetal and neonatal periods, but locomotion is not reflexive or hardwired. Rather, locomotion is creative and infants must learn to control locomotion adaptively. Locomotion improves with practice, and practice can lead to extraordinary performance4, 7, 40.
Newborn Reflexes
When newborns are held upright with their feet on a hard surface, they move their legs in an alternating pattern that resembles walking. This phenomenon is called the “newborn stepping reflex” because the movements appear to be elicited by contact with the ground surface and do not require cortical control41. Stepping typically disappears by 2 months of age and reappears at 8–10 months when infants begin walking with support. The fact that newborns produce alternating, upright leg movements led researchers to believe that walking is hardwired in the nervous system42–44. Similarly, the curious disappearance and reappearance of stepping was attributed to a hardwired developmental mechanism: Cortical maturation inhibits the reflex and increased myelination of the corticospinal tract allows stepping to return under volitional control.
However, the so-called stepping reflex is not, in fact, reflexive, and alternating leg movements do not, in fact, disappear. Newborns “air-step” without an eliciting physical stimulus and they step in response to optic flow45, 46. Infants can deliberately modify their leg movements47 in various configurations of alternating, simultaneous, and single-leg kicks48, 49. They spontaneously kick their legs while supine50 and supine leg kicks are kinematically equivalent to upright steps, and are produced by the same muscle activations50. Moreover, supine kicking continues unabated throughout the period when upright stepping disappears51, and upright steps instantly reappear when infants are held on a motorized treadmill52, 53 or when their legs are submerged in a tank of water54. With daily practice in an upright posture, the stepping movements never disappear55, 56. Changes in the body, not the brain, explain the U-shaped trajectory of upright stepping: Between 2–8 months of age, gains in leg fat typically outstrip gains in muscle54. In an upright position, infants cannot lift their chubby legs against gravity, but in a supine position gravity helps to flex the legs; on a treadmill, the moving belt does the work of pulling infants’ legs backward and in a tank of water, the medium alleviates the effects of gravity. Upright practice makes leg muscles stronger.
Creative Solutions
Individual infants find different ways to solve the problem of moving. Their first success at mobility likely involves a prone position with minimal balance constraints. They may log roll from place to place or pivot in circles using auditory information to calculate the shortest rotational distance to their caregivers57. As shown in Figure 3, some infants belly crawl, using limbs, head, and belly in various combinations for support and propulsion58, 59. The belly rests continually on the floor or bumps up and down during each cycle. Every form of precursory prone movement helps: Infants who pivot, belly crawl, and so on are twice as proficient when they begin crawling on hands and knees compared with infants who don't display the earlier forms58. In fact, simply spending a few minutes a day in a prone position accelerates the onset of rolling and crawling60.
Figure 3.
Variations in infants’ crawling patterns. Left column shows four different crawling styles: “army” crawling with the abdomen continually resting on the ground, “inchworm” crawling with the belly on and off the ground during each cycle, standard hands-and-knees crawling, and hands-and-feet “bear” crawling. Center column represents combinations of arms and legs used to propel the body. Right column shows combinations of belly, hands, knees, and feet used to maintain balance. Each row shows a unique crawling pattern. Adapted from58.
On hands and knees, balance constraints increase because the belly is off the floor. As a consequence, most infants quickly settle into a relatively stable, near-trot gait pattern58, 61. But they also crawl on hands and feet, and combine hands, knees, feet, and buttocks into various forms of hitching and bum-shuffling positions that blur the line between sitting and crawling40. Balance constraints are more severe while upright, but learning to walk is likewise an exercise in creative problem solving with various falling, twisting, and stepping strategies for inducing enough disequilbrium to take steps but not so much loss of stability to cause a fall43, 62, 63.
Generating new forms of locomotion can involve cognitive skills such as means-ends problem solving, representing goals and spatial locations, and tool use. As illustrated in Figure 4, when confronted with challenging obstacles such as steep slopes, cliffs, and stairs, infants search for alternative means of descent—scooting, crawling, sliding, and backing strategies64–66. Backing is most difficult because it requires infants to initially turn away from the goal, coordinate backward movements, and steer without visual guidance. On narrow bridges, infants use a sturdy wooden handrail as a tool to augment their balance, but they reject the handrail if it is too far from the bridge67–69. With only a wobbly rubber handrail for support, they test the potential utility of the rail, and invent various strategies for distributing body weight over the bridge and handrail (Figure 4).
Figure 4.
(A) Some of the strategies infants use to descend slopes: Scooting down in a sitting position, crawling on hands and knees, sliding head-first while prone, and turning their bodies to back down feet first. (B) Some of the strategies infants use to cross bridges holding a wobbly handrail for support. Infants employ a “hunchback” strategy by pushing down on the rail to make it taut, walk sideways while leaning backward as if “windsurfing”, walking forward and pulling back on the rail as if “mountain climbing”, and “drunkenly” leaning against the rail as they staggered forward. Adapted from64, 68.
Learning to Walk
Infants take their first walking steps at 12 months, on average19, but like all motor milestones, onset ages have a wide range (8–18 months). Walking onset awaits sufficient strength and balance to support the body on one leg as the other leg swings forward43, 70, 71. Both experimental and cross-cultural studies show that experience standing, stepping, and moving upright facilitates gains in strength and balance and accelerates the onset of walking4, 7, 40. A few minutes of daily practice with upright stepping causes infants to begin walking weeks earlier than infants who receive only passive exercise55, 56. Similarly, in Caribbean and African cultures where parents deliberately exercise their infants’ upright skills as part of daily massage and bathing routines (Figure 5), infants walk sooner than those from the same ethnic backgrounds who do not receive practice16.
Figure 5.
Formal massage and exercise routines used in Africa, India, and the Caribbean that facilitate motor development. (A) Massage; (B–C) Suspending the infant from the arms and feet; (D) Mother providing sitting practice; (E–F) Practicing stepping in an upright posture. Adapted from179, 180.
Infants’ first steps are wobbly and uneven, with a wide side-to-side distance between feet, a small front-to-back distance between steps, long periods when both feet are on the floor, and short periods when one foot is in the air72, 73. But soon the base of support narrows, step length increases, double support periods decrease, and infants are racing across the floor. The steep developmental trajectory for walking resembles the negatively accelerated performance curves characteristic of most motor learning tasks. Initial rapid improvements in the first 3–6 months of walking reflect infants’ discovery of the relevant parameters that control upright balance and propulsion72, 74–76. A protracted tapering-off period ending between 5–7 years of age reflects subtle fine-tuning of gait parameters70, 77. Practice, not merely maturation, underlies improvements72, and infants accumulate immense amount of practice. In one hour of free play, the average toddler takes about 2400 steps, travels the length of about 8 American football fields, and falls 17 times78.
