Abstract
The aged population is growing rapidly, which has sparked tremendous interest in elucidating mechanisms of aging in both the body and the brain. Animal models have become an indispensable tool in biomedical science, but because of the cost and extended timeframe associated with aging animals to appropriate time points, studies that rely on using aged animals are often not feasible. Somewhat surprisingly, there are relatively few animal models that have been specifically engineered to mimic physiological changes known to occur during “normal” aging. Developing transgenic animal models that faithfully mimic key aspects of aging would likely be of great utility in studying both age-related deficits in the absence of overt pathology as well as an adjunct for transgenic models of diseases where aging is a primary risk factor.
In particular, there are several alterations in the aged brain that are amenable to being modeled genetically. We have focused on one key aspect that has been repeatedly demonstrated in aged animals - an increase in the L-type voltage-gated calcium channel CaV1.3. Here we present a novel transgenic mouse line in which expression of CaV1.3 is increased by approximately 50% in the forebrain of young mice. These mice do not display any overt physical or non-cognitive deficits, exhibiting normal exploratory behavior, motor function, and affective-like responses, suggesting that these mice can be successfully deployed to assess the impact of an “aged brain” in a variety of conditions.
Keywords: aged brain, transgenic mice, L-type calcium channels, CaV1.3, neurobattery
Introduction
As people age, they often experience declines in physical, physiological, and mental function which not only reduce the lifespan but also adversely impact the quality of life. The aged population is one of the fastest growing segments of society, which has been predicted to double in size by the year 2050, potentially representing over 20% of the total population in the United States [1]. As such, there has been increased interest in elucidating mechanisms that underlie age-related alterations so that potential therapeutic interventions can be identified and developed to ameliorate declining function.
Animal models have become an invaluable tool in biomedical science, and are used to study myriad aspects of biological mechanisms. Often, animal models are generated by introducing mutations into the genome to alter the function of target proteins or processes or to replicate mutations linked to specific human disease states. The “aged” phenotype, however, comprises a constellation of various changes throughout the body and brain that are complex and often interdependent, making it difficult to produce an accurate model. Thus, researchers interested in studying aging often have to literally “age” animals, simply waiting for them to reach a time point that is comparable to that of interest in humans. In the case of rodents, which are commonly used as animal models, this time point is generally 2–3 years, making this a very time-consuming and expensive experimental design.
One approach that has been taken in order to accelerate the time frame necessary for generating phenotypically aged mice is selective breeding. The senescence-accelerated-prone (SAMP) mouse lines were established by continuous interbreeding of littermates, which were selected on the basis of early senescence, shortened lifespan, and the development of age-associated pathologies, for many successive generations [2]. This process yielded a number of separate lines that each mimic specific characteristics of aged animals, including both physical deficits and cognitive deficits [3]. However, it is important to note that while these mice mimic some aspects of aging, it is difficult to determine whether the same mechanisms that underlie aging are contributing to the deficits observed in the SAMP mice. Thus, alterations in the expression, activity, and/or function of genes and proteins in the SAMP lines need to be validated by comparison to control mouse lines. While this represents a powerful unbiased method for identifying potential targets of interest, the selection of the “control” mouse line(s) introduces another significant caveat. Because the SAMP lines are a result of selective breeding, all mice in these lines are senescence-accelerated; therefore, there are no unaffected littermates to use as controls. Instead, control mice must be selected from other (genetically inbred) lines, each of which may have distinct mechanisms that contribute to an “aged” phenotype resulting in different catalogs of potential targets depending on the “control” chosen. A final crucial point is that each of the SAMP lines generally comprises multiple alterations that may contribute to the observed phenotypes. While this is similar to aging in which multiple factors likely interact to produce observed deficits, it confounds the understanding of the mechanism(s) responsible for specific age-related impairments. For example, the SAMP8 mouse line has garnered considerable interest because these mice exhibit cognitive deficits (particularly in learning and memory), which is a hallmark of aging in humans [4, 5]. However, these mice display both an increase in oxidative stress as well as an increase in amyloid beta [5], making it is difficult to determine which mechanism is responsible for the deficits in learning and memory, and, moreover, to precisely elucidate the relative contribution of these processes to the cognitive decline that is observed in aged subjects.
In light of these limitations, our goal was to design a mouse model in which functional as well as mechanistic aspects of brain aging could be interrogated. There are many well-documented age-related changes that occur in the brain [6–8] and a number of these alterations could be modeled transgenically. We have chosen to focus our initial efforts on one key aspect of the aged brain: dysregulation of neuronal calcium (Ca2+) homeostasis [9–11]. As a ubiquitous signaling molecule [12, 13], a crucial regulator of gene transcription [14, 15], and a critical modulator of both neuronal excitability [16] and plasticity [17], even small changes in Ca2+ homeostasis can significantly alter brain function. Several lines of evidence support the idea that Ca2+ homeostasis is dysregulated in the aged brain and that this contributes to age-related impairments in brain function (for example, the deficits in learning and memory that are often observed during aging), even in the absence of overt pathology. Calcium imaging experiments in the hippocampus showed that neurons from aged rats exhibited a significantly larger increase in intracellular Ca2+ concentration in response to depolarization than neurons from young rats [17]. This difference was only observed when the neurons fired action potentials, which suggested that the high voltage-activated class of voltage-gated calcium channels was involved [17]. Indeed, additional electrophysiological experiments demonstrated that the increase in whole-cell calcium currents was a result of an increase in the density of L-type voltage-gated calcium channels (L-VGCC) [18]. Further, the magnitude of the increase in calcium current and channel density was correlated with the degree of cognitive impairment in a hippocampus-dependent learning and memory task (the Morris water maze) [18]. Another line of evidence comes from electrophysiological recordings of the slow afterhyperpolarization (sAHP), a key determinant of neuronal excitability [19], which has been shown to require activation of L-VGCCs [20–23]. The sAHP in neurons from aged animals is significantly increased relative to that in young animals [24–27], consistent with the model of an increase in the number of available L-VGCCs that in turn activate more of the current underlying the sAHP. Interestingly, the magnitude of the sAHP recorded from hippocampal neurons has also been shown to be correlated with the degree of impairment in hippocampus-dependent learning and memory tasks (trace eyeblink conditioning and trace fear conditioning) – animals with a larger sAHP are impaired relative to those with a smaller sAHP [28, 29].