Sufficient practice can lead to improvements in endurance, strength, coordination, and balance far beyond the norm for Western walkers4, 40. Tarahumaran children engage in long-distance running as part of daily activity. As a consequence, endurance running exceeds the abilities of most Western ultra-marathoners: Tarahumaran children routinely run 10–40 km in a few hours and adults race 150–300 km over 24–48 hours79. From childhood, East African women and Nepalese porters of both genders carry prodigious loads on their heads. Adults in these cultures have learned to alter the biomechanics of gait so as to carry loads greater than their body weight with reduced energetic cost80, 81.
Obstacle Navigation
Perception-action coupling makes locomotion functional. To navigate the everyday cluttered environment, children must select the appropriate movements and modify them accordingly, whether crawling, walking, or riding a bicycle82. Children generate the requisite perceptual information through exploratory movements—looking, touching, and testing various options7, 64, 83. The first studies of obstacle navigation tested infants on a “visual cliff,” a drop-off covered in safety glass84. But infants in such studies can feel the glass and, after one trial, they learn that the drop-off is only illusory—and so they cross85. As shown in Figure 6, recent researchers have used real cliffs, bridges, waterbeds, foam pits, water pits, slippery surfaces, barriers, apertures, monkey bars, car-filled streets, and other obstacles to test the development of prospective control of locomotion65, 66, 82, 86–92. Because visual and haptic information are not in conflict on these apparatuses, children can be tested in dozens of trials (an experimenter follows alongside to ensure their safety). Many of the apparatuses are adjustable, allowing precise assessment of children’s ability to gauge possibilities for locomotion.
Figure 6.
(A) “Visual cliff” with safety glass covering an apparent drop-off. (B) Real cliff with adjustable height of drop-off. (C) Sitting at the edge of an adjustable gap. (D) Cruising an adjustable gap in the handrail. (E) Walking across adjustable bridges. (F) Crawling down an adjustable slope. (G) Walking down a slope with a Teflon-coated section. Adapted from180.
Prelocomotor infants are sensitive to visual flow for heading93 and depth information for a drop-off94, but sensitivity is not enough. Mobile infants must learn to navigate. In their first weeks after acquiring a new posture—sitting, crawling, cruising, and walking—infants plunge repeatedly over the edge of impossibly steep slopes, high cliffs, and wide gaps. Over weeks of experience with each posture, judgments improve so that infants attempt safe increments within their ability and avoid risky obstacles beyond their ability7, 40.
A surprising finding is that learning does not transfer from earlier to later developing postures. The same infants who perceive that a large gap precludes scooting or leaning over the edge in an experienced sitting posture will repeatedly attempt to crawl over the gap in a novice crawling posture95. The same infants who perceive that a large cliff or slope is impossible to descend in an experienced crawling posture will repeatedly attempt to walk when tested in a novice walking posture64–66. In a cruising posture, pre-walking infants perceive precisely how large of a gap they can span with their arms, but not with their legs14. Clearly, failure to transfer from earlier to later developing postures is not due to fear of heights because the gaps, slopes, and drop-offs are high above the ground in every posture96. Moreover, infants are not learning fixed facts about the environment or their abilities because possibilities for action change from week to week as locomotor skills improve. Instead, infants are learning to generate and use perceptual information about the current status of their body relative to the environment7, 40. They are learning the relevant parameters for each new posture in development and the relevant exploratory behaviors for calibrating those parameters in a new situation.
Summary: Locomotion
Fetuses and neonates can produce leg and arm movements that grossly resemble locomotion, but locomotion is not hardwired or reflexive. Instead locomotor development is tremendously plastic and responsive to caregiving practices. And locomotion is wildly creative. Every infant discovers a unique solution for their first crawling, walking, bum shuffling, or rolling “steps.” And then they must learn to generate information for perception and cognition to find the right solution to suit the local constraints of the cluttered, obstacle strewn everyday environment.
Manual Action
Manual actions begin prenatally, but outside the womb, infants require a stable postural base to support arm movements and perceptual information to guide movements adaptively. Tools extend children’s manual abilities7, 97, 98.
Spontaneous Motility
Like all actions, manual actions appear long before birth. Ten-week-old fetuses flex and extend their arms, wiggle their fingers, and clench their fists99, 100. By 14 weeks, fetuses manually explore their own bodies, the umbilical cord, and the surface of the uterine wall101. By 16 weeks, fetuses bring hand to mouth to suck their thumbs102. Even these early actions are perceptually guided and planned: Infants open their mouths in anticipation of, not in reaction to, the arrival of their thumb103.
Spontaneous arm and hand movements continue after birth. Throughout the first year, infants flap their arms, rotate their hands, and wiggle their fingers, and exhibit bouts of rhythmical waving, rubbing, and banging while holding objects51, 104, 105. Ironically, such so-called “stereotypies” may not be stereotyped at all. Infants’ first banging movements are highly variable in terms of arm trajectory. Banging becomes increasingly uniform with the arm repeatedly tracing the same upward and downward path98, 106.
Reaching and Grasping
As in locomotion, the contextual influences of infants’ bodies, physical environment, and social/cultural environment affect the development of manual skills. And individual infants forge their own developmental trajectories. Some infants learn to reach in the context of spontaneous arm flaps; they stiffen the arm joints to dampen inertial forces and direct the arm more in the direction of the target107. Other infants learn to reach more conventionally, by powering up their stationary arms in the presence of a target. In both cases, initial attempts are usually unsuccessful.
Goal-directed reaching requires perceptual information about the location of the object vis-a-vis the hand. Given appropriate postural support, neonates and young infants show precursors of visually guided reaching, extending their arms more frequently while looking at a toy108, 109. Successful toy contacts appear between 11 and 24 weeks of age110–112, but reaches are jerky and crooked; the arm speeds up, slows down, and changes direction several times prior to contact. It takes years before children’s reaches become as smooth and straight as those of adults110, 113. Infants “reach” with their feet at a slightly younger age than they reach with their hands (Figure 7), showing that cephalocaudal (head to feet) development is only a rule of thumb, not an obligatory law of development114.
Figure 7.
Three-month-old infant “feet reaching” by contacting an object with the foot. From180.
At about the same age that infants contact stationary targets, they show evidence of prospective control in dynamic situations by intercepting moving targets. As the toy moves along a horizontal path, infants time their arm movements so that their hand arrives at the location where the object will be, rather than where it was at the start of the reach115–117. Visual information for frontally approaching targets is different: The toy expands in the field of view. By 8 to 9 months of age, infants precisely gauge whether balls approaching at different speeds are catchable and they initiate interceptive arm movements based on visual information for time to contact118.
Older children and adults rely on view of the hand as well as view of the target to guide reaching119, 120. However, young infants do not benefit from being able to see their moving hand. Researchers can measure the importance of continual visual feedback by turning off the lights once a reach begins (the toy glows to mark its location) or by occluding sight of the hand and arm with a cloth barrier. Infants begin reaching for objects in the light and dark at the same age, and are equally successful in both conditions112. Moreover, the kinematics of infants’ reaching trajectories in the light and dark are indistinguishable121 and do not require sight of the hand122. In other words, infants’ zigzag reach trajectories do not necessarily mean that they visually track their hand because infants display equally jerky reaches when they cannot see their hand. Jerky trajectories may result in part from postural constraints17 and unanticipated reactive forces110.