There are two primary L-VGCC pore forming subunits that are expressed in the mammalian brain: CaV1.2, which comprises ~80% of the total L-VGCC expression, and CaV1.3. While these subtypes have unique subcellular distributions [30], distinct electrophysiological properties [31], and have been implicated in different physiological roles [32], they cannot be differentiated by pharmacological agents. Thus, one approach that has been used to distinguish between the roles of these two L-VGCC subtypes is to generate transgenic mice lacking either CaV1.2 or CaV1.3. Previous work from our lab has suggested that the age-related increase in the sAHP is predominated by CaV1.3 because genetic deletion of CaV1.3 significantly reduces the magnitude of the sAHP [16, 33], whereas deletion of CaV1.2 does not appear to impact the sAHP [16]. Researchers have also used molecular and biochemical techniques to elucidate the contribution of the L-VGCC subtypes to the observed age-related increase in calcium current and channel density. The majority of work, which has been performed in the hippocampus of rats, has revealed an increase in the amount of CaV1.3 mRNA [34, 35] and/or protein [36, 37], although recent evidence suggests that surface levels [38] and/or phosphorylation [38, 39] of CaV1.2 may be increased in some hippocampal subfields. However, single-cell analysis using reverse transcription polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR) showed that the magnitude of the increase in CaV1.3 mRNA from individual hippocampal neurons correlated with the magnitude of the recorded calcium current [34], and additionally, the increase in CaV1.3 protein expression has been shown to be inversely correlated with the degree of impairment in the Morris water maze [37]. Further, aged mice, unlike aged rats, do not exhibit increased phosphorylation of CaV1.2 (at serine-1928), which can increase the calcium current mediated by this channel, nor do they exhibit changes in expression levels of CaV1.2 [27].
Taken together, the available evidence, especially in mice, strongly suggests that an increase in CaV1.3 is a primary contributor to the age-related dysregulation of neuronal calcium that underlies brain aging. In light of these observations, we have designed a novel line of transgenic mice that are engineered to over-express CaV1.3 in excitatory forebrain neurons, including the hippocampus and cortex. Here we describe the generation of these mice and their initial behavioral characterization, which demonstrates that they have no overt physical or non-cognitive deficits. Thus, we anticipate that these mice will be useful in studies investigating the putative role of CaV1.3 in “normal” brain aging as well as in studies aimed at determining the impact of age-related dysregulation of Ca2+ homeostasis on diseases in which age is a primary risk factor.
Materials and Methods
2.1 Transgene construction and generation of transgenic mice
The general cloning strategy is similar to that previously described [40, 41]. The original construct (sHA-CaV1.3a; a kind gift from I. Bezprozvanny, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX) was provided in a pCDNA6/V5-hisB plasmid and contained the full length rat CaV1.3 cDNA that included a surface-expressed hemagglutinin (HA) epitope, which has previously been shown not to affect protein folding, trafficking to the cellular membrane, cell surface levels, or channel function [42, 43]. The pCDNA6/V5-hisB/sHA-CaV1.3a plasmid was initially modified to replace the 5’ NheI site just adjacent to the ATG start codon with an XmaI site and a 3’ flanking SacII site was converted to an FseI site. These sites were used to excise the full length transgene, which was subsequently ligated into the multiple cloning site (MCS) of pNN265 that had been previously modified by the addition of a XmaI site 5’ adjacent to an existing FseI site. Thus, the resulting plasmid contained the full length CaV1.3HA transgene flanked by both a 5’ and a 3’ synthetic intron, which were included to help stabilize the mRNA and increase transgene expression during transcription [44]. In addition, the 3’ synthetic intron contained the a poly(A) signal from simian virus 40 necessary for mRNA translocation out of the nucleus. The CaV1.3 transgene and synthetic UTR sequences (a 7218bp fragment) were excised from the plasmid using two NotI sites that flanked the entire construct. This fragment was gel purified and subsequently cloned into a NotI site downstream of the full-length (8.5kb) rat alpha Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II (αCaMKII) promoter in vector pMM403 (both pNN265 and pMM403 were kindly provided by M. Mayford, The Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA). We elected to use the αCaMKII promoter because it has been shown to effectively localize transgene expression to forebrain glutamatergic neurons [40, 41]. The resulting plasmid (pJNS01) containing the αCaMKII promoter, the 3’ and 5’ synthetic UTRs, and the CaV1.3HA sequence, was approximately 18 kb in size. Proper orientation of the transgene was confirmed by sequencing.
Approximately 50µg of pJNS01 was digested with SfiI, removing the pBS backbone fragment (3 kb) which originated from pMM403. The injection construct (15 kb), which included the αCaMKII promoter, synthetic UTRs, and CaV1.3HA cDNA (Figure 1A), was purified using the SNAP UV-Free Extraction Kit and sent to the University of Michigan Transgenic Core for pronuclear injection into fertilized C57BL/6 eggs that were then transferred into C57BL/6 pseudopregnant female mice.
2.2 Genotyping
Mice were genotyped for the presence of the CaV1.3HA transgene by polymerase chain reaction (PCR). After tail samples were digested overnight at 55°C in One-Step Tail Buffer (50mM KCl, 10mM Tris-HCl, 0.1% Triton X-100, 0.15mg/mL Proteinase K, pH 9.0), PCR was performed using GoTaq Green Master Mix (Promega, Madison, WI) with the following parameters: initial denaturation for 5:00 at 94°C, 30 cycles (0:30 at 94°C, 0:30 at 60°C, 1:00 at 72°C), and final elongation for 5:00 at 72°C. Primers for transgene were designed to span a ~350bp region in the synthetic 3’ UTR (Figure 1A), which ensured that endogenous CaV1.3 DNA would not be detected. Primers for β-actin, spanning ~250bp, were also included as a control for the presence of DNA in the tail sample. Transgene primers were (CCACACCTCCCCCTGAACCTGAAACATAA) and (TGGAGGGTGTAATGAGTTCGGAGAC). Primers for β-actin were (CTCTCAGCTGTGGTGGTGAA) and (GCTCCCATTCATCAGTTCCATAGGT).