Much to infants’ frustration, getting the hand to the right place is only part of the problem. Reaching precedes grasping because control of the arms precedes control of the hands. Normally, 3-month-olds merely swat at objects because they lack the requisite hand control for grasping and do not use perceptual information about object properties to plan the grasp. However, with the help of sticky Velcro mittens and Velcro covered toys, swats are sufficient to pick up an object, thereby allowing “pre-graspers” to reap some of the benefits of grasping objects that only older infants normally experience123. These early benefits have both immediate and long-lasting effects on manual skill124–126. With increased hand/finger control, infants adapt their grip configuration to object properties, but they do so after contacting the object, not prospectively during the reach127. Prospective control of grasping based on visual information for object size, orientation, and substance appears months after infants begin reaching128–131.
Exploring Objects
An object in hand opens up new opportunities for visual, manual, and oral exploration, and with increasing skill, object exploration becomes increasingly multi-modal132, 133. At first, infants use their hands only to bring objects up to the face for looking and mouthing134. Increased grip strength allows infants to alternate between looking and mouthing, providing multimodal information about object properties. Soon, manual skills progress beyond mere holding. Infants heft, rub, squeeze, and finger objects31, 133, 135. Later, infants coordinate visual and manual exploration by transferring objects from hand to hand and rotating them in front of their eyes25. Hands begin to serve complementary functions, one supporting the object and keeping it in view, the other generating information about object properties by fingering or palpating136. Infants explore the relations between object and surface properties by banging a hard block against a rigid surface to make a noise or rubbing a soft block against the surface105.
Extending Abilities with Tools
Tool use has its roots in early motor actions and relies on motor actions for its execution97, 98, 106. Young infants’ spontaneous banging and rubbing become preschoolers’ hammering and drawing. Fetal hand-to-mouth behaviors become self-feeding with a spoon. Exploring relations between objects and surfaces sets the stage for using objects as effective tools.
Tool use requires infants to perceive that a goal is beyond their abilities, recognize that an object can serve as a means to augment their abilities, and execute the necessary movements to use the tool. Each of these steps in real time must first be acquired in development. For example, very young infants perceive when an object is out of reach137. Months later, they use hooks, canes, and rakes to acquire out-of-reach objects, but only if the target object is already placed inside the crook of the tool138–140. And still later they perceive the full implications of the spatial relations by orienting the tools to place the target in the crook. Observing caregivers or other adults use a tool effectively provides a powerful impetus for learning140
Implementation often stands in the way of functional tool use. Nine-month-olds grasp a spoon filled with applesauce by the bowl end rather than by the handle (getting a handful of applesauce), or with a grip that points the food away from the mouth so that they cannot eat (Figure 8). Eighteen-month-olds perceive the optimal grasp for delivering food to their own mouth and plan their grasp prospectively, but their planning is less efficient when feeding a doll141–143. Two-year-olds adapt their grasp to use a spoon with a bent handle144. But even 4-year-olds fail to realize that they must use an underhand grip to grasp a spoon or hammer pointing away from their dominant hand145. Implementing a writing or drawing instrument poses similar problems for older children146. Three-year-olds use eleven different grip configurations to draw straight lines (including using both hands to hold the pen) and individual children vary their grips from trial to trial (Figure 8). Variability decreases by 5 years of age when most children begin formal schooling, and children converge on one of the two common adult grips.
Figure 8.
Nine-month-old infant grasping a spoon (A) by the bowl or (B) with an ulnar grip that points the bowl away from the mouth. (C) An 18-month-old using a radial grip that correctly brings food to the mouth. (D) Variety of pen grips used by 3- and 5-year-olds and adults. Adapted from142, 146.
Summary: Manual Action
Beginning prenatally, manual actions are perceptually guided and serve exploratory functions. Many of infants’ spontaneous arm and hand movements are co-opted for goal-directed manual actions and tool use. Infants use vision to locate the target of a reach and to preshape their hand for grasping, but they do not require sight of their hand to get it to a target. Exploring objects is a multimodal activity involving eyes, hands, fingers, and mouth. Boosting up manual skills can jump-start the cascade of opportunities for learning.
Facial Action
All the parts of the face begin moving prenatally, including the eyes while they are still fused shut. After birth, infants continue to produce spontaneous facial movements, but facial actions become integral to everyday function. The simple ability to swallow is critical for suckling, eating, and talking. Vocalizations and facial expressions are fundamental for communication. Head and eye movements provide the basis for visual exploration of the environment.
Swallowing, Sucking, and Chewing
Actions like swallowing are normally so innocuous that we don’t recognize the tremendous coordination required. Fetuses make swallowing, sucking, and breathing movements, but since they do not breathe air or eat, the movements are not coordinated147. However, to nurse without choking or swallowing air, newborns must coordinate movements of tongue, jaws, and lips to create suction, draw liquid into the mouth, pull the liquid into the pharynx, and divert the liquid to the esophagus while pulling air into the trachea148–150. Infants solve the timing problem by coordinating suck-swallow-respiration patterns at a ratio of 1:1:1 or 2:2:1151–153.
Chewing solid food is more complicated because the food must be masticated before it can be swallowed. Newborns can mush up a small piece of banana and move it around the mouth with jaws and tongue154. However, infants rely on lateral jaw movements to do most of the work of chewing, whereas older children and adults use rotary jaw movements and incorporate more prospective actions of the lips and tongue155. Infants produce the same chewing movements regardless of the type of food, whereas older children select appropriate jaw movements and muscle forces based on the food consistency155.
Facial Gestures and Speech
Facial expressions and vocalizations appear long before infants can convey feelings and communicate ideas. Fetuses produce smiles, grimaces, and facial movements that resemble adult-like expressions of laughter, crying, and pain156, 157. Neonates produce characteristic facial gestures to strong stimuli such as nose wrinkling and furrowed brows to noxious smells158. Newborns smile most while asleep, about one smile every five minutes159, 160. Awake infants begin to display social smiles and laughter by 2 to 5 months of age while gazing at caregivers or in response to positive stimulation161. Perhaps because they are so critical for social interaction, facial expressions are highly redundant so that muscles distributed throughout the face work in concert; eyebrows can convey basic facial expressions as effectively as the mouth. In fact, infants who lack the ability to move specific parts of their faces due to severe craniofacial anomalies, cleft lip/palate, or hemangiomas produce recognizable smiles, cries, and interested expressions162.