Mice that were genotyped as positive for the CaV1.3HA transgene are herein referred to as CaV1.3HA mice, and mice that were genotyped as negative for the CaV1.3HA transgene are referred to as wild-type (WT) mice. All mice were fed ad libitum and kept on a 14:10-hour light:dark schedule, with husbandry provided by the veterinarians and staff of the Unit for Laboratory Animal Medicine (ULAM) at the University of Michigan. Experimental procedures were approved by and performed in accordance with the University of Michigan Committee on Use and Care of Animals (UCUCA).
2.3 Western blot analysis
To detect the presence of the CaV1.3HA transgenic protein, Western blotting was performed on membrane fractions prepared from CaV1.3HA and WT littermate mice. After being deeply anesthetized using isoflurane (VetOne, Boise, ID), each mouse was decapitated and the brain was rapidly removed, bisected, and placed in a petri dish submerged in ice-cold phosphate-buffered saline (PBS; 20mM phosphate, 150mM NaCl, pH 7.4). The hippocampus, cortex, and cerebellum were isolated and placed in separate 1.5mL microcentrifuge tubes containing 500µL of ice-cold HEPES sucrose EDTA buffer (HSE; 10mM HEPES, 350mM Sucrose, 5mM ETDA) with a protease inhibitor (cOmplete; Roche Diagnostics, Indianapolis, IN). Samples were homogenized with a pestle and centrifuged at 6,000rpm at 4°C for 5 minutes. The resulting supernatant was then transferred to a 10.4mL ultracentrifuge tube containing HSE buffer, and centrifuged at 100,000 × g at 4°C for 1 hour. The supernatant was carefully removed, and the pellet was resuspended in 200µL HSE buffer with cOmplete protease inhibitor and 1% Triton X (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) to solubilize membranes proteins. Protein quantification was performed using the Bradford assay (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA) with bovine serum albumin (BSA) as a standard. Samples were solubilized in Laemmli Buffer (Bio-Rad Hercules, CA), then boiled for five minutes at 95°C to denature the proteins. Proteins (20–30µg total protein loaded per lane) were separated on a 7.5% SDS-PAGE gel (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA) run at 65V for 3 hours, and then transferred to a PVDF membrane overnight. To detect transgenic protein, the gel was first incubated overnight at 4°C with mouse anti-HA primary antibody (1:1000; Covance, Princeton, NJ), then washed 3 times for 10 minutes each in PBS with 0.1% Tween 20 (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), and finally incubated at room temperature for 1 hour with horseradish-peroxidase (HRP) conjugated goat anti-mouse secondary antibody (1:5000; Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA). Following additional washes (3 × 10 min in PBS with 0.1% Tween 20), immunoreactivity was visualized using Amersham Prime ECL (GE Healthcare, Waukesha, WI). To generate a loading control, the blot was then stripped and reprobed with mouse anti-transferrin primary antibody (1:500; Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) and HRP-conjugated goat anti-mouse secondary antibody (both using the same protocols as above). The blot was again visualized using Amersham Prime ECL.
To detect CaV1.3 protein (both endogenous and transgenic), Western blotting was performed on samples prepared from CaV1.3HA and WT littermate mice. After being deeply anesthetized using isoflurane, each mouse was decapitated, the brain was rapidly removed and then submerged in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF, in mM: 120 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 2.5 CaCl2, 1 MgSO4, 10 glucose, 0.4 ascorbic acid, pH 7.3). The forebrain was harvested, transferred to a Dounce tissue grinder containing 1mL ice-cold HSE buffer with cOmplete protease inhibitor, and homogenized. Homogenates were then centrifuged at 5,000rpm at 4°C for 5 minutes after which the supernatant was moved to a new tube and the pellet was discarded. Protein quantification was performed using the Bradford assay with BSA as a standard. Samples were solubilized in Laemmli Buffer and boiled for five minutes at 95°C to denature the proteins. Proteins (25µg of total protein loaded per lane) were separated on a 4–15% gradient SDS-PAGE gel (run at 65V for three hours) and then transferred to a PVDF membrane overnight. Before antibody incubation the membranes were cut at approximately 100kD; then, the section containing the larger proteins was incubated overnight at 4°C with rabbit anti-Cav1.3 primary antibody (1:3000; [45]), while the section containing the smaller proteins was incubated overnight at 4°C with rabbit anti-β-actin (1:2000; ThermoFisher Scientific, Wayne, MI), which served as a loading control. After primary antibody incubation, membranes were washed 3 times for 10 minutes each in PBS with 0.1% Tween 20, and then incubated at room temperature for 1 hour with HRP-conjugated goat anti-rabbit secondary antibody (1:5000; Milipore, Temecula, CA). Following additional washes (3 × 10 min in PBS with 0.1% Tween 20), immunoreactivity was visualized using Amersham Prime ECL (GE Healthcare, Waukesha, WI).
2.4 Neurobattery
At least one week before the start of behavioral characterization, mice were moved to a holding room near the behavior testing facilities so they could acclimate. The same cohort of mice, which consisted of CaV1.3HA (n=13) and WT (n=18) littermates ranging in age from 3–5 months (average age: CaV1.3HA = 3.5 ± 0.2mo, WT = 3.4 ± 0.1mo), was used for all of the behavioral testing. Experimenters were blind to the genotype of the mice during behavioral testing. All testing was performed at approximately the same time each day, during the animals’ light cycle.
2.4.1 Open Field
The open field arena was a 71 × 71cm white acrylic box placed in a room lit by indirect white light. Mice were singly placed in the center of the box and allowed to explore for five minutes, during which time activity was recorded with LimeLight2 software (Actimetrics, Evanston, IL). The arena was thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol between each trial. For analysis, the arena was divided into an “edge” zone (a 10cm perimeter at the periphery of the arena, next to the wall) and a “center” zone, and distance traveled as well as time spent in each zone were computed by LimeLight2.