The movements needed for speech production are perhaps the most complex movements children learn163. The jaws, lips, and tongue must be precisely positioned to shape each sound as air travels through the oral and nasal cavities. Both speed and accuracy are major challenges in speech development. Adult-like speech is incredibly fast, encompassing up to 15 sounds per second163. As in the development of chewing, infants discover functional strategies to produce speech sounds, but their movements are not adult-like. For example, adults use quick simultaneous movements of the jaws and lips to babble (baba, mama), whereas infants rely primarily on jaw movements, which are easier to control164. Between 2–6 years of age, children gain better control over their lips and incorporate those movements into the previously established jaw movements, allowing them to produce a greater variety of speech sounds148.
Looking
Visual perception involves pointing the eyes in the right direction. But looking usually involves more than moving the eyes. It involves coordination among body, head, and eyes to bring a desired location into view165. For newborns who can barely turn their heads, the control problem is simplified: They typically watch whatever happens to be in front of them, whether faces, hands, objects, or more complex scenes166–168. Even after posture improves and infants can sit, crawl, and walk, much of what they see is opportunistic. Toddlers are less likely to tilt their head up to look at mother’s face than to point their gaze straight ahead at her knees; they see what’s in their hands or someone else’s hands because the hands are already in their field of view23, 169–171.
Like other motor actions, looking is more functional and adaptive when eye, head, and body movements are controlled prospectively. To track a moving object, infants must anticipate its speed and trajectory to keep their eyes moving at the right pace. Large targets moving in predictable ways are easier for young infants to smoothly pursue with their eyes172. When the target moves too quickly, the eyes lag behind, so infants often make corrective saccades to catch up to the target. Over months of practice, infants track smaller targets at faster speeds173, resorting less often to corrective saccades. Initially, infants keep their eyes on the target, but their head lags behind. By 4–5 months, infants coordinate movements of eyes and head to smoothly follow moving objects174. While watching an object move repeatedly behind an occluder, 4-month-olds keep their eyes on the point where the object disappeared and then struggle to catch up to its motion after it appears on the other side; 6-month-olds visually anticipate the location where the object will reappear, indicating their understanding that the object exists even when it is out of sight175.
As in the case of walking, infants amass tremendous amounts of experience while learning to look. In one day, infants shift their gaze roughly 50,000 times176. By 2 months of age, infants have accumulated 200 hours of visual experience175 and by 3.5 months of age, researchers estimate that infants have produced 3–6 million eye movements177. However, researchers know very little about what infants actually see outside of the laboratory. Head-mounted eye tracking provides a new method for observing infants’ eye movements during unconstrained, spontaneous activity. As shown in Figure 9, infants wear two small cameras that record their eye movements and field of view for real-time gaze processing. Toddlers seamlessly distribute visual attention among multiple motor tasks, looking toward obstacles to guide crawling and walking, fixating objects to guide the hand while reaching, and glancing occasionally at caregivers to initiate or respond to social interactions169. Visual guidance becomes increasingly efficient over the course of development. Quick glances toward obstacles from a distance elicit more costly types of exploration such as touching83. Infants’ short bodies serendipitously contribute to successful obstacle navigation because their field of view includes more of the floor compared with older children and adults, who primarily guide locomotion with visual information from the periphery178.
Figure 9.
(A) Head-mounted eye-tracker worn by a 14-month-old infant. An outward facing “scene camera” records the infant’s field of view, and an inward facing “eye camera” records movements of the infant’s right eye. Computer software calculates point of gaze. (B) Processed gaze video with red crosshair showing the infant’s point of gaze. From169.
Summary: Facial Action
Facial actions include many of our most prized and basic social skills—talking, facial gestures, eating and drinking, and looking at others and at the environment. And each of these skills sets off a new cascade of interactions. Infants’ solutions for moving the various parts of their face often differ from those of adults, but they get the job done in that developmental niche.
Conclusion
The study of motor development is really the study of behavioral development. As such, it can provide a useful window into general processes of development because the topic of study—movement—is directly observable. Researchers in motor development have always recognized the importance of the bodily context7. How could they do otherwise? Movements depend on physical forces and the moment-to-moment changes and developmental status of the body affect forces. The developmental systems perspective encourages researchers also to consider a larger context that includes the physical and social/cultural environment, and to view motor behaviors as potentially both cause and consequence of developmental change in other psychological domains. Although prominent developmental theorists have long recognized the importance of motor development for psychological development more generally5, 6, only recently have researchers begun to systematically map out these developmental pathways.
Acknowledgments
The writing of this article was supported by NICHD Grant R37-HD33486 to K. E. Adolph.
Contributor Information
Karen E. Adolph, New York University
John M. Franchak, University of California, Riverside
References
- 1.Bernstein NA. Dexterity and its development. In: Latash ML, Turvey MT, editors. Dexterity and its development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1996. pp. 3–244. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gibson EJ. Has psychology a future? Psychological Science. 1994;5:69–76. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Keen R. The development of problem solving in young children: A critical cognitive skill. Annual Review of Psychology. 2011;62:1–21. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Adolph KE, Karasik LB, Tamis-LeMonda CS. Motor skills. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of cultural development science. Vol. 1. Domains of development across cultures. New York, NY: Taylor and Francis; 2010. pp. 61–88. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gibson EJ. Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology. 1988;39:1–41. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Piaget J. The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books; 1954. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Adolph KE, Robinson SR. Motor development. In: Liben L, Muller U, editors. Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. 7th. Vol. 2. New York: Wiley; 2015. pp. 114–157. Cognitive Processes. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Thelen E. Dynamic systems theory and the complexity of change. Psychoanalytic Dialogues. 2005;15:255–283. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Campos JJ, Anderson DI, Barbu-Roth MA, Hubbard EM, Hertenstein MJ, Witherington DC. Travel broadens the mind. Infancy. 2000;1:149–219. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0102_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Reed ES. An outline of a theory of action systems. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1982;14:98–134. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1982.10735267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.D'Elia A, Pighetti M, Moccia G, Santangelo N. Spontaneous motor activity in normal fetuses. Early Human Development. 2001;65:139–147. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(01)00224-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.de Vries JIP, Hopkins B. Fetal movements and postures: What do they mean for postnatal development? In: Hopkins B, Johnson SP, editors. Prenatal development of postnatal functions. Westport, CT: Praeger; 2005. pp. 177–220. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gesell A. The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In: Carmichael L, editor. Manual of child psychology. New York, NY: John Wiley; 1946. pp. 295–331. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Adolph KE, Berger SE, Leo AJ. Developmental continuity? Crawling, cruising, and walking. Developmental Science. 2011;14:306–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00981.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Atun-Einy O, Berger SE, Scher A. Pulling to stand: Common trajectories and individual differences. Developmental Psychobiology. 2012;54:187–198. doi: 10.1002/dev.20593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hopkins B, Westra T. Motor development, maternal expectations, and the role of handling. Infant Behavior and Development. 