2.4.2 Light/Dark Box
The light/dark box was a 46 × 37cm rectangle made of acrylic placed in a room lit by indirect white light. The box was divided into two zones: a dark zone (approximately 1/3 of the total area), made of black acrylic with a black lid that remained closed, and a light zone (approximately 2/3 of the total area), which was made of white acrylic and was open at the top. A small opening in the center of the black acrylic wall separating the two zones allowed mice to cross from one zone into the other. Mice were singly placed in the center of the light zone and allowed to explore for five minutes, during which time activity was recorded with LimeLight2. The open field was thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol between trials for each animal. The percent time spent in each zone and the number of transitions (crossings) between the light and dark zones were analyzed using LimeLight2.
2.4.3 Rotarod
Mice were placed on a rotating rod that accelerated from 4 to 40 rpm over a five-minute time period (Stoelting, Chicago, IL) in groups of up to five (each cage of mice was run together, with 2–5 mice per cage), separated by dividers. Mice were given five trials over the course of one day, with approximately 1.5hr between each trial. Latency to fall was recorded by the experimenter, with a maximum of five minutes per trial.
2.5 Data Analysis
ImageJ (National Institutes of Health, Bethesda MD) was used to analyze pixel density of Western blot images. Data collected from behavioral experiments was analyzed with LimeLight2 software. StatView (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) or GraphPad Prism 6 (GraphPad Software, Inc., La Jolla, CA) was used to calculate statistical significance (α =0.05) using Student’s t-test, 2-factor ANOVA, or repeated measures ANOVA, as appropriate.
Results
An increase in expression of the L-VGCC, CaV1.3, has been repeatedly demonstrated in the brains of aged rodents [34–37] and is thought to represent a key aspect of the aged brain [9–11]. In order to determine the relative contribution of this alteration to age-related changes in brain function without the confounds of other pathologies associated with aging, we generated the CaV1.3HA transgenic mouse line to genetically mimic the age-related increase in CaV1.3. Here we describe the establishment and initial characterization of the CaV1.3HA transgenic mouse line, showing that there is an approximately 50% increase in CaV1.3 in forebrain regions and that these mice have normal baseline behavioral function. Thus these mice represent a useful genetic tool with which to investigate the mechanisms of brain function in a variety of physiological conditions and diseases that are impacted by aging.
3.1 Generation of injection construct and transgenic mouse lines
In order to generate transgenic mouse lines, we first synthesized an injection construct, which contained the HA-tagged CaV1.3 transgene flanked by synthetic 5’ and 3’UTR regions downstream of the αCaMKII promoter (Figure 1A). Importantly, the surface-expressed HA epitope (located on the extracellular side of domain II; Figure 1B) allows the transgenic protein (CaV1.3HA) to be distinguished from endogenous CaV1.3 protein and the positioning of the HA epitope in this region does not affect the cell surface levels or functional properties of the channel [42, 43]. The synthetic 5’ and 3’ UTRS were included because they have been shown to help stabilize mRNA and to increase transgene expression during transcription [44], and also to provide the poly-A sequence needed for mRNA translocation out of the nucleus. We chose to use the αCaMKII promoter for two main reasons: 1) the expression of αCaMKII (and therefore expression of the CaV1.3HA protein) peaks postnatally (around p7–p10) [46–48], which limits potential deleterious effects of transgenic protein expression during embryonic development ; and 2) the αCaMKII promoter has been shown to localize transgenic protein expression to excitatory forebrain neurons in transgenic animals [40, 48], which confines expression of CaV1.3HA protein to the population of neurons in which expression of CaV1.3 has been examined in aged rodents [34, 36, 37]. In addition, this minimizes the possibility that transgene expression in the cerebellum might interfere with normal motor function, which could confound behavioral tasks.
The linearized construct was sent to the University of Michigan Transgenic Core for pronuclear injection into fertilized eggs, which were then implanted into C57BL/6 pseudopregnant female mice. Out of 18 individual injection and implantation procedures, 6 pups (4 male, 2 female) that were genotyped as positive for the CaV1.3HA transgene (CaV1.3HA mice) were obtained. Each one of these CaV1.3HA mice (referred to as founders) was paired with a C57BL/6NTac mouse (Taconic Laboratories, Cambridge City, IN) of the opposite sex to establish individual mouse lines. The 2 female founders did not produce any viable progeny, but all 4 male founders produced pups that lived to be weaned. Of these, 3 of the 4 lines contained progeny that were genotyped as positive for the CaV1.3HA transgene. However, Western blot analysis detected CaV1.3HA transgenic protein expression in only 2 of these 3 lines (see below). Thus, this procedure has established two viable transgenic mouse lines that express exogenous CaV1.3HA protein and therefore represent candidates for a mouse model of an aged brain (see Table 1).
Table 1. Establishment of CaV1.3HA transgenic mouse lines.
Founding Mouse Number |
Sex of Founder |
Weaned Progeny? |
Transgene in Progeny? |
Transgenic Protein in Progeny? |
---|---|---|---|---|
100 | Male | yes | yes | no |
101 | Male | yes | no | n/a |
102 | Male | yes | yes | yes |
103 | Male | yes | yes | yes |
104 | Female | no | n/a | n/a |
105 | Female | no | n/a | n/a |
3.2 Characterization of CaV1.3HA transgene expression
Our initial characterization efforts focused on CaV1.3HA mice from line 102. First, we determined the regional specificity of transgenic protein expression by Western blot analysis using an antibody against the HA epitope (anti-HA). To ensure that we were detecting CaV1.3HA transgenic protein that had been successfully folded, appropriately trafficked, and inserted into the plasma membrane, we prepared membrane fractions from cortex, hippocampus, and cerebellum of CaV1.3HA mice and their WT littermates. A band was detected at approximately 250kD (the same size as endogenous CaV1.3 protein) in both cortical and hippocampal, but not cerebellar, membrane fractions from CaV1.3HA but not WT mice (Figure 2A). This experiment demonstrates that transgenic protein expression is restricted to forebrain regions, due to the specificity of the αCaMKII promoter, and that the HA antibody specifically detects only transgenic, and not endogenous, protein.
While we predicted that transgene expression would result in an increase in total CaV1.3 protein, it is possible that endogenous CaV1.3 would be down-regulated in areas where the transgene was expressed, possibly as a result of a compensatory or homeostatic mechanism. Therefore, to measure total CaV1.3 protein levels, we performed Western blot analysis on whole brain samples (including cortex and hippocampus, but not cerebellum) using an antibody against CaV1.3 (anti-CaV1.3), which will identify both endogenous CaV1.3 and transgenic CaV1.3HA protein. A band was detected at approximately 250kD in both CaV1.3HA and WT mice, demonstrating the presence of CaV1.3 in both genotypes, but more protein was evident in the lane from CaV1.3HA mice (Figure 2B1). This strongly suggests that expression of the CaV1.3HA transgene did not cause down-regulation of endogenous CaV1.3 protein and instead resulted in an increase in total CaV1.3 protein.
Finally, to quantify the increase in CaV1.3 expression in transgenic mice, Western blots were performed on whole-brain samples (including cortex and hippocampus, but not cerebellum) from five CaV1.3HA mice and five WT littermates. Blots were probed with anti-CaV1.3 (which will detect both transgenic protein and endogenous protein) and with anti-β-actin (to serve as a loading control). The resulting blot was subjected to densitometric analysis using ImageJ software whereby the pixel density of each protein band (CaV1.3 and β-actin) was measured. Then the ratio of the CaV1.3 band to the β-actin band (CaV1.3: β-actin) was normalized to the average CaV1.3: β-actin ratio for all the WT samples. This analysis revealed that there was a significant increase in CaV1.3 protein expression in CaV1.3HA mice, relative to their WT littermates, of approximately 50% (p < 0.05, Student’s t-test; Figure 2B2).
3.3 Initial behavioral characterization of the CaV1.3HA transgenic mouse line
In addition to the Western blot analysis described above, a separate cohort containing CaV1.3HA (n=13) and WT (n=18) littermate mice was characterized in a series of standard neurobattery tests to determine whether CaV1.3 transgene expression resulted in any overt alterations in exploration, locomotion, anxiety-like behavior, or motor learning.
3.3.1 Open Field
The open field test was performed to assess overall exploratory and anxiety-like behavior. Mice were individually placed in the open field for a five-minute trial, during which they were allowed to freely explore the arena while being recorded with LimeLight2 tracking software. Subsequent analysis revealed no difference between groups in the total distance traveled (Figure 3A1), suggesting that expression of the CaV1.3 transgene did not affect exploratory behavior and that CaV1.3HA mice are neither hyper- nor hypo-mobile. Further, dividing the arena into an “edge” zone (by the arena wall) and “center” zone showed that although both CaV1.3HA and WT mice spent significantly more time in the “edge” zone (which is typical behavior for mice, who generally avoid open spaces), there was no difference between genotypes in the amount of time spent in either zone (Figure 3A2), suggesting CaV1.3HA mice do not exhibit altered anxiety-like behavior.
3.3.2 Light/Dark Box
While both exploratory and anxiety-like behavior can be assayed in the open field test, the light/dark box is generally considered a more sensitive measure of anxiety. Therefore, the CaV1.3HA and WT mice were also assessed in the light/dark box, where each mouse received a five-minute trial during which they were allowed to freely explore the arena while being recorded with LimeLight2 tracking software. Similar to the results from open field, although both CaV1.3HA and WT mice spent significantly more time on the dark side compared to the light side of the box (which is also typical behavior for mice, who generally avoid brightly lit spaces), there was no difference between genotypes in the amount of time spent in either the light or dark areas (Figure 3B1) or in the number of crossings between the light and dark sides (Figure 3B2). These data provide further strong evidence that CaV1.3HA mice, like WT mice, prefer closed, dark spaces but are willing to explore other areas to a similar extent as their WT counterparts, and thus do not exhibit over- or under-exaggerated anxiety-like behavior.
3.3.3 Rotarod
The accelerating rotarod was used to assess both gross locomotor function as well as motor learning. CaV1.3HA and WT cagemates were placed on a slowly (4rpm) rotating rod, separated by dividers. Over a 5-minute (300s) period, the rotation speed accelerated from 4 to 40rpm, and the latency to fall off the rod was recorded. Mice were given 5 trials in 1 day, with an inter-trial interval of approximately 1.5 hours. Performance (latency to fall) on the first trial was not different between genotypes, indicating that CaV1.3HA mice have normal gross locomotor function (Figure 3C). Additionally, over the course of training, both CaV1.3HA and WT mice exhibited a significant increase in latency to fall, but their performance was not different from each other, which demonstrates that mice in both groups display normal motor learning. These results are consistent with our Western blot analysis showing that expression of the transgenic protein was restricted to the forebrain by the αCamKII promoter as the cerebellum is critically involved in both locomotor performance and motor learning. Finally, because body weight can affect motor performance and differences may occlude the observation of a behavioral phenotype, a subset of mice used in neurobattery experiments were weighed after completion of the rotarod task (Figure 3D). There was no significant difference in weight between the CaV1.3HA and WT mice, further suggesting that expression of the CaV1.3 transgene does not result in any gross physical defects or deficits in motor function.
Discussion
The transgenic model of an aged brain presented here has several distinct advantages over obtaining aged mice from suppliers or aging mice in-house. Perhaps the most practical concerns are cost and time. Buying aged mice from suppliers is often prohibitively expensive, and while aged mice can be obtained from some institutions at little to no cost (e.g. National Institute on Aging), there are restrictions on the number of mice available to investigators. Further, only wild-type mice are available, so it is not possible to use these resources to examine aged mice that have been genetically modified or treated with pharmacological agents. Instead, researchers must bred their own mice, or at least obtain transgenic or treated mice at substantially younger ages, and then age them in their own facilities, which requires paying per diem costs for mouse housing and husbandry (typically $0.70 – $1.00 a day per cage) that can exceed $500 for a single cage of 24-month old mice. This cost is compounded by the fact that not all mice will survive to the 24-month mark, thus requiring an additional allocation of mice (often 50% or more of the total number needed for the experiment) simply to account for mouse mortality. Considering that each experiment therefore requires 5–10 cages of mice per condition, the total cost for one experiment can easily exceed several thousand dollars. Additionally, breeding mice to be used for studies that involve aging, by definition, takes many months or years so it can be difficult and time-consuming to test different experimental manipulations. Therefore, using a transgenic model of an aged brain means that mice can be used at much younger ages, saving time and money, as well as allowing a greater extent of freedom in investigating multiple experimental manipulations.