1990;13:117–122. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hopkins B, Ronnqvist L. Facilitating postural control: Effects on the reaching behavior of 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology. 2002;40:168–182. doi: 10.1002/dev.10021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Saavedra SL, van Donkelaar P, Woollacott MH. Learning about gravity: Segmental assessment of upright control as infants develop independent sitting. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108:2215–2229. doi: 10.1152/jn.01193.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Martorell R, Onis M, Martines J, Black M, Onyango A, Dewey KG. WHO motor development study: Windows of achievement for six gross motor development milestones. Acta Paediatrica. 2006;95(S450):86–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2006.tb02379.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Karasik LB, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Adolph KE, Bornstein MH. Places and postures: A cross-cultural comparison of sitting in 5-month-olds. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 2015;46:1023–1038. doi: 10.1177/0022022115593803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kretch KS, Adolph KE. Active vision in passive locomotion: Real-world free viewing in infants and adults. Developmental Science. 2015;18:736–750. doi: 10.1111/desc.12251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fogel A, Messinger DS, Dickson KL, Hsu H. Posture and gaze in early mother-infant communication: synchronization of developmental trajectories. Developmental Science. 1999;2:325–332. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kretch KS, Franchak JM, Adolph KE. Crawling and walking infants see the world differently. Child Development. 2014 doi: 10.1111/cdev.12206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rachwani J, Santamaria V, Saavedra SL, Wood S, Porter F, Woollacott MH. Segmental trunk control acquisition and reaching in typically developing infants. Experimental Brain Research. 2013;228:131–139. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3544-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Soska KC, Adolph KE. Postural position constrains multimodal object exploration in infants. Infancy. 2014;19:138–161. doi: 10.1111/infa.12039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Harbourne RT, Lobo MA, Karst GM, Galloway JC. Sit happens: Does sitting development perturb reaching development, or vice versa? Infant Behavior and Development. 2013;36:438–450. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Carvalho RP, Tudella E, Calijouw SR, Savelsbergh GJP. Early control of reaching: Effects of experience and body orientation. Infant Behavior and Development. 2008;31:23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Soska KC, Adolph KE, Johnson SP. Systems in development: Motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. Developmental Psychology. 2010;46:129–138. doi: 10.1037/a0014618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Baumgartner HA, Oakes LM. Investigating the relation between infants' manual activity with objects and their perception of dynamic events. Infancy. 2013;18:983–1006. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Libertus K, Gibson J, Hidayatallah NZ, Hirtle J, Adcock RA, Needham AW. Size matters: How age and reaching experience shape infants' preferences for different sized objects. Infant Behavior and Development. 2013;36:189–198. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Eppler MA. Development of manipulatory skills and the deployment of attention. Infant Behavior and Development. 1995;18:391–405. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Loucks J, Sommerville JA. The role of motor experience in understanding action function: The case of the precision grasp. Child Development. 2012;83:801–809. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01735.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Daum MM, Prinz W, Aschersleben G. Perception and production of object-related grasping in 6-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2011;108:810–818. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Deffeyes JE, Harbourne RT, Stuberg WA, Stergiou N. Approximate entropy used to assess sitting postural sway of infants with developmental delay. Infant Behavior and Development. 2011;34:81–99. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Metcalfe JS, Chen LC, Chang TY, McDowell K, Jeka JJ, Clark JE. The temporal organization of posture changes during the first year of independent walking. Experimental Brain Research. 2005;161:405–416. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2082-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Chen LC, Metcalfe JS, Chang TY, Jeka JJ, Clark JE. The development of infant upright posture: Sway less or sway differently? Experimental Brain Research. 2008;186:293–303. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1236-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dahl A, Campos JJ, Anderson DI, Uchiyama I, Witherington DC, Ueno M, Poutrain-Lejeune L, Barbu-Roth M. The epigenesis of wariness of heights. Psychological Science. 2013;24:1361–1367. doi: 10.1177/0956797613476047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Chen LC, Jeka J, Clark JE. Development of adaptive sensorimotor control in infant sitting posture. Gait and Posture. 2016;45:157–163. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bertenthal BI, Bai DL. Infants' sensitivity to optical flow for controlling posture. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25:936–945. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Adolph KE, Robinson SR. The road to walking: What learning to walk tells us about development. In: Zelazo P, editor. Oxford handbook of developmental psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 403–443. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Andre-Thomas M, Autgaerden S. Locomotion from pre- to post-natal life. Lavenham, Suffolk: Spastics Society Medical Education and Information Unit in association with William Heinemann Medical Books, Ltd; 1966. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Forssberg H. Ontogeny of human locomotor control. I. Infant stepping, supported locomotion, and transition to independent locomotion. Experimental Brain Research. 1985;57:480–493. doi: 10.1007/BF00237835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.McGraw MB. The neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York, NY: Columbia University Press; 1945. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Dominici N, Ivanenko YP, Cappellini G, d'Avella A, Mondi V, Cicchese M, Fabiano A, Silei T, Di Paolo A, Giannini C, et al. Locomotor primitives in newborn babies and their development. Science. 2011;334:997–999. doi: 10.1126/science.1210617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Barbu-Roth M, Anderson DI, Despres A, Provasi J, Cabrol D, Campos JJ. Neonatal stepping in relation to terrestrial optic flow. Child Development. 2009;80:8–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01241.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Barbu-Roth M, Anderson DI, Despres A, Streeter RJ, Caborl D, Trukillo M, Campos JJ, Provasi J. Air stepping in response to optic flows that move toward and away from the neonate. Developmental Psychobiology. 2014;56:1142–1149. doi: 10.1002/dev.21174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Angulo-Kinzler RM, Ulrich BD, Thelen E. Three-month-old infants can select specific leg motor solutions. Motor Control. 2002;6:52–68. doi: 10.1123/mcj.6.1.52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Rovee-Collier CK, Morrongiello BA, Aron M, Kupersmidt J. Topographical response differentiation and reversal in 3-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development. 1978;1:323–333. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Thelen E. Three-month-old infants can learn task-specific patterns of interlimb coordination. Psychological Science. 1994;5:280–285. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Thelen E, Fisher DM. Newborn stepping: An explanation for a "disappearing reflex". Developmental Psychology. 1982;18:760–775. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Thelen E. Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behavior. 1979;27:699–715. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Thelen E. Treadmill-elicited stepping in seven-month-old infants. Child Development. 1986;57:1498–1506. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Yang JF, Lam T, Pang MYC, Lamont E, Musselman K, Seinen E. Infant stepping: A window to the behavior of the human pattern generator for walking. Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology. 2004;82:662–674. doi: 10.1139/y04-070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Thelen E, Fisher DM, Ridley-Johnson R. The relationship between physical growth and a newborn reflex. Infant Behavior and Development. 1984;7:479–493. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Zelazo PR. The development of walking: New findings on old assumptions. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1983;2:99–137. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1983.10735292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Zelazo PR, Zelazo NA, Kolb S. “Walking” in the newborn. Science. 1972;176:314–315. doi: 10.1126/science.176.4032.314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.van der Meer ALH, Ramstad M, van der Weel FR. Choosing the shortest way to mum: Auditory guided rotation in 6- to 9-moth-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development. 2008;31:207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Adolph KE, Vereijken B, Denny MA. Learning to crawl. Child Development. 1998;69:1299–1312. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Patrick SK, Noah JA, Yang JF. Developmental constraints of quadrupedal coordination across crawling styles in human infants. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;107:3050–3061. doi: 10.1152/jn.00029.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Majnemer A, Barr RG. Influence of supine sleep positioning on early motor milestone acquisition. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2005;47:370–376. doi: 10.1017/s0012162205000733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Patrick SK, Noah JA, Yang JF. Interlimb coordination in human crawling reveals similarities in development and neural control with quadrupeds. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;101:603–613. doi: 10.1152/jn.91125.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.McCollum G, Holroyd C, Castelfranco AM. Forms of early walking. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1995;176:373–390. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.1995.0206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Snapp-Childs W, Corbetta D. Evidence of early strategies in learning to walk. Infancy. 2009;14:101–116. doi: 10.1080/15250000802569835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Adolph KE. Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1997;62 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kretch KS, Adolph KE. Cliff or step? Posture-specific learning at the edge of a drop-off. Child Development. 2013;84:226–240. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01842.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Karasik LB, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Adolph KE. Decisions at the brink: Locomotor experience affects infants' use of social information on an adjustable drop-off. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:797. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Berger SE, Adolph KE. Infants use handrails as tools in a locomotor task. Developmental Psychology. 2003;39:594–605. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Berger SE, Adolph KE, Lobo SA. Out of the toolbox: Toddlers differentiate wobbly and wooden handrails. Child Development. 2005;76:1294–1307. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00851.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Berger SE, Adolph KE, Kavookjian AE. Bridging the gap: Solving spatial means-ends relations in a locomotor task. Child Development. 2010;81:1367–1375. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01478.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Bril B, Ledebt A. Head coordination as a means to assist sensory integration in learning to walk. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1998;22:555–563. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(97)00044-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hallemans A, De Clercq D, Aerts P. Changes in 3D joint dynamics during the first 5 months after the onset of independent walking: A longitudinal follow-up study. Gait and Posture. 2006;24:270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Adolph KE, Vereijken B, Shrout PE. What changes in infant walking and why. Child Development. 2003;74:474–497. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.7402011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bril B, Breniere Y. Postural requirements and progression velocity in young walkers. Journal of Motor Behavior. 1992;24:105–116. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Bril B, Breniere Y. Posture and independent locomotion in early childhood: Learning to walk or learning dynamic postural control? In: Savelsbergh GJP, editor. The development of coordination in infancy. North-Holland, The Netherlands: Elsevier; 1993. pp. 337–358. [Google Scholar]
- 75.Ivanenko YP, Dominici N, Cappellini G, Lacquaniti F. Kinematics in newly walking toddlers does not depend upon postural stability. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2005;94:754–763. doi: 10.1152/jn.00088.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Bril B, Dupuy L, Dietrich G, Corbetta D. Learning to tune the antero-posterior propulsive forces during walking: A necessary skill for mastering upright locomotion in toddlers. Experimental Brain Research. 2015;233:2903–2912. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4378-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Sutherland DH, Olshen RA, Biden EN, Wyatt MP. The development of mature walking. London: Mac Keith Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Adolph KE, Cole WG, Komati M, Garciaguirre JS, Badaly D, Lingeman JM, Chan G, Sotsky RB. How do you learn to walk? Thousands of steps and dozens of falls per day. Psychological Science. 2012;23:1387–1394. doi: 10.1177/0956797612446346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Bennett WC, Zingg RM. The Tarahumara, an Indian tribe of northern Mexico. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1935. [Google Scholar]
- 80.Bastien GJ, Schepens B, Willems PA, Heglund NC. Energetics of load carrying in Nepalese porters. Science. 2005;308:1755. doi: 10.1126/science.1111513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Maloiy GMO, Heglund NC, Prager LM, Cavagna GA, Taylor CR. Energetic costs of carrying loads: Have African women discovered an economic way? Nature. 1986;319:668–669. doi: 10.1038/319668a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Plumert JM, Kearney JK. How do children perceive and act on dynamic affordances in crossing traffic-filled roads? Child Development Perspectives. 2014;8:207–212. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Kretch KS, Adolph KE. The organization of exploratory behaviors in infant locomotor planning. Developmental Science. doi: 10.1111/desc.12421. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Gibson EJ, Walk RD. The "visual cliff". Scientific American. 1960;202:64–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Campos JJ, Hiatt S, Ramsay D, Henderson C, Svejda M. The emergence of fear on the visual cliff. In: Lewis M, Rosenblum L, editors. The development of affect. New York: Plenum; 1978. pp. 149–182. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Adolph KE, Joh AS, Eppler MA. Infants' perception of affordances of slopes under high and low friction conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2010;36:797–811. doi: 10.1037/a0017450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Burnay C, Cordovil R. Crawling experience predicts avoidance of real cliffs and water cliffs: Insight from a new paradigm. Infancy. in press. [Google Scholar]
- 88.Franchak JM, Adolph KE. What infants know and what they do: Perceiving possibilities for walking through openings. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48:1254–1261. doi: 10.1037/a0027530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Gibson EJ, Riccio G, Schmuckler MA, Stoffregen TA, Rosenberg D, Taormina J. Detection of the traversability of surfaces by crawling and walking infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1987;13:533–544. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.13.4.533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Joh AS, Adolph KE. Learning from falling. Child Development. 2006;77:89–102. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00858.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Schmuckler MA. Development of visually guided locomotion: Barrier crossing by toddlers. Ecological Psychology. 1996;8:209–236. [Google Scholar]
- 92.van der Meer ALH. Visual guidance of passing under a barrier. Early Development and Parenting. 1997;6:149–157. [Google Scholar]
- 93.Gilmore RO, Baker TJ, Grobman KH. Stability in young infants' discrimination of optic flow. Developmental Psychology. 2004;40:259–270. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Campos JJ, Bertenthal BI, Kermoian R. Early experience and emotional development: The emergence of wariness of heights. Psychological Science. 1992;3:61–64. [Google Scholar]
- 95.Adolph KE. Specificity of learning: Why infants fall over a veritable cliff. Psychological Science. 2000;11:290–295. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Adolph KE, Kretch KS, LoBue V. Fear of heights in infants? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2014;23:60–66. doi: 10.1177/0963721413498895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Smitsman AW, Bongers RM. Tool use and tool making: A developmental action perspective. In: Valsiner J, Connolly KJ, editors. Handbook of developmental psychology. London, England: Sage; 2003. pp. 172–193. [Google Scholar]