Of course, “aging” is a complicated and multifaceted process, comprising myriad changes in the body and the brain. Because studying only one aspect of aging in isolation does not reproduce the entire aging milieu, it may be argued that this approach will yield limited insight into the function of the aged brain. However, precisely because “aging” encompasses a large number of variables, even with well-controlled experiments that focus on individual aspects, it is difficult to enumerate the relative contribution of distinct mechanisms to age-related alterations and to elucidate potential therapeutic targets that may ameliorate declines in function. For example, there is substantial interest in identifying mechanisms that contribute to age-related cognitive decline – especially impairments in learning and memory – which has been repeatedly demonstrated in humans, non-human primates, and rodents [4, 49]. In elderly human subjects, cognitive capacity can be determined using a wide variety of assessment tools [50], but most utilize verbal assessment (e.g. mini mental state exam). In contrast, almost all of the behavioral tasks that have been developed to study cognition in animal models like mice require some form of motor output (or suppression of motor output). Although we and others typically go to great lengths to control for deficits in motor ability and/or alterations in emotional reactivity, it is difficult if not impossible to completely remove the inherent confounds of using aged animals on the interpretation of cognitive performance. Therefore, the lack of any phenotypic deficits in the neurobattery of the CaV1.3HA mice compared to their WT littermates represents an important advantage of this model in that future studies utilizing these mice will not be confounded by other age-related changes that may confuse or obscure the interpretation of experimental results in the context of cognitive function.
One of the biggest advantages of the CaV1.3HA transgenic mouse line is that it can be combined with other mouse models that emulate distinct aspects of normal aging or pathology to investigate the impact of an aged brain in a variety of conditions. For example, advancing age is the leading risk factor for many neurodegenerative disorders, including Alzheimer’s disease (AD), Parkinson’s disease (PD), and Huntington’s disease (HD) [51–53]. However, because of the time and cost of aging animals to time points that would be comparable to the average age of disease onset in humans (e.g. approximately 2 years or more in rodents), many studies utilize genetic models of disease that are designed to be aggressive and manifest at relatively young ages (e.g. 2–6 months in rodents), which then lack important aspects of the aged phenotype that may fundamentally contribute to the disease in humans. While this approach has provided important information about some of the mechanisms involved, it remains likely that the brain environment (i.e. a “young brain” versus an “aged brain”) substantially impacts the initiation, progression, and cognitive outcomes associated with these diseases. Thus, using the CaV1.3HA mouse line as a background on which to investigate the cellular and molecular mechanisms will likely provide a more representative and comprehensive understanding of these diseases. In addition, transgenic mouse models that mimic different age-related insults, such as an increase in reactive oxygen species (ROS) [54–56], changes in vascular function [57–60], or disrupted cholinergic signaling [61–64], could be crossed with CaV1.3HA mice to investigate the impact of interactions between different age-related alterations on cognitive outcomes. Taken together, the CaV1.3HA transgenic mouse line represents a significant step forward in the development of methodologies that will allow consideration of crucial aspects of the aged brain on both “normal” aging and in pathological conditions, improving our understanding of these processes and elucidating potential therapeutic interventions to ameliorate age-related cognitive decline.
Conclusions
We have successfully generated a novel transgenic mouse line in which expression of the L-VGCC subtype CaV1.3 is increased by approximately 50% in forebrain tissue, which mimics a key aspect of the aged brain. Notably, the transgenic CaV1.3 protein includes a surface-expressed HA epitope, which allows endogenous CaV1.3 to be distinguished from transgenic CaV1.3 protein. Importantly, mice positive for the transgene (CaV1.3HA mice) exhibit no gross exploratory, locomotor, or non-cognitive deficits compared to their WT littermates, suggesting that their performance in behavioral tasks can be successfully assessed without being impacted by other age-related pathologies. Thus, CaV1.3HA mice represent a promising model that will facilitate studies in which age is an important factor without the time, expense, and additional confounds associated with directly aging animals. We anticipate that the use of CaV1.3HA mice in future studies, either independently or in concert with other transgenic mouse models, will provide new insights into the mechanisms underlying the disruption of cognitive function that occurs with “normal” aging or in combination with other age-related diseases.
Highlights.