- 98.Lockman JJ, Kahrs BA. Tool using. Child Development Perspectives. 2014;8:231–236. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Prechtl HFR. Ultrasound studies of human fetal behaviour. Early Human Development. 1985;12:91–98. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(85)90173-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Prechtl HFR. Prenatal motor development. In: Wade MG, Whiting HTA, editors. Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff Publishing; 1986. pp. 53–64. [Google Scholar]
- 101.Sparling JW, van Tol J, Chescheir NC. Fetal and neonatal hand movement. Physical Therapy. 1999;79:24–39. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Hepper PG, Shahidullah S, White R. Handedness in the human fetus. Neuropsychologia. 1991;29:1107–1111. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90080-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Reissland N, Francis B, Aydin E, Mason J, Schaal B. The development of anticipation in the fetus: A longitudinal account of human fetal mouth movements in reaction to and anticipation of touch. Developmental Psychobiology. 2014;56:955–963. doi: 10.1002/dev.21172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Piek JP, Carman R. Developmental profiles of spontaneous movements in infants. Early Human Development. 1994;39:109–126. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(94)90160-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Fontenelle SA, Kahrs BA, Neal SA, Newton AT, Lockman JJ. Infant manual exploration of composite substrates. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2007;98:153–167. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Kahrs BA, Jung WP, Lockman JJ. Motor origins of tool use. Child Development. 2013;84:810–816. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Thelen E, Corbetta D, Kamm K, Spencer JP, Schneider K, Zernicke RF. The transition to reaching: Mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. Child Development. 1993;64:1058–1098. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Lee HM, Bhat A, Scholz JP, Galloway JC. Toy-oriented changes during early arm movements IV: Shoulder-elbow coordination. Infant Behavior and Development. 2008;31:447–469. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Lee MH, Ranganathan R, Newell KM. Changes in object-oriented arm movements that precede the transition to goal-directed reaching in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology. 2011;53:685–693. doi: 10.1002/dev.20541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Berthier NE, Keen RE. Development of reaching in infancy. Experimental Brain Research. 2006;169:507–518. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0169-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Konczak J, Dichgans J. The development toward stereotypic arm kinematics during reaching in the first 3 years of life. Experimental Brain Research. 1997;117:346–354. doi: 10.1007/s002210050228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Clifton RK, Muir DW, Ashmead DH, Clarkson MG. Is visually guided reaching in early infancy a myth? Child Development. 1993;64:1099–1110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Schneiberg S, Sveistrup H, McFadyen B, McKinley P, Levin MF. The development of coordination of reach-to-grasp movements in children. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;146:142–154. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1156-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Galloway JC, Thelen E. Feet first: Object exploration in young infants. Infant Behavior and Development. 2004;27:107–112. [Google Scholar]
- 115.von Hofsten C. Catching skills in infancy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1983;9:75–85. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.9.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.von Hofsten C, Lindhagen K. Observations on the development of reaching for moving objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1979;28:158–173. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(79)90109-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Fagard J, Spelke ES, von Hofsten C. Reaching and grasping a moving object in 6-, 8-, and 10-month-old infants: Laterality and performance. Infant Behavior and Development. 2009;32:137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.van Hof P, van der Kamp J, Savelsbergh GJP. The relation between infants' perception of catchableness and the control of catching. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:182–194. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Churchill A, Hopkins B, Ronnqvist L, Vogt S. Vision of the hand and environmental context in human prehension. Experimental Brain Research. 2000;134:81–89. doi: 10.1007/s002210000444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Stolze H, Boczek-Funcke A, Johnk K, Heinrichs H, Illert M. Kinematic analysis of prehension movements in children. Behavioral Brain Research. 1998;93:131–141. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00147-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Clifton RK, Rochat P, Robin DJ, Berthier NE. Multimodal perception in the control of infant reaching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1994;20:876–886. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.4.876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Lee MH, Newell KM. Visual feedback of hand trajectory and the development of infant prehension. Infant Behavior and Development. 2012;35:273–279. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Needham AW, Barrett T, Peterman K. A pick-me-up for infants' exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using "sticky" mittens enhances young infants' object exploration skills. Infant Behavior and Development. 2002;25:279–295. [Google Scholar]
- 124.Libertus K, Needham AW. Teach to reach: The effects of active vs. passive reaching experiences on action and perception. Vision Research. 2010;50:2750–2757. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Wiesen SE, Watkins RM, Needham AW. Active motor training has long-term effects on infants' object exploration. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Libertus K, Joh AS, Needham AW. Motor training at 3 months affects object exploration 12 months later. Developmental Science. doi: 10.1111/desc.12370. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Vollmer B, Forssberg H. Development of grasping and object manipulation. In: Nowak DA, Hermsdorfer J, editors. Sensorimotor control of grasping: Physiology and Pathophysiology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2009. pp. 235–249. [Google Scholar]
- 128.Barrett TM, Traupman E, Needham AW. Infants' visual anticipation of object structure in grasp planning. Infant Behavior and Development. 2008;31:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Fagard J. Linked proximal and distal changes in the reaching behavior of 5- to 12-month-old human infants grasping objects of different sizes. Infant Behavior and Development. 2000;23:317–329. [Google Scholar]
- 130.Witherington DC. The development of prospective grasping control between 5 and 7 months: A longitudinal study. Infancy. 2005;7:143–161. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0702_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.von Hofsten C, Johansson K. Planning to reach for a rotating rod: Developmental aspects. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und pädagogische Psychologie. 2009;41:207–213. [Google Scholar]
- 132.Needham AW. Improvements in object exploration skills may facilitate the development of object segregation in early infancy. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2000;1:131–156. [Google Scholar]
- 133.Rochat P. Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25:871–884. [Google Scholar]
- 134.Norris B, Smith S. Research into the mouthing behaviour of children up to 5 years old. 2002 [Google Scholar]
- 135.Palmer CF. The discriminating nature of infants' exploratory actions. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25:885–893. [Google Scholar]
- 136.Kotwica KA, Ferre CL, Michel GF. Relation of stable hand-use preferences to the development of skill for managing multiple objects from 7 to 13 months of age. Developmental Psychobiology. 2008;50:519–529. doi: 10.1002/dev.20311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Yonas A, Hartman B. Perceiving the affordance of contact in four- and five-month-old infants. Child Development. 1993;64:298–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02911.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Chen Z, Siegler RS. Across the great divide: Bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers' and older children's thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 2000;65 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.van Leeuwen L, Smitsman A, van Leeuwen C. Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1994;20:174–191. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.20.1.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Rat-Fischer L, O'Regan JK, Fagard J. The emergence of tool use during the second year of life. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2012;113:440–446. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Claxton LJ, McCarty ME, Keen R. Self-directed action affects planning in tool-use tasks with toddlers. Infant Behavior and Development. 2009;32:230–233. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.McCarty ME, Clifton RK, Collard RR. Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:1091–1101. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.4.1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.McCarty ME, Clifton RK, Collard RR. The beginnings of tool use by infants and toddlers. Infancy. 2001;2:233–256. [Google Scholar]
- 144.Steenbergen B, van der Kamp J, Smitsman A, Carson RG. Spoon-handling in two- to four-year-old children. Ecological Psychology. 1997;9:113–129. [Google Scholar]
- 145.Keen R, Lee MH, Adolph KE. Planning an action: A developmental progression in tool use. In: Pick Herbert L, Lockman JJ, Rieser J., editors. Ecological Psychology, Collection to honor. Vol. 26. 2014. pp. 96–108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Greer T, Lockman JJ. Using writing instruments: Invariances in young children and adults. Child Development. 1998;69:888–902. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Miller JL, Sonies BC, Macedonia C. Emergence of oropharyngeal, laryngeal and swallowing activity in the developing fetal upper aerodigestive tract: An ultrasound evaluation. Early Human Development. 2003;71:61–87. doi: 10.1016/s0378-3782(02)00110-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Wilson EM, Green JR, Yunusova YY, Moore CA. Task specificity in early oral motor development. Seminars in Speech and Language. 2008;29:257–266. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1103389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Burton P, Deng J, McDonald D, Fewtrell MS. Real-time 3D ultrasound imaging of infant tongue movements during breastfeeding. Early Human Development. 2013;89:635–641. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Geddes DT, Sakalidis VS, Hepworth AR, McClellan HL, Kent JC, Lai CT, Hartmann PE. Tongue movement and intra-oral vacuum of term infants during breastfeeding and feeding from an experimental teat that released milk under vacuum only. Early Human Development. 2012;88:443–449. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Barlow SM. Oral and respiratory control for preterm feeding. Current Opinion in Otolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery. 2009;17:179–186. doi: 10.1097/MOO.0b013e32832b36fe. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.van der Meer ALH, Holden G, van der Weel FR. Coordination of sucking, swallowing, and breathing in healthy newborns. Journal of Pediatric Neonatology. 2005;2:69–72. [Google Scholar]
- 153.Fucile S, MacFarland DH, Gisel EG, Lau C. Oral and nonoral sensorimotor interventions facilitate suck-swallow-respiration functions and their coordination in preterm infants. Early Human Development. 2012;88:345–350. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Sheppard JJ, Mysak ED. Ontogeny of infantile oral reflexes and emerging chewing. Child Development. 1984;55:831–843. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Wilson EM, Green JR. The development of jaw motion for mastication. Early Human Development. 2009;85:303–311. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Reissland N, Francis B, Mason J. Can healthy fetuses show facial expressions of "pain" or "distress"? PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e65530. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Reissland N, Francis B, Mason J, Lincoln K. Do facial expressions develop before birth? PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e24081. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Loos HM, Doucet S, Roussignan R, Hartmann C, Durand K, Dittrich R, Sagot P, Buettner A, Schaal B. Responsiveness of human neonates to the odor of 5a-Androst-16-en-3-one: A behavioral paradox? Chemical Senses. 2014;39:693–703. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bju041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Messinger DS. Positive and negative: Infant facial expressions and emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2002;11:1–6. [Google Scholar]
- 160.Messinger DS. Smiling. In: Haith MM, Benson JB, editors. Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development. Vol. 3. Oxford: Elsevier; 2008. pp. 186–198. [Google Scholar]
- 161.Messinger DS. Smiling. In: Salkind NJ, editor. Encyclopedia of human development. Vol. 3. Thousand Oakes, CA: Sage; 2006. pp. 1183–1185. [Google Scholar]
- 162.Oster H. Emotion in the infant's face: Insights from the study of infants with facial anomalies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2003;1000:197–204. doi: 10.1196/annals.1280.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Green JR, Nip ISB. Some organization principles in early speech development. In: Maaseen B, van Lieshout PHHM, editors. Speech motor control: New development in basic and applied research. New York: Oxford University Press; 2010. pp. 171–188. [Google Scholar]
- 164.Green JR, Moore CA, Higashikawa M, Steeve RW. The physiologic development of speech motor control: Lip and jaw coordination. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2000;43:239–255. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4301.239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Gibson JJ. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 166.Fausey CM, Jayaraman S, Smith LB. From faces to hands: Changing visual input in the first two years. Cognition. 2016;152:101–107. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Daniel BM, Lee DN. Development of looking with head and eyes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1990;50:200–216. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(90)90039-b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Jayaraman W, Fausey CM, Smith LB. The faces in infant-perspective scenes change over the first year of life. PloS One. 2015 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Franchak JM, Kretch KS, Soska KC, Adolph KE. Head-mounted eye tracking: A new method to describe infant looking. Child Development. 2011;82:1738–1750. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01670.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Smith LB, Yu C, Pereira AF. Not your mother's view: The dynamics of toddler visual experience. Developmental Science. 2011;14:9–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00947.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Yu C, Smith LB. Multiple sensory-motor pathways lead to coordinated visual attention. Cognitive Science. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12366. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.von Hofsten C, Rosander K. Development of smooth pursuit tracking in young infants. Vision Research. 1997;37:1799–1810. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(96)00332-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Rosander K, von Hofsten C. Development of gaze tracking of small and large objects. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;146:257–264. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1161-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Rosander K, von Hofsten C. Infants' emerging ability to represent occluded object motion. Cognition. 2004;91:1–22. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00166-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Johnson SP, Amso D, Slemmer JA. Development of object concepts in infancy: Evidence for early learning in an eye tracking paradigm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2003;100:10568–10573. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1630655100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.Bronson GW. Infant's transitions toward adult-like scanning. Child Development. 1994;65:1243–1261. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00815.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Haith MM, Hazan C, Goodman GS. Expectations and anticipation of dynamic visual events by 3.5-month-old babies. Child Development. 1988;59:467–479. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Franchak JM, Adolph KE. Visually guided navigation: Head-mounted eye-tracking of natural locomotion in children and adults. Vision Research. 2010;50:2766–2774. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Hopkins B, Westra T. Maternal handling and motor development: An intracultural study. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs. 1988;114:379–408. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Adolph KE, Berger SE. Physical and motor development. In: Bornstein MH, Lamb ME, editors. Development science: An advanced textbook. 7. New York: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis; 2015. pp. 261–333. [Google Scholar]
Further Reading
- Adolph KE, Berger SE. Motor development. In: Kuhn D, Siegler RS, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2 Cognition, perception, and language. 6th. New York: Wiley; 2006. pp. 161–213. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson EJ, Pick AD. An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Smitsman AW, Corbetta D. Action in infancy: Perspectives, concepts, and challenges. In: Bremner JG, Wachs TD, editors. The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Infant Development. 2. Chichester, West Sussex, England: Wiley-Blackwell Ltd; 2010. pp. 167–203. [Google Scholar]
- Thelen E. Motor development: A new synthesis. American Psychologist. 1995;50:79–95. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.50.2.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]