Generation of a novel transgenic mouse model of the “aged brain”
Expression of HA-tagged CaV1.3 driven by αCamKII promoter
Results in ~50% increase in CaV1.3 channels in forebrain tissue
Mice exhibit normal behavioral responses in a series of neurobattery tasks
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank Dr. Mark Mayford and Dr. Ilya Bezprozvanny for their generous gifts of plasmids. This work was funded by NIH (to GGM: R01AG052934 and R01AG028488; to AL: NS084190, DC009433) and the University of Michigan Endowment for Basic Science.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Cesari MJ. The demographic transition of the United States. Hauppauge, N.Y.: Nova Science Publishers; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hosokawa M, Abe T, Higuchi K, Shimakawa K, Omori Y, Matsushita T, et al. Management and design of the maintenance of SAM mouse strains: an animal model for accelerated senescence and age-associated disorders. Exp Gerontol. 1997;32:111–116. doi: 10.1016/s0531-5565(96)00078-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Takeda T. Senescence-accelerated mouse (SAM): a biogerontological resource in aging research. Neurobiol Aging. 1999;20:105–110. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(99)00008-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Barnes CA. Aging and the physiology of spatial memory. Neurobiol Aging. 1988;9:563–568. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(88)80114-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Butterfield DA, Poon HF. The senescence-accelerated prone mouse (SAMP8): a model of age-related cognitive decline with relevance to alterations of the gene expression and protein abnormalities in Alzheimer's disease. Exp Gerontol. 2005;40:774–783. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2005.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Blalock EM, Chen KC, Sharrow K, Herman JP, Porter NM, Foster TC, et al. Gene microarrays in hippocampal aging: statistical profiling identifies novel processes correlated with cognitive impairment. J Neurosci. 2003;23:3807–3819. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-09-03807.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kadish I, Thibault O, Blalock EM, Chen KC, Gant JC, Porter NM, et al. Hippocampal and cognitive aging across the lifespan: a bioenergetic shift precedes and increased cholesterol trafficking parallels memory impairment. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1805–1816. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4599-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Rowe WB, Blalock EM, Chen KC, Kadish I, Wang D, Barrett JE, et al. Hippocampal expression analyses reveal selective association of immediate-early, neuroenergetic, and myelinogenic pathways with cognitive impairment in aged rats. J Neurosci. 2007;27:3098–3110. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4163-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Foster TC, Kumar A. Calcium dysregulation in the aging brain. Neuroscientist. 2002;8:297–301. doi: 10.1177/107385840200800404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Khachaturian ZS. Toward theories of brain aging. In: Kay DWK, Burrows GD, editors. Handbook of studies on psychiatry and old age. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1984. pp. 7–30. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Khachaturian ZS, Cotman CW, Pettegrew JW. Calcium, membranes, aging, and Alzheimer's disease. New York, N.Y.: New York Academy of Sciences; 1989. John Douglas French Foundation for Alzheimer's Disease., Pharmaceutical Manufacturers Association., National Institute on Aging. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Clapham DE. Calcium signaling. Cell. 2007;131:1047–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Simms BA, Zamponi GW. Neuronal voltage-gated calcium channels: structure, function, and dysfunction. Neuron. 2014;82:24–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Barbado M, Fablet K, Ronjat M, De Waard M. Gene regulation by voltage-dependent calcium channels. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2009;1793:1096–1104. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamcr.2009.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Alonso MT, Garcia-Sancho J. Nuclear Ca(2+) signalling. Cell Calcium. 2011;49:280–289. doi: 10.1016/j.ceca.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gamelli AE. Deletion of the L-type calcium channel CaV1.3 but not CaV1.2 results in a diminished sAHP in mouse CA1 pyramidal neurons. Hippocampus. 2011;21:133–141. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Thibault O, Hadley R, Landfield PW. Elevated postsynaptic [Ca2+]i and L-type calcium channel activity in aged hippocampal neurons: relationship to impaired synaptic plasticity. J Neurosci. 2001;21:9744–9756. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09744.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Thibault O, Landfield PW. Increase in single L-type calcium channels in hippocampal neurons during aging. Science. 1996;272:1017–1020. doi: 10.1126/science.272.5264.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Madison DV, Nicoll RA. Control of the repetitive discharge of rat CA 1 pyramidal neurones in vitro. J Physiol. 1984;354:319–331. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bowden SE, Fletcher S, Loane DJ, Marrion NV. Somatic colocalization of rat SK1 and D class (CaV1.2) L-type calcium channels in rat CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 2001;21:RC175. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-20-j0006.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lima PA, Marrion NV. Mechanisms underlying activation of the slow AHP in rat hippocampal neurons. Brain Res. 2007;1150:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Marrion NV, Tavalin SJ. Selective activation of Ca2+-activated K+ channels by co-localized Ca2+ channels in hippocampal neurons. Nature. 1998;395:900–905. doi: 10.1038/27674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Goldberg JA, Wilson CJ. Control of spontaneous firing patterns by the selective coupling of calcium currents to calcium-activated potassium currents in striatal cholinergic interneurons. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10230–10238. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2734-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Moyer JR, Jr, Thompson LT, Black JP, Disterhoft JF. Nimodipine increases excitability of rabbit CA1 pyramidal neurons in an age- and concentration-dependent manner. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:2100–2109. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.6.2100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Landfield PW, Pitler TA. Prolonged Ca2+-dependent afterhyperpolarizations in hippocampal neurons of aged rats. Science. 1984;226:1089–1092. doi: 10.1126/science.6494926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Power JM, Wu WW, Sametsky E, Oh MM, Disterhoft JF. Age-related enhancement of the slow outward calcium-activated potassium current in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons in vitro. J Neurosci. 2002;22:7234–7243. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-16-07234.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Murphy GG, Shah V, Hell JW, Silva AJ. Investigation of age-related cognitive decline using mice as a model system: neurophysiological correlates. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;14:1012–1021. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000209404.54310.b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Moyer JR, Jr, Power JM, Thompson LT, Disterhoft JF. Increased excitability of aged rabbit CA1 neurons after trace eyeblink conditioning. J Neurosci. 2000;20:5476–5482. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-14-05476.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kaczorowski CC, Disterhoft JF. Memory deficits are associated with impaired ability to modulate neuronal excitability in middle-aged mice. Learn Mem. 2009;16:362–366. doi: 10.1101/lm.1365609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hell JW, Westenbroek RE, Warner C, Ahlijanian MK, Prystay W, Gilbert MM, et al. Identification and differential subcellular localization of the neuronal class C and class D L-type calcium channel alpha 1 subunits. J Cell Biol. 1993;123:949–962. doi: 10.1083/jcb.123.4.949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Xu W, Lipscombe D. Neuronal Ca(V)1.3alpha(1) L-type channels activate at relatively hyperpolarized membrane potentials and are incompletely inhibited by dihydropyridines. J Neurosci. 2001;21:5944–5951. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-05944.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Zamponi GW, Striessnig J, Koschak A, Dolphin AC. The Physiology, Pathology, and Pharmacology of Voltage-Gated Calcium Channels and Their Future Therapeutic Potential. Pharmacol Rev. 2015;67:821–870. doi: 10.1124/pr.114.009654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.McKinney BC, Sze W, Lee B, Murphy GG. Impaired long-term potentiation and enhanced neuronal excitability in the amygdala of CaV1.3 knockout mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009;92:519–528. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Chen KC, Blalock EM, Thibault O, Kaminker P, Landfield PW. Expression of α1D subunit mRNA is correlated with L-type Ca2+ channel activity in single neurons of hippocampal "zipper" slices. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:4357–4362. doi: 10.1073/pnas.070056097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Herman JP, Chen KC, Booze R, Landfield PW. Up-regulation of α1D Ca2+ channel subunit mRNA expression in the hippocampus of aged F344 rats. Neurobiol Aging. 1998;19:581–587. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(98)00099-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Veng LM, Browning MD. Regionally selective alterations in expression of the α1D subunit (CaV1.3) of L-type calcium channels in the hippocampus of aged rats. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2002;107:120–127. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(02)00453-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Veng LM, Mesches MH, Browning MD. Age-related working memory impairment is correlated with increases in the L-type calcium channel protein α1D (CaV1.3) in area CA1 of the hippocampus and both are ameliorated by chronic nimodipine treatment. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2003;110:193–202. doi: 10.1016/s0169-328x(02)00643-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Nunez-Santana FL, Oh MM, Antion MD, Lee A, Hell JW, Disterhoft JF. Surface L-type Ca2+ channel expression levels are increased in aged hippocampus. Aging Cell. 2014;13:111–120. doi: 10.1111/acel.12157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Davare MA, Hell JW. Increased phosphorylation of the neuronal L-type Ca2+ channel CaV1.2 during aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:16018–16023. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2236970100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mayford M, Bach ME, Huang YY, Wang L, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER. Control of memory formation through regulated expression of a CaMKII transgene. Science. 1996;274:1678–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5293.1678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kushner SA, Elgersma Y, Murphy GG, Jaarsma D, van Woerden GM, Hojjati MR, et al. Modulation of presynaptic plasticity and learning by the H-ras/extracellular signal-regulated kinase/synapsin I signaling pathway. J Neurosci. 2005;25:9721–9734. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2836-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Zhang H. CaV1.2 and CaV1.3 neuronal L-type calcium channels: differential targeting and signaling to pCREB. The European journal of neuroscience. 2006;23:2297–2310. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04734.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Altier C, Dubel SJ, Barrere C, Jarvis SE, Stotz SC, Spaetgens RL, et al. Trafficking of L-type calcium channels mediated by the postsynaptic scaffolding protein AKAP79. J Biol Chem. 2002;277:33598–33603. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M202476200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Callis J. Introns increase gene expression in cultured maize cells. Genes & development. 1987;1:1183–1200. doi: 10.1101/gad.1.10.1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gregory FD, Bryan KE, Pangrsic T, Calin-Jageman IE, Moser T, Lee A. Harmonin inhibits presynaptic Cav1.3 Ca(2)(+) channels in mouse inner hair cells. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1109–1111. doi: 10.1038/nn.2895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Burgin KE, Waxham MN, Rickling S, Westgate SA, Mobley WC, Kelly PT. In situ hybridization histochemistry of Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase in developing rat brain. J Neurosci. 1990;10:1788–1798. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-06-01788.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Liu W, Chang L, Song Y, Gao X, Ling W, Lu T, et al. Immunolocalization of CaMKII and NR2B in hippocampal subregions of rat during postnatal development. Acta Histochem. 2013;115:264–272. doi: 10.1016/j.acthis.2012.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hanson PI, Schulman H. Neuronal Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinases. Annu Rev Biochem. 1992;61:559–601. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bi.61.070192.003015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cognitive Aging: Progress in Understanding and Opportunities for Action. Washington D.C.: 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Woodford HJ, George J. Cognitive assessment in the elderly: a review of clinical methods. QJM. 2007;100:469–484. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcm051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Bishop NA, Lu T, Yankner BA. Neural mechanisms of ageing and cognitive decline. Nature. 2010;464:529–535. doi: 10.1038/nature08983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Douglas PM, Dillin A. Protein homeostasis and aging in neurodegeneration. J Cell Biol. 2010;190:719–729. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201005144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Reeve A, Simcox E, Turnbull D. Ageing and Parkinson's disease: why is advancing age the biggest risk factor? Ageing Res Rev. 2014;14:19–30. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2014.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Massaad CA, Klann E. Reactive oxygen species in the regulation of synaptic plasticity and memory. Antioxid Redox Signal. 2011;14:2013–2054. doi: 10.1089/ars.2010.3208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Maier CM, Chan PH. Role of superoxide dismutases in oxidative damage and neurodegenerative disorders. Neuroscientist. 2002;8:323–334. doi: 10.1177/107385840200800408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Melov S, Coskun PE, Wallace DC. Mouse models of mitochondrial disease, oxidative stress, and senescence. Mutat Res. 1999;434:233–242. doi: 10.1016/s0921-8777(99)00031-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Iadecola C, Davisson RL. Hypertension and cerebrovascular dysfunction. Cell Metab. 2008;7:476–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2008.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Jiwa NS, Garrard P, Hainsworth AH. Experimental models of vascular dementia and vascular cognitive impairment: a systematic review. J Neurochem. 2010;115:814–828. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2010.06958.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Popescu BO, Toescu EC, Popescu LM, Bajenaru O, Muresanu DF, Schultzberg M, et al. Blood-brain barrier alterations in ageing and dementia. J Neurol Sci. 2009;283:99–106. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.02.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wiesmann M, Kiliaan AJ, Claassen JA. Vascular aspects of cognitive impairment and dementia. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2013;33:1696–1706. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kolisnyk B, Al-Onaizi MA, Hirata PH, Guzman MS, Nikolova S, Barbash S, et al. Forebrain deletion of the vesicular acetylcholine transporter results in deficits in executive function, metabolic, and RNA splicing abnormalities in the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33:14908–14920. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1933-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Prado VF, Roy A, Kolisnyk B, Gros R, Prado MA. Regulation of cholinergic activity by the vesicular acetylcholine transporter. Biochem J. 2013;450:265–274. doi: 10.1042/BJ20121662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Bazalakova MH, Blakely RD. The high-affinity choline transporter: a critical protein for sustaining cholinergic signaling as revealed in studies of genetically altered mice. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2006:525–544. doi: 10.1007/3-540-29784-7_21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bartus RT, Dean RL, 3rd, Beer B, Lippa AS. The cholinergic hypothesis of geriatric memory dysfunction. Science. 1982;217:408–414. doi: 10.1126/science.7046051